Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2017 Apr 4;6:e18558. doi: 10.7554/eLife.18558

A causal relationship between face-patch activity and face-detection behavior

Srivatsun Sadagopan 1, Wilbert Zarco 2, Winrich A Freiwald 2,*
Editor: Jack L Gallant3
PMCID: PMC5380432  PMID: 28375078

Abstract

The primate brain contains distinct areas densely populated by face-selective neurons. One of these, face-patch ML, contains neurons selective for contrast relationships between face parts. Such contrast-relationships can serve as powerful heuristics for face detection. However, it is unknown whether neurons with such selectivity actually support face-detection behavior. Here, we devised a naturalistic face-detection task and combined it with fMRI-guided pharmacological inactivation of ML to test whether ML is of critical importance for real-world face detection. We found that inactivation of ML impairs face detection. The effect was anatomically specific, as inactivation of areas outside ML did not affect face detection, and it was categorically specific, as inactivation of ML impaired face detection while sparing body and object detection. These results establish that ML function is crucial for detection of faces in natural scenes, performing a critical first step on which other face processing operations can build.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.18558.001

Research Organism: Rhesus Macaque

Introduction

For primates, faces are among the most behaviorally salient and socially relevant visual stimuli (Leopold and Rhodes, 2010). Reflecting this importance , the primate brain contains multiple interconnected cortical areas specialized for face processing, the face-patch network (Tsao et al., 2006, 2008). The middle-lateral face patch (ML), one of the earliest nodes in the face processing hierarchy, consists of neurons highly selective for faces (Tsao et al., 2006). Half of ML neurons exhibit sensitivity for coarse contrast relationships between face regions such as vertical adjacency of a bright forehead region above a darker eye region (Ohayon et al., 2012). Such qualitative contrast signatures for faces have been shown to provide a computationally powerful means for face detection (Sinha, 2002; Viola and Jones, 2004; Gilad et al., 2009), the most fundamental computation for all subsequent face-processing (Tsao and Livingstone, 2008). The hierarchically early location of face patch ML and the functional specificity of ML neurons led us to hypothesize that ML neurons might be critical for detection of faces, but not objects, in natural scenes. Testing this hypothesis required development of a naturalistic and demanding face and object detection task, and monitoring of real-world face-detection performance during causal manipulation of face patch ML.

Results and discussion

We first trained two macaques on a touchscreen-based object detection task. From a visual scene displayed on the touchscreen, subjects were required to select an object belonging to one of three target categories – human faces, macaque bodies (socially relevant control category represented near face patches; Popivanov et al., 2012), or shoes (newly learned object category that, like faces, exhibits high within-class similarity). The visual scene was composed of 24 objects, at one of ten visibility levels. To discourage the use of low-level features for solving the task, the face targets were randomly resized, rotated in-plane, and blended into a cluttered background at random locations (Figure 1A; see Materials and methods). Face patch ML was localized by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) using a standard face localizer. To inactivate ML, we stereotaxically targeted its fMRI-identified coordinates and infused 5 μL (8 μL in one experiment) of muscimol (5 μg/μL), a GABAA agonist. We co-infused the MR contrast agent gadolinium (Gd), the cortical spread of which closely tracks the diffusion extent of muscimol (Heiss et al., 2010; Wilke et al., 2012), as a surrogate tracer so that the extent of muscimol spread could be detected in an anatomical MR scan (Figure 1B). Overlap of muscimol diffusion with the functionally defined extent of ML was determined post-hoc by aligning the post-injection anatomical volume and previous functional scan data (see Materials and methods; Figure 1B–D).

Figure 1. Naturalistic face-detection task and targeted inactivation of face-patch ML.

Figure 1.

(A) Subjects performed a face-detection task on a touchscreen device that was mounted to their home cages. Subjects were unconstrained, initiated trials at will, and scanned the array freely. (B) Upon initiating a trial, an array of 24 objects appeared on the screen, one of which was the target belonging to one of three categories – faces, bodies, or shoes (green circles). (C) Neural activity in face-patch ML (yellow regions) was silenced by targeted microinjections of muscimol. (Top) The extent of diffusion and targeting accuracy was evaluated by coinjecting gadolinium, which appeared as a bright volume in post-injection anatomical scans. Labels: SAG - sagittal slice, HOR - horizontal slice, COR - coronal slice, A - anterior, P - posterior, D - dorsal, V – ventral. (Bottom) The locations of all four targeted muscimol microinjections (heatmap) from one subject's left hemisphere relative to face-patch locations (green outlines, face area labels as in Tsao et al., 2008). (D) Volumetric overlap between the inactivation injections and ML (red; n = 11) and off-target injections and ML (blue; n = 10). Lines correspond to ±1 s.e.m., and the intersection point is the mean along each axis. Symbols correspond to the mean values from individual hemispheres (circle – Subject ‘I’, LH; square – Subject ‘O’, RH; triangle – Subject ‘O’, LH).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.18558.002

The goal of the injections was to inactivate as much of ML as possible, with as little inactivation of outside regions as possible – with the converse goal for the off-target injections. To determine how well these goals were met, we computed the percentage overlap of injections with ML and the fraction of ML volume overlapping with the injection (Figure 1D). First, we determined the average volume of ML exceeding a threshold of t > 20 as ~58 mm3, corresponding to a ~5 mm-diameter sphere, consistent with published neurophysiological data (Aparicio et al., 2016). The average volume of the injection at a threshold of 50% peak Gd brightness was ~37 mm3, corresponding to a ~4 mm-diameter sphere. At the chosen thresholds, the theoretical maximum of volumetric ML coverage by the injection, assuming concentric spheres for both volumes, is 63.8%. We achieved 44% coverage on average. Of the total injection volume, 76% was contained within the ML volume (Figure 1D). The high overlap of target injections with ML suggests that we silenced a large fraction of highly face-selective neurons, primarily found in the center of the face patches (Aparicio et al., 2016). The corresponding numbers for off-target injections were 4% ML coverage with 10% of the injection contained within ML. It is thus unlikely that off-target injections, although overlapping slightly with ML, silenced many face-selective neurons. The subject was transferred to its home cage to perform the detection tasks between ~1 and 3 hr after muscimol injection and subsequent anatomical scan, well within the time period of maximal muscimol effects (~0.5 hr to ~4 hr) (Hikosaka and Wurtz, 1985; Lomber, 1999; Dias and Segraves, 1999; Sommer and Wurtz, 2004). In a typical experimental sequence, we gathered baseline performance data on Day 1, inactivation data on Day 2, and recovery data on Day 3 or 4.

We first compared average face-detection performance across all 11 inactivation sessions (two monkeys, three hemispheres) and their corresponding baseline sessions. Face-detection performance was reduced by ~11% during ML inactivation (pooled over all visibilities and sessions, p<0.001, paired t-test after logit transform). Separated by visibility level, these performance decrements were significant for 9 out of 10 visibility levels at p<0.05 (FDR-corrected paired t-tests after logit transform; Figure 2A). A comparison of single session performances during baseline and inactivation showed this performance reduction to be uniform across visibility levels (Figure 2B): it was well-described by a line (R2 = 0.74; p<0.001) parallel to the diagonal (slope = 0.96) with a significant negative offset (p<0.001), providing a second, visibility-independent, single-session level estimate of the performance deficit of ~15%. In Figure 2C and D, we plot the same quantities as above separated into each hemisphere and animal to demonstrate that the reduction in performance was consistent between subjects and inactivated hemispheres. At the single-hemisphere level, we observed significant differences mainly at intermediate visibility levels (p<0.05; FDR-corrected paired t-test after logit transform). Thus, inactivation of 1 out of 12 temporal lobe face patches (Tsao et al., 2008) caused a significant and sizable reduction in face-detection performance.

Figure 2. ML inactivation causes face-detection deficits.

Figure 2.

(A) Face-detection performance as a function of stimulus visibility under baseline (black) and ML-inactivated (red) conditions. Circles correspond to average performance, error bars to ±1 s.e.m. Lines are psychometric function fits to all performance data. Filled circles: p<0.05, FDR-corrected paired t-tests after logit transform. (B) Performance in the inactivated condition plotted as a function of performance in the corresponding baseline condition, sorted by stimulus visibility (grayscale luminance). Filled circles are the significant points from (A). These data were well-fit by a line (red; R2 = 0.74; p<0.001) with a slope close to 1, and a significantly shifted intercept (p<0.001) compared withthe diagonal. Dashed red lines represent the 95% confidence intervals. (C and D) The data in (A) and (B), but separated into individual inactivated hemispheres. Filled symbols denote statistically significant differences between the control and inactivated conditions (p<0.05, FDR-corrected paired t-test after logit transform). Symbols are as in Figure 1. (E and F) Face-detection performance as quantified by shift α (E) and lapse λ (F) during three conditions (ML-inactivated (red), recovery (green) and off-target (blue)) versus baseline condition. Shift and lapse were derived from psychometric function fits to face-detection data, and were significantly altered from baseline during inactivation (filled red circles; p<0.001 for shift and p<0.05 for lapse; paired t-test). Note that the axes in (B) and (D) are logit scaled.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.18558.003

To determine which behavioral parameters were affected by inactivation, psychometric functions (Equation 1 in Materials and methods; Wichmann and Hill, 2001) with three parameters – shift along the abscissa (α), slope (β), and lapse rate (λ, the difference between maximum and perfect performance) - were then fit to each session’s performance data (R2 > 0.9 for face detection in all sessions). Shift (α) and lapse rate (λ) were significantly higher in the inactivated conditions (p<0.001 and p = 0.016 respectively; paired t-test) than during baseline (red disks in Figure 2E; Figure 2F). In other words, during inactivation, the psychometric function was shifted rightwards and asymptotically lowered. The slope of the function (β) remained unaltered by inactivation. As a result, the stimulus visibility required for 50% performance (the average face-detection threshold) increased from 0.37 ± 0.02 at baseline to 0.45 ± 0.03 during the inactivation conditions.

We next tested whether the behavioral impairment following inactivation was specific to faces. Figure 3A shows the cumulative (summed across sessions) performance curves for face (left), body (middle), and shoe (right) detection. ML inactivation caused a shift of the psychometric function for face detection, but neither body nor shoe detection were significantly impaired. Effects were specific to the injection both in time and space. In recovery sessions when the effect of muscimol was expected to have subsided, behavioral performance returned to baseline levels across all target categories (Figure 3B; green), and fit parameters for performance in the recovery condition were not different from baseline for faces (Figure 2E; Figure 2F; green) or the other target categories (not shown). The transience of this effect demonstrates that muscimol was a causative agent. We also injected muscimol into cortex or white matter near ML (off-target injection, see Materials and methods). The off-target injection volumes barely overlapped with ML (9.6%; Figure 1D) or with any other face patch, and consequently, we observed only a weak and non-significant reduction in detection performance (Figure 2E; Figure 2F; Figure 3B; blue).

Figure 3. Deficits after ML inactivation are reversible and largely categorically specific to faces.

Figure 3.

(A) Behavioral performance in the baseline (black) and ML-inactivation (red) conditions for face-, body-, and shoe-detection as a function of stimulus visibility, showing the categorical specificity of the effect to faces. Data are summed across all 11 inactivation sessions. Lines are fits to the psychometric function in Equation 1. Shading shows the 95% confidence intervals. Gray dashed lines indicate threshold stimulus visibilities for each stimulus category. (B) Behavioral performance measured the day after ML inactivation (recovery; green) was not different from the baseline condition (black). Behavioral performance when muscimol was infused into the surrounding cortical areas, but not into ML (off-target; blue), was not different from the baseline condition, showing the anatomical specificity of the effect. (C) The performance deficit, averaged across all visibilities and sessions, for all tested category/condition combinations (colors as earlier). Face/inactivated performance deficit was significantly different for all pairwise comparisons (ANCOVA accounting for effect of repeated muscimol exposure on logit-transformed data; black asterisk: p<0.05, Tukey's test). An ANOVA not accounting for repeated exposure resulted in two cross-category comparisons reaching significance (gray asterisks: p<0.01, Tukey’s test). (D) The change in detection threshold for all tested category/condition combinations. The face/inactivated threshold was significantly shifted to higher stimulus visibilities for all pairwise comparisons (ANOVA; black asterisk, p<0.05, Tukey's test). Labels 'I', 'R', and 'O' refer to the inactivated, recovery, and off-target conditions. (E) The effect of repeated muscimol exposure on the performance deficit caused by muscimol. Dots correspond to mean effect pooled over visibilities, error bars are s.e.m. Red line corresponds to linear fit, dashed lines correspond to 95% confidence intervals.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.18558.004

We then compared the grand average performance deficits (across all stimulus visibilities and sessions) and threshold shifts (across all sessions) for all category/condition combinations (Figure 3C and D). We used an ANCOVA with each target category and experimental condition as an independent group (seven groups) and muscimol exposure session number as a covariate, with all fits constrained to have equal slopes on logit-transformed data to evaluate statistical significance. Only the face-inactivated condition showed significant performance deficits (F(6, 752)=17.89; p<0.0001) and threshold shifts (ANOVA; F(6, 65)=5.74; p<0.0001) that were pairwise significantly different from all of the other category/condition combinations including the body- and shoe-inactivated conditions (*p<0.05; Tukey's test). The mean face-detection performance deficit was not significantly correlated with the ML-injection overlap (Spearman’s Rho rs = 0.42; p=0.1). An ANCOVA analysis with session number as independent groups and ML-injection overlap as a covariate did not reveal a significant effect of ML-injection overlap on the observed face-detection deficit (F(1,102) = 1.35; p=0.25). These data suggest that although there are differences in the extent of ML-injection overlap between sessions, as the central portion of ML was targeted by the injection, performance deficits were largely unaffected by these small differences in injection volume and overlap. Using an ANOVA (on logit-transformed data) that did not account for the effect of repeated muscimol exposure, we observed two additional pairwise comparisons that showed cross-category significance (light gray lines in Figure 3C; **p<0.01; Tukey’s test), but which did not reach significance for the appropriate within-category comparisons. Thus ML inactivation is largely category-selective for faces, and anatomically specific for ML compared with its immediate surroundings.

Although ML inactivation did not cause statistically significant performance deficits or threshold shifts for the other object categories, it is possible that the data might contain a non-specific component, as indicated by the non-zero values of performance deficits and threshold shifts for body parts and shoes (Figure 3C and D). To gain more statistical power for testing for a non-specific effect, we repeated the ANCOVA analysis that yielded the statistically significant performance deficit in Figure 3C, but with the ‘body’ and ‘shoe’ categories grouped into a single ‘non-face’ category. This procedure also failed to yield statistically significant differences between ‘non-face’ control and inactivated conditions. Similarly, grouping the ‘body’ and ‘shoe’ categories in the ANOVA analysis of threshold failed to yield a significant difference in threshold. If a weak non-specific effect truly existed, we could not attribute this to the motivation level of the animal post injection and MRI scan, as there were no significant differences between the total number of trials the animal initiated on control or inactivation days (paired t-test, p=0.47). Alternately, although 76% of the injection volume was contained within ML, it is possible that some muscimol diffused out of ML and into neighboring body patches (Popivanov et al., 2012) in some cases, causing a smaller, non-significant effect on that object category.

Our behavioral paradigm was designed to emphasize natural face detection, thus allowing free viewing. The paradigm therefore encouraged the use of behavioral strategies that made maximal use of face-processing resources in the unaffected hemisphere. Compatible with the idea that such strategies might have been learned and used, we found muscimol-induced performance deficits to decrease with repeated muscimol exposure. This accounted for some of the variability seen in Figure 2B, and could be quantified using the ANCOVA analysis described above. We observed a highly significant effect of session number on the performance deficit magnitude (F(1,752)=15.33; p<0.0001) with a positive slope of 0.01 ± 0.002, that is, the performance deficit caused by muscimol decreased by ~1% each session (relative to baseline performance level; Figure 3E). It is also possible that over the course of the inactivation experiments, animals gradually learned to use stimulus features represented outside of ML such as low-level features of faces, despite our efforts to prevent this through scaling and in-plane rotation of targets and varying the texture backgrounds. When we performed the ANCOVA with subject identity as a covariate, we did not observe a significant effect, demonstrating that the gradual reduction of the effect of muscimol and the variability in the data (Figure 2B) did not arise from pooling data from the two subjects.

To illustrate the putative perceptual impact of ML inactivation on face detection, we derived equivalent inactivated visibility values from the psychometric curves (Figure 4A; similar to Carrasco et al., 2004). At a high visibility of 0.85 (gray dashed line, marked '1' in Figure 4A), performance in the inactivated condition was ~75% correct, and the same performance level was achieved in the baseline condition at a visibility of 0.49 (red dashed line, marked '1' in Figure 4A). Therefore, when stimulus visibility was 0.85 in the inactivated condition, the subject was behaving as if the visibility was 0.49, and so forth (Figure 4B). As expected from the increased detection threshold, equivalent inactivated visibilities for faces were significantly lower than baseline visibilities (p<0.01; paired t-test). In Figure 4C, we plot example face stimuli for two visibility pairs (filled circles in Figure 4B). Thus, the induced shift in the psychometric curve is potentially reflected as a perceptually profound reduction of equivalent face visibility.

Figure 4. Perceptual effects of ML inactivation.

Figure 4.

(A) Performance under baseline (black) and inactivated (red) conditions for face detection reproduced from Figure 2A (summed data). Dotted lines outline how equivalent inactivated visibility was calculated. For example, the ML-inactivated performance at a stimulus visibility of 0.85 (gray dashed line and gray '1') was the same as baseline performance when stimulus visibility was 0.49 (red dashed line and red '1'), that is, when ML was inactivated, the subject behaved as if it was viewing the stimulus at a decreased visibility. (B) As expected from the shifted psychometric curves, equivalent inactivated visibility was always lower than baseline visibility at each of the visibility levels tested (p=0.002; paired t-test). Filled circles marked '1' and '2' correspond to the visibility levels marked in A and C. (C and D) Example face stimuli plotted at baseline (0.85, marked '1', and 0.35, marked '2') and equivalent inactivated visibility levels corresponding to these baseline levels.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.18558.005

The free-viewing nature of the task exploits natural visual search behavior to demonstrate the causal dependence of this behavior on face-patch activity. This design is similar to one used in many human visual search and object recognition studies, but, because reliable methods for eye-tracking in freely moving primates are not available, we did not track the eye position of subjects as they performed the task. Recent evidence suggests that object recognition performance in monkeys does not depend on response modality – performance measured using saccades with eye-tracking, or touch with no gaze tracking, was similar to human object recognition performance measured without eye-tracking (Rajalingham et al., 2015). Therefore, we do not expect the lack of eye-tracking to affect the main results of our study.

Lack of eye-tracking, however, introduces two main sources of variability into our data. First, we cannot clearly assess the spatial effects of unilateral inactivation. Because we inactivated ML unilaterally in our experiments, we attempted to address whether performance deficits were also lateralized by analyzing recognition performance when the target image was located in three vertically oriented bands – ipsilateral to injection site, contralateral, and in the center of the screen. This analysis reduced our overall statistical power and did not yield significant results. Previous experiments in head-fixed preparations have demonstrated that local inactivation of smaller cortical regions can cause face-processing deficits specific to the visual field they represent (e.g., Afraz et al., 2015). As face-patch ML shows a foveal bias (Janssens et al., 2014), and because the center of ML contains neurons with the strongest face selectivity (Aparicio et al., 2016), by targeting our injections to the center of ML, we may have interfered with face detection in a large foveal area that would force the subject to use alternate and slower face-detection strategies. Thus performance deficits may have been higher at target locations that correspond to retinotopic locations of the injection, and effect sizes reported here should be taken as lower bound estimates.

Second, the lack of eye-tracking limits our ability to quantify behavioral strategies that may have been used by the subject to solve the task. In particular, it adds variability to the psychometric curve and affects our estimate of the ‘chance level’ of the task because the subject may have looked away from the screen after initiating a trial. We designed our task to minimize this variability by: (1) requiring the animals to voluntarily initiate trials; (2) requiring subjects to direct their gaze to the center of the screen and perform a deliberate and accurate movement to initiate the trial; (3) requiring subjects to pay attention to the color of the central cue to determine target block identity; (4) requiring subjects to complete the task in a short time span; and (5) randomizing visibility levels within blocks and interleaving block types to prevent systematic biases. Through post-hoc analysis we determined that: (1) the animal performed similar numbers of trials on control and inactivation days suggesting no change in motivation level; (2) animals performed at a high rate on high-visibility trials on all days, suggesting no distraction at the time scale of trials. The experimental configuration ensured that the screen covered a large portion of the animal’s visual field, minimizing space available for distractions. Therefore, we believe that we have minimized the effects of extraneous sources on behavior. Additional sources of variability may remain, however, such as variations in eccentricity of the target stimulus as the subject freely views the display.

Direct cortical stimulation and suppression have been used successfully to alter face perception in humans and macaques (Allison et al., 1994; Parvizi et al., 2012; Chong et al., 2013; Jonas et al., 2014; Afraz et al., 2006; Roy et al., 2014; Afraz et al., 2015). We provide four critical advances over these past experiments: (1) We directly visualized and limited the spread of muscimol to the functionally defined coordinates of an fMRI-identified face area. Comparing on- and off-target injections, we could thus establish anatomical specificity of inactivation. (2) We used several interleaved object categories as behavioral targets. We could thus establish categorical specificity of ML inactivation. (3) We focused on face detection, the most fundamental and robust component of face processing (Sinha, 2002; Viola and Jones, 2004; Tsao and Livingstone, 2008; Gilad et al., 2009) and selected the target site of inactivation based on the known physiology of neurons (Ohayon et al., 2012). (4) We used a behavioral paradigm that was naturalistic, yet allowed for quantification of perceptual thresholds. Thus we could provide first causal evidence for the role of ML in real-life face detection. This is despite only unilaterally inactivating 1 out of 12 nodes (Tsao et al., 2006, 2008) and allowing free viewing. Receptive fields in posterior IT, the region in which ML is located, are spatially specific (Kobatake and Tanaka, 1994), and thus the two hemispheres cover different parts of the visual field (Rajimehr et al., 2014). Therefore, unilateral inactivation is expected to affect only part of the visual field (Afraz et al., 2015). That significant impairments occur during free viewing emphasizes the causal role of ML in everyday face detection. Effects were specific to faces, with little or no effects on one object category, bodies, that is represented in the direct vicinity of ML (Popivanov et al., 2012), and another one, shoes, a newly learned object category with exemplars, as in faces, exhibiting high within-class similarity. Our results thus extend our understanding of ML as a face-specific processing module containing a large number of cells tuned to facial features that are known to be useful for face detection in an important way: highlighting its causal role for real-world face-detection behavior.

Materials and methods

All procedures conformed to the NIH Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All animals were handled and all procedures and experiments conducted according to approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) protocols at Weill Cornell Medicine (protocol number 2010–0029) and The Rockefeller University (protocol numbers 12585-H and 15849-H). All surgery was performed under isoflurane and a 70% nitrous oxide and 30% oxygen mixture. Every effort was made to minimize suffering.

Subjects

We studied one male rhesus (Subject O) and one male cynomolgus (Subject I) macaque, 3.5–5 years in age and weighing between 4.5 and 9 kgs. An acrylic cement-based cranial implant (C&B Metabond, Parkell; Palacos LV+G, Zimmer) was anchored to the skull with ceramic screws (Thomas Recording or Rogue Research) using sterile surgical methods under standard anesthetic and postoperative protocols. A custom MRI-compatible headpost made of Ultem (SABIC) was then secured in each implant.

MRI imaging

We acquired all MRI data on a 3T Siemens Tim Trio MRI scanner and used an AC88 gradient insert (Siemens) for functional scans. A dose of Feraheme (AMAG Pharmaceuticals) containing 8-10 mg/kg of iron was injected into a saphenous vein before each functional scan to increase functional contrast. Functional images were obtained with a custom eight-channel surface coil (Lawrence Wald, MGH Martinos Center) and a gradient-echo echo-planar imaging sequence with 54 horizontal slices, a 96 × 96 in-plane matrix, an isotropic resolution of 1 mm3, TR = 2 s, TE = 16 ms, and 2x GRAPPA acceleration. For anatomical registration and cortical surface reconstruction, we obtained images using a custom single-channel surface coil and a T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo sequence (MPRAGE) with 240 sagittal slices, an in-plane matrix of 256 × 256, and isotropic resolution of 0.5 mm3 (averaged over four to six scans) under isoflurane anesthesia. After each muscimol injection, we acquired T1-weighted images using a fast MPRAGE sequence with 192 slices, an in-plane matrix of 256 × 256, and isotropic resolution of 0.5 mm3 (one or two repetitions).

Visual stimuli

Stimuli for fMRI imaging were projected on a screen placed ~35 cm in front of the subject. Stimuli consisted of grayscale pictures of rhesus macaque faces, cynomolgus macaque faces, fruits and vegetables, monkey bodies, manmade objects, and phase-scrambled versions of the fruit and vegetable pictures, presented in separate 24 s blocks. Each block of this set consisted of 15 exemplar images shown for 0.4 s, with each exemplar repeated four times in a block. Images were matched within and across all categories for total screen area, placed on a pink noise background and normalized for luminance and frequency amplitude using the SHINE toolbox (http://www.mapageweb.umontreal.ca/gosselif/shine; Willenbockel et al., 2010). The pink noise background subtended 15° visual angle, and the average foreground image subtended ~6° visual angle. A Latin square design was used to create six orders of the stimulus blocks, and the relative position of each block within a run and the identity of the immediately preceding block were balanced.

fMRI analysis

We used standard, previously published analyses to localize the face patches (Tsao et al., 2006). Briefly, we used standard preprocessing methods to correct for artifacts common to imaging head-fixed, awake macaques. We corrected for motion in three dimensions, shifts, rotations, and warping, and aligned the functional images to the high-resolution anatomical images. We used a sign-reversed hemodynamic response function to account for MION increasing contrast by decreasing the local MR signal. A general linear model was then fit, which included predictors for each stimulus type, as well as nuisance predictors for the motion parameters. To map the face patches, we used the contrast [(rhesus faces + cynomolgus faces) > all other object categories]. We constructed a model of each subject's cortical surface from the high-resolution anatomical scans using FreeSurfer (Fischl, 2012). The statistical significance values of the differential signal changes from the functional scans were projected onto the high-resolution anatomical volume and surface model for visualization.

Behavioral training

We trained subjects on a touchscreen-based object detection task. Stimuli were presented using MATLAB software (Kofiko, provided by Dr. Shay Ohayon, MIT; https://github.com/shayo/Kofiko) on an Elo IntelliTouch 1537L 15-inch LCD touchscreen (Tyco Electronics) mounted to the subject's cage. The subject initiated the trial by touching a central disc (4.3 cm dia.), the color of which indicated the target category (red-faces; blue-body parts; green-shoes). On initiating the trial, subjects were presented with a stimulus array consisting of 24 objects, one of which belonged to the target category, presented at 36 possible random locations, balanced to ensure that each quadrant had an equal number of stimuli. The objects were alpha-blended into a cluttered background, based on a set of visual textures (Portilla and Simoncelli, 2000) pre-generated using exemplar stimuli from the three target categories. For each trial, a random 600 × 600 region of one of 32 pre-generated textures, at one of four possible rotations, was used as the background. The texture occupied a 26 × 26 cm area of the touchscreen. Subjects were free to choose viewing distance, but typically positioned themselves to minimize movement required to reach the centrally placed reward spout, resulting in a viewing distance of ~20–25 cm. At that viewing distance, the full extent of the stimulus array subtended a visual angle of ~54°–64°. Individual stimuli varied in size and subtended ~4°–7° of visual angle. Subjects had 3 s to choose the correct target object, upon which they were rewarded with a small amount of juice or water. Wrong choices, but not time-outs, resulted in an increased inter-trial delay of 0.5–1.5 s. Sessions were organized into interleaving blocks for each target category (faces, bodies, or shoes), with each block consisting of 10 stimuli at the 10 visibility levels in random order. Subjects were trained until they performed ~800–1000 trials per 2 hr session, and attained at least 75% face-detection performance at the highest visibility level.

Behavioral stimuli

Grayscale images of human faces were constructed from a multiracial face database (http://wiki.cnbc.cmu.edu/Face_Place, Michael Tarr). We chose 32 faces with neutral expression, uniform lighting, and viewing angle ranging from half profile to frontal views. Images included hair and in some cases, facial hair. On each face trial, we further applied a random scaling factor (0.5x – 1.5x) and random in-plane rotation (±45 deg.). The goal of these manipulations was to minimize the consistent spatial occurrence of low-level features and ensure that face stimuli possessed highly variable outlines. We used an initial set of 16 frontal face stimuli to train the subjects to criterion, and then switched to this new set just before commencing the inactivation experiments. No performance decrease was triggered by this switch, suggesting that the subjects had learned the stimulus category, and not the individual exemplars. For bodies, we used a set of 16 images consisting of various views of macaque torsos and limbs. Shoe images were downloaded from various online sources. We chose these categories to control for two effects – bodies controlled for the ethological relevance of the stimuli, and shoes controlled for the global shape similarity of objects. The target stimuli did not overlap with the stimuli used for mapping the face patches. Distractor images were drawn from a set of 178 images of airplanes, cars, chairs, flowers, fruits, manmade objects, motorbikes, familiar animal toys, technological objects, Fribbles, and Yadgits (Barry et al., 2014). The entire stimulus set was normalized for luminance and frequency amplitude using the SHINE toolbox. All stimuli were alpha-blended into the cluttered background using a transparency mask with feathered edges to achieve smooth blending. We randomized the location of the objects to one of 36 equally spaced locations on the background for every trial, and ensured that: (1) objects did not overlap, and (2) equal numbers of objects were present in each quadrant to maintain a uniform extent and density of visual search.

Task design considerations

The behavioral task used was the result of theoretical considerations and refinement from pilot behavioral experiments. The visual texture background was introduced to both disrupt the normal coarse contrast relationships in faces, and to introduce face-like coarse contrast relationships at non-target locations. We reasoned that by doing so, pooling over a large number of ML neurons might become necessary for successful task performance. In pilot experiments without a background or with pixel-noise background, animals detected faces rapidly (<1 s) and at high performance levels for all stimulus visibility levels tested. Imposing the texture background resulted in a more typically shaped psychometric curve. Even with a textured background, when a time constraint was not imposed on behavioral response, subjects could perform the face-detection task at near-perfect levels at moderate to high levels of stimulus visibility. However, we noticed that the response time distributions were multi-peaked and had a heavy tail, indicating that the subjects may perform several ~1.5 s-long scans of the array until the target was detected. We imposed a hard time constraint of 3 s, giving subjects time for about two scans of the array. Trials that would have taken longer response times (had the time constraint not been imposed) were thus converted to timeouts or misses attributable to forced guessing, allowing us to obtain a psychometric function that better reflects rapid face detection in scenes.

Muscimol injection

After functional mapping, MR-compatible chambers were implanted over the functionally identified location of ML under anesthesia, and a craniotomy (~6 mm dia.) was performed within the chamber under sedation just before the inactivation experiments. We injected 5 μL (8 μL in one experiment) of a cocktail of 5 μg/μL muscimol (Sigma Aldrich) and 5 mM gadolinium (Magnevist) in phosphate-buffered saline. Injections were performed through a 32-gauge metal cannula (Hamilton) attached to a precision microinfusion pump (World Precision Instruments) using non-compressible fused silica tubing (Polymicro) and PEEK connectors (Labsmith), adapted from an earlier design (Noudoost and Moore, 2011). The cannula was introduced into the cortex via a custom 3D-printed grid, using MATLAB-based planning software (Ohayon and Tsao, 2012) to select the appropriate grid hole. We typically injected in 0.2 μL steps, at a rate of 0.01 μL/s, with 20–30 s between steps (average rate of 0.4 μL /min), spread over three cortical depths spaced 500 μm apart (1.6–1.8 μL per depth, deepest first). We waited 3–5 min after each set of injections before moving the cannula to a different depth or withdrawing the cannula from the cortex. The entire procedure lasted ~25 min. Immediately after the injections, the subject was transferred to the MRI scanner to obtain anatomical scans, after which the subject was transferred to its home cage where the touchscreen was already mounted. Subjects thus performed the task between ~1 and 3 hr from the end of the injection procedure. This window was well within the time period of maximal muscimol effects (~0.5 hr to ~4 hr) (Hikosaka and Wurtz, 1985; Lomber, 1999; Dias and Segraves, 1999; Sommer and Wurtz, 2004). Off-target sites included locations dorsal to the STS (n = 3; one into STS and two in gray matter 3.0 mm dorsal to STS), deeper locations comprising the white matter beneath ML (n = 3; depths 4–5.5 mm from cortical surface), and locations posterior to ML (n = 4; 2.9–3.6 mm from the center of ML at t > 20). The first sub-group of off-target injections was intended to function as sham injections, as the animal undergoes identical experimental procedures without inactivation of visual areas. We used off-target shams rather than injecting saline into ML to minimize risks of causing tissue damage and to preserve the face patches for future electrophysiological studies. We performed 11 on-target injections and 10 off-target injections in three hemispheres. To evaluate overlap between the muscimol injection and the functionally defined bounds of ML, we used AFNI to construct a binary mask volume that isolated t-values greater than 20 in the [(rhesus faces + cynomolgus faces) > all other objects] contrast. We resampled this mask volume to the resolution of the anatomical volume, and manually selected voxels corresponding to ML. In the anatomical volume, we first constructed a manual mask that included the area around the injection site. Then, we determined the maximum value of Gd brightness within this mask, and constructed a second binary mask that included all values greater than 50% of the maximum value (full-width at half maximum of the injection site). We then used AFNI to compute the volume of the injection (in voxels), and the volumetric overlap between the injection and ML (at t > 20). The injection overlap was defined as the volume of overlap divided by the volume of the injection. ML overlap was defined as the volume of overlap divided by the volume of ML.

Behavior and behavioral data analysis

Subjects were required to select, within 3 s, an object belonging to the target category from an array of 24 objects, blended into a cluttered background at one of 10 visibility levels (Figure 1A,B). Subjects had been trained on three target categories: human faces, macaque bodies, or shoes. Each trial had three possible outcomes – target object selected, distractor object selected, or a timeout (3s elapsed without a touch). For all analyses, we focused on the proportion of correct trials. We first compared the average performance difference between each experimental condition (inactivated, recovery or off-target) and the paired baseline condition for each category, using FDR-corrected paired t-tests to evaluate statistical significance. We then fit psychometric functions to the data from individual sessions in each experimental condition for each stimulus category. These functions were of the form (Wichmann and Hill, 2001):

ψ(x;α,β,λ)=γ+(1γλ) F(x;α,β) (1)

where F is the Weibull function, defined as F(x;α,β)=1exp((xα)β), α is the shift parameter, β is the slope parameter, λ is the lapse rate, and γ = 1/24 = 0.0417 is the chance performance level (assuming that the animal initiates trials with the intention of finishing them). A logit transform was applied to all proportional data (such as percent correct) before applying parametric statistical tests. Paired t-tests were used to evaluate whether fit parameters were different for each experimental condition from baseline. Thresholds were derived from these psychometric fits. We used ANOVAs with post-hoc Tukey's tests to compare performance deficits across experimental conditions and stimulus categories. In preliminary analyses, we noticed that performance deficits decreased as we exposed the subject to an increasing number of inactivation sessions. To control for this effect, we also performed an ANCOVA to compare effect sizes between the various groups after accounting for session number, which was modeled as a covariate. All analyses were performed in MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA). We derived equivalent inactivated visibilities from the fit curves, and plotted example face stimuli blended into the background texture at the resulting alpha levels. We caution that these example stimuli render differently in print and on different monitor types (LCD or CRT).

Acknowledgements

We thank Shay Ohayon (Massachusetts Institute of Technology) for providing the behavioral control software environment, and the Planner software package for stereotactic targeting. We thank Akinori Ebihara, Rizwan Huq, Sofia Landi, Clark Fisher, Caspar Schwiedrzik, Stephen Shepherd, Pablo Polosecki, Ilaria Sani, Julia Sliwa and Margaret Fabiszak (The Rockefeller University) for assistance with imaging and training; Skye Rasmussen, Leslie Diaz, Alejandra Gonzales, Susan Hinklein, Jorge Fajardo, and Vadim Sherman (The Rockefeller University) for veterinary and technical support, Jon Dyke (Weill Cornell Medicine) for scanner support, and Lawrence L Wald for MRI coil design and support. We thank Melanie Wilke and Igor Kagan for advice on targeted muscimol injections, and Gaby Maimon and Stephen Shepherd for comments on the manuscript. We are grateful for the reviewers’ constructive input towards improving this manuscript. This manuscript is based upon work supported by the Center for Brains, Minds and Machines (CBMM), funded by NSF STC award CCF-1231216, funds from The Rockefeller University, and National Eye Institute grant R01 EY021594-01A1 to WAF. WAF is a New York Stem Cell Foundation-Robertson Investigator. SS was supported by a Leon Levy postdoctoral fellowship. WZ was supported by the Pew Fellows Program in Biomedical Sciences and a NIH-International Neuroscience Fellowship F05MH094113.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Science Foundation STC award CCF-1231216 to Winrich A Freiwald.

  • National Eye Institute R01 EY021594-01A1 to Winrich A Freiwald.

  • New York Stem Cell Foundation Robertson Investigator to Winrich A Freiwald.

  • Pew Charitable Trusts Fellows and Investigator Programs to Wilbert Zarco, Winrich A Freiwald.

  • Leon Levy Foundation Postdoctoral fellowship to Srivatsun Sadagopan.

  • National Institutes of Health International Neuroscience Fellowship F05MH094113 to Wilbert Zarco.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

SS, Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing, Designed the experiments, performed functional imaging and inactivation experiments, analyzed data.

WZ, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Collected behavioral data, performed functional imaging, performed inactivation experiments, assisted with data analysis.

WAF, Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Writing—review and editing, Designed the experiments, performed inactivation experiments, provided inputs for data analysis, wrote and revised the manuscript.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All procedures conformed to the NIH Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All of the animals were handled and all procedures and experiments conducted according to the approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) protocols at Weill Cornell Medicine (protocol number 2010-0029) and The Rockefeller University (protocol numbers 12585-H and 15849-H). All surgery was performed under isoflurane and in a 70% nitrous oxide and 30% oxygen mixture. Every effort was made to minimize suffering.

References

  1. Afraz A, Boyden ES, DiCarlo JJ. Optogenetic and pharmacological suppression of spatial clusters of face neurons reveal their causal role in face gender discrimination. PNAS. 2015;112:6730–6735. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1423328112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Afraz SR, Kiani R, Esteky H. Microstimulation of inferotemporal cortex influences face categorization. Nature. 2006;442:692–695. doi: 10.1038/nature04982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Allison T, Ginter H, McCarthy G, Nobre AC, Puce A, Luby M, Spencer DD. Face recognition in human extrastriate cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1994;71:821–825. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.71.2.821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aparicio PL, Issa EB, DiCarlo JJ. Neurophysiological organization of the middle face patch in macaque inferior temporal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:12729–12745. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0237-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barry TJ, Griffith JW, De Rossi S, Hermans D. Meet the fribbles: novel stimuli for use within behavioural research. Frontiers in Psychology. 2014;5:103. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Carrasco M, Ling S, Read S. Attention alters appearance. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:308–313. doi: 10.1038/nn1194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chong SC, Jo S, Park KM, Joo EY, Lee MJ, Hong SC, Hong SB. Interaction between the electrical stimulation of a face-selective area and the perception of face stimuli. NeuroImage. 2013;77:70–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Dias EC, Segraves MA. Muscimol-induced inactivation of monkey frontal eye field: effects on visually and memory-guided saccades. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1999;81:2191–2214. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.5.2191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Fischl B. FreeSurfer. NeuroImage. 2012;62:774–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gilad S, Meng M, Sinha P. Role of ordinal contrast relationships in face encoding. PNAS. 2009;106:5353–5358. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812396106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Heiss JD, Walbridge S, Asthagiri AR, Lonser RR. Image-guided convection-enhanced delivery of muscimol to the primate brain. Journal of Neurosurgery. 2010;112:790–795. doi: 10.3171/2009.7.JNS09652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hikosaka O, Wurtz RH. Modification of saccadic eye movements by GABA-related substances. II. effects of muscimol in monkey substantia nigra pars reticulata. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1985;53:292–308. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.53.1.292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Janssens T, Zhu Q, Popivanov ID, Vanduffel W. Probabilistic and single-subject retinotopic maps reveal the topographic organization of face patches in the macaque cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:10156–10167. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2914-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Jonas J, Rossion B, Krieg J, Koessler L, Colnat-Coulbois S, Vespignani H, Jacques C, Vignal JP, Brissart H, Maillard L. Intracerebral electrical stimulation of a face-selective area in the right inferior occipital cortex impairs individual face discrimination. NeuroImage. 2014;99:487–497. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Kobatake E, Tanaka K. Neuronal selectivities to complex object features in the ventral visual pathway of the macaque cerebral cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1994;71:856–867. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.71.3.856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Leopold DA, Rhodes G. A comparative view of face perception. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2010;124:233–251. doi: 10.1037/a0019460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Lomber SG. The advantages and limitations of permanent or reversible deactivation techniques in the assessment of neural function. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 1999;86:109–117. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(98)00160-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Noudoost B, Moore T. A reliable microinjectrode system for use in behaving monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2011;194:218–223. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.10.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ohayon S, Freiwald WA, Tsao DY. What makes a cell face selective? the importance of contrast. Neuron. 2012;74:567–581. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ohayon S, Tsao DY. MR-guided stereotactic navigation. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2012;204:389–397. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.11.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Parvizi J, Jacques C, Foster BL, Witthoft N, Withoft N, Rangarajan V, Weiner KS, Grill-Spector K. Electrical stimulation of human fusiform face-selective regions distorts face perception. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:14915–14920. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2609-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Popivanov ID, Jastorff J, Vanduffel W, Vogels R. Stimulus representations in body-selective regions of the macaque cortex assessed with event-related fMRI. NeuroImage. 2012;63:723–741. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Portilla J, Simoncelli EP. A parametric texture model based on joint statistics of complex wavelet coefficients. International Journal of Computer Vision. 2000;40:49–70. doi: 10.1023/A:1026553619983. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Rajalingham R, Schmidt K, DiCarlo JJ. Comparison of object recognition behavior in human and monkey. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:12127–12136. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0573-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Rajimehr R, Bilenko NY, Vanduffel W, Tootell RB. Retinotopy versus face selectivity in macaque visual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2014;26:2691–2700. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Roy A, Shepherd SV, Platt ML. Reversible inactivation of pSTS suppresses social gaze following in the macaque (Macaca mulatta) Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2014;9:209–217. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Sinha P. Proceedings of the Annual Workshop on Biologically Motivated Computer Vision. 2002. Qualitative representations for recognition; pp. 249–262. [Google Scholar]
  28. Sommer MA, Wurtz RH. What the brain stem tells the frontal cortex. II. role of the SC-MD-FEF pathway in corollary discharge. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004;91:1403–1423. doi: 10.1152/jn.00740.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Tsao DY, Freiwald WA, Tootell RB, Livingstone MS. A cortical region consisting entirely of face-selective cells. Science. 2006;311:670–674. doi: 10.1126/science.1119983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Tsao DY, Livingstone MS. Mechanisms of face perception. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2008;31:411–437. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Tsao DY, Moeller S, Freiwald WA. Comparing face patch systems in macaques and humans. PNAS. 2008;105:19514–19519. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809662105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Viola P, Jones MJ. Robust Real-Time face detection. International Journal of Computer Vision. 2004;57:137–154. doi: 10.1023/B:VISI.0000013087.49260.fb. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Wichmann FA, Hill NJ. The psychometric function: I. fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Perception & Psychophysics. 2001;63:1293–1313. doi: 10.3758/BF03194544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Wilke M, Kagan I, Andersen RA. Functional imaging reveals rapid reorganization of cortical activity after parietal inactivation in monkeys; PNAS; USA. 2012. pp. 8274–8279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Willenbockel V, Sadr J, Fiset D, Horne GO, Gosselin F, Tanaka JW. Controlling low-level image properties: the SHINE toolbox. Behavior Research Methods. 2010;42:671–684. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.3.671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2017 Apr 4;6:e18558. doi: 10.7554/eLife.18558.006

Decision letter

Editor: Jack L Gallant1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "A Causal Relationship Between Face-Patch Activity and Face-Detection Behavior" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor (who separately reviewed the paper) and by David Van Essen as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal his identity: Rufin Vogels (Reviewer #1).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

The present study shows that inactivation of the macaque face selective area ML impairs the detection of faces in cluttered displays. The main finding of the study is that ML plays a causal role in face detection.

The reviewers found the work to be interesting and worthwhile, and potentially publishable in eLife after appropriate revision. However, they also had several concerns about methods and interpretation of the results that must be addressed before the paper can be considered for publication.

Essential revisions:

1) Most importantly, one reviewer and the reviewing editor were both concerned about the behavioral procedures used in the experiment, and the apparent lack of head or eye tracking during the experiment. The animals apparently freely viewed the stimulus during the 3 second response period, and no attempt was made to deal with variability in post-processing or analysis. In most vision studies the eyes are tracked closely to ensure that eye movement differences across conditions could not have accounted for the results, but apparently that was not done here. Could this have affected the results? If the eye movement data are available then the paper should be revised appropriately, incorporating those data. If not, then the authors should include a detailed rebuttal indicating why this is not a concern.

2) The behavioral data from individual hemispheres/animals should be shown so that readers can gain a better sense of how consistent the impairment is across hemispheres/animals. The overlap between the estimated injection site and the fMRI defined face patches for each individual hemisphere should also be shown.

3) Although the largest effects seem to occur for faces, inspection of Figure 3 suggests that inactivation affects detection of all three categories, although faces appear most affected. Thus, it is unclear whether the effect is truly limited to faces, or whether it a smaller impairment is also found in other categories. This issue should be addressed clearly in revision.

4) All reviewers had concerns about the location of the muscimol injections in both experimental and control conditions. This information needs to be provided in a very clear form, and the interpretation of the results of each specific injection should be made clear.

Reviewer #1:

The present study has several shortcomings, however. Although I feel that these drawbacks do not affect their main result – an impairment of face detection when inactivating ML -, they make a quantitative assessment of the impairment impossible, which is a missed opportunity. First, their "naturalistic" task suffers from a major problem when one wants to quantify detection behavior: if the animal fails to respond to the display that does not mean that the animal did not detect the stimulus. The authors did not measure eye movements of the animals during the detection task and thus an unknown portion of "no go" responses may have resulted not from detection failures but simply from the animals not looking at the display or not performing the task. This causes problems when attempting to compute the proportion of correct trials: what to take as an "executed" trial?

Second, they made unilateral inactivations which will have affected mainly the contralateral visual field (see Afraz et al., 2015). Thus, whether the target stimulus was in the affected part of the visual field will depend on the fixation pattern, which may have varied from trial to trial. This will have added noise to the psychometric functions for the inactivated condition and may have underestimated the deficit to an unknown degree. The data show that ML activation has an effect, but the size of the effect cannot be estimated.

Third, the authors should show the behavioral data of the individual hemispheres/monkeys so that one can appreciate how consistent the impairment is across hemispheres/animals. Furthermore, they should show the overlap between the estimated injection site and the fMRI defined face patches for each individual hemisphere. Related to this, it appears from Figure 1D that in that monkey the injections were rather posterior and at best affected only part of ML. Is this true?

Figure 3C and D suggest an impairment after each muscimol injection, although the effect is greater for face detection after ML injection than for the other 3 conditions. Could this aspecific part of the effect result from the possibility that the animals did not work well after the injection and following the MRI scan? Control injections with saline – standard in these sort of experiments – could have answered this concern.

I am puzzled by the site of the control injections: dorsal to the STS (where? In gray matter? Non-visual!) and deeper locations in the white matter near ML (subsection “Muscimol injection”). Especially the latter is puzzling since fibers are not affected by muscimol. What was the purpose of this? Sham injections? It would have been more informative to inject in nonface-patch regions of IT.

Reviewer #2:

Sadagopan and colleagues studied the behavioral effects that result from inactivation of a face selective area in the temporal lobe of monkeys. The results show that inactivating area ML results in an increased threshold for face detection. Importantly, this increased detection threshold seems to be specific for monkey faces and not other visual categories such as body parts or shoes. In addition, the authors performed muscimol injections in nearby sites that did not affect face detection, demonstrating that the effect is also site specific.

This is a carefully planned experiment that convincingly demonstrates the causal role that area ML has in the processing and detection of visual information related to faces. I have some comments that the authors could address to further improve the manuscript.

1) It is clear that ML inactivation has its strongest effect on the detection of faces. However, Figure 3A shows that ML inactivation also affects the detection of body and shoe categories. These non-selective effects did not reach statistical significance but I think that some readers will find hints of threshold increases for the shoe and body categories. I think no reasonable reader will expect 100% selectivity in any cortical inactivation study. Maybe the authors could make a more guarded interpretation in relation to category selectivity, or maybe they could comment on this.

2) Figure 3B. There seems to be no data in relation to performance obtained from off-target inactivation in the shoe and body categories. Why is this?

3) I wonder if the authors have data about the location and spread of the off-target injections. A schematic like the one in Figure 1D but for the off-target inactivation could help readers evaluate how off were the these injections from the ML area.

4) From the data on Figure 2C-D one can imagine that there were some off-target and recovery sessions that reached a statistical significant effect. Am I correct? Please mark them on those panels.

5) The analyses described in the sixth paragraph of the Results and Discussion seemed redundant to me. Yes, a displacement in detection threshold can be illustrated as a reduction in face visibility but I am not sure what it is gained by doing that.

6) The effects of muscimol decreased with each exposure. This is an interesting finding that I think is worth showing in a data figure. Please show the effect of muscimol as a function of experimental session.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "A Causal Relationship Between Face-Patch Activity and Face-Detection Behavior" for further consideration at eLife. Your revised article has been favorably evaluated by David Van Essen (Senior editor), a Reviewing editor, and two reviewers.

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed before a final decision can be made, as outlined below:

1) Please address the injection overlap issue identified by the first reviewer, and if possible provide a more quantitative relationship between inactivation and behavior.

Reviewer #1:

Although the authors attempt to defend the use of a "naturalistic" free-viewing task to study the effect of inactivation of a face patch (located in a region with a rough retinotopy), I still feel that this study would have provided more information on the contribution of that face patch to face detection if they had use a well-controlled task (with at least proper eye movement control). Despite this shortcoming, the study shows clear face detection performance deficits, which is a valuable addition to the literature.

I have the following comment on the revised text, which needs to be addressed.

The injection overlap was defined as the volume of overlap (between ML and injection) divided by the injection volume. However, this implies that a small injection volume inside a larger ML volume will get a 100% overlap, while only part of ML was inactivated. They should also provide the proportion of overlap computed relative to the ML volume. This metric reflects how much of ML was inactivated, which is unknown now (especially given the "distorted" map shown in Figure 1), but is important for evaluation of the behavioral effects.

Reviewer #2:

The authors have addressed my questions and concerns satisfactorily.

1) I think they now have a more balanced discussion on the selectivity of the muscimol inactivations.

2) They now provide data on the overlap between off-target injections and ML area for each hemisphere.

However, they do not discuss my query: "6) The effects of muscimol decreased with each exposure. This is an interesting finding that I think is worth showing in a data figure. Please show the effect of muscimol as a function of experimentalsession."

I think they missed pasting the response on the Response to Reviewers file because this effect is now shown and analyzed in the revised version of the manuscript (new panel on Figure 3E).

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "A Causal Relationship Between Face-Patch Activity and Face-Detection Behavior" for further consideration at eLife. Your revised article has been favorably evaluated by David Van Essen (Senior Editor), a Reviewing Editor, and two reviewers.

The reviewers were very happy with your revisions and we are ready to accept the manuscript. However, one reviewer requested a small technical change to correct some missing information. In the manuscript you currently report one degree of freedom for the ANCOVA, but there should be two degrees of freedom reported [e.g., in the following format F(df1,df2) = x]. Please provide the other degree of freedom here, or provide an explanation for why this is not required.

After we receive your revised submission we will be happy to publish this work.

eLife. 2017 Apr 4;6:e18558. doi: 10.7554/eLife.18558.007

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) Most importantly, one reviewer and the reviewing editor were both concerned about the behavioral procedures used in the experiment, and the apparent lack of head or eye tracking during the experiment. The animals apparently freely viewed the stimulus during the 3 second response period, and no attempt was made to deal with variability in post-processing or analysis. In most vision studies the eyes are tracked closely to ensure that eye movement differences across conditions could not have accounted for the results, but apparently that was not done here. Could this have affected the results? If the eye movement data are available then the paper should be revised appropriately, incorporating those data. If not, then the authors should include a detailed rebuttal indicating why this is not a concern.

We will address the general and most important concern (eye-tracking) raised here in quite some detail. In brief, our study is a behavioral visual search study. Much of the classical and recent behavioral work on visual search (in humans and monkeys) has been conducted without eye movement monitoring. The analyses of behavior we are using here follows these established techniques. Like the reviewer, we do not think that lack of eye tracking detracts from the main results of the study.

In our lab, we use eye tracking regularly, since it is necessary for most of the paradigms we employ. This is especially the case for electrophysiological recordings, to account for variability of neural firing rates arising from shifts of receptive fields over the stimulus array.

But because the study in this manuscript is purely behavioral, we could make a fundamental design choice when we started this study, given that reliable eye tracking for freely moving primates is not available: 1) do a traditional head-fixed experiment with eye-tracking, or 2) exploit a natural visual search behavior by using a touchscreen. We deliberately chose the latter option, fully recognizing that we were trading off the advantages we gained (almost unconstrained natural behavior) against disadvantages (lack of eye tracking).

We did so mainly for one reason: in the landscape of the existing literature on face processing, which virtually exclusively relies on the head-fixed preparation, we believed we could be truly innovative and contribute to the ongoing debates about the relevance of face-representations for real-world face- recognition behavior by studying the dependence of a natural behavior on neural activity. We believe that this is a transformative innovation for the field of face recognition as a whole, and of fundamental value to the current study.

As we mentioned earlier, this choice was influenced by many classical and recent studies on visual search and object recognition. Many recent human behavioral studies on visual search and object recognition are conducted on engines such as Amazon MTurk where there is no control on gaze, experimental settings, or distractions. For example, a recent study on core object recognition obtained human performance data from MTurk by pooling the responses of a large number (~600) of human subjects performing a small number (~100) of trials, without eye tracking data (Rajalingham et al., J. Neurosci. 2015). In the same study, they performed monkey object recognition experiments on a touchscreen (one subject) or with eye tracking (one subject), and found that performance was similar between the monkey subjects. Indeed, the study concluded using these data that core object recognition was similar between humans and monkeys. This illustrates that studies without eye tracking can be consistent with those using eye tracking and highly informative. A possible reason for this high degree of similarity is that humans and monkeys naturally tend to foveate the most interesting or task-relevant regions of the visual field.

As we will detail below in our response to specific reviewer comments, we agree with the reviewer that this means that the “true” underlying effect size is likely larger than what we report. Thus all our behavioral quantifications are more precisely seen as lower bound estimates. But the true value of an inactivation study like ours really lies in providing evidence for the existence and for the specificity of an effect during a naturalistic behavior, and this is what the current study accomplishes.

We further argue that the difficulties quantifying behavior that reviewer 1 points out are difficulties resulting from the logic of go/no-go tasks, not of the lack of eye position measurements. Recognizing the need for tight behavioral control in the absence of eye position monitoring, we designed the task to minimize potential for unwanted distractions by: 1) Requiring the animals to voluntarily initiate trials; 2) Requiring subjects to direct their gaze to the center of the screen and perform a deliberate and accurate movement to initiate the trial; 3) Requiring subjects to pay attention to the color of the central cue to determine target identity; 4) Requiring subjects to complete the task in a short time span; and 5) Randomizing visibility levels within blocks and interleaving block types to prevent systematic biases. Through post-hoc analysis we determined that: 1) The animal performed similar numbers of trials on control and inactivation days suggesting no change in motivation level; 2) Animals performed at a high rate on high-visibility trials on all days, suggesting no distraction at the time scale of trials. The experimental configuration ensured that the screen covered a large portion of the animal’s visual field, minimizing space available for distractions. Therefore, we believe that we have minimized the effect of extraneous factors on behavior.

From the information we had provided with the first submission, it was probably difficult for the reviewers to get a sense of the behavioral paradigm and the degree of focus subjects had to pay in order to succeed at it. Perhaps the advantages of the touchscreen were overstated in the original submission; we have now revised the manuscript for a more balanced view. Overall, we believe that we are in agreement with the reviewer that this does not detract from the main results of our study.

Please see our responses to reviewer 1 for details. We have also made the following changes in the revised manuscript:

“The free-viewing nature of the task exploits natural visual search behavior to demonstrate the causal dependence of this behavior on face patch activity. […] The experimental configuration ensured that the screen covered a large portion of the animal’s visual field, minimizing space available for distractions. Therefore, we believe that we have minimized the effects of extraneous sources on behavior.”

2) The behavioral data from individual hemispheres/animals should be shown so that readers can gain a better sense of how consistent the impairment is across hemispheres/animals. The overlap between the estimated injection site and the fMRI defined face patches for each individual hemisphere should also be shown.

A) We have now included two new figure panels (Figures 2C and D) that now show the data from individual hemispheres/animals, demonstrating the consistency of the effect of inactivating ML on face detection.

“In Figures 2C and D, we plot the same quantities as above separated into each hemisphere and animal to demonstrate that the reduction in performance was consistent between subjects and inactivated hemispheres. At the single-hemisphere level, we observed significant differences mainly at intermediate visibility levels (p<0.05; FDR-corrected paired t-test after logit transform).”

We have now quantified the overlap between the injection site and ML, and provided a summary in new figure panel Figure 1D that is also broken down into individual hemispheres/animals.

“On average, 76% of the half-maximal extent of the injection volume overlapped with ML volume exceeding a threshold of t>20.”

“The off-target injection volumes barely overlapped with ML (9.6%; Figure 1D) or with any other face patch, and consequently, we observed only a weak and insignificant reduction in detection performance (Figures 2C,D, and 3B; blue).”

“To evaluate overlap between the muscimol injection and the functionally-defined bounds of ML, we used AFNI to construct a binary mask volume that isolated t-values greater than 20 in the (rhesus faces + cynomolgus faces) > all other objects contrast. […] The injection overlap was then defined as the volume of overlap divided by the volume of the injection.”

For statistical power, we now correct for multiple comparisons using the false discovery rate (FDR) instead of using the Bonferroni-Holm correction (mentioned in manuscript wherever changed).

3) Although the largest effects seem to occur for faces, inspection of Figure 3 suggests that inactivation affects detection of all three categories, although faces appear most affected. Thus, it is unclear whether the effect is truly limited to faces, or whether it a smaller impairment is also found in other categories. This issue should be addressed clearly in revision.

We approached this question in three ways:

A) We grouped the ‘body’ and ‘shoe’ categories into a single ‘non-face’ category and used the same ANCOVA analysis that yielded significant results for the Face/body/shoe analysis to test whether there was a significant difference between non-face control and inactivated conditions. We did not find a significant difference. A similarly grouped ANOVA analysis of thresholds also failed to reveal any non- face effects if inactivation.

“Although ML inactivation did not cause statistically significant performance deficits or threshold shifts for the other object categories, it is possible that the data might contain a non-specific component, as indicated by the non-zero values of performance deficits and thresholds for body parts and shoes (Figures 3C and D). […] Similarly, grouping the ‘body’ and ‘shoe’ categories in the ANOVA analysis of threshold failed to yield a significant difference in threshold.”

B) To determine if the cause was a change in motivation on inactivation days (per reviewer 1), we compared the number of trials the animal initiated on control and inactivation days, and did not find a significant difference.

“If a weak non-specific effect truly existed, we could not attribute this to the motivation level of the animal post injection and MRI scan, as there were no significant differences between the total number of trials the animal initiated on control or inactivation days (paired t-test, p = 0.47).”

C) We have tempered our claim with regard to category selectivity (per reviewer 2) by discussing a potential drawback of our method:

“Alternately, although 76% of the injection volume was contained within ML, it is possible that some muscimol diffused out of ML and into neighboring body patches (Popivanov et al., 2012) in some cases, causing a smaller, non-significant effect on that object category. “

4) All reviewers had concerns about the location of the muscimol injections in both experimental and control conditions. This information needs to be provided in a very clear form, and the interpretation of the results of each specific injection should be made clear.

Please see response to Essential revisions #1. We have now quantified the overlap between the injection and ML in each case, and present these data for individual subjects/hemispheres in new figure panel Figure 1D. As we demonstrate, the experimental muscimol injections largely overlapped with ML (76% overlap) whereas the control injections did not (9.6% overlap).

Secondly, we now describe the locations of the control injections in much more detail. We chose these off-target sites because we attempted to generate a range of injections that were in the near vicinity of the face patch. The sham injections (n=10) fell into three broad subgroups: injections into the white matter directly beneath the face patch (n=3), injections dorsal to the STS (n=3; 2 of which were in gray matter), and injections posterior and lateral to face patch ML (n=4). The first subgroup was intended to function as a sham group, where the animal experiences identical experimental procedures, has muscimol in the brain, but likely does not affect visual cortex activity. In the second and third subgroup, non-face selective areas were inactivated. We did not find statistically significant differences between these subgroups, which is why they were combined into an ‘off-target’ group for analysis. Please see our response to reviewer 1 for more details.

The revised manuscript now contains these details:

“Off-target sites included locations dorsal to the STS (n = 3, 2 in gray matter), deeper locations comprising the white matter beneath ML (n = 3), and locations posterior to ML (n = 4). […] We used off-target shams rather than injecting saline into ML in order to minimize risks of causing tissue damage and preserve the face patches for future electrophysiological studies.”

Reviewer #1:

The present study has several shortcomings, however. Although I feel that these drawbacks do not affect their main result – an impairment of face detection when inactivating ML -, they make a quantitative assessment of the impairment impossible, which is a missed opportunity. First, their "naturalistic" task suffers from a major problem when one wants to quantify detection behavior: if the animal fails to respond to the display that does not mean that the animal did not detect the stimulus. The authors did not measure eye movements of the animals during the detection task and thus an unknown portion of "no go" responses may have resulted not from detection failures but simply from the animals not looking at the display or not performing the task. This causes problems when attempting to compute the proportion of correct trials: what to take as an "executed" trial?

We agree that the lack of eye-tracking in our data was not sufficiently discussed in the original submission. As we stated earlier in the “Essential revisions” section, this is a question that we have asked ourselves since the inception of this project. In the majority of paradigms we use in the lab, eye- tracking plays a central role. However, we realized that the behavioral study we planned had a lot of similarity to human visual search and object recognition studies that are based on free viewing, and realized that by using a “naturalistic” task, we could add a lot more to the debate on the dependence of behavior on neural activity than would have been possible using head-fixed preparations.

We emphasize the reason we decided to use a touchscreen-based, free-viewing task: previously published studies that have manipulated the activity of ventral stream areas have almost exclusively used a head-fixed preparation. As the reviewer points out in his comment below, these studies (such as the ones by Afraz) have shed light on specific areas of the visual field that are impacted by inactivating the topographically corresponding areas of cortex. Our approach is complementary to the head-fixed studies which may offer better quantification of behavior, but at the expense of a constrained experimental prep.

Having chosen a free-viewing paradigm, we designed our task to minimize possibilities of distraction. First, animals initiated individual trials voluntarily by touching a central disc that was about the same size as the target objects. This required that the animal direct its attention and gaze to the center of the screen, and perform a deliberate and precise action to initiate a trial. The color of the central disc cued the animal to the target category; therefore, it was beneficial for the animal to pay attention to the disc while initiating the trial. For these reasons, our design ensures that the animal is ready and attentive when initiating the trial. From this, we conclude that the animal voluntarily initiates trials with the intent to complete them.

As with most behavioral studies, we worked under the assumption that the animal sought to maximize its reward within the time available for behavior. In our case, the animals knew that they had ~2 hours to complete the task (trained as such because of the time scale of muscimol effectiveness). On a day to day basis, we did not find significant differences in the number of trials completed by the animal in the control and inactivation days, suggesting that motivation on these two days was roughly constant. On the timescale of blocks, the animal still performed at 75% for high visibility levels, and performance decreased <10% after inactivation. Since the visibilities are interleaved and randomized, it is unlikely that distractions were happening at this time scale as well.

Thus, the only remaining theoretical possibility is that the animal was distracted after it initiated a trial (with intention to complete). Our task design minimizes this possibility because: 1) the animal only had 3 seconds to respond, restricting the time available for distractions, and 2) the touchscreen covers a large portion of the visual field, restricting the space available for distractions.

Finally, what the reviewer is pointing out here is a general problem with go/no-go tasks that is logically difficult to address even with eye tracking. When an animal (or human) does not generate a ‘go’ response after voluntarily initiating a trial, is it the result of a true rejection, or because of other factors such as a distraction that shifts the covert attention of the animal?

Therefore, while we agree with the reviewer that eye-tracking could offer better quantification of behavior, we believe that: 1) given previous studies in head-fixed animals, we could contribute innovatively by using free-viewing behavior, and 2) we designed our task to minimize potential for distractions. We also agree that this should not affect our main result.

In the revised manuscript, we now discuss this issue at some length:

“The free-viewing nature of the task exploits natural visual search behavior to demonstrate the causal dependence of this behavior on face patch activity. […] Therefore, while the lack of eye tracking does not affect the main results of our study, it introduces two sources of variability into our data.”

“Second, the lack of eye-tracking limits our ability to quantify behavioral strategies that may have been used by the subject to solve the task. […] Therefore, we believe that we have minimized the effects of extraneous sources on behavior.”

Second, they made unilateral inactivations which will have affected mainly the contralateral visual field (see Afraz et al., 2015). Thus, whether the target stimulus was in the affected part of the visual field will depend on the fixation pattern, which may have varied from trial to trial. This will have added noise to the psychometric functions for the inactivated condition and may have underestimated the deficit to an unknown degree. The data show that ML activation has an effect, but the size of the effect cannot be estimated.

The reviewer is correct. Our goal was to provide a lower bound on the effect of inactivation during natural behavior, not to estimate the effect precisely. We believe that we are in agreement with the reviewer in that this does not affect the main conclusions of the study.

We designed our task to test the dependence of one of the most fundamental of behaviors (face detection) on neural activity in a naturalistic context. A few previous studies (for example, Afraz et al., 2015) have demonstrated spatially restricted deficits using head-fixed preparations. As these studies show, and as the reviewer suggests, it is quite likely the case that the deficit is stronger in the contralateral visual hemifield. Nonetheless, the cited study reports drops in behavioral performance in the order of 2.0% to 5.5%, which can be considered modest effect sizes for a head fixed controlled experiment. With our current approach, we have shown that the effect size is at least an 8% threshold shift even in conditions where the animal is free to choose an optimal strategy. We think that this is a significant contribution to the debates around the causal effect of neural activity on behavior.

ML is also known to be largely foveal (Janssens et al., 2014, as well as electrophysiological data from our own lab). During visual search, animals and humans foveate a series of points that contain the most task-relevant information. Thus, by affecting ML activity, it is likely that we have affected a critically informative region of the visual field that leads to the observed deficits.

In the original submission, we had perhaps overstated the advantages of the touchscreen. In the revised manuscript, we now present a more balanced discussion of its disadvantages as well:

“The free-viewing nature of the task exploits natural visual search behavior to demonstrate the causal dependence of this behavior on face patch activity. […] Thus, performance deficits may have been higher at target locations that correspond to retinotopic location of the injection, and effect sizes reported here are to be taken as lower bound estimates.”

Third, the authors should show the behavioral data of the individual hemispheres/monkeys so that one can appreciate how consistent the impairment is across hemispheres/animals. Furthermore, they should show the overlap between the estimated injection site and the fMRI defined face patches for each individual hemisphere. Related to this, it appears from Figure 1D that in that monkey the injections were rather posterior and at best affected only part of ML. Is this true?

A) We have now included two new figure panels (Figures 2C and D) that now show the data from individual hemispheres/animals, demonstrating the consistency of the effect of inactivating ML on face detection.

“In Figures 2C and D, we plot the same quantities as above separated into each hemisphere and animal to demonstrate that the reduction in performance was consistent between subjects and inactivated hemispheres. At the single-hemisphere level, we observed significant differences mainly at intermediate visibility levels (p<0.05; FDR-corrected paired t-test after logit transform).”

B) We have now quantified the overlap between the injection site and ML, and provided a summary in new figure panel Figure 1D that is also broken down into individual hemispheres/animals.

“On average, 76% of the half-maximal extent of the injection volume overlapped with ML volume exceeding a threshold of t>20.”

“The off-target injection volumes barely overlapped with ML (9.6%; Figure 1D) or with any other face patch, and consequently, we observed only a weak and insignificant reduction in detection performance (Figures 2C, D, and 3B; blue).”

“To evaluate overlap between the muscimol injection and the functionally- defined bounds of ML, we used AFNI to construct a binary mask volume that isolated t-values greater than 20 in the (rhesus faces + cynomolgus faces) > all other objects contrast. […] We then used AFNI to compute the volume of the injection (in voxels), and the volumetric overlap between the injection and ML (at t>20). The injection overlap was then defined as the volume of overlap divided by the volume of the injection.”

With regard to the overlap of injection sites and ML in Figure 1D – the volumetric representation (Figure 1C) is a more realistic depiction of the injection site and ML than the surface visualization, for two reasons. First, the surface transform creates distortions especially at the lips of sulci. Second, we choose a depth mapping for projection (either a specific depth such as halfway in the gray matter, or a maximum projection) that does not present all the information on the display. Experimentally, we used a grid hole that gave us the best access to the center of the ML volume. This particular hemisphere was no different, and as our analysis shows, the injection was contained within ML in all animals.

Figure 3C and D suggest an impairment after each muscimol injection, although the effect is greater for face detection after ML injection than for the other 3 conditions. Could this aspecific part of the effect result from the possibility that the animals did not work well after the injection and following the MRI scan? Control injections with saline – standard in these sort of experiments – could have answered this concern.

We agree that this warrants additional analyses.

To answer the specific concern about motivation level, we compared the total number of trials the animal initiated on control and inactivation days. Animals performed about 900 trials on both days and we did not find a significant difference.

“If a weak non-specific effect truly existed, we could not attribute this to the motivation level of the animal post injection and MRI scan, as there were no significant differences between the total number of trials the animal initiated on control or inactivation days (paired t-test, p = 0.47).”

We did not perform saline injections into ML to minimize the risk of causing any tissue damage within the face patch and to preserve the face patch for future electrophysiological experiments. The off-target white-matter injections were intended to function as sham injections (see below).

Moreover, we conducted additional analyses where we grouped the ‘body’ and ‘shoe’ categories into a single ‘non-face’ category and used the same ANCOVA analysis that yielded significant results for the Face/body/shoe analysis to test whether there was a significant difference between non-face control and inactivated conditions. We did not find a significant difference. A similarly grouped ANOVA analysis of thresholds also failed to reveal any significant effects of inactivation for non-face stimuli.

“Although ML inactivation did not cause statistically significant performance deficits or threshold shifts for the other object categories, it is possible that the data might contain a non-specific component, as indicated by the non-zero values of performance deficits and thresholds for body parts and shoes (Figures 3C and D). […] Similarly, grouping the ‘body’ and ‘shoe’ categories in the ANOVA analysis of threshold failed to yield a significant difference in threshold.”

Therefore, while the effect sizes are indeed non-zero as the reviewer observes, we are not sure if we can draw any conclusions from those data in the absence of statistically significant results. Therefore, we have revised the manuscript with the above additional analyses to help the reader evaluate the data themselves.

Finally, as we demonstrate through new analyses, 76% of the injection volume was contained within ML, and it is possible that some muscimol diffused into neighboring body patches, causing a smaller effect. We now acknowledge this in the revised manuscript.

“Alternately, although 76% of the injection volume was contained within ML, it is possible that some muscimol diffused out of ML and into neighboring body patches (Popivanov et al., 2012) in some cases, causing a smaller, non-significant effect on that object category.”

I am puzzled by the site of the control injections: dorsal to the STS (where? In gray matter? Non-visual!) and deeper locations in the white matter near ML (subsection “Muscimol injection”). Especially the latter is puzzling since fibers are not affected by muscimol. What was the purpose of this? Sham injections? It would have been more informative to inject in nonface-patch regions of IT.

We chose these off-target sites because we attempted to generate a range of injections that were in the near vicinity of the face patch. The reviewer is correct in pointing out that these injections were intended to serve both as shams and as controls for inactivating non-face regions of IT cortex, and we now state so clearly in the revised version of the manuscript.

As we outlined earlier, we did not inject saline into the face patch as a control because we wanted to minimize the risk of causing tissue damage within the face patch. We took this precaution because: 1) the volumes of the injections were large (5uL), and 2) the sham and inactivation sessions were interleaved, and we did not want to adversely affect future inactivation sessions or electrophysiological recordings.

The off-target injections (n=10) fell into three broad categories: injections into the white matter directly beneath the face patch (n=3), injections dorsal to the STS (n=3; 2 of which were in gray matter), and injections posterior and lateral to face patch ML (n=4).

As the reviewer rightly points out, the white matter injections were intended to function as shams, as fibers do not express the GABA receptor and should therefore be unaffected by muscimol. These injections replicate all experimental procedures but leaves face patch activity intact. Injections dorsal to the STS again replicate all experimental procedures and do inactivate portions of cortex at the same A- P coordinates as ML, but do not affect face patch activity. Finally, injections posterior and lateral to ML were intended to inactivate non-face but visual regions of IT cortex. We did not find significant differences in the performance deficit between these three groups (ANOVA followed by pairwise post- hoc tests), which is why they were all combined into a single off-target group for analysis. In our new analysis of Injection overlap, the injections dorsal to the STS showed significantly lower overlap with ML (0%) than the other two sham groups.

In the revised manuscript, we now provide the following additional details:

“Off-target sites included locations dorsal to the STS (n = 3, 2 in gray matter), deeper locations comprising the white matter beneath ML (n = 3), and locations posterior to ML (n = 4). […] We used off-target shams rather than injecting saline into ML in order to minimize risks of causing tissue damage and preserve the face patches for future electrophysiological studies.”

Reviewer #2:

[…] 1) It is clear that ML inactivation has its strongest effect on the detection of faces. However, Figure 3A shows that ML inactivation also affects the detection of body and shoe categories. These non-selective effects did not reach statistical significance but I think that some readers will find hints of threshold increases for the shoe and body categories. I think no reasonable reader will expect 100% selectivity in any cortical inactivation study. Maybe the authors could make a more guarded interpretation in relation to category selectivity, or maybe they could comment on this.

We performed additional analyses to address this observation that the effect sizes are non-zero for body and shoe detection.

First, we grouped the ‘body’ and ‘shoe’ categories into a single ‘non-face’ category to increase statistical power – the number of trials in the non-face category doubles, providing a better estimate of the mean effect size, and the number of categories decrease, making multiple comparisons correction more powerful. We then used the same ANCOVA analysis that yielded significant results for the Face/body/shoe performance deficit analysis to test whether there was a significant difference between non-face control and inactivated conditions. We did not find a significant difference. A similarly grouped ANOVA analysis of thresholds also failed to reveal any non-face effects if inactivation.

“Although ML inactivation did not cause statistically significant performance deficits or threshold shifts for the other object categories, it is possible that the data might contain a non-specific component, as indicated by the non-zero values of performance deficits and thresholds for body parts and shoes (Figures 3C and D). […] Similarly, grouping the ‘body’ and ‘shoe’ categories in the ANOVA analysis of threshold failed to yield a significant difference in threshold.”

Second, to determine if the cause was a change in motivation on inactivation days (per Reviewer 1) that could result in a non-selective decrease in performance, we compared the number of trials the animal initiated on control and inactivation days, and did not find a significant difference.

“If a weak non-specific effect truly existed, we could not attribute this to the motivation level of the animal post injection and MRI scan, as there were no significant differences between the total number of trials the animal initiated on control or inactivation days (paired t-test, p = 0.47).”

Finally, following the reviewer’s suggestion, we offer a more cautious claim with regard to category selectivity by discussing a potential drawback of our method:

“Alternately, although 76% of the injection volume was contained within ML, it is possible that some muscimol diffused out of ML and into neighboring body patches (Popivanov et al., 2012) in some cases, causing a smaller, non-significant effect on that object category.”

“Thus ML inactivation is largely category-selective for faces, and anatomically specific for ML compared to its immediate surrounding.”

2) Figure 3B. There seems to be no data in relation to performance obtained from off-target inactivation in the shoe and body categories. Why is this?

This is because ‘off-target’ is well-defined for face patch ML as this is functionally mapped, but:

A) is not defined in this study with respect to ‘body’ and ‘shoe’ processing, and

B) ‘off-target’ ML inactivation may not be the same as an ‘off-target’ body injection or ‘off-target’ shoe injection.

Thus, for the purposes of this study and to avoid confusion, we only present analysis of off-target injections with respect to ML.

3) I wonder if the authors have data about the location and spread of the off-target injections. A schematic like the one in Figure 1D but for the off-target inactivation could help readers evaluate how off were the these injections from the ML area.

It might be misleading to display off-target data using a similar schematic because the sham injections are at different depths (including some in white matter). The surface mapping procedure typically maps values at a particular depth to the surface image. If we use a ‘max’ mapping, it will appear as though some sham injections are located within the face patch, but only because the difference in depth is ignored. Second, it is difficult to quantify the overlap between the injection and ML because of how the surface is generated from the volume (using triangular tessellation). For these reasons, we used analysis based on the anatomical volumes themselves to determine the bounds of the injection, of face patch ML, and quantify the overlap. Through this analysis, we show that ~76% of ML injection volumes are contained within ML, whereas only an ~10% volume of off-target injections overlap with ML.

We have now quantified the overlap between the injection site and ML, and provided a summary in new figure panel Figure 1D that is also broken down into individual hemispheres/animals.

“On average, 76% of the half-maximal extent of the injection volume overlapped with ML volume exceeding a threshold of t>20.”

“The off-target injection volumes barely overlapped with ML (9.6%; Figure 1D) or with any other face patch, and consequently, we observed only a weak and insignificant reduction in detection performance (Figures 2C, D, and 3B; blue).”

“To evaluate overlap between the muscimol injection and the functionally- defined bounds of ML, we used AFNI to construct a binary mask volume that isolated t-values greater than 20 in the (rhesus faces + cynomolgus faces) > all other objects contrast. […] The injection overlap was then defined as the volume of overlap divided by the volume of the injection.”

The locations of off-target injections are now better described in the revised manuscript.

“Off-target sites included locations dorsal to the STS (n = 3, 2 in gray matter), deeper locations comprising the white matter beneath ML (n = 3), and locations posterior to ML (n = 4). […] We used off-target shams rather than injecting saline into ML in order to minimize risks of causing tissue damage and preserve the face patches for future electrophysiological studies.”

4) From the data on Figure 2C-D one can imagine that there were some off-target and recovery sessions that reached a statistical significant effect. Am I correct? Please mark them on those panels.

There are some outliers for off-target and recovery sessions in Figures 2C and D as the reviewer rightly notes. But these represent single sessions, and we are not sure how we can test for statistical significance for these single samples. We analyzed the population of n=10 off-target sessions and n=11 recovery sessions, and found no statistical differences. We are happy to perform any specific analyses the reviewer might have in mind.

5) The analyses described in the sixth paragraph of the Results and Discussion seemed redundant to me. Yes, a displacement in detection threshold can be illustrated as a reduction in face visibility but I am not sure what it is gained by doing that.

This analysis was included because to a general reader, it may be unclear what a “11% deficit” or an “8% threshold shift” might represent. By performing these analyses, we transformed these “percent deficits” into actual images, illustrating the perceptually dramatic effects that an 8% threshold shift represents. A ‘small’ perceived effect size (8%) is a perceptually dramatic effect because of the nonlinearity of the psychophysical contrast-response function, and this is illustrated by these analyses.

As both reviewers point out, there was one result that should be completely expected by the displacement in threshold, and we have now stated this explicitly:

“As expected from the increased detection threshold, equivalent inactivated visibilities for faces were significantly lower than baseline visibilities (p<0.01; paired t-test).”

6) The effects of muscimol decreased with each exposure. This is an interesting finding that I think is worth showing in a data figure. Please show the effect of muscimol as a function of experimental session.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed before a final decision can be made, as outlined below:

1) Please address the injection overlap issue identified by the first reviewer, and if possible provide a more quantitative relationship between inactivation and behavior.

We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. In our past revision, we chose a metric with respect to the injection volume because we had interpreted the concerns of both reviewers in the previous round of reviews as being about the extent to which non-target injections may have leaked into ML. We agree with reviewer 1’s point, however, and for this revision, we:

1) calculated the overlap metric with respect to ML volume inactivated as well,

2) updated Figure 1 to include both overlap metrics, and

3) presented the actual volumes of ML and the injection (at the chosen thresholds) to address the concern that our injections were much smaller in volume than ML.

“The goal of injections was to inactivate as much of ML as possible with as little inactivation of outside regions as possible – and the converse for the off-target injections. […] The corresponding numbers for off-target injections were 4% ML coverage with 10% of the injection contained within ML, and it is thus unlikely that off-target injections, though spaced closely enough to ML to generate an overlap, silenced many face-selective neurons.”

We thank the reviewer for suggesting further analyses of the relationship between the precise properties of the injections and behavioral effects. We performed a number of such analyses. In particular, we correlated the average deficit in face-detection against injection overlap using a couple of different methods. We saw trends, but no significant correlations. In hindsight, this was probably expected, given the relative consistency of injection overlap with ML across experiments, as well as the complexity of injection geometry and of the behavior under study, but it is good to verify this as suggested. The main quantitative difference really is the one between injections inside and outside of area ML. We are now including them in the revised manuscript as follows:

“The mean face detection performance deficit was not significantly correlated with the ML-injection overlap (Spearman’s Rho rs = 0.42; p = 0.1). An ANCOVA analysis with session number as independent groups and ML-injection overlap as a covariate did not reveal a significant effect of ML-injection overlap on the observed deficit (df = 1; F = 1.35; p = 0.25). These data suggest that although there are differences in the extent of ML-injection overlap between sessions, since the central portion of ML was targeted by the injection, performance deficits were largely unaffected by these small differences in injection volume and overlap.”

Reviewer #1:

Although the authors attempt to defend the use of a "naturalistic" free-viewing task to study the effect of inactivation of a face patch (located in a region with a rough retinotopy), I still feel that this study would have provided more information on the contribution of that face patch to face detection if they had use a well-controlled task (with at least proper eye movement control). Despite this shortcoming, the study shows clear face detection performance deficits, which is a valuable addition to the literature.

I have the following comment on the revised text, which needs to be addressed.

The injection overlap was defined as the volume of overlap (between ML and injection) divided by the injection volume. However, this implies that a small injection volume inside a larger ML volume will get a 100% overlap, while only part of ML was inactivated. They should also provide the proportion of overlap computed relative to the ML volume. This metric reflects how much of ML was inactivated, which is unknown now (especially given the "distorted" map shown in Figure 1), but is important for evaluation of the behavioral effects.

We chose the metric relative to injection volume because from the comments of both reviewers in our previous revision, we understood that the concern was about off-target injections diffusing into ML. In this revision, we also provide a metric relative to ML volume, and provide actual volumes of the injection and ML. Figure 1D now plots both metrics for individual hemispheres. We hope that this addresses the reviewers concern about a small injection contained inside a large ML volume.

“The goal of injections was to inactivate as much of ML as possible with as little inactivation of outside regions as possible – and the converse for the off-target injections. […] The corresponding numbers for off-target injections were 4% ML coverage with 10% of the injection contained within ML, and it is thus unlikely that many face-selective neurons were silenced during off-target injections.”

Reviewer #2: […] However, they do not discuss my query: "6) The effects of muscimol decreased with each exposure. This is an interesting finding that I think is worth showing in a data figure. Please show the effect of muscimol as a function of experimentalsession."

I think they missed pasting the response on the Response to Reviewers file because this effect is now shown and analyzed in the revised version of the manuscript (new panel on Figure 3E).

This was indeed the case – we missed pasting our response into the response file. Figure 3E in Revision 1 was meant to address this point.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

The reviewers were very happy with your revisions and we are ready to accept the manuscript. However, one reviewer requested a small technical change to correct some missing information. In the manuscript you currently report one degree of freedom for the ANCOVA, but there should be two degrees of freedom reported [e.g., in the following format F(df1,df2) = x]. Please provide the other degree of freedom here, or provide an explanation for why this is not required.

The source of this confusion is now clear to us – while we had taken the reviewer comment from the last review as an analysis problem (running one-way instead of two-way ANOVA), we realize now that it is actually a reporting style issue. We have now updated our reporting to APA guidelines as suggested:

“(F(6, 752)=17.89; p<0.0001)”

“(ANOVA; F(6, 65)=5.74; p<0.0001)”

“face-detection deficit (F(1,102) = 1.35; p = 0.25)”

“(F(1,752)=15.33; p<0.0001)”


Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

RESOURCES