Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2017 Apr 5;12(4):e0174882. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0174882

Effects of living near an urban motorway on the wellbeing of local residents in deprived areas: Natural experimental study

Louise Foley 1,*, Richard Prins 1, Fiona Crawford 2, David Humphreys 3, Richard Mitchell 4, Shannon Sahlqvist 5, Hilary Thomson 6, David Ogilvie 1; on behalf of the M74 study team
Editor: Hajo Zeeb7
PMCID: PMC5381791  PMID: 28379993

Abstract

Background

Health and wellbeing are partly shaped by the neighbourhood environment. In 2011, an eight kilometre (five mile) extension to the M74 motorway was opened in Glasgow, Scotland, constructed through a predominantly urban, deprived area. We evaluated the effects of the new motorway on wellbeing in local residents.

Methods

This natural experimental study involved a longitudinal cohort (n = 365) and two cross-sectional samples (baseline n = 980; follow-up n = 978) recruited in 2005 and 2013. Adults from one of three study areas—surrounding the new motorway, another existing motorway, or no motorway—completed a postal survey. Within areas, individual measures of motorway proximity were calculated. Wellbeing was assessed with the mental (MCS-8) and physical (PCS-8) components of the SF-8 scale at both time points, and the short Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (SWEMWBS) at follow-up only.

Results

In multivariable linear regression analyses, cohort participants living nearer to the new M74 motorway experienced significantly reduced mental wellbeing over time (MCS-8: -3.6, 95% CI -6.6 to -0.7) compared to those living further away. In cross-sectional and repeat cross-sectional analyses, an interaction was found whereby participants with a chronic condition living nearer to the established M8 motorway experienced reduced (MCS-8: -3.7, 95% CI -8.3 to 0.9) or poorer (SWEMWBS: -1.1, 95% CI -2.0 to -0.3) mental wellbeing compared to those living further away.

Conclusions

We found some evidence that living near to a new motorway worsened local residents’ wellbeing. In an area with an existing motorway, negative impacts appeared to be concentrated in those with chronic conditions, which may exacerbate existing health inequalities and contribute to poorer health outcomes. Health impacts of this type of urban regeneration intervention should be more fully taken into account in future policy and planning.

Introduction

The activity, health and wellbeing of individuals and populations are shaped by the social, physical and economic environments in which they live [13]. Urban regeneration projects are often touted as improving health and prosperity in deprived populations; however, there is limited evidence to support these claims [4, 5].

Urban regeneration refers to a myriad of activities including housing improvements and broader changes to neighbourhood public spaces [6]. Research indicates that urban regeneration has the potential to improve the wellbeing of local residents [79]. However, the evidence is inconclusive, and different aspects of urban regeneration, such as the construction of new motorways, might have different effects. Though motorways may improve mobility, roads and traffic have been shown in cross-sectional research to contribute to noise disturbance and severance (separation of residents from facilities or social networks) in local communities [1012]. Other studies indicate an association between noise disturbance from traffic [13], or living in industrial areas characterised by noise disturbance and air pollution [14], and poor quality of life or wellbeing. However, there are currently no longitudinal studies examining the long-term effects of motorways on wellbeing in local residents.

Urban regeneration in deprived neighbourhoods may also have implications for health inequalities, as deprivation is itself associated with poorer health and wellbeing [15, 16]. Positive impacts from regeneration in deprived areas might plausibly reduce inequalities at the population level. However, whilst previous regeneration projects have been associated with modest improvements in socioeconomic outcomes, the effects were not larger than corresponding national trends [5]. New major roads may contribute to area-level economic revival, but may also degrade the local environment, contributing to a process of ‘deprivation amplification’ [17] in vulnerable communities and widening existing inequalities.

It is not easy to parse urban regeneration ‘interventions’ into their components and establish causal relationships with behaviour or health, because such interventions are typically both complex and ill-suited to evaluation using randomised study designs. Natural experiments are a burgeoning field of public health research in which exposure to an intervention is not manipulated by the researcher, but is nevertheless used to enable controlled comparisons of outcomes over time [18].

The M74 motorway extension in Glasgow, Scotland was a long-standing dormant transport infrastructure project that was revived in the early 2000s, with the primary aim of reducing traffic congestion on the existing motorway network. The construction of this eight kilometre (five mile), six-lane section of motorway through a predominantly urban, deprived area involved a major change to the landscape. The new motorway was mostly raised above ground, and in addition to the road itself, involved the construction of four motorway junctions, new bridges over existing local roads, the demolition of buildings along the proposed route and the concurrent construction of a new residential development close to one of the new junctions.

This presented an opportunity to examine the activity and health impacts of new transport infrastructure using a natural experimental design. In this study, we aimed to contribute to this evidence base by (a) evaluating the effects of living near an urban motorway on wellbeing in local communities, and (b) exploring potential moderators of this relationship.

Methods

Context

Glasgow (593,200 inhabitants) [19] is the fourth largest city in the United Kingdom (UK), has the lowest life-expectancy in the UK [20], and is characterised by extremes of affluence and deprivation [21].

Design

We conducted a quasi-experimental evaluation of a natural experiment, examining the effects of the M74 motorway extension on the travel and activity patterns, injuries and wellbeing of residents in the local area. The study consisted of a longitudinal cohort within two distinct cross-sectional samples recruited at baseline (2005) and follow-up (2013).

The study was approved by University of Glasgow Ethics Committees (baseline reference FM01304; follow-up reference 400120077). If participants completed and returned a postal survey to the study team, this was taken as implied consent for the data to be used for the purpose of the study. This approach was reviewed and approved by University of Glasgow Ethics Committees at both time points.

Further information on the baseline study hypotheses, methods [22] and sample characteristics [23] can be found elsewhere.

Study areas

Prior to baseline data collection, three local study areas in Glasgow were defined: an area surrounding the new M74 motorway extension (South); an area surrounding the established M8 motorway, which was built in the 1960s (East); and a control area containing a railway segment but no comparable motorway infrastructure (North) [23]. For a map of the study areas, see Ogilvie 2008 [23]. The areas were iteratively delineated in a Geographic Information System, using spatially referenced census and transport infrastructure data combined with field visits. This process ensured that the study areas had similar overall socioeconomic (e.g. levels of deprivation and unemployment, home and car ownership, and prevalence of chronic illness) and topographical characteristics, but differed in terms of containing a motorway [23]. All areas contained a mixture of residential and other land uses, a mixture of housing stock from traditional high-density tenement housing to new developments, and other major arterial roads.

Intervention

The M74 motorway extension cost approximately £800 million and opened in 2011. The motorway passes through or adjacent to several residential areas, with some homes situated within 50 metres of the carriageway (Fig 1).

Fig 1. Proximity of housing to M74 motorway extension.

Fig 1

Image copyright Amy Nimegeer.

Though it was primarily intended to reduce traffic congestion, health-related claims were made about the motorway by supporters within government, and by opponents including members of Scottish parliament, advocacy groups, local businesses and residents. These claims identified potential positive and negative effects of the new motorway on (active) travel, physical activity and wellbeing. At baseline, this dialogue was summarised into two competing overarching ‘hypotheses’: a virtuous cycle in which active travel, physical activity and wellbeing improved, and a vicious cycle in which all declined [22]. At follow-up, these were further developed into a logic model describing the putative causal chains and relationships to be tested a priori.

For wellbeing specifically, projected impacts that might worsen wellbeing included loss of green space, visual intrusion, increased traffic noise or vibration, reductions in air quality, severance, the undermining of community facilities and increasing inequalities. Projected benefits that could improve wellbeing included easing traffic on some local roads improving amenity for pedestrians, improved mobility and connectivity to the wider area, and economic regeneration.

Sampling and recruitment of participants

We recruited participants prior to motorway construction in 2005 (T1), and approximately two years after motorway opening in 2013 (T2). From the three defined study areas, eligible unit postcodes (the smallest unit of postal geography in the UK, corresponding to approximately 15 addresses on average) were identified and a random sample of private residential addresses was drawn from the Royal Mail Postcode Address File. Participants were adults aged 16 years or over who responded to the postal survey delivered to their home address. If more than one householder was eligible, the individual with the most recent birthday completed the survey. At baseline, participants were asked to return an optional consent form giving their permission to be contacted again in the future. Brief contact was maintained via yearly mailings which was intended to promote study retention by providing an ongoing reminder of the study and an opportunity for participants to alert the study team to potential changes in address or circumstance. At follow-up, all baseline participants with current contact details, as well as a new random sample drawn from the Royal Mail Postcode Address File, were mailed a survey.

At baseline, 3,000 surveys were mailed to each study area– 9,000 in total. At follow-up, baseline participants who could still be contacted were accounted for, and the sample for each study area was then topped up to 3,000 with new cross-sectional participants. Therefore, 9,000 surveys in total were mailed at each time point. A minimum achieved sample of 1,200 participants was required at each time point to adequately power the analysis of the primary outcome of the study (travel behaviour).

We followed recommendations to maximise response to postal surveys [24]. Potential participants were sent an initial notification postcard of the survey to come. The next week (the first week of October at both time points), they were mailed a survey and associated study documentation. Those who did not respond were sent the full study documentation a second time approximately one month later. All mailings were staggered over multiple days to maximise the probability that surveys would be completed on different days of the week. Respondents were entered into a £50 prize draw (at baseline) or received a £5 voucher (at follow-up). Responses received more than three months after the first mailing were disregarded, to minimise any effect of seasonal variation in activity patterns.

Measurement

The survey included items on demographic and socioeconomic characteristics, travel behaviour (including a recall of all travel undertaken on the previous day), physical activity (the short form of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire), health and wellbeing (including the SF-8 scale) and perceptions of the local neighbourhood (S1 Appendix).

Wellbeing

Wellbeing was assessed using the SF-8 at both time points, and the short version of the Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale [SWEMWBS] at follow-up only. While some researchers make a distinction between the concepts of health status, (health-related) quality of life and wellbeing, for ease we use the blanket term ‘wellbeing’ here to describe both the SF-8 and the SWEMWBS, acknowledging that these tools do capture somewhat different underlying constructs. The SWEMWBS measures psychological and eudemonic wellbeing, whereas the SF-8 has a dual focus on physical and mental health status, with a specific emphasis on functioning in daily life.

The SF-8 scale is an eight item survey assessing health status in the previous four weeks, derived from (and highly correlated with) the original 36 item version (SF-36) [25]. Items are scored on either 5- or 6-point Likert scales. Using standard procedures, physical and mental component scores (PCS-8 and MCS-8, respectively) were derived [25], whereby higher scores reflect better wellbeing. The SF-8 has been normalised in the general United States population, with mean PCS-8 and MCS-8 scores of 49 [25]. Longitudinal validation in a clinical population has indicated that the SF-8 is sensitive to change, with a clinically meaningful reduction in overall quality of life corresponding to a reduction of 3.0 units for PCS-8 and 3.3 units for MCS-8 [25]. Studies assessing the predictive validity of the original SF-36 scale indicate associations with job loss, use of primary care services, hospitalisation and five-year survival [26].

The SWEMWBS [27] is a seven item survey assessing positive mental wellbeing in the previous two weeks, derived from the original 14 item version (WEMWBS). Items are scored on 5-point Likert scales and summed to produce a total score, whereby higher scores reflect greater wellbeing. Using standard procedures, the raw total score was transformed into a metric score [27]. WEMWBS has acceptable psychometric properties [28] and its mean value in the 2012 Scottish Health Survey was 50 [29].

Exposure

In addition to the three study areas, we defined individual-level exposures. Using a GIS, we calculated the distance (metres) from the weighted population centroid of the unit postcode for each participant’s home address in a straight line to the nearest motorway infrastructure. We transformed this exposure using the negative natural log to produce a measure of proximity, whereby higher values reflected greater exposure. Hereafter, we use the term ‘proximity’ to refer specifically to this individual-level exposure.

Analysis

We explored differences in sample characteristics between study areas and time points, and between the longitudinal cohort and the remainder of the T1 sample, using one-way ANOVA, t and chi-squared tests. We then undertook three main analyses. The first examined within-participant change in the cohort, using SF-8. The second examined population-level change in the repeat cross-sectional sample (in which each participant provided SF-8 data at one time point). The third examined cross-sectional relationships in the full T2 sample, using SWEMWBS.

Linear regression analyses were carried out using Stata13 to assess the relationships of (a) study area and (b) individual-level exposure stratified by study area with (i) PCS-8, (ii) MCS-8 and (iii) SWEMWBS score. The final models were adjusted for age, sex, home ownership, car ownership, working status, perceived financial strain, presence of a chronic condition and years lived in the local area. Additionally, in the longitudinal analysis, we adjusted for the baseline value of the outcome of the model in question. When using study area as the exposure, we used the North area (no motorway) as the reference. For the repeat cross-sectional analyses we added a time point variable, whereby the coefficient of the interaction between time point and motorway exposure gave an indication of the population shift in the outcome over time. We did not impute data as there was less than 5% missing values for all variables.

Finally, we tested all maximally adjusted models for interactions with perceived financial strain and presence of a chronic condition. In models using individual-level exposure, interactions were tested only in the South and East (the areas with a new and an existing motorway, respectively).

Results

Response rate

1,345 completed surveys were returned at T1 and 1,343 at T2. After accounting for undeliverable survey packs, the response rate was 16.1% at T1 and 15.8% at T2. 365 participants formed the longitudinal cohort. The remaining 980 (T1) and 978 (T2) participants formed the repeat cross-sectional sample.

Differences between time points, study areas and samples

Changes in sociodemographic characteristics over time (i.e. age, working status and presence of a chronic condition) were consistent with an ageing cohort. However in the repeat cross-sectional sample, there was a higher proportion of men, car owners and participants with a chronic condition at T2 compared to T1, and the T2 sample was older on average than the T1 sample (Table 1).

Table 1. Characteristics of the longitudinal cohort, repeat cross-sectional sample and full T2 sample.

Variable Longitudinal cohort (n = 365) Repeat cross-sectional sample (T1 n = 980; T2 n = 978) Full T2 sample (n = 1343)
T1 T2 T1 T2 T2
n mean (SD) / % n mean (SD) / % n mean (SD) / % n mean (SD) / % n mean (SD) / %
Age (years) 360 50.4 (13.6) 363 58.5 (13.6)** 962 48.8 (18.3) 970 52.6 (16.5)** 1333 54.2 (16.0)
% male 361 43.5 363 44.4 970 37.1 972 42.8** 1335 43.2
% home ownership 360 61.1 363 62.5 965 47.9 971 49.6 1334 53.2
% car ownership 361 58.5 362 60.5 951 48.8 969 53.4** 1331 55.3
% working* 359 58.5 364 48.1** 961 48.3 972 48.3 1336 48.2
% with chronic condition 360 38.9 361 47.9** 955 39.0 964 43.9** 1325 45.0
% perceived financial strain 361 361 955 950 1311
    Quite comfortably off 11.9 12.5 4.9 5.2 7.2
    Can manage without difficulty 20.2 24.4 24.0 20.5 21.6
    Have to be careful with money 52.9 47.1 51.9 52.4 51.0
    Find it a strain to get by 15.0 16.1 19.2 21.9 20.3
Years lived in local area 365 18.3 (15.3) 362 24.9 (16.6)** 980 18.2 (18.0) 965 19.0 (17.4) 1327 20.6 (17.4)
SWEMWBS 1318 21.9 (4.1)
SF-8 PCS-8 352 47.4 (11.0) 360 45.9 (11.7)** 935 46.8 (11.8) 960 45.3 (12.1)**
SF-8 MCS-8 352 45.5 (11.1) 360 46.4 (11.1) 935 43.8 (11.6) 960 44.4 (12.1)

n–number; T–time point; SD–standard deviation; SF-8 MCS-8 –SF-8 mental component score; SF-8 PCS-8 –SF-8 physical component score; SWEMWBS–Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (short version)

*In paid employment (full or part-time), full-time student, or undertaking voluntary work

**Significant difference between time points within the same study sample (p<0.05)

In the longitudinal cohort, there were no significant sociodemographic differences between study areas at either time point. In the repeat cross-sectional sample, there were no significant sociodemographic differences between study areas at T1. However, at T2 on average participants in the North (no motorway) area were older, and participants in the South (new motorway) area perceived less financial strain and had lived in the local area for less time, than those in the other areas. In the T2 cross-sectional sample, participants in the South (new motorway) area perceived significantly less financial strain than those in the other areas (Table 2).

Table 2. Sociodemographic characteristics and unadjusted measures of wellbeing by study area and time point.

Variable Longitudinal cohort (n = 365) Repeat cross-sectional sample (T1 n = 980; T2 n = 978) Full T2 sample (n = 1343)
T1 T2 T1 T2 T2
n mean (SD) n mean (SD) n mean (SD) n mean (SD) n mean (SD)
Age (years)
    Total 360 50.4 (13.6) 363 58.5 (13.6) 962 48.8 (18.3) 970 52.6 (16.5)** 1333 54.2 (16.0)
    North 124 49.0 (13.3) 126 57.3 (13.4) 333 49.7 (18.2) 337 54.6 (16.0) 463 55.3 (15.4)
    East 111 51.3 (13.3) 112 59.4 (13.3) 317 48.5 (18.7) 329 51.8 (17.0) 441 53.7 (16.4)
    South 125 51.0 (14.1) 125 59.0 (14.1) 312 48.1 (17.8) 304 51.2 (16.4) 429 53.5 (16.1)
% male
    Total 361 43.5 363 44.4 970 37.1 972 42.8 1335 43.2
    North 125 37.6 126 38.9 337 36.2 337 43.3 463 42.1
    East 111 44.1 112 44.6 318 34.0 331 40.2 443 41.3
    South 125 48.8 125 49.6 315 41.3 304 45.1 429 46.4
% home ownership
    Total 360 61.1 363 62.5 965 47.9 971 49.6 1334 53.2
    North 125 60.8 126 62.7 337 46.3 336 50.3 462 53.7
    East 111 61.3 112 62.5 313 51.1 331 48.6 443 52.1
    South 124 61.3 125 62.4 315 46.4 304 50.0 429 53.6
% car ownership
    Total 361 58.5 362 60.5 951 48.8 969 53.4 1331 55.3
    North 125 61.6 126 65.9 332 49.4 336 54.8 462 57.8
    East 111 52.3 112 55.4 312 49.4 329 52.3 441 53.1
    South 125 60.8 124 59.7 307 47.6 304 53.0 428 54.9
% working*
    Total 359 58.5 364 48.1 961 48.3 972 48.3 1336 48.2
    North 125 60.8 127 50.4 333 47.2 338 44.4 465 46.0
    East 110 54.6 112 46.4 315 48.9 330 49.7 442 48.9
    South 124 59.7 125 47.2 313 48.9 304 51.0 429 49.9
% with chronic condition
    Total 360 38.9 361 47.9 955 39.0 964 43.9 1325 45.0
    North 126 34.9 125 49.6 329 38.0 334 45.8 459 46.8
    East 110 45.5 112 52.7 310 41.0 329 44.1 441 46.3
    South 124 37.1 124 41.9 316 38.0 301 41.5 425 41.7
% perceived financial strain
    Total 361 361 955 950 1311
    Quite comfortably off 11.9 12.5 4.9 5.2** 7.2**
    Can manage without difficulty 20.2 24.4 24.0 20.5 21.6
    Have to be careful with money 52.9 47.1 51.9 52.4 51.0
    Find it a strain to get by 15.0 16.1 19.2 21.9 20.3
    North 125 126 328 332 458
    Quite comfortably off 12.8 11.1 5.2 3.6 5.7
    Can manage without difficulty 23.2 27.8 20.4 20.5 22.5
    Have to be careful with money 47.2 42.1 54.9 57.5 53.3
    Find it a strain to get by 16.8 19.1 19.5 18.4 18.6
    East 110 111 315 322 433
    Quite comfortably off 9.1 9.0 3.8 4.0 5.3
    Can manage without difficulty 13.6 21.6 25.7 18.9 19.6
    Have to be careful with money 59.1 52.3 51.4 52.5 52.4
    Find it a strain to get by 18.2 17.1 19.1 24.5 22.6
    South 126 124 312 296 420
    Quite comfortably off 13.5 16.9 5.8 8.1 10.7
    Can manage without difficulty 23.0 23.4 26.0 22.3 22.6
    Have to be careful with money 53.2 47.6 49.4 46.6 46.9
    Find it a strain to get by 10.3 12.1 18.9 23.0 19.8
Years lived in local area
    Total 365 18.3 (15.3) 362 24.9 (16.6) 980 18.2 (18.0) 965 19.0 (17.4)** 1327 20.6 (17.4)
    North 127 16.9 (13.1) 126 22.7 (14.1) 338 18.9 (18.7) 332 19.7 (16.9) 458 20.5 (16.2)
    East 112 17.5 (13.5) 110 24.9 (14.0) 319 18.2 (16.9) 330 20.7 (18.1) 440 21.7 (17.2)
    South 126 20.3 (18.4) 126 27.0 (20.3) 323 17.3 (18.4) 303 16.3 (17.1) 429 19.5 (18.7)
SWEMWBS
    Total 1318 21.9 (4.1)
    North 456 21.9 (4.0)
    East 439 21.8 (4.1)
    South 423 22.0 (4.1)
SF-8 PCS-8
    Total 352 47.4 (11.0) 360 45.9 (11.7) 935 46.8 (11.8) 960 45.3 (12.1)
    North 125 47.5 (10.8) 126 46.2 (11.2) 323 46.7 (11.7) 333 44.9 (12.5)
    East 105 46.7 (11.1) 111 44.7 (12.0) 307 46.7 (11.6) 327 45.0 (11.9)
    South 122 47.7 (11.2) 123 46.7 (11.9) 305 47.0 (12.1) 300 46.2 (12.1)
SF-8 MCS-8
    Total 352 45.5 (11.1) 360 46.4 (11.1) 935 43.8 (11.6) 960 44.4 (12.1)
    North 125 45.2 (11.6) 126 45.7 (11.9) 323 44.3 (11.6) 333 45.1 (11.7)
    East 105 44.7 (10.6) 111 46.4 (9.7) 307 43.2 (11.7) 327 44.0 (12.7)
    South 122 46.4 (11.1) 123 47.1 (11.5) 305 43.9 (11.6) 300 44.1 (11.8)

n–number; T–time point; SD–standard deviation; SF-8 MCS-8 –SF-8 mental component score; SF-8 PCS-8 –SF-8 physical component score; SWEMWBS–Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (short version). North–study area containing no motorway infrastructure; East–study area containing existing M8 motorway; South–study area containing new M74 motorway

*In paid employment (full or part-time), full-time student, or undertaking voluntary work

**Significant difference between study areas within the same time point and study sample (p<0.05)

Compared to the rest of the T1 sample, cohort participants were significantly more likely to be men, to own a home or a car, to be employed or studying, and to describe themselves as financially “comfortably off”, though there were no differences for age, time lived in the local area or presence of a chronic condition.

Longitudinal analysis of SF-8

There were no significant differences in wellbeing between study areas. In the East (existing motorway) and South (new motorway) areas, participants living closer to a motorway experienced reduced mental wellbeing (MCS-8) over time compared to those further away. In the South, this remained statistically significant in the maximally adjusted model (-3.6, 95% confidence interval [CI] -6.6 to -0.7) (Table 3).

Table 3. Longitudinal associations between exposure to a motorway and change in SF-8 physical and mental component score.

Beta coefficient (95% CI)
Exposure Outcome: SF-8 physical component score
n Model 1 n Model 2 n Model 3 n Model 4
Area: East (reference: North) 348 -0.8 (-3.1, 1.5) 346 -0.7 (-2.9, 1.6) 336 -1.1 (-3.4, 1.2) 336 -0.7 (-2.9, 1.4)
Proximity within East study area 103 -0.3 (-2.8, 2.3) 103 0.1 (-2.6, 2.7) 100 0.4 (-2.4, 3.2) 100 0.0 (-2.6, 2.6)
Area: South (reference: North) 348 0.0 (-2.2, 2.2) 346 0.4 (-1.8, 2.6) 336 0.5 (-1.8, 2.8) 336 0.5 (-1.6, 2.6)
Proximity within South study area 116 -0.9 (-3.7, 1.9) 115 -0.4 (-3.2, 2.5) 110 -0.9 (-4.0, 2.3) 110 -0.5 (-3.3, 2.4)
Outcome: SF-8 mental component score
n Model 1 n Model 2 n Model 3 n Model 4
Area: East (reference: North) 348 0.8 (-1.9, 3.5) 346 0.7 (-2.0, 3.4) 336 0.5 (-2.2, 3.2) 336 0.8 (-1.6, 3.1)
Proximity within East study area 103 -3.5 (-6.7, -0.3)* 103 -2.9 (-6.2, 0.5) 100 -1.2 (-4.6, 2.2) 100 0.2 (-2.5, 2.9)
Area: South (reference: North) 348 -0.1 (-2.7, 2.5) 346 0.0 (-2.6, 2.6) 336 0.3 (-2.3, 2.9) 336 0.7 (-1.6, 3.0)
Proximity within South study area 116 -3.2 (-6.4, -0.1)* 115 -3.6 (-6.8, -0.5)* 110 -3.9 (-7.2, -0.6)* 110 -3.6 (-6.6, -0.7)*

CI–confidence interval; n–number

*p<0.05

**p<0.01

***p<0.001

Model 1 is unadjusted. Model 2 is adjusted for age and sex. Model 3 is adjusted for variables in model 2 plus home ownership, car ownership, working status, perceived financial strain, presence of a chronic condition and years lived in the local area. Model 4 is adjusted for variables in model 3 plus baseline value of the outcome of the model in question. North–study area containing no motorway infrastructure; East–study area containing existing M8 motorway; South–study area containing new M74 motorway. Proximity refers to the distance from each participant’s home address in a straight line to the nearest motorway infrastructure

Repeat cross-sectional analysis of SF-8

There were no significant differences in wellbeing between study areas. In the South (new motorway) area, physical wellbeing (PCS-8) reduced over time in people living closer to the motorway compared to those living further away, but this was not statistically significant in the maximally adjusted model (Table 4). In the East (existing motorway) area, a borderline significant (p = 0.06) interaction with chronic condition was found for mental wellbeing (MCS-8). Stratified analysis suggested a reduction in MCS-8 over time among participants with a chronic condition living closer to a motorway compared to those further away (-3.7, 95% CI -8.3 to 0.9).

Table 4. Repeat cross-sectional associations between exposure to a motorway and change in SF-8 physical and mental component score.

Beta coefficient (95% CI)
Exposure Outcome: SF-8 physical component score
obs Model 1 obs Model 2 obs Model 3
Area: East (reference: North) 1895 0.1 (-2.6, 2.7) 1870 -0.5 (-2.9, 1.9) 1778 -0.8 (-2.6, 1.0)
Proximity within East study area 634 2.1 (-1.0, 5.2) 628 0.7 (-2.0, 3.4) 591 1.5 (-0.7, 3.6)
Area: South (reference: North) 1895 1.0 (-1.7, 3.6) 1870 0.2 (-2.2, 2.6) 1778 -0.2 (-2.0, 1.7)
Proximity within South study area 604 -6.0 (-10.6, -1.5)* 593 -5.2 (-9.4, -0.9)* 571 -1.5 (-4.8, 1.7)
Outcome: SF-8 mental component score
obs Model 1 obs Model 2 obs Model 3
Area: East (reference: North) 1895 0.0 (-2.6, 2.6) 1870 -0.1 (-2.7, 2.5) 1778 0.5 (-1.8, 2.8)
Proximity within East study area 634 -1.3 (-4.5, 1.9) 628 -1.6 (-4.9, 1.6) 591 -0.7 (3.5, 2.1)
Area: South (reference: North) 1895 -0.6 (-3.2, 2.0) 1870 -0.6 (-3.3, 2.0) 1778 -0.8 (-3.1, 1.5)
Proximity within South study area 604 -3.3 (-7.8, 1.1) 593 -3.7 (-8.2, 0.8) 571 1.4 (-2.6, 5.4)

CI–confidence interval; obs–observations

*p<0.05

**p<0.01

***p<0.001

Model 1 is unadjusted. Model 2 is adjusted for age and sex. Model 3 is adjusted for variables in model 2 plus home ownership, car ownership, working status, perceived financial strain, presence of a chronic condition and years lived in the local area. North–study area containing no motorway infrastructure; East–study area containing existing M8 motorway; South–study area containing new M74 motorway. Proximity refers to the distance from each participant’s home address in a straight line to the nearest motorway infrastructure

T2 cross-sectional analysis of SWEMWBS

There were no significant differences in wellbeing between study areas. In the East (existing motorway) and South (new motorway) areas, participants living closer to a motorway had poorer wellbeing than those living further away; however, these findings were not statistically significant in the maximally adjusted models (Table 5). A significant interaction with chronic condition was found in the East. Stratified analysis indicated that participants with a chronic condition living closer to a motorway had significantly poorer wellbeing than those living further away (-1.1, 95% CI -2.0 to -0.3).

Table 5. Cross-sectional associations between exposure to a motorway and Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (short version) score at T2.

Beta coefficient (95% CI)
Exposure Outcome: SWEMWBS score
n Model 1 n Model 2 n Model 3
Area: East (reference: North) 1318 -0.2 (-0.7, 0.4) 1310 -0.2 (-0.7, 0.4) 1253 0.0 (-0.5, 0.5)
Proximity within East study area 437 -0.8 (-1.4, -0.1)* 433 -0.8 (-1.4, -0.1)* 411 -0.4 (-1.0, 0.2)
Area: South (reference: North) 1318 0.1 (-0.4, 0.7) 1310 0.1 (-0.4, 0.7) 1253 0.0 (-0.5, 0.5)
Proximity within South study area 419 -1.0 (-1.8, -0.2)* 418 -1.0 (-1.8, -0.2)* 404 -0.1 (-0.9, 0.7)

CI–confidence interval; n–number; SWEMWBS–Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (short version)

*p<0.05

**p<0.01

***p<0.001

Model 1 is unadjusted. Model 2 is adjusted for age and sex. Model 3 is adjusted for variables in model 2 plus home ownership, car ownership, working status, perceived financial strain, presence of a chronic condition and years lived in the local area. North–study area containing no motorway infrastructure; East–study area containing existing M8 motorway; South–study area containing new M74 motorway. Proximity refers to the distance from each participant’s home address in a straight line to the nearest motorway infrastructure.

Discussion

Main findings

We found some evidence that living near to either a newly-constructed or an existing urban motorway had a negative impact on local residents’ mental wellbeing. In addition, we found no evidence to suggest any positive effects of living near a motorway on wellbeing.The pattern of findings across the South (new motorway) and East (existing motorway) study areas indicate how adaptation might occur in the short and long term. The negative impacts on wellbeing appeared to be broadly distributed in the short term, becoming concentrated in those with poorer health in the long term.

Strengths and limitations

This is one of very few intervention studies examining how changes in the environment influence changes in health, particularly in deprived populations. In accordance with calls for more evidence of this nature [18, 30], we objectively defined exposure using multiple methods, used two extensively validated tools to capture the nuances of the wellbeing construct, accounted for a series of potential confounders and used both longitudinal and repeat cross-sectional analyses to offset the limitations of each approach and corroborate findings.

We also acknowledge the limitations of our study. There was relatively high attrition of the longitudinal cohort, though the rate was comparable to that of other similar studies [8, 31] and the repeat cross-sectional design was chosen to buffer against this specific weakness. We also found some differences between study areas for sociodemographic variables at follow-up, despite having delimited comparable study areas and recruited comparable samples at baseline [23]. A natural experimental study design cannot eliminate the possibility of unmeasured confounding related to other concurrent changes, such as the ongoing Clyde Gateway initiative (a regeneration project incorporating parts of the South study area) and the 2014 Commonwealth Games. Additionally, the findings are likely to be at least somewhat specific to the context.

Comparison with other studies

The reduction in MCS-8 attributable to motorway exposure was approximately 3.5 units in both the longitudinal analysis and the stratified repeat cross-sectional analysis. With the log transformation, this represents the average difference between those living approximately 100 metres from a motorway and those living 300 metres away, or between those living 300 and 800 metres away. This 3.5 unit reduction is similar in magnitude to that observed in a clinical population experiencing reduced overall quality of life (3.3 units) [25]. In a general population, it is comparable to the difference between those not completing high school and tertiary graduates (4.2 units), or between those with and without a physical chronic condition (2.0 units) [25].

Our findings are consistent with previous cross-sectional studies linking traffic noise disturbance with lower wellbeing [13, 14]. In particular, one study found that mental wellbeing assessed using SF-36 was 4.2 units lower in those experiencing traffic noise disturbance than those not [13]. However, our findings are inconsistent with evaluations of other types of urban regeneration initiatives in the UK, which have found either no change [32] or modest improvements [8, 9] in wellbeing. A recent study of neighborhood demolition and housing improvement (also in Glasgow) found a significant increase in mental wellbeing in participants receiving housing improvements relative to controls, measured using SF-12 (2.4, 95% CI 0.0 to 4.8) [8].

Implications for policy and practice

There is currently little public health evidence to guide policy decisions about investing in expensive urban regeneration projects. We found negative impacts of a new motorway on wellbeing. However, more time may be necessary for some benefits, such as economic revival (which we have not assessed directly), to be fully realised and impact on wellbeing. Those with chronic conditions living near an existing motorway experienced the greatest adverse effects on wellbeing, which may entrench existing health inequalities. From a social justice perspective, there did not appear to be a fair distribution of benefits and harms for those living near a motorway, particularly as approximately half of our sample did not own a car. Previous work on the socio-spatial patterning of busy roads and industrial sites indicates these are disproportionately located near deprived neighbourhoods [33, 34].

While transport policy in Scotland and other countries highlights the need to promote active travel and public transport on health and sustainability grounds [35], urban design continues to prioritise car use despite the adverse health effects associated with a car-dominant transport system [36]. This study will help inform future policy in the UK and further afield.

Implications for research

Several issues may be of interest to researchers. Firstly, while delimiting area-based exposures in natural experimental research is relatively straightforward [37], in this study individual proximity to a motorway appeared to be a more meaningful exposure. This seems intuitive given that some of the hypothesised contributors to poor wellbeing, including visual intrusion and traffic noise, are restricted to those in close proximity. Graded proximity exposures have been employed in other recent natural experimental studies [31, 38]. In future studies, the optimal definition of exposure will depend on the particular combination of intervention, study design and outcome.

Secondly, the longitudinal and repeat cross-sectional analyses did not fully corroborate. At baseline, the cohort was significantly wealthier and had higher mental wellbeing on average than the rest of the T1 sample. It is therefore plausible that their response to the intervention differed from that of the repeat cross-sectional sample. The longitudinal analysis examined within-participant change over time and provided the greatest support for causal inference, but was limited by the smaller sample size. The repeat cross-sectional analysis examined population-level shifts over time, bolstering the sample size but providing a lower level of confidence for causal inference at individual level. It is likely that the intervention operated differently at the individual and population levels, reflecting the differences we found. This will be explored further in complementary quantitative mediation analyses and qualitative research.

Finally, in natural experimental research, replication is unlikely to involve multiple studies of the same intervention–rather, multiple studies between which researchers can synthesise the effects of altering the same general characteristics of the environment in different contexts. The cumulation of this work will allow researchers to make more generalisable causal statements about the effects of environmental change [18, 22].

Conclusions

Living near to a new motorway appeared to worsen residents’ wellbeing. In an area with an existing motorway, negative impacts were concentrated in those with chronic conditions, which may exacerbate health inequalities and contribute to poorer health. Health impacts of this type of urban regeneration intervention should be considered in future policy and planning.

Supporting information

S1 Appendix. Study survey.

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

The baseline phase of the M74 study was developed by David Ogilvie, Richard Mitchell, Nanette Mutrie, Mark Petticrew and Stephen Platt. The follow-up phase of the study was developed by David Ogilvie, Lyndal Bond, Fiona Crawford, Simon Griffin, Shona Hilton, David Humphreys, Andrew Jones, Richard Mitchell, Nanette Mutrie, Shannon Sahlqvist and Hilary Thomson.

We thank all study participants; staff from the MRC/CSO Social and Public Health Sciences Unit survey office, in particular Kate Campbell, Catherine Ferrell, John Gibbons, Elaine Hindle, Sally Stewart and Matthew Tolan; Emma Coombes at CEDAR for the derivation of exposures; and Stephen Sharp at the MRC Epidemiology Unit for his advice on the analysis strategy.

The M74 study team consists of:

  • David Ogilvie. Lead author. MRC Epidemiology Unit & UKCRC Centre for Diet and Activity Research (CEDAR), University of Cambridge. Email: dbo23@medschl.cam.ac.uk

  • Louise Foley. MRC Epidemiology Unit & CEDAR, University of Cambridge

  • Amy Nimegeer. MRC/CSO Social and Public Health Sciences Unit, University of Glasgow

  • Jonathan Olsen. Centre for Research on Environment, Society and Health, University of Glasgow

  • Richard Mitchell. Centre for Research on Environment, Society and Health, University of Glasgow

  • Hilary Thomson. MRC/CSO Social and Public Health Sciences Unit, University of Glasgow

  • Fiona Crawford. NHS Greater Glasgow & Clyde and Glasgow Centre for Population Health

  • Richard Prins. MRC Epidemiology Unit & CEDAR, University of Cambridge

  • Shona Hilton. MRC/CSO Social and Public Health Sciences Unit, University of Glasgow

  • Andrew Jones. CEDAR, Norwich Medical School, University of East Anglia

  • David Humphreys. Department of Social Policy and Intervention, University of Oxford

  • Shannon Sahlqvist. Centre for Physical Activity and Nutrition Research (C-PAN), School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences, Deakin University

  • Nanette Mutrie. Physical Activity for Health Research Centre, University of Edinburgh

Data Availability

Study meta-data will be made fully available without restriction at http://epimeta.medschl.cam.ac.uk/. Non-identifiable individual-level data are available on request. The MRC Epidemiology Unit Senior Data Manager, Adam Dickinson, manages the processing of data requests (datasharing@mrc-epid.cam.ac.uk). Requests for data sharing will be considered by the principal investigator (DO) in consultation with the other investigators. Their consent prevents the authors from making these data available publicly; third-party researchers would need to sign a collaborative agreement. The authors' data sharing policies and processes meet the requirements and expectations of MRC policy on sharing of data from population and patient cohorts: http://www.mrc.ac.uk/research/research-policy-ethics/data-sharing/policy/. These policies and processes are in place to ensure that the use of data from this study is within the bounds of consent given previously by study members, complies with MRC guidance on ethics and research governance, and meets rigorous MRC data security standards.

Funding Statement

LF is supported by the National Institute for Health Research Public Health Research programme (NIHR PHR; project number 11/3005/07) and the Centre for Diet and Activity Research (CEDAR), a UKCRC Public Health Research Centre of Excellence. Funding from the British Heart Foundation, Cancer Research UK, Economic and Social Research Council, Medical Research Council, the National Institute for Health Research, and the Wellcome Trust, under the auspices of the UK Clinical Research Collaboration, is gratefully acknowledged. DO and RP are supported by the Medical Research Council (MC_UU_12015/6). HT is supported by the Medical Research Council (MC_UU_12017/4 and MC_UU_12017-15) and the Chief Scientist Office of the Scottish Government Health Directorate (SPHSU1). FT is employed in a hybrid role between NHS Greater Glasgow and Clyde and Glasgow Centre for Population Health (GCPH). GCPH is a partnership between NHS Greater Glasgow and Clyde, Glasgow City Council, and the University of Glasgow, funded by the Scottish Government. The baseline phase of the M74 study was developed by David Ogilvie, Richard Mitchell, Nanette Mutrie, Mark Petticrew and Stephen Platt and supported by a Medical Research Council Special Training Fellowship in Health of the Public Research (award number G106/1203). The follow-up phase of the study was developed by Lyndal Bond, Fiona Crawford, Simon Griffin, Shona Hilton, David Humphreys, Andrew Jones, Richard Mitchell, Nanette Mutrie, Shannon Sahlqvist and Hilary Thomson. This article presents independent research funded by the NIHR (project number 11/3005/07). The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Sallis JF, Cervero RB, Ascher W, Henderson KA, Kraft MK, Kerr J. An ecological approach to creating active living communities. Annual Review of Public Health. 2006;27(297–322). 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.27.021405.102100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Jackson RJ. The impact of the built environment on health: an emerging field. American Journal of Public Health. 2003;93(9):1382–4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Evans GW. The built environment and mental health. Journal of Urban Health: Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine. 2003;80(4):536–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Thomson H. A dose of realism for healthy urban policy: lessons from area-based initiatives in the UK. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2008;62:932–6. 10.1136/jech.2007.068775 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Thomson H, Atkinson R, Petticrew M, Kearns A. Do urban regeneration programmes improve public health and reduce health inequalities? A synthesis of the evidence from UK policy and practice (1980–2004). Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2006;60:108–15. 10.1136/jech.2005.038885 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kearns A, Tannahill C, Bond L. Regeneration and health: conceptualising the connections. Journal of Urban Regeneration & Renewal. 2009;3(1):56–76. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Thomson H, Thomas S, Sellström E, Petticrew M. Housing improvements for health and associated socio-economic outcomes: a systematic review. Campbell Systematic Reviews. 2013;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Egan M, Katikireddi SV, Kearns A, Tannahill C, Kalacs M, Bond L. Health effects of neighborhood demolition and housing improvement: a prospective controlled study of 2 natural experiments in urban renewal. American Journal of Public Health. 2013;103(6):e47–e53. 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301275 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Batty E, Beatty C, Foden F, Lawless P, Pearson S, Wilson I. The New Deal for Communities Experience: A Final Assessment. Vol. 7 London, UK: Department for Communities and Local Government, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Egan M, Petticrew M, Ogilvie D, Hamilton V. New roads and human health: a systematic review. Journal of Public Health. 2003;93:1463–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Appleyard D, Gerson MS, Lintell M. Livable Streets. Berkeley, California: University of California Press; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hart J. Driven to excess—impacts of motor vehicle traffic on residential quality of life in Bristol, UK. Bristol: University of the West of England; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Dratva J, Zemp E, Dietrich DF, Bridevaux P, Rochat T, Schindler C, et al. Impact of road traffic noise annoyance on health-related quality of life: results from a population-based study. Quality of Life Research. 2010;19:37–46. 10.1007/s11136-009-9571-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Marques S, Lima ML. Living in grey areas: Industrial activity and psychological health. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2011;31:314–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Erdem O, Prins RG, Voorham T, van Lenthe FJ, Burdorf A. Structural neighbourhood conditions, social cohesion and psychological distress in the Netherlands. European Journal of Public Health. 2015: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Breeze E, Jones DA, Wilkinson P, Bulpitt CJ, Grundy C, Latif AM, et al. Area deprivation, social class, and quality of life among people aged 75 years and over in Britain. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2005;34(2):276–83. 10.1093/ije/dyh328 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Macintyre S, Maciver S, Sooman A. Area, class and health: should we be focusing on places or people? Journal of Social Policy. 1993;22(2):213–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Craig P, Cooper C, Gunnell D, Haw S, Lawson K, Macintyre S, et al. Using natural experiments to evaluate population health interventions: new Medical Research Council guidance. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2012;66:1182–6. 10.1136/jech-2011-200375 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.National Records of Scotland. 2011 Census: First Results on Population and Household Estimates for Scotland—Release 1B. Edinburgh: National Records of Scotland, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.National Records of Scotland. Life expectancy for areas within Scotland. Edinburgh: National Records of Scotland, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Watt G. All together now: why social deprivation matters to everyone. British Medical Journal. 1996;312:1026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ogilvie D, Mitchell R, Mutrie N, Petticrew M, Platt S. Evaluating health effects of transport interventions. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2006;31(2):118–26. 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.03.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Ogilvie D, Mitchell R, Mutrie N, Petticrew M, Platt S. Personal and environmental correlates of active travel and physical activity in a deprived urban population. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2008;5:43 10.1186/1479-5868-5-43 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Edwards PJ, Roberts I, Clarke MJ, DiGuiseppi C, Wentz R, Kwan I, et al. Methods to increase response to postal and electronic questionnaires. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2009;Issue 3. Art. No.: MR000008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ware JE, Kosinski M, Dewey JE, Gandek B. How to score and interpret single-item health status measures: a manual for users of the SF-8 Health Survey. Lincoln RI: QualityMetric Incorporated; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ware JE, Kosinski M. SF-36 Physical & Mental Health Summary Scales: a manual for users of version 1. Second edition Lincoln, RI: QualityMetric Incorporated; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Stewart-Brown S, Tennant A, Tennant R, Platt S, Parkinson J, Weich S. Internal construct validity of the Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (WEMWBS): a Rasch analysis using data from the Scottish Health Education Population Survey. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes. 2009;7:15 10.1186/1477-7525-7-15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Tennant R, Hiller L, Fishwick R, Platt S, Joseph S, Weich S, et al. The Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (WEMWBS): development and UK validation. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes. 2007;5:63 10.1186/1477-7525-5-63 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Scottish Government. The Scottish Health Survey—main report. Edinburgh: Scottish Government, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (NICE). Physical activity and the environment: guidance on the promotion and creation of physical environments that support increased levels of physical activity. London: National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Goodman A, Sahlqvist S, Ogilvie D. New walking and cycling routes and increased physical activity: one- and 2-year findings from the UK iConnect study. American Journal of Public Health. 2014;104(9):e38–e46. 10.2105/AJPH.2014.302059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kearns A, Petticrew M, Mason P, Whitley E. SHARP Survey Findings: Mental Health and Well-Being Outcomes. Edinburgh: Scottish Government Social Research, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Walker G, Mitchell G, Fairburn J, Smith G. Industrial pollution and social deprivation: Evidence and complexity in evaluating and responding to environmental inequality. Local Environment: The International Journal of Justice and Sustainability. 2005;10(4):361–77. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.World Health Organisation. Environmental health inequalities in Europe. Assessment report. Copenhagen: World Health Organisation, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Strathclyde Partnership for Transport. A catalyst for change: the regional transport strategy for the west of Scotland 2008–21. Glasgow: Strathclyde Partnership for Transport, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.British Medical Association. Healthy transport = Healthy lives. London: British Medical Association, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Humphreys DK, Panter J, Sahlqvist S, Goodman A, Ogilvie D. Changing the environment to improve population health: a framework for considering exposure in natural experimental studies. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2016; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Panter J, Heinen E, Mackett R, Ogilvie D. Impact of new transport infrastructure on walking, cycling and physical activity. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2016;50(2):e45–e53. 10.1016/j.amepre.2015.09.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Appendix. Study survey.

(PDF)

Data Availability Statement

Study meta-data will be made fully available without restriction at http://epimeta.medschl.cam.ac.uk/. Non-identifiable individual-level data are available on request. The MRC Epidemiology Unit Senior Data Manager, Adam Dickinson, manages the processing of data requests (datasharing@mrc-epid.cam.ac.uk). Requests for data sharing will be considered by the principal investigator (DO) in consultation with the other investigators. Their consent prevents the authors from making these data available publicly; third-party researchers would need to sign a collaborative agreement. The authors' data sharing policies and processes meet the requirements and expectations of MRC policy on sharing of data from population and patient cohorts: http://www.mrc.ac.uk/research/research-policy-ethics/data-sharing/policy/. These policies and processes are in place to ensure that the use of data from this study is within the bounds of consent given previously by study members, complies with MRC guidance on ethics and research governance, and meets rigorous MRC data security standards.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES