Skip to main content
PMC Canada Author Manuscripts logoLink to PMC Canada Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Apr 4.
Published in final edited form as: J Biol Rhythms. 2012 Jun;27(3):257–264. doi: 10.1177/0748730412441172

Does pupil constriction under blue and green monochromatic light exposure change with age?

Véronique Daneault 1,2,6,#, Gilles Vandewalle 1,2,6,#, Marc Hébert 3, Petteri Teikari 4,5, Ludovic S Mure 4,5, Julien Doyon 1,6, Claude Gronfier 4,5, Howard M Cooper 4,5, Marie Dumont 2, Julie Carrier 1,2,6,*
PMCID: PMC5380439  CAMSID: CAMS6599  PMID: 22653894

Abstract

Many non-visual functions are regulated by light through a photoreceptive system involving melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion cells that are maximally sensitive to blue light. Several studies have suggested that the ability of light to modulate circadian entrainment and to induce acute effects on melatonin secretion, subjective alertness and gene expression, decreases during aging, particularly for blue light. This could contribute to the documented changes in sleep and circadian regulatory processes with aging. However, age-related modification in the impact of light on steady-state pupil constriction, which regulates the amount of light reaching the retina, is not demonstrated. We measured pupil size in 16 young (22.8±4y) and 14 older (61±4.4y) healthy subjects during 45s exposures to blue (480nm) and green (550nm) monochromatic lights at low (7×1012 photons/cm2/s), medium (3×1013 photons/cm2/s), and high (1014 photons/cm2/s) irradiance levels. Results showed that young subjects had consistently larger pupils than older subjects, for dark adaptation and during all light exposures. Steady-state pupil constriction was greater under blue than green light exposure in both age groups and increased with increasing irradiance. Surprisingly, when expressed in relation to baseline pupil size, no significant age-related differences were observed in pupil constriction. The observed reduction in pupil size in older individuals, both in darkness and during light exposure, may reduce retinal illumination and consequently affect non-visual responses to light. The absence of a significant difference between age groups for relative steady-state pupil constriction suggests that other factors such as tonic, sympathetic control of pupil dilation, rather than light sensitivity per se, account for the observed age difference in pupil size regulation. Compared to other nonvisual functions, the light sensitivity of steady-state pupil constriction appears to remain relatively intact and is not profoundly altered by age.

Keywords: pupil constriction, pupil light reflex (PLR), aging, monochromatic light, circadian, non-visual responses to light, melanopsin

INTRODUCTION

Light is perceived by the visual system to allow conscious vision but also by a photoreceptive system based on light sensitive melanopsin-expressing, intrinsically photosensitive ganglion cells (ipRGCs) optimized to detect changes in light irradiance rather than to participate in image formation (Brown et al. 2010; Hatori and Panda 2010; Schmidt et al. 2011). This non-image-forming (NIF) photoreceptive system is responsible for circadian entrainment but also acutely regulates many other non-visual functions such as melatonin secretion, sleep, alertness, performance, cognitive brain functions and pupillary constriction (Brainard and Hanifin 2005; Chellappa et al. 2011; Hatori and Panda 2010; Schmidt et al. 2011; Vandewalle et al. 2009). The NIF system shows a peak sensitivity at around 460–480 nm (blue light) (Brainard and Hanifin 2005; Mure et al. 2009) so that the impact of light on non-visual functions is greater under shorter-wavelength blue light compared to lights of longer-wavelength such as green light (Cajochen et al. 2005; Lockley et al. 2003; Lockley et al. 2006; Mure et al. 2009; Mure et al. 2007; Vandewalle et al. 2011; Vandewalle et al. 2009). This maximal sensitivity to short wavelength light is primarily due to the recruitment of melanopsin ipRGCs, which also receive modulatory inputs from rods and cones (Berson 2003; Hatori and Panda 2010; Provencio et al. 2000; Schmidt et al. 2011). IpRGCs project directly to numerous brain regions including the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN), site of the master circadian clock, the ventro-lateral preoptic nucleus (VLPO) which contains sleep active neurons, and the olivary pretectal nucleus (OPN), crucial for pupillary constriction (Berson et al. 2010; Hatori and Panda 2010; Schmidt et al. 2011).

Aging is associated with changes in functions affected by the non-visual photoreception system including sleep and circadian entrainment (Turner et al. 2010). For instance, studies have demonstrated that healthy aging is associated with advanced sleep timing, more wakefulness during the sleep episode, and increased rate of napping (Buysse et al. 1992; Carrier et al. 2001; Carrier et al. 1997). Age-related changes in the circadian timing system such as phase advance, reduced amplitude of circadian rhythms and problems to adapt to circadian challenges (e.g., shift work, jet lag) have also been reported (Carrier et al. 1996; Duffy et al. 1998; Kawinska et al. 2005; Monk et al. 1995). These age-related modifications may be triggered in part by a decreased sensitivity to light or by the decreased ability for light to drive non-visual functions. Indeed, the impact of light on the circadian phase, and therefore on circadian entrainment, seems to diminish with aging [significant impact found in (Duffy et al. 2007) but not in (Sletten et al. 2009)]. Recent evidence also suggests that acute effects of light such as melatonin suppression, subjective alertness enhancement and induction of PERIOD2 expression, decrease with age for blue but not for green light (Herljevic et al. 2005; Jud et al. 2009; Sletten et al. 2009). However, age-related alterations in the impact of light on pupil light reflex (PLR) that regulates the amount of light reaching the retina, remain poorly understood.

Major modifications occur with aging at the level of the eye such as a decrease in scotopic and photopic sensitivity, as well as in the number of retinal photoreceptors (Freund et al. 2011; Hebert et al. 2004; Sturr et al. 1997). Due to progressive opacification and yellowing of the lens, light transmission decreases with age, especially for shorter wavelength (blue) light (Kessel et al. 2010). In addition, aging is associated with a substantial decrease in pupil size (senile miosis) in darkness and under various light irradiance levels (Bitsios et al. 1996; Winn et al. 1994). PLR is characterized by an initial, transient and robust phasic constriction at light onset followed by a sustained steady-state tonic response at a lower constriction level (McDougal and Gamlin 2010; Mure et al. 2009). Age-related changes in the phasic response have been observed (Bitsios et al. 1996). Yet, how aging affects the wavelength sensitivity of steady-state pupil constriction and its response as a function of changes in irradiance is unknown.

In the present study, we first assessed how the amount of light reaching the retina changes with age by measuring pupil size under green (550nm) and blue (480nm) light exposures at different irradiance levels in healthy young and older individuals. We then assessed whether age-related changes in pupil size affected steady-state PLR, defined here as the relative change in pupil size under these light conditions, and whether the ability of light to trigger this non-visual response changes with aging. We anticipated that, in older compared to young subjects, a smaller pupil size would be observed under all light conditions, accompanied with a reduction in steady-state PLR, particularly for blue light.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

Healthy subjects, 16 young (22.8 ± 4y; 10 females) and 14 older (61 ± 4.4y; 10 females), participated in the present study. Recruitment interviews established the absence of medical, traumatic, psychiatric, or sleep disorders. Questionnaires were used to exclude candidates with extreme chronotypes (Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire, MEQ, ≤ 30; ≥ 70) (Horne and Ostberg 1976), poor sleep quality (Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index, PSQI, ≥ 7) (Buysse et al. 1989), high anxiety (Beck Anxiety Inventory, BAI, ≥ 11) (Beck et al. 1988) or depression (Beck Depression Inventory-II, BDI-II, ≥ 11) (Steer et al. 1997) scores (Table 1). Candidates with a body mass index > 27 were excluded. None had worked on night shifts during the preceding year or travelled across more than one time zone during the last two months. All participants were nonsmokers, moderate caffeine and alcohol consumers and were not using medication, except for hormonal contraceptive in 9 out of 10 young women. Prior to participation, subjects went through an extensive professional ocular examination to confirm normal color vision and absence of ocular problems. Lens opacification as well as the absence of cataract was assessed subjectively using the Lens Opacities Classification System III [LOCS-III, from 1, clear lens, to 5, cataract; (Chylack et al. 1993)]. All young subjects had clear lenses (level 1) and all older subjects’ lenses were classified below level 4. This experiment was performed in accordance with institutional guidelines and received the necessary ethical approvals. Written informed consent was obtained from each subject.

Table 1.

Subjects’ characteristics (number of subjects or mean ± SD).

Older Subjects (≥ 55y; n=14) Younger Subjects (18–30y; n=16) P value

Age 61.0 ± 4.4 22.8 ± 4.0 ---
Gender (Men / Women) 4M / 10W 6M / 10W 0.6
Depression Score (BDI-II) 3 ± 4.3 3 ± 2.0 1.0
Anxiety Score (BAI) 1.9 ± 3.0 3.9 ± 3.7 0.13
Sleep Disturbance Score (PSQI) 3.2 ± 1.4 2.9 ± 1.3 0.5
Chronotype Score (MEQ) 55.9 ± 8.3 54.1 ± 10.7 0.6
Ethnicity (Afro-American / Caucasian) 0 / 14 1 / 15 0.9
Date of study (dd/mm/yy) 27/10/09 ± 30 days 26/10/09 ± 34 days 1.0
Clock time of study 14:04 ± 2:57 13:58 ± 2:44 0.9
Lens Opacification (LOCS-III) 2.4 ± 0.7 1.0 ± 0 <0.001
Stimuli Order: Blue before Green (BG) / Green before Blue (GB) 9 BG / 5 GB 8 BG / 8 GB 0.4

BDI-II: Beck Depression Inventory -II; BAI: Beck Anxiety Inventory; PSQI: Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; MEQ: Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire; LOCS-III: Lens Opacities Classification System -III. See text for inclusion criteria and references.

Protocol

Upon arrival, subjects were dark-adapted for 15 minutes (0 lux) and baseline pupil size was assessed at the end of this adaptation period (Figure 1). Pupil size was then measured under blue (480nm, full width at half maximum -FWHM -: 10nm) and green (550nm, FWHM: 10nm) monochromatic light exposures presented at low (7×1012 photons/cm2/s), medium (3×1013ph/cm2/s), and high (1014 ph/cm2/s) irradiance levels. Light exposures lasted 45s, during which subjects were asked to maintain a fixed gaze, and were separated by 2 min of darkness (0 lux, free gaze or eyes closed). Three light exposures of each wavelength were administered for the three irradiance levels (pseudo-random order; high irradiance was never administered first). The order of blue and green monochromatic lights was counterbalanced between subjects and groups. Pre-exposure pupil size was assessed at the end of each 2 min period in darkness. Note that we verified that pseudo-randomization prevented an exposure to significantly affect the subsequent exposure (supplementary material). Data were collected during the day between 10:00h and 20:00h. Average clock time of testing did not differ between the two groups and all experiments were carried in the Fall season.

Figure 1. Experimental Protocol.

Figure 1

Example for one subject. Wavelength and irradiance were counterbalanced between subjects and groups. *↓ = baseline pupil value; **↓ = pre-exposure pupil value; D = Darkness; BL, BM, BH = Blue light; low, medium and high irradiances, respectively; GL, GM, GH = Green light; low, medium, high irradiance, respectively.

Light settings and data acquisition

Narrow interference band-pass filters (Edmund Optic, NJ, USA) were used to produce blue and green monochromatic lights using a white light source (PL950, Dolan-Jenner Industries, MA, USA). Switch between filters was achieved using a filter wheel (AB301-T, Spectral Products, NM, USA) inserted between the light source and an optic fiber (Dolan-Jenner Industries). The latter brought light to a diffusing glass placed 2cm in front of the subject’s left eye. The light source and the filter wheel were computer-controlled and synchronized with pupil data acquisition using COGENT2000 (http://www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/Cogent/) under Matlab 7.0.4 (Mathwork Inc., MA, USA). Light spectra were assessed at the level of the diffuser (Lightspex, GretagMacbeth, NY, USA) and confirmed to peak at 480nm and 550nm. Irradiance levels were verified using a calibrated radiometer (PM100D, Thorlabs, NJ, USA).

Pupil size was recorded using an eye tracking device (EyeFrame Scene systems, Arrington Research Inc., AZ, USA) consisting of an infrared camera mounted on eyeglass frames maintained at the level of the nasium. As PLR is a consensual mechanism (i.e. light in one eye constricts the pupil in the other eye), light was administered to the subjects’ left eye while pupil size was captured from the right eye. Pupils were not pharmacologically dilated because the aim of the present study was to investigate whether changes in pupil size affected steady-state PLR. In addition, it was shown that pupils of older individuals remain smaller relative to that of younger individuals even when pupil constriction is pharmacologically blocked (Kergoat et al. 2001). Pupil width and height (arbitrary units relative to frame of the display window on the computer screen) were acquired at a sampling rate of 60Hz and the display frame was constant for all acquisitions to ensure equivalence of the unit size.

Data analysis

Data were analyzed using Matlab 7.10. Artifacts in pupil measures, including eye movements and blinks, were excluded using a circularity coefficient exclusion criterion (ratio between width and height <0.70 or >1.30). Following published procedures (Canver et al. 2010; Wyatt 2010), pupil data was smoothed using a non-parametric locally weighted linear regression (smoothing type: rlowess; span: 0.8). In our experimental setting, the transition between light-on and light-off was not sharply regulated (~ 500ms for full illumination or extinction) therefore not allowing a reliable analysis of phasic PLR. As a consequence, we excluded the first 6s of each illumination to make sure that we only included a stable response and averaged pupil size during the remaining 39s of each light exposure (representative data are displayed in Supplemental Figure S1). Baseline pupil size and pre-exposure pupil size were estimated by averaging the last second (60 data points) of the 15 or 2 min of darkness prior to illumination, respectively. Before analyses, pupil size was inferred by computing the ellipsoidal surface obtained with width and height arbitrary units (π × 1/2 width × 1/2 height). Steady-state PLR was estimated by normalizing mean pupil size during the illumination according to baseline pupil size or pre-exposure pupil size. Statistical analyses were computed in SPSS 17.0 (IBM SPSS Statistics) using a two-tailed Student t test to assess difference in baseline pupil size, and a 3-way repeated-measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) with wavelength (blue, green) and irradiance (low, medium, high) as within-subject factors and age (young, older) as a between-subject factor. Due to technical issues, three older subjects had missing values (two men with data of 1 light condition missing and one woman with data of 2 light conditions missing). We estimated these missing values with the Yates replacement technique (Kirk 1968).

RESULTS

Analysis of baseline (dark adapted) pupil size (i.e. following 15min in darkness) showed that older subjects (arbitrary units –a.u.-, mean ± SEM: 0.08±0.009) had smaller pupil size than young subjects (0.11±0.008) [T (28) = 2.73, P =0.01]. This represents an average reduction in surface area of about 27% in older subjects.

The repeated-measure 3-way ANOVA on absolute pupil size during illumination (no normalization) revealed a significant interaction between age groups and irradiance [F (2, 56) = 7.29, P =0.006]. Older subjects showed smaller absolute pupil size at each irradiance level but the age-related difference was greater at low irradiances (low: young = 0.049±0.004, older = 0.030±0.004 [F (1, 28) = 11.47, P =0.002] -medium: young = 0.038±0.003, older = 0.024±0.003 [F (1, 28) = 10.05, P =0.004] -high: young = 0.031±0.002, older = 0.020±0.003 [F (1, 28) = 9.35, P =0.005]). This analysis also revealed a significant effect of wavelength [F (1, 28) = 4.69, P =0.039], with smaller pupil sizes under blue light (0.031±0.002) as compared to green light (0.033±0.002) (see Supplemental Figure S2 for display of absolute pupil size results). No other interactions were significant (light wavelength by age group: [F (1, 28) = 2.73, P =0.110] -light wavelength by light irradiance: [F (2, 56) = 0.287, P =0.683] -light wavelength by light irradiance by age group: [F (2, 56) = 0.055, P =0.898]).

We then normalized sustained pupil size during light exposure according to baseline pupil size to analyze PLR (Figure 2). The repeated-measure 3-way ANOVA of normalized pupil values revealed that blue light induced significantly more constriction (66.67% ± 2.10), than green light (64.47% ± 2.25) [F (1, 28) = 6.43, P =0.017]. Moreover, constriction was greater with increasing irradiances (low: 57.83% ± 2.71 -medium: 66.67% ± 2.04 -high: 72.21% ± 1.81) [F (2, 56) = 89.18, P < 0.001]. The main effect of age was not significant [F (1, 28) = 0.514, P = 0.479] and none of the interactions, including the age group by irradiance, were significant (irradiance by age group: [F (2, 56) = 1.78, P = 0.19], wavelength by age group: [F (2, 56) = 1.75, P = 0.20], wavelength by irradiance: [F (2, 56) = 0.24, P =0.75], wavelength by irradiance by age group: [F (2, 56) = 0.33, P =0.68]). Finally, statistical analyses performed on normalized pupil size data according to pre-exposure pupil size gave similar results (supplementary material).

Figure 2. Steady-State pupil constriction in young and older individuals.

Figure 2

Mean pupillary constriction relative to baseline ± SEM in each age group. Blue bars: blue light at low (L), medium (M), and high (H) irradiances; Green bars: green light at low (L), medium (M) and high irradiances (H). Effects of wavelength and irradiance levels were significant (*; p < 0.05) but there was no difference between age groups and no interaction with age (n.s.: non significant).

We also computed supplementary analyses, in which only the last 5 seconds of each illumination were included (from 40–45 seconds). The statistical results obtained are identical to those obtained previously both with absolute and normalized data (supplementary material).

DISCUSSION

This study investigated the effects of age on pupil size and steady-state PLR using shorter (blue, 480 nm) and longer (green, 550 nm) wavelength monochromatic light exposures at three different irradiance levels. Our results confirm that older subjects have smaller pupil size than young subjects (Bitsios et al. 1996; Winn et al. 1994). This difference was observed both after prolonged dark adaptation and during blue and green light exposures at the three irradiance levels. The observed interaction between irradiance level and age groups is in agreement with previous results that reported larger age-related decreases in pupil size under lower, compared to higher irradiance levels (Winn et al. 1994). Our results also support previous reports showing that PLR is greater under blue than green light exposure and for higher irradiances (Mure et al. 2009; West et al. 2011). However, despite the fact that irradiance levels exerted a stronger impact on the absolute pupil size of younger compared to older individuals, steady-state PLR did not differ between the two age groups for either blue or green light exposures at the 3 irradiance levels.

A previous study by Biotsis et al. (1996) showed no age-related differences under white light for the latency of the phasic PLR, while other parameters of the phasic response were affected by age (amplitude, maximum constriction velocity, maximum constriction acceleration), although steady-state pupil constriction was not investigated. At the eye level, senile miosis is one of the primary functional mechanisms reducing the amount of light reaching the retina (Winn et al. 1994). In our sample, the observed age-related reduction in pupil size (~30%) is comparable with previous reports in young and older populations (Bitsios et al. 1996). Senile miosis may constitute a contributing factor to the reduced impact of light on non-visual functions in older subjects (Herljevic et al. 2005; Jud et al. 2009; Sletten et al. 2009). Indeed, studies have reported a correlation between pupil size and light-induced melatonin suppression in young subjects (Gaddy et al. 1993; Higuchi et al. 2008). In our study, despite the 30% reduction in pupil size and the increase in lens opacification (professional assessment, see LOCS-III (Steer et al. 1997) score on Table 1), older subjects showed a steady-state PLR similar to that of younger subjects. Future studies should investigate the functional impact of age-related reduction in pupil size on other non-visual responses to light.

One explanation for the lack of difference could be that compensatory mechanisms may allow a normal steady-state PLR despite the age-related decrease in the amount of light reaching the retina. For instance, an increased sensitivity to light may develop with age to compensate for chronic exposure to lower levels of light due to lens opacification. Several studies in young individuals have shown that, following prolonged exposure to low light level, exposure to light induces stronger suppression and phase shift of melatonin secretion (Chang et al. 2011; Hebert et al. 2002; Jasser et al. 2006). An alternative explanation for the smaller pupil size in the elderly, despite the lack of a difference in relative pupil constriction, is a loss of tonic control of pupil dilation, previously suggested in a study of non-human primates (Clarke et al. 2003). The authors of the study reported that pharmacological blockade of the sympathetic pupillodilator pathway reduces pupil size but does not affect PLR dynamics to different broadband white light intensities. The idea of a loss of autonomic control in the aged is consistent with the results in our study that showed smaller absolute pupil areas in the older subjects, but no difference in steady-state PLR under blue or green wavelength light exposures.

A statistical power analysis on steady-state PLR indicated a small size effect for the main effect of age (0.13) that would require 230 subjects per group to reach a power of 0.80 (alpha = 0.05), supporting the notion that age did not affect the PLR response in the present study. However, interactions between group and irradiance and between group and wavelength showed medium effect sizes (0.24 and 0.30, respectively) that would require 42 and 45 subjects per group to reach a power of 0.80 (alpha = 0.05). Studies with larger samples are required to gain a better understanding of age-related interactions according to wavelength and irradiance light levels. Furthermore, future studies should consider to extend the duration of light exposure.

In general agreement with previous hypotheses (Revell and Skene 2010), our data suggest that, although aging alters several non-visual functions regulated by light, the degree to which the impact of light on these functions is reduced may vary considerably. It is worth noting that our sample was slightly younger than that of previous studies showing age-related differences in non-visual effects of light [61 y.o on average in our sample vs 65.8 y.o (Jud et al. 2009; Sletten et al. 2009) or 68.3 y.o. (Duffy et al. 2007)]. However, such small age differences appear unlikely to fully explain the very limited effect of aging on steady-state PLR in our data compared to that of other studies. Interestingly, another recent investigation did not find a significant impact of age on the ability of light to phase shift circadian rhythms (Sletten 2009), contrary to a previous investigation (Duffy et al. 2007).

The lack of age-related differences in the steady-state PLR response compared to that reported in other non-visual responses may be related to the involvement of different melanopsin ganglion cell types and their target brain structures, or the output pathways involved. For instance, recent studies in rodents have found that light sensitivity is not equivalent for all non-visual functions regulated by light. The sensitivity of the circadian system for entrainment and phase shift was greater than that of PLR or masking (Butler and Silver 2011; Hut et al. 2008). There is evidence that different populations of melanopsin-expressing ipRGCs may mediate these light sensitivity differences. Five types of these ipRGCs (M1 to M5) have been identified to date, their projections overlapping only partially (Berson et al. 2010; Ecker et al. 2010). In addition, two subtypes of M1 cells have been identified, with one subtype innervating the mainly SCN while the other projects to all other known brain targets of ipRGCs including the OPN (Chen et al. 2011). In this respect, it appears plausible that the impact of aging varies for different non-visual functions regulated by light, such as circadian entrainment, melatonin secretion, and PLR, which present different light sensitivities and are mediated, at least in part, by different populations of ipRGCs.

Supplementary Material

1

Acknowledgments

We thank Jean Paquet for his support on statistical analyses as well as CARMS members for their critical comments on the manuscript.

GRANTS

Financial support: IRSC, CRSNG, FRSQ, Fonds France-Canada pour la recherche, Fonds échanges FRSQ/INSERM, ARN, Rhône-Alpes Cible.

Footnotes

DISCLOSURE

No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the authors.

References

  1. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;56:893–897. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.56.6.893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Berson DM. Strange vision: ganglion cells as circadian photoreceptors. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26:314–320. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00130-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Berson DM, Castrucci AM, Provencio I. Morphology and mosaics of melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion cell types in mice. J Comp Neurol. 2010;518:2405–2422. doi: 10.1002/cne.22381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bitsios P, Prettyman R, Szabadi E. Changes in autonomic function with age: a study of pupillary kinetics in healthy young and old people. Age Ageing. 1996;25:432–438. doi: 10.1093/ageing/25.6.432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brainard GC, Hanifin JP. Photons, clocks, and consciousness. J Biol Rhythms. 2005;20:314–325. doi: 10.1177/0748730405278951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brown TM, Gias C, Hatori M, Keding SR, Semo M, Coffey PJ, Gigg J, Piggins HD, Panda S, Lucas RJ. Melanopsin contributions to irradiance coding in the thalamo-cortical visual system. PLoS Biol. 2010;8:e1000558. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Butler MP, Silver R. Divergent photic thresholds in the non-image-forming visual system: entrainment, masking and pupillary light reflex. Proc Biol Sci. 2011;278:745–750. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Buysse DJ, Browman KE, Monk TH, Reynolds CF, 3rd, Fasiczka AL, Kupfer DJ. Napping and 24-hour sleep/wake patterns in healthy elderly and young adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1992;40:779–786. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1992.tb01849.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, 3rd, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28:193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cajochen C, Munch M, Kobialka S, Krauchi K, Steiner R, Oelhafen P, Orgul S, Wirz-Justice A. High sensitivity of human melatonin, alertness, thermoregulation, and heart rate to short wavelength light. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2005;90:1311–1316. doi: 10.1210/jc.2004-0957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Canver MC, Canver AC, Revere KE, Amado D, Bennett J, Chung DC. Algorithm for pupillometric data analysis. Bioengineering Conference, Proceeding of the 2010 IEEE 36th Annual Northeast; 2010. pp. 1–2. [Google Scholar]
  12. Carrier J, Land S, Buysse DJ, Kupfer DJ, Monk TH. The effects of age and gender on sleep EEG power spectral density in the middle years of life (ages 20–60 years old) Psychophysiology. 2001;38:232–242. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Carrier J, Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Kupfer DJ. Amplitude reduction of the circadian temperature and sleep rhythms in the elderly. Chronobiol Int. 1996;13:373–386. doi: 10.3109/07420529609012661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Carrier J, Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Kupfer DJ. Sleep and morningness-eveningness in the ‘middle’ years of life (20–59 y) J Sleep Res. 1997;6:230–237. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1997.00230.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chang AM, Scheer FA, Czeisler CA. The human circadian system adapts to prior photic history. J Physiol. 2011;589:1095–1102. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.201194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chellappa SL, Steiner R, Blattner P, Oelhafen P, Gotz T, Cajochen C. Non-visual effects of light on melatonin, alertness and cognitive performance: can blue-enriched light keep us alert? PLoS One. 2011;6:e16429. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chen SK, Badea TC, Hattar S. Photoentrainment and pupillary light reflex are mediated by distinct populations of ipRGCs. Nature. 2011;476:92–95. doi: 10.1038/nature10206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chylack LT, Jr, Wolfe JK, Singer DM, Leske MC, Bullimore MA, Bailey IL, Friend J, McCarthy D, Wu SY. The Lens Opacities Classification System III. The Longitudinal Study of Cataract Study Group. Arch Ophthalmol. 1993;111:831–836. doi: 10.1001/archopht.1993.01090060119035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Clarke RJ, Zhang H, Gamlin PD. Characteristics of the pupillary light reflex in the alert rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol. 2003;89:3179–3189. doi: 10.1152/jn.01131.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Duffy JF, Dijk DJ, Klerman EB, Czeisler CA. Later endogenous circadian temperature nadir relative to an earlier wake time in older people. Am J Physiol. 1998;275:R1478–1487. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1998.275.5.r1478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Duffy JF, Zeitzer JM, Czeisler CA. Decreased sensitivity to phase-delaying effects of moderate intensity light in older subjects. Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28:799–807. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ecker JL, Dumitrescu ON, Wong KY, Alam NM, Chen SK, LeGates T, Renna JM, Prusky GT, Berson DM, Hattar S. Melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion-cell photoreceptors: cellular diversity and role in pattern vision. Neuron. 2010;67:49–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Freund PR, Watson J, Gilmour GS, Gaillard F, Sauve Y. Differential changes in retina function with normal aging in humans. Doc Ophthalmol. 2011;122:177–190. doi: 10.1007/s10633-011-9273-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gaddy JR, Rollag MD, Brainard GC. Pupil size regulation of threshold of light-induced melatonin suppression. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1993;77:1398–1401. doi: 10.1210/jcem.77.5.8077340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hatori M, Panda S. The emerging roles of melanopsin in behavioral adaptation to light. Trends Mol Med. 2010;16:435–446. doi: 10.1016/j.molmed.2010.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hebert M, Beattie CW, Tam EM, Yatham LN, Lam RW. Electroretinography in patients with winter seasonal affective disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2004;127:27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hebert M, Martin SK, Lee C, Eastman CI. The effects of prior light history on the suppression of melatonin by light in humans. J Pineal Res. 2002;33:198–203. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-079x.2002.01885.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Herljevic M, Middleton B, Thapan K, Skene DJ. Light-induced melatonin suppression: age-related reduction in response to short wavelength light. Exp Gerontol. 2005;40:237–242. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2004.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Higuchi S, Ishibashi K, Aritake S, Enomoto M, Hida A, Tamura M, Kozaki T, Motohashi Y, Mishima K. Inter-individual difference in pupil size correlates to suppression of melatonin by exposure to light. Neurosci Lett. 2008;440:23–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.05.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 1976;4:97–110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hut RA, Oklejewicz M, Rieux C, Cooper HM. Photic sensitivity ranges of hamster pupillary and circadian phase responses do not overlap. J Biol Rhythms. 2008;23:37–48. doi: 10.1177/0748730407311851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jasser SA, Hanifin JP, Rollag MD, Brainard GC. Dim light adaptation attenuates acute melatonin suppression in humans. J Biol Rhythms. 2006;21:394–404. doi: 10.1177/0748730406292391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jud C, Chappuis S, Revell VL, Sletten TL, Saaltink DJ, Cajochen C, Skene DJ, Albrecht U. Age-dependent alterations in human PER2 levels after early morning blue light exposure. Chronobiol Int. 2009;26:1462–1469. doi: 10.3109/07420520903385564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kawinska A, Dumont M, Selmaoui B, Paquet J, Carrier J. Are modifications of melatonin circadian rhythm in the middle years of life related to habitual patterns of light exposure? J Biol Rhythms. 2005;20:451–460. doi: 10.1177/0748730405280248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kergoat H, Kergoat MJ, Justino L. Age-related changes in the flash electroretinogram and oscillatory potentials in individuals age 75 and older. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49:1212–1217. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49239.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kessel L, Lundeman JH, Herbst K, Andersen TV, Larsen M. Age-related changes in the transmission properties of the human lens and their relevance to circadian entrainment. J Cataract Refract Surg. 2010;36:308–312. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrs.2009.08.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kirk RE. In: Experimental design: Procedures for the behavioral sciences. Brooks, editor. Cole Publishing Company; 1968. pp. 146–147. [Google Scholar]
  38. Lockley SW, Brainard GC, Czeisler CA. High sensitivity of the human circadian melatonin rhythm to resetting by short wavelength light. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2003;88:4502–4505. doi: 10.1210/jc.2003-030570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lockley SW, Evans EE, Scheer FA, Brainard GC, Czeisler CA, Aeschbach D. Short-wavelength sensitivity for the direct effects of light on alertness, vigilance, and the waking electroencephalogram in humans. Sleep. 2006;29:161–168. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. McDougal DH, Gamlin PD. The influence of intrinsically-photosensitive retinal ganglion cells on the spectral sensitivity and response dynamics of the human pupillary light reflex. Vision Res. 2010;50:72–87. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, 3rd, Kupfer DJ. Inducing jet lag in an older person: directional asymmetry. Exp Gerontol. 1995;30:137–145. doi: 10.1016/0531-5565(94)00059-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Mure LS, Cornut PL, Rieux C, Drouyer E, Denis P, Gronfier C, Cooper HM. Melanopsin bistability: a fly’s eye technology in the human retina. PLoS One. 2009;4:e5991. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Mure LS, Rieux C, Hattar S, Cooper HM. Melanopsin-dependent nonvisual responses: evidence for photopigment bistability in vivo. J Biol Rhythms. 2007;22:411–424. doi: 10.1177/0748730407306043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Provencio I, Rodriguez IR, Jiang G, Hayes WP, Moreira EF, Rollag MD. A novel human opsin in the inner retina. J Neurosci. 2000;20:600–605. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-02-00600.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Revell VL, Skene DJ. Impact of age on human non-visual responses to light. Sleep and biological rhythms. 2010;8:84–94. [Google Scholar]
  46. Schmidt TM, Chen SK, Hattar S. Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells: many subtypes, diverse functions. Trends Neurosci. 2011 doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sletten TL, Revell VL, Middleton B, Lederle KA, Skene DJ. Age-related changes in acute and phase-advancing responses to monochromatic light. J Biol Rhythms. 2009;24:73–84. doi: 10.1177/0748730408328973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri WF, Beck AT. Further evidence for the construct validity of the Beck depression Inventory-II with psychiatric outpatients. Psychol Rep. 1997;80:443–446. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1997.80.2.443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sturr JF, Zhang L, Taub HA, Hannon DJ, Jackowski MM. Psychophysical evidence for losses in rod sensitivity in the aging visual system. Vision Res. 1997;37:475–481. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(96)00196-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Turner PL, Van Someren EJ, Mainster MA. The role of environmental light in sleep and health: effects of ocular aging and cataract surgery. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14:269–280. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Vandewalle G, Archer SN, Wuillaume C, Balteau E, Degueldre C, Luxen A, Dijk DJ, Maquet P. Effects of light on cognitive brain responses depend on circadian phase and sleep homeostasis. J Biol Rhythms. 2011;26:249–259. doi: 10.1177/0748730411401736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Vandewalle G, Maquet P, Dijk DJ. Light as a modulator of cognitive brain function. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13:429–438. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. West KE, Jablonski MR, Warfield B, Cecil KS, James M, Ayers MA, Maida J, Bowen C, Sliney DH, Rollag MD, Hanifin JP, Brainard GC. Blue light from light-emitting diodes elicits a dose-dependent suppression of melatonin in humans. J Appl Physiol. 2011;110:619–626. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01413.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Winn B, Whitaker D, Elliott DB, Phillips NJ. Factors affecting light-adapted pupil size in normal human subjects. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1994;35:1132–1137. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Wyatt HJ. The human pupil and the use of video-based eyetrackers. Vision Research. 2010;50:1982–1988. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES