Abstract
Attacks on peoples’ dignity help to produce and maintain stigmatization and interpersonal hostility. As part of an effort to develop innovative measures of possible pathways between structural interventions or socially-disruptive Big Events and HIV outbreaks, we developed items to measure dignity denial. These measures were administered to 300 people who inject drugs (PWID), 260 high-risk heterosexuals who do not inject drugs (HRH), and 191 men who have sex with men who do not inject drugs (MSM). All of the PWID and many of the high risk heterosexuals and MSM were referred to our study in 2012–2015 by a large New York City study that used respondent-driven sampling; the others were recruited by chain-referral. Members of all three key populations experienced attacks on their dignity fairly often and also reported frequently seeing others’ dignity being attacked. Relatives are major sources of dignity attacks. MSM were significantly more likely to report having their dignity attacked by police officers than were the other groups. 40% or more of each key population reported that dignity attacks are followed “sometimes” or more often both by using more drugs and also by using more alcohol. Dignity attacks and their health effects require more research and creative interventions, some of which might take untraditional forms like social movements.
Keywords: Dignity attacks, stigma, key populations, Big Events, HIV, health, people who inject drugs, men who have sex with men, heterosexuals
INTRODUCTION
Attacks on people’s dignity are quite common (1). To some extent such attacks are implicit in patterns of the subordination or oppression of people based on race/ethnicity, job type or work status, sex, sexual orientation, religion or drug use. In addition, a culture based to a large degree on concepts of the relative value and status of people is quite prone to interpersonal dignity attacks between people who might fall into the same categories of an intersectional analysis. Such attacks can create many forms of pain ranging from stigmatization to interpersonal hostility.
Neither dignity nor dignity attacks are new topics for public health. Jacobson has reviewed and described research on them quite extensively (2 – 5). Furthermore, unlike some social and behavioral science concepts like stigma or micro-aggression, attacks on dignity are clearly emic categories rather than academic inventions. The streets of the US South resounded with calls for dignity during the civil rights movement of the 1960s, as did the streets of Egypt and Tunisia in 2011 and as do conversations by workers about their treatment at work. In spite of this, however, the field has lacked adequate measures of dignity denial as an event and as a social process. (To some extent, of course, social science concepts do diffuse into wider populations. “Stigma” has become somewhat emic in some mental health populations and HIV-related target populations such as men who have sex with men (MSM) and people who inject drugs (PWID), particularly among those who interact with HIV programs frequently.)
We thus developed questionnaire items to measure attacks on dignity in the context of a larger research project to develop measures of hypothesized pathways by which major social (or “structural”) interventions such as neighborhood-uprooting urban renewal or reductions in fire service coverage that result in urban “burnouts” (7, 8) or large-scale economic, political, social, and climate-driven “Big Events” might lead some people to become members of Key Populations such as PWID or sex workers; and how they can lead members of Key Populations such as PWID, men who have sex with men (MSM) or other populations such as heterosexuals to engage in high-risk behaviors (6, 9 – 11). Big Events are an important macro-social phenomenon of considerable interest to HIV research since a number of them have been followed by major HIV outbreaks (6, 9 – 11). We developed a theorization of pathways from macro-social change or Big Events to HIV risk in terms of CHAT (Cultural-Historical Activity Theory); this included an outline of relevant measures that need to be developed (6). Pouget et al (12) showed that a number of measures that were developed—including the measures described here--showed promising reliability and validity among a New York City PWID sample. (Similar analyses for heterosexual and MSM samples are being conducted.)
In this paper, we focus on a set of measures developed as part of this project that focus on attacks on peoples’ dignity (6, 11, 12). Attacks on dignity seem to be an inherent part of current economic and political structures and cultures (1). Thus, research on dignity attacks should be important for issues like micro-aggression and other ongoing experiences that may be particularly likely to affect some populations at high risk for HIV. We describe these measures and discuss how they might be used in studies of current HIV risk and/or in studies of social intervention or change might unleash HIV outbreaks.
Some attacks on peoples’ dignity are deeply structural, but others are interpersonal (1). Our focus in this paper is on interpersonal attacks on people’s dignity, although some of these attacks are carried out by people in the process of fulfilling their systemic roles such as police officer or counselor.
One result of dignity attacks is that they can be internalized as stigma (13, 14), which has many negative health consequences through creating stress, producing obstacles to accessing health care, and much else (15 – 20). Link and Phelan (15) developed the concept of “structural stigma” which has some parallels to the concept of structural attacks on dignity. Hatzenbuehler recently edited a special issue of Social Science and Medicine on “Structural stigma and health” which contains a range of papers on this issue, including Hatzenbuehler’s useful introduction to the thoughts and literature in the field as well as to the special issue (21, 22). Hatzenbuehler and his colleagues have themselves published extensively on ways in which structural stigma can have negative consequences for the health of gay men (23 – 26).
Other related concepts are “enacted stigma” and “microaggression” (13, 27, 28). In a relatively early definition of enacted stigma (towards people with epilepsy), Jacoby defined the term as follows: “In this dichotomy, enacted stigma refers to episodes of discrimination against people with epilepsy, solely on the grounds of their social unacceptability; whereas felt stigma refers to the shame associated with being epileptic and the fear of enacted stigma.” (29) More recently, a USAID document on HIV/AIDS related discrimination and stigma offers the following definition: “We define discrimination (or enacted stigma) as the negative acts that result from stigma and that serve to devalue and reduce the life chances of the stigmatized.” (30) Enacted stigma thus differs from dignity attacks in that it is purposive and aimed specifically at a given stigmatized characteristic of its target. Dignity attacks, on the other hand, may be much less specific statements or actions. Indeed, at the more macro-level, we have described ways in which the implicit devaluation of workers as opposed to machinery can serve to attack workers’ dignity without any action or words that explicitly target the workers (1).
It should be noted that the concept of microaggression is based on experiences of “indignities,” and thus is a recent re-formulation of the concept of dignity denial or dignity attack. Swann et al. argue that microaggressions “are frequently non-deliberate and unconscious on the part of the actor,” which is in accordance with the concept of dignity attacks (28). We use the term dignity attacks both because dignity and dignity attacks are firmly rooted in the vocabularies of the victims of these attacks (as shown by the widespread explicit mention of dignity issues in the grievances expressed by social movements) and also because there is a well-rooted literature on dignity and its denial in philosophy, social science and in the health sciences. Both terms, it should be noted, refer to behaviors frequently though not always enacted as part of larger systems of oppression or subordination such as racism, sexism, heterosexism, prejudice against the jobless, or drug wars.
Research on macro-social (structural) interventions, Big Events, structural stigma, structural and individual dignity attacks, enacted stigma, micro-aggression, individual stigma, and HIV outbreaks can and should be integrated. One way in which these concepts might be tied together is that macro-social interventions or Big Events can sometimes exacerbate, and sometimes ameliorate, both the ways in which social structures attack individuals’ dignity and the degree of structural stigma in a society. These, in turn, can influence the extent to which individuals experience enacted stigma and various micro-aggressions, some of which can lead some individuals to internalize these as stigma (spoiled identity). Since these concepts are still being developed, and research in this area is in its early stages, we hasten to warn that dignity attacks may well affect HIV via pathways like exacerbating interpersonal hostilities that do not involve stigma at all. Furthermore, we would argue, it is useful to differentiate between processes like dignity attacks or intravention (31) that create or maintain social psychological effects such as stigma or internal norms and the social psychological effects themselves. These supra-individual and interactional processes often are rooted in macrosocial and interpersonal processes, institutions and structures that are quite independent of what we usually think of when we think of terms like “stigma,” micro-aggression, or even “structural stigma.”
This paper is descriptive and exploratory. It aims to introduce measures of dignity attacks to a public health audience and to describe and compare their frequencies among three key populations in New York City—PWID, high-risk heterosexuals (HRH) and MSM. Since we hope that other researchers will use these or similar items to investigate if and how dignity attacks lead to personal or public health consequences, we present extensive data in our tables that others can use as benchmarks in planning their own research.
METHODS
Samples
The sample consists of 300 PWID, 260 high-risk heterosexuals who do not inject drugs, and 191 men who have sex with men who do not inject drugs. All of the PWID and many of the high risk heterosexuals and MSM were referred to our study by a large New York City RDS study in 2012–2015. For HRH and MSM we supplemented this by asking participants who had been referred to us to help us recruit others who would qualify. These potential participants were screened for eligibility by our field director (usually over the telephone). Eligibility criteria for each group included (1) age 18 or older, (2) residency in the NYC metropolitan area, and (3) fluency in English. In addition, PWID participants had to report having injected illicit drugs in the past 12 months; high-risk heterosexuals had to report having had sex with an opposite sex partner in the last 12 months; and MSM had to be male and to report having had sex with a man in the last 12 months.
Measures
Participants were interviewed, with informed consent, using a long interview form that covered activities, experiences, norms and roles in specific contexts, and how normative conflicts and other situations are perceived and resolved (3, 16). These interviews typically took between 90 minutes and two hours. Study methods and questionnaire items were approved by National Development and Research Institutes (NDRI)’s Institutional Review Board. Participants generally reported that they enjoyed the interview topics and felt respected. They were reimbursed $30 for their time and effort.
In addition to standard demographic and behavioral items, respondents were asked a number of questions about attacks on their dignity. Each participant was asked how often people had spoken or acted towards them in the last year in a way that felt like they were attacking their dignity or demeaning them. They were also asked “In the last year, how often have you been present when someone spoke or acted towards someone else in a way that attacked their dignity or demeaned them?” For each of these questions, response categories were: 1. Never; 2. 1 to 2 times; 3. 3 – 10 times; 4. About once a month; 5. About once or twice a week; 6. Daily or more. For these analyses, responses of “don’t know” or refusal to answer were treated as missing.
Respondents were also asked “What aspects of you or your life did you think they were demeaning you about?” They were given a list of possible answers to endorse or not to endorse as one that had been attacked.
They were also asked “When they attack your dignity, how often do you react in each of these ways?” We gave them a list of possible reactions, and asked them to tell us for each whether they reacted in this way 1. Never; 2. Rarely; 3. Sometimes; 4. Often; 5. Very often. (Again, “don’t know” and refusals to answer are treated as missing.)
They were also asked about who attacked their dignity: “Please tell me who are the people and/or groups who have attacked your dignity or demeaned you in the way that hurt you the most (circle all that apply).” We provided them a list of 41 categories of such potential attackers for them to choose among.
Analyses of associations of key socioeconomic variables (lacking a job, being homeless and very low income) with dignity denial experiences were conducted for two reasons. First, these socioeconomic variables are likely to be affected by major social change or macro-social interventions, so their associations with dignity denial are crude proxies for how these might affect dignity denial experiences. Secondly, we would anticipate that homelessness, joblessness and very low income would be associated with a higher level of dignity denial experiences. To the extent that we find such associations, it tends to provide a degree of convergence validation for the indicators. It also suggests that social changes or interventions that worsen these socioeconomic indicators might increase attacks on dignity.
To define “very low income” we approximated median splits for each Key Population sample. For PWID and HRH, this led us to define “very low income” as less than $10,000 per year. MSM had a slightly higher income distribution, but even so the “very low income” split for them was defined as “less than $12,500 per year.”
Analyses
Analyses were conducted with IBM SPSS (ver. 22). These analyses were exploratory ones, since no prior research has been done on how these measures might differ among the three Key Populations and very little empirical research has been done on dignity attacks in these populations.
Statistical analyses included frequency descriptions and cross-tabulations. Statistical significance was estimated by chi square (and, if needed, exact tests). We used linear-by-linear association tests to compare ordinal variables (32). Pearson correlation (r), linear regression and logistic regression analyses are used to study associations of dignity attacks and behaviors. P-values are presented in the tables rather than the text to improve readability.
RESULTS
Participants’ characteristics and p-values for differences between key populations are presented in Table 1. For some analyses, the sample sizes are smaller because of the non-applicability of a question to some respondents. PWID were older than either the MSM or the heterosexual sample; and heterosexuals were alder than MSM. High-risk heterosexuals were more likely than either PWID or MSM to be Black or Hispanic. PWID were most likely never to have married, followed by MSM and then HRH. PWID were more likely to be homeless than the HRH. Most PWID reported income less than $10,000 a year, as did almost half of the HRH and 23% of MSM. (The low incomes of the HRH and many characteristics of all three groups derive in part from the sampling frame, as discussed above.) Approximately a quarter of each group report having exchanged sex for money or drugs in the past 30 days.
Table 1.
People Who Inject Drugs | High-Risk Heterosexuals (Who Do Not inject Drugs) | Men (who do not themselves inject drugs) who have sex with men | p (PWID vs. HRH) | p (PWID vs. MSM) | p (HRH vs MSM) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||
N (may vary for some rows due to missing values) | 300 | 257 | 191 | |||
| ||||||
Age | ||||||
Mean (p by t-test) | 40.9 | 32.6 | 25.6 | .097 | .008 | .803 |
Standard deviation | 9.85 | 11.62 | 4.41 | |||
Median (p by independent samples median test) (25%-ile, 75%-ile) | 41.0 (32.9, 49.1) | 28.3 (23.7, 43.9) | 25.9 (23.8, 29.7) | <.001 | <.001 | .001 |
| ||||||
Women* | 44% | 48% | NA | .244 | NA | NA |
| ||||||
Black/African American Race | 54% | 71% | 56% | .000 | .7809 | .006 |
| ||||||
Hispanic/Latino Ethnicity | 57% | 66% | 54% | .026 | .530 | .009 |
| ||||||
Did not graduate High School or Obtain GED | 38% | 36.2% | 30% | .826 | .0756 | .1594 |
| ||||||
Never Married | 44% | 72% | 63% | .000 | .000 | .0317 |
| ||||||
Homeless | 19% | 10% | 15% | .0026 | .2387 | .109 |
| ||||||
Income < $10,000/year | 62% | 44% | 23% | .00 | .000 | .000 |
| ||||||
Self-reported HIV-Positivity | 21% | 7% | 13% | .000 | .039 | .105 |
| ||||||
Any military service | 19% | 12% | 18% | .0388 | .7241 | .1180 |
| ||||||
Exchanged Sex for Money or Drugs (past 30 days) | 22% | 26% | 26% | .210 | .288 | .932 |
There were also 4 transgender high-risk heterosexuals who are not considered in the statistical comparisons
Note. Injectable drugs are: amphetamine/methamphetamine, heroin/opioids, cocaine/crack.
Self-reported HIV prevalence levels of all three groups are high, with 21% of PWID, 13% of MSM and 7% of HRH reporting that they have been diagnosed as HIV-positive.
Frequency of being attacked and of seeing others attacked
Attacks on the dignity of all groups occurred relatively frequently, with dignity attacks occurring about once a month or more for 46% of PWID, 39% of HRH, and 53% of MSM. As Table 2 describes, such attacks were reported more by MSM and PWID than by HRH. They observed other people’s dignity being attacked even more, with 80% of PWID, 64% of HRH and 93% of MSM reporting observing dignity attacks about monthly or more. About a third of PWID report seeing others’ dignity attacked daily; and more than half of both MSM and PWID report seeing this on a weekly basis or more. As Table 2 shows, taking the distributions as a whole, PWID and MSM were more likely to observe attacks on others’ dignity than were HRH.
Table 2.
None | 1–2 times | 3 – 10 times | About once a month | About once or twice a week | Daily or more | Total | p (Linear-by-linear association)** | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Having one’s dignity attacked | ||||||||
PWID | 49 16.8% |
42 14.4% |
67 23.0% |
52 17.9% |
45 15.5% |
36 12.4% |
291 | |
HRH | 58 22.7% |
47 18.4% |
50 19.6% |
55 21.6% |
32 12.5% |
13 5.1% |
255 | .003 |
Observing someone else have their dignity attacked | ||||||||
PWID | 2 0.7% |
18 6.1% |
38 12.8% |
70 23.6% |
66 22.3% |
102 34.5% |
296 | |
HRH | 13 5.1% |
26 10.2% |
53 20.8% |
27 10.6% |
45 17.6% |
91 35.7% |
255 | .010 |
Having one’s dignity attacked | ||||||||
HRH | 58 22.7% |
47 18.4% |
50 19.6% |
55 21.6% |
32 12.5% |
13 5.1% |
255 | |
MSM | 11 5.8% |
24 12.6% |
54 28.4% |
83 43.7% |
18 9.5% |
0 0% |
190 | .002 |
Observing someone else have their dignity attacked | ||||||||
HRH | 13 5.1% |
26 10.2% |
53 20.8% |
27 10.6% |
45 17.6% |
91 35.7% |
255 | |
MSM | 0 0% |
1 0.5% |
14 7.3% |
55 28.8% |
87 45.5% |
34 17.8% |
191 | .002 |
Having one’s dignity attacked | ||||||||
PWID | 49 16.8% |
42 14.4% |
67 23.0% |
52 17.9% |
45 15.5% |
36 12.4% |
291 | |
MSM | 11 5.8% |
24 12.6% |
54 28.4% |
83 43.7% |
18 9.5% |
0 0% |
190 | .962 |
Observing someone else have their dignity attacked | ||||||||
PWID | 2 0.7% |
18 6.1% |
38 12.8% |
70 23.6% |
66 22.3% |
102 34.5% |
296 | |
MSM | 0 0% |
1 0.5% |
14 7.3% |
55 28.8% |
87 45.5% |
34 17.8% |
191 | .413 |
- In the last year, how often have people spoken or acted towards you in a way that felt like they were attacking your dignity or demeaning you?
- In the last year, how often have you been present when someone spoke or acted towards someone else in a way that attacked their dignity or demeaned them?
Linear-by-linear association tests compare differences for ordinal variables (Agresti, 2002, p. 369–373).
What their dignity was attacked for
There are interesting commonalities and differences among groups in what they report were the reasons why their dignity was attacked (Table 3). Approximately half of each group had their dignity attacked because of the people they hang out with; and 30% or more of each group were attacked for being drug users. (Although this was much more common for PWID than for these samples of HRH and MSM who do not inject drugs.) All three groups had sizeable proportions who reported dignity attacks for not having a job (although this was particularly common for HRH), the way they look (which was particularly common for MSM), and their clothing. People’s dignity was attacked on the basis of their personality for 25% of HRH and 18% of MSM.
Table 3.
People Who Inject Drugs | High-Risk Heterosexuals (Who Do Not inject Drugs) | Men (who do not themselves inject drugs) who have sex with men | p (PWID vs. HRH) (p’s by chi-square unless otherwise specified) | p (PWID vs. MSM) (p’s by chi-square unless otherwise specified) | p (HRH vs MSM) (p’s by chi-square unless otherwise specified) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N (may vary for some rows due to missing values) | 242 | 197 | 179 | |||
My drug use | 93% | 40% | 30% | <.001 | <.001 | .044 |
The way I use drugs | 77% | 10% | 4% | <.001 | <.001 | .036 |
My personality | 7% | 25% | 18% | <.001 | <.001 | .131 |
The way I look | 28% | 29% | 54% | .831 | <.001 | <.001 |
My clothing | 15% | 33% | 33% | <.001 | <.001 | .994 |
Not having a job | 32% | 56% | 32% | <.001 | .899 | <.001 |
Lack of education | 7% | 26% | 9% | <.001 | .276 | <.001 |
Being a single parent | 3% | 14% | 0% | <.001 | .022 (Fisher’s exact test) | <.001 |
People I hang out with | 45% | 53% | 47% | .102 | .679 | .260 |
People I have sex with | 16% | 31% | 56% | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 |
Everything about me | 4% | 2% | 3% | .350 | .587 | .758 |
My race | 6% | 10% | 18% | .055 | .016 | <.001 |
My gender | 1% | 0% | 10% | .127 (Fisher’s exact test) | <.001 | <.001 |
My sexual orientation | 7% | 4% | 58% | .104 | <.001 | <.001 |
My weight (fat or skinny) | 21% | 13% | 9% | .019 | .134 | <.001 |
My personal hygiene | 10% | 1% | 4% | .00 | .084 | .019 |
The way I speak | 4% | 8% | 28% | .064 | <.001 | <.001 |
How I earn my money | 20% | 26% | 11% | .083 | <.001 | .007 |
Being poor | 8% | 12% | 3% | .066 | <.001 | .019 |
Being over-educated | 2% | 0.4% | 0% | .223 (Fisher’s exact test) | 1.000 (Fisher’s exact test) | .162 (Fisher’s exact test) |
Being dumb | 5% | 2% | 2% | .050 | 1.000 (Fisher’s exact test) | .104 |
Having children from different men | 7% | 9% | 4% | .334 | .029 | .155 |
My housing situation | 23% | 14% | 11% | .004 | .466 | .001 |
The neighborhood where I live | 1% | 3% | 2% | .241 (Fisher’s exact test) | .522 | .717 (Fisher’s exact test) |
My HIV or HCV status | 13% | 5% | 7% | <.001 | .315 | .023 |
PWID were particularly likely to be attacked for being injectors, and (to lesser degrees) for being fat or skinny, having a poor housing situation, or being infected with HIV or hepatitis C. HRH were particularly likely to report having their dignity attacked for lack of education or being a single parent or (along with PWID) for how they earn their money. Over half of MSM were attacked for their sexual orientation and who they had sex with, and 28% of them for the way they speak. Interestingly, MSM were particularly likely to have their dignity attacked because of their race, suggesting enhanced vulnerability to racist attacks among MSM in New York.
Who were the perpetrators of the attacks
Table 4 presents data on who attacked respondents’ dignity. Relatives—and perhaps surprisingly, particularly mothers—were major sources of attacks on PWIDs’ dignity. Program staff or case managers were sources of attack for 18% of PWID (and 17% of HRH), and hospital or clinical staff for 13% of PWID—which suggests that sources of assistance may sometimes be sources of attack on PWIDs’ dignity. MSM and HRH were also likely to have their dignity attacked by relatives, though not to the same extent as PWID. MSM reported having their dignity attacked by neighbors (30%), bosses (20%; 31% among those with jobs) and co-workers (21%; 34% among those with jobs), police officers (25%), store owners (27%) and remarkably (but perhaps unsurprisingly) 53% reported having their dignity attacked by strangers. Police officers were significantly more likely to be reported as attackers of their dignity by MSM than either HRH (16%) or PWID (12%); in further analyses, this did not appear to be related to race within any key population although the statistical power of this test was low due to small N’s for mixed race.
Table 4.
People Who Inject Drugs | High-Risk Heterosexuals (Who Do Not inject Drugs) | Men (who do not themselves inject drugs) who have sex with men | p (PWID vs. HRH) (p’s by chi-square unless otherwise specified) | p (PWID vs. MSM) (p’s by chi-square unless otherwise specified) | p (HRH vs MSM) (p’s by chi-square unless otherwise specified) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N (may vary for some rows due to missing values) | 300 | 260 | 191 | |||
Mother | 41% | 18% | 15% | <.001 | <.001 | .243 |
Father | 13% | 5% | 14% | <.001 | .845 | .002 |
Brother | 24% | 11% | 21% | <.001 | .571 | .002 |
Sister | 29% | 11% | 15% | <.001 | <.001 | .189 |
Ex-spouse | 13% | 11% | 8% | .417 | .076 | .297 |
Ex-lover | 13% | 18% | 16% | .097 | .268 | .683 |
Other relatives | 21% | 24% | 22% | .414 | .863 | .578 |
Best friend | ||||||
Friends | 10% | 12% | 9% | .550 | .743 | .399 |
Neighbors | 5% | 11% | 30% | .005 | <.001 | <.001 |
Boss or employer at work | 11% | 8% | 20% | .182 | .004 | <.001 |
Boss or employer at work (among those 60 with full time or part-time jobs) | 43% | 17% | 31% | .001 | .125 | .041 |
Co-Workers | 7% | 4% | 21% | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 |
Co-workers (among respondents with full-time or part-time jobs) | 27% | 11% | 34% | .024 | .311 | .001 |
“Fellow gays” | 1% | 0% | 14% | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 |
Drug dealer | 15% | 7% | 10% | .003 | .149 | .180 |
Counselor/Social Worker | 9% | 16% | 3% | .007 | .005 | <.001 |
Probation/Parole Officer | 5% | 10% | 9% | .016 | .056 | .737 |
Police Officer | 12% | 16% | 25% | .242 | <.001 | .014 |
Storeowner | 11% | 8% | 27% | .314 | <.001 | <.001 |
Strangers | 19% | 13% | 53% | .078 | <.001 | <.001 |
Hospital or clinical staff | 13% | 7% | 14% | .027 | .719 | .018 |
Program Staff/Case Manager | 18% | 17% | 6% | .296 | <.001 | <.001 |
Security guards | 11% | 8% | 7% | .296 | .216 | .770 |
How do they react?
More than 50% of each group report that when their dignity is attacked, they very often or often shrug it off, pretend they do not care, and do nothing about it (Table 5). HRH are more likely than members of the other groups to tell the perpetrator off; PWID to cry inside, to use more drugs or to try to change their behavior or what they look like so as to prove the perpetrator wrong. MSM, it should be noted, say they never (47%) or rarely (16%) try to change themselves in reaction to dignity attacks.
Table 5.
Never | Rarely | Sometimes | Often | Very often | Number of cases | p (PWID vs. HRH) p (Linear-by-linear association)) | p (PWID vs. MSM) p (Linear-by-linear association) | p (HRH vs MSM) p (Linear-by-linear association) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
I tell them off | <.001 | <.001 | .011 | ||||||
PWID | 25% | 19% | 40% | 8% | 8% | 248 | |||
HRH | 7% | 16% | 46% | 24% | 7% | 200 | |||
MSM | 7% | 19% | 57% | 16% | 1% | 181 | |||
I cry inside | <.001 | <.001 | .120 | ||||||
PWID | 26% | 25% | 23% | 13% | 13% | 249 | |||
HRH | 49% | 21% | 24% | 3% | 4% | 191 | |||
MSM | 40% | 23% | 25% | 9% | 2% | 170 | |||
I cry openly | .001 | .002 | .660 | ||||||
PWID | 57% | 20% | 13% | 5% | 5% | 247 | |||
HRH | 73% | 10% | 15% | 1% | 1% | 190 | |||
MSM | 63% | 23% | 12% | 1% | 0% | 170 | |||
I tried to hurt them physically | .343 | .016 | .186 | ||||||
PWID | 74% | 16% | 5% | 2% | 2% | 191 | |||
HRH | 74% | 10% | 10% | 4% | 2% | 187 | |||
MSM | 57% | 26% | 15% | 2% | 1% | 168 | |||
I tried to hurt myself physically | <.001 | .466 | <.001 | ||||||
PWID | 61% | 18% | 10% | 6% | 5% | 192 | |||
HRH | 86% | 7% | 3% | 2% | 1% | 184 | |||
MSM | 61% | 17% | 14% | 5% | 0% | 160 | |||
I shrug it off, pretend I don’t care, and do nothing | .348 | .884 | .264 | ||||||
PWID | 6% | 5% | 24% | 26% | 39% | 249 | |||
HRH | 2% | 3% | 36% | 32% | 27% | 199 | |||
MSM | 1% | 7% | 29% | 28% | 35% | 181 | |||
I use more drugs | <.001 | <.001 | .260 | ||||||
PWID | 16% | 21% | 36% | 10% | 17% | 248 | |||
HRH | 25% | 35% | 28% | 4% | 8% | 191 | |||
MSM | 20% | 33% | 29% | 15% | 2% | 168 | |||
I use more alcohol | .579 | .077 | .012 | ||||||
PWID | 34% | 23% | 24% | 7% | 12% | 228 | |||
HRH | 26% | 33% | 31% | 4% | 6% | 194 | |||
MSM | 18% | 23% | 41% | 15% | 2% | 165 | |||
I have more sex | .034 | .006 | <.001 | ||||||
PWID | 68% | 21% | 6% | 3% | 2% | 173 | |||
HRH | 73% | 22% | 4% | 1% | 0% | 187 | |||
MSM | 44% | 39% | 13% | 1% | 2% | 164 | |||
I think about changing what I do, or what I look like to prove them wrong | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 | ||||||
PWID | 18% | 16% | 30% | 16% | 20% | 244 | |||
HRH | 29% | 18% | 28% | 17% | 7% | 196 | |||
MSM | 47% | 16% | 28% | 8% | 2% | 173 |
The question asked was: When they attack your dignity, how often do you react in each of these ways? (We ask them to circle each of a list of possible answers)
Social characteristics and frequency of dignity denial experiences
Table 6 provides data within each Key Population for associations between dignity attack experiences and socioeconomic indicators. In seven of nine associations, the jobless, homeless and very low income members of each key population were significantly (p < .05) more likely to report having their own dignity attacked more often. For homelessness among HRH, the directionality is the same, but only indicate a trend towards significance (p < 0.06). Among MSM, it appears that homelessness is not associated with the frequency of dignity attacks. Neither sex nor engaging in sex work in the last 30 days was associated with being attacked within either the PWID or HRH groups; but among MSM, those who received some form of payment for sex had their dignity attacked more frequently.
Table 6.
Table 6.a. For people who inject drugs (PWID | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
None | 1–2 times | 3–10 times | About once a month | About once or twice a week | Daily or more | Total | p (Linear-by-linear association) | |
Having one’s dignity attacked | ||||||||
Not currently employed | 15% | 11% | 25% | 18% | 16% | 15% | 233 | |
Currently employed | 26% | 29% | 14% | 15% | 14% | 2% | 58 | <.001 |
Currently domiciled | 21% | 18% | 21% | 19% | 15% | 5% | 225 | |
Currently homeless | 2% | 2% | 25% | 11% | 16% | 44% | 55 | <.001 |
Income $10,000+ | 24% | 26% | 14% | 19% | 14% | 1% | 110 | |
Income less than $10,000 | 12% | 8% | 28% | 17% | 16% | 18% | 179 | <.001 |
Did not sell sex for money or goods in last 30 days | 18% | 14% | 25% | 17% | 14% | 12% | 242 | |
Sold sex (participant received money/drugs/gifts) | 12% | 16% | 12% | 20% | 22% | 16% | 49 | .089 |
Male | 22% | 14% | 19% | 19% | 12% | 13% | 164 | |
Female | 10% | 15% | 23% | 18% | 16% | 12% | 127 | .163 |
Table 6b. For high-risk heterosexuals (HRH) | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
None | 1–2 times | 3–10 times | About once a month | About once or twice a week | Daily or more | Total | p (Linear-by-linear association) | |
Having one’s dignity attacked | ||||||||
Not currently employed | 18% | 15% | 21% | 25% | 16% | 5% | 191 | |
Currently employed | 37.5% | 28% | 16% | 12% | 2% | 5% | 64 | <.001 |
Currently domiciled | 24% | 18% | 20% | 20% | 12% | 5% | 229 | |
Currently homeless | 8% | 20% | 16% | 32% | 16% | 8% | 25 | .058 |
Income $10,000+ | 29% | 22% | 20% | 17% | 8% | 3% | 143 | |
Income less than $10,000 | 14% | 14% | 19% | 28% | 18% | 7% | 112 | <.001 |
Did not sell sex for money or goods in last 30 days | 25% | 17% | 17% | 23% | 13% | 5% | 210 | |
Sold sex (participant received money/drugs/gifts) | 11% | 27% | 31% | 16% | 11% | 4% | 45 | .838 |
Male | 23% | 16% | 21% | 20% | 12% | 7% | 129 | |
Female | 23% | 20% | 19% | 22% | 12% | 3% | 122 | .498 |
Table 6c. For men who have sex with men (MSM) | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
None | 1–2 times | 3–10 times | About once a month | About once or twice a week | Daily or more | Total | p (Linear-by-linear association) | |
Having one’s dignity attacked | ||||||||
Not currently employed | 5% | 7% | 22% | 56% | 9% | 0% | 85 | |
Currently employed | 7% | 17% | 33% | 33% | 9% | 0% | 105 | .013 |
Currently domiciled | 6% | 14% | 27% | 42% | 10% | 0% | 162 | |
Currently homeless | 4% | 4% | 36% | 54% | 4% | 0% | 28 | .514 |
Income $12,500+ | 4% | 15% | 40% | 34% | 7% | 0% | 104 | |
Income less than $12,500 | 8% | 9% | 14% | 56% | 13% | 0% | 86 | .032 |
Did not sell sex for money or goods in last 30 days | 7% | 15% | 29% | 41% | 7% | 0% | 149 | |
Sold sex (participant received money/drugs/gifts) | 0% | 2% | 27% | 54% | 17% | 0% | 41 | .001 |
The question asked was: In the last year, how often have people spoken or acted towards you in a way that felt like they were attacking your dignity or demeaning you?
For MSM, we used an income cut-off of $12,500 due to MSM sample having higher incomes than the PWID or HRH samples
Social characteristics and changes in drug use, alcohol use and sexual frequency
Table 7 provides data within each Key Population for associations between social characteristics and how participants react when their dignity is attacked. Among people who inject drugs, a higher income and selling sex make participants more likely to react to attacks on their dignity by using more drugs, and sex sellers and the homeless by using more alcohol and having more sex. Among high-risk heterosexuals, sex workers report more drug use and alcohol use when their dignity is attacked, lower income people report more alcohol use, and those without jobs report that they react with more alcohol use and more sex. Among MSM, those without jobs, with low incomes and who engage in sex work react by using more drugs and alcohol, and the homeless and sex traders by having more sex.
Table 7.
Never | Rarely | Sometimes | Often | Very often | Number of cases | p (Linear-by-linear association) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
a. I use more drugs | <.001 | ||||||
PWID | |||||||
Not currently employed | 18% | 20% | 37% | 9% | 16% | 204 | |
Currently employed | 9% | 25% | 30% | 14% | 23% | 44 | .144 |
Currently domiciled | 17% | 22% | 34% | 19% | 17% | 182 | |
Currently homeless | 13% | 16% | 38% | 13% | 29% | 55 | .247 |
Income $10,000+ | 7% | 19% | 33% | 13% | 27% | 84 | |
Income less than $10,000 | 22% | 22% | 36% | 9% | 12% | 162 | <.001 |
Did not sell sex for money or goods in last 30 days | 18% | 21% | 38% | 9% | 14% | 205 | |
Sold sex (participant received money/drugs/gifts) | 7% | 16% | 28% | 16% | 33% | 43 | .001 |
Male | 20% | 20% | 28% | 11% | 20% | 134 | |
Female) | 12% | 21% | 45% | 9% | 13% | 114 | .923 |
HRH | |||||||
Not currently employed | 21% | 37% | 32% | 4% | 6% | 153 | |
Currently employed | 42% | 29% | 10% | 5% | 13% | 38 | .328 |
Currently domiciled | 255 | 36% | 27% | 2% | 8% | 167 | |
Currently homeless | 22% | 26% | 30% | 17% | 4% | 23 | .338 |
Income $10,000+ | 34% | 35% | 16% | 3% | 12% | 96 | |
Income less than $10,000 | 16% | 35% | 40% | 5% | 4% | 95 | .121 |
Did not sell sex for money or goods in last 30 days | 29% | 34% | 29% | 1% | 6% | 148 | |
Sold sex (participant received money/drugs/gifts) | 8% | 38% | 23% | 15% | 15% | 39 | <.001 |
Male | 25% | 30% | 36% | 2% | 7% | 97 | |
Female) | 24% | 41% | 19% | 7% | 9% | 90 | .873 |
MSM | |||||||
Not currently employed | 11% | 30% | 34% | 22% | 2% | 82 | |
Currently employed | 28% | 36% | 24% | 9% | 2% | 86 | .001 |
Currently domiciled | 21% | 33% | 29% | 15% | 1% | 140 | |
Currently homeless | 11% | 36% | 29% | 18% | 7% | 28 | .130 |
Income $12,500+ | 26% | 39% | 24% | 10% | 1% | 88 | |
Income less than $12,500 | 12% | 27% | 35% | 21% | 4% | 80 | .001 |
Did not sell sex for money or goods in last 30 days | 24% | 32% | 27% | 17% | 0% | 126 | |
Sold sex (participant received money/drugs/gifts) | 7% | 36% | 36% | 12% | 9% | 42 | .017 |
b. I use more alcohol | |||||||
PWID | |||||||
Not currently employed | 34% | 21% | 26% | 6% | 11% | 186 | |
Currently employed | 33% | 31% | 14% | 7% | 14% | 42 | ,978 |
Currently domiciled | 40% | 24% | 20% | 6% | 10% | 164 | |
Currently homeless | 21% | 23% | 18% | 7% | 21% | 53 | .003 |
Income $10,000+ | 32% | 25% | 15% | 7% | 20% | 80 | |
Income less than $10,000 | 36% | 23% | 28% | 6% | 7% | 146 | .105 |
Did not sell sex for money or goods in last 30 days | 38% | 23% | 25% | 5% | 9% | 185 | |
Sold sex (participant received money/drugs/gifts) | 16% | 26% | 21% | 14% | 23% | 43 | <.001 |
Male | 38% | 20% | 22% | 7% | 12% | 125 | |
Female) | 29% | 27% | 26% | 6% | 12% | 103 | ,600 |
HRH | |||||||
Not currently employed | 21% | 32% | 36% | 4% | 6% | 156 | |
Currently employed | 45% | 37% | 10% | 3% | 5% | 38 | .005 |
Currently domiciled | 24% | 35% | 32% | 2% | 6% | 170 | |
Currently homeless | 35% | 22% | 22% | 17% | 4% | 23 | .920 |
Income $10,000+ | 32% | 39% | 17% | 3% | 8% | 97 | |
Income less than $10,000 | 20% | 27% | 44% | 5% | 4% | 97 | .049 |
Did not sell sex for money or goods in last 30 days | 29% | 34% | 31% | 1% | 4% | 151 | |
Sold sex (participant received money/drugs/gifts) | 13% | 31% | 28% | 13% | 15% | 39 | <.001 |
Male | 23% | 29% | 40% | 2% | 6% | 98 | |
Female) | 28% | 39% | 21% | 5% | 6% | 92 | .314 |
MSM | |||||||
Not currently employed | 8% | 17% | 52% | 19% | 2% | 82 | |
Currently employed | 28% | 29% | 30% | 11% | 2% | 83 | <.001 |
Currently domiciled | 20% | 23% | 41% | 14% | 1% | 138 | |
Currently homeless | 11% | 22% | 41% | 18% | 7% | 27 | .118 |
Income $12,500+ | 26% | 28% | 34% | 12% | 1% | 86 | |
Income less than $12,500 | 10% | 18% | 49% | 19% | 4% | 79 | .001 |
Did not sell sex for money or goods in last 30 days | 23% | 24% | 37% | 15% | 1% | 123 | |
Sold sex (participant received money/drugs/gifts) | 5% | 19% | 55% | 14% | 7% | 42 | .004 |
c. I have more sex | |||||||
PWID | |||||||
Not currently employed | 69% | 21% | 3% | 4% | 2% | 132 | |
Currently employed | 63% | 19% | 15% | 9% | 2% | 41 | .505 |
Currently domiciled | 70% | 21% | 5% | 1% | 2% | 131 | |
Currently homeless | 57% | 22% | 8% | 11% | 3% | 37 | .027 |
Income $10,000+ | 56% | 29% | 8% | 2% | 5% | 79 | |
Income less than $10,000 | 79% | 14% | 4% | 3% | 0% | 94 | .004 |
Did not sell sex for money or goods in last 30 days | 78% | 13% | 4% | 2% | 1% | 135 | |
Sold sex (participant received money/drugs/gifts) | 32% | 47% | 10% | 5% | 5% | 38 | <.001 |
Male | 72% | 17% | 5% | 3% | 2% | 98 | |
Female) | 63% | 25% | 7% | 3% | 3% | 75 | .371 |
HRH | |||||||
Not currently employed | 69% | 25% | 5% | 1% | 0% | 150 | |
Currently employed | 89% | 11% | 0% | 0% | 0% | 37 | .012 |
Currently domiciled | 73% | 23% | 4% | 0% | 0% | 163 | |
Currently homeless | 70% | 17% | 4% | 9% | 0% | 23 | .110 |
Income $10,000+ | 77% | 21% | 1% | 1% | 0% | 95 | |
Income less than $10,000 | 68% | 24% | 6% | 1% | 0% | 92 | .115 |
Did not sell sex for money or goods in last 30 days | 75% | 21% | 3% | 1% | 0% | 145 | |
Sold sex (participant received money/drugs/gifts) | 61% | 32% | 5% | 3% | 0% | 38 | .066 |
Male | 75% | 29% | 4% | 1% | 0% | 95 | |
Female) | 69% | 26% | 3% | 1% | 9% | 88 | .594 |
MSM | |||||||
Not currently employed | 37% | 44% | 16% | 1% | 2% | 82 | |
Currently employed | 52% | 34% | 11% | 1% | 1% | 82 | .070 |
Currently domiciled | 48% | 38% | 12% | 1% | 1% | 137 | |
Currently homeless | 26% | 44% | 18% | 4% | 7% | 27 | .003 |
Income $12,500+ | 51% | 34% | 13% | 1% | 1% | 85 | |
Income less than $12,500 | 38% | 39% | 13% | 1% | 2% | 79 | .186 |
Did not sell sex for money or goods in last 30 days | 50% | 38% | 10% | 2% | 0% | 123 | |
Sold sex (participant received money/drugs/gifts) | 27% | 41% | 24% | 0% | 7% | 41 | ,000 |
The question asked was: When they attack your dignity, how often do you react in each of these ways? (We ask them to circle each of a list of possible answers)
Exploratory associations with risk behaviors
Although we see dignity attacks as most importantly related to longitudinal dynamics that will lead youth and, perhaps to a lesser extent, their elders, to be more likely to take up high-risk sexual and drug behaviors and roles, it is also useful to understand the cross-sectional associations of dignity attacks with measures of risk behavior. For these analyses (Table 8), we looked at the correlations of the frequency of having one’s dignity attacked with selected risk behaviors and with how often participants responded to dignity attacks by increasing their drug use, alcohol use, and sexual frequency. We also looked at the correlations of how often participants responded to dignity attacks by increasing their drug use, alcohol use, and sexual frequency with the specific risk behaviors. Multivariate logistic regressions were run controlling for age, race/ethnicity (Non-Hispanic White vs. other), and (for PWID and HRH) sex. In these cells, in addition to r, the adjusted odds ratio and 95% confidences intervals are also given. For MSM, the regression coefficients and confidence intervals for the analysis of number of sex partners are from linear regression since the dependent variable is continuous.
Table 8.
A. People who inject drugs | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Dignity attack variables | ||||
Behavioral variable | Frequency of having dignity attacked3 | React to dignity attacks by using more drugs | React to dignity attacks by using more alcohol | React to dignity attacks by having more sex |
React to dignity attacks by using more drugs | .454* | NA | ||
React to dignity attacks by using more alcohol | .361* | .613* | NA | |
React to dignity attacks by having more sex | .217* | .498* | .574* | NA |
Any unprotected sex with non-main partner | .040 1.06 (.88, 1.28) |
.214* 1.18 (.90, 1.54) |
.263* 1.27 (1.0, 1.63) |
.380* 2.04 (1.17, 3.57) |
Any sex partner2 | .048 1.04 (.89, 1.23) |
.146* 1.04 (.83, 1.32) |
.133* 1.07 (.86, 1.34) |
.229** 1.41 (.87, 2.30) |
More than one sex partner2 | .029 1.04 (.86, 1.27) |
.278** 1.35 (1.02, 1.80) |
.197** 1.13 (.87, 1.45) |
.316** 1.42 (.91, 2.22) |
Any exchange sex | .138* 1.26 (1.04, 1.54) |
.323* 1.75 (1.29, 2.36) |
.398* 1.80 (1.36, 2.38) |
.483* 6.31 (2.65, 15.03) |
Had sex at a group sex event | −.041 .88 (.68, 1.13) |
.256* 1.68 (1.12, 2.52) |
.205* 1.25 (.90, 1.72) |
.434* 2.11 (1.24, 3.58) |
Any receptive syringe sharing | .285* 1.67 (1.33, 2.11) |
.177 1.04 (.78, 1.39) |
.325* 1.49 (1.15, 1.93) |
.197* 1.16 (.76, 1.77) |
Any distributive syringe sharing | .226* 1.42 (1.17, 1.72) |
.142** .97 (.75, 1.26) |
.314* 1.42 (1.12, 1.80) |
.201* 1.17 (.78, 1.76) |
B. High risk heterosexuals | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Dignity attack variables | ||||
Behavioral variable | Frequency of having dignity attacked2 | React to dignity attacks by using more drugs | React to dignity attacks by using more alcohol | React to dignity attacks by having more sex |
React to dignity attacks by using more drugs | .364* | NA | ||
React to dignity attacks by using more alcohol | .482* | .769* | NA | |
React to dignity attacks by having more sex | .235* | .387* | .420* | NA |
Any unprotected sex with non-main partner | −.136* .76 (.62, .94) |
.149* 1.54 (1.15, 2.06) |
.141* 1.45 (1.07, 1.95) |
.058 1.23 (.73, 2.08) |
More than one sex partner2 | −.003 .95 (.80, 1.14) |
.192* 1.45 (1.10, 1.92) |
.258* 1.71 (1.27, 2.31) |
.184* 1.94 (1.12, 3.37) |
Any exchange sex | .108 1.18 (.97, 1.44) |
.288* 1.66 (1.23, 2.22) |
.315* 1.85 (1.36, 2.52) |
.185* 1.67 (1.00, 2.78) |
Had sex at a group sex event | −.016 .94 (.74, 1.20) |
.134* 1.45 (1.03, 2.03) |
.159* 1.53 (1.08, 2.17) |
.124 1.65 (.90, 3.00) |
C. Men who have sex with men | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Dignity attack variables | ||||
Behavioral variable | Frequency of having dignity attacked | React to dignity attacks by using more drugs | React to dignity attacks by using more alcohol | React to dignity attacks by having more sex |
React to dignity attacks by using more drugs | .183* | NA | ||
React to dignity attacks by using more alcohol | .235* | .805* | NA | |
React to dignity attacks by having more sex | .133 | .515* | .550* | NA |
Any unprotected sex with non-main partner | −.074 .95 (.69, 1.31) |
.287* 1.81 (1.28, 2.56) |
.212* 1.47 (1.05, 2.06) |
.097 1.21 (.83, 1.77) |
Number of sex partners2 | −.263** −.64 (−1.05, −.24) |
.294** .76 (.36, 1.16) |
.186* .51 (.09, .94) |
.167* .64 (.14, 1.15) |
Any exchange sex | .222* 1.67 (1.12, 2.47) |
.323* 1.80 (1.26, 2.58) |
.357* 1.95 (1.33, 2.86) |
.371* 2.30 (1.49, 3.56) |
Had sex at a group sex event | .088 1.43 (.97, 2.13) |
.256* 1.70 (1.18, 2.46) |
.189* 1.52 (1.05, 2.23) |
.234* 1.81 (1.19, 2.77) |
Cell entries are Pearson’s correlations. An * indicates that the parameter has p < .05, and ** indicates p < .01. For selected cells, multivariate logistic regressions were run controlling for Age, Race/Ethnicity (Non-Hispanic White vs. other), and (for PWID and HRH) sex. In these cells, in addition to r, the adjusted odds ratio and 95% confidences intervals are also given. For MSM, the regression coefficients and confidence intervals are from linear regression since the dependent variable is continuous.
For analyses of sex partner numbers, different ways of categorizing these were used for the different risk groups based on the distributions in the sample. For PWID, the reference group is those who reported having no sex partners, and the other categories are one sex partner and more than one sex partner. For high-risk heterosexuals, we dichotomized into more than one sex partner vs. one or zero partners. Correlations are not given for this set of variables this the underlying variable on number of partners is a trichotomy. For MSM, we used the number of sex partners they reported.
Among PWID, dignity denial frequency has significant positive associations with receptive syringe sharing, distributive syringe sharing, and any exchange sex, as well as with more often responding to dignity denial with more drugs, more alcohol and more sex.
Among HRH, dignity denial frequency has significant positive associations with more often responding to dignity denial with more drugs, more alcohol and more sex. It has a weak negative correlation with unprotected sex with a non-main partner.
Among MSM, dignity denial frequency significant positive associations with any exchange sex and with more often responding to dignity denial with more drugs and more alcohol (and a tendency (p .068) with more sex. It has a negative correlation with the number of sex partners (which could mean that people more “into the scene” are less often in a position to have their dignity attacked.)
For all groups, responding to dignity denial with more drugs, more alcohol and more sex are positively related to most indicators of sexual risk behavior (and for PWID, with receptive and with distributive syringe sharing.)
DISCUSSION
Overall, members of all three key populations experienced attacks on their dignity fairly often. Such attacks were reported more often by people who inject drugs and by men who have sex with men, which is not terribly surprising since both drug injection and same-sex-sex are socially stigmatized. Furthermore, members of all groups report seeing other’s dignity being attacked fairly often, with a third of PWID seeing this daily and more than half of both PWID and HRH seeing it weekly or more.
Our findings are of course limited by possible errors in self-report. We would in particular mention that there may be differences in proclivities to report or recognize dignity attacks among the different populations being sampled. This could occur if populations that are frequently attacked or stigmatized are more sensitive and thus interpret non-attacks as attacks in some cases.
Findings are limited in several other ways as well. Since the study is cross-sectional, what can be learned about causation is extremely limited and tentative. The relatively small study sizes of each of the three sub-samples has limited the extent to which we can conduct multivariate analysis. Furthermore, there are undoubtedly experiential and cultural differences in the ways that the three subpopulations (PWID, HRH, and MSM) interpret some of the questionnaire items. Differences in race/ethnicity, sex, age, HIV status and many other variables may also be associated with differences in how participants interpret some items.
Nonetheless it is noteworthy that almost two-fifths of “high-risk heterosexuals” reported having their dignity attacked in the last month. Since some of them use non-injected drugs and/or engage in sex trade, some of these attacks may be due to this. From Table 3, however, we see that the two most frequent reasons they give for their dignity being attacked are their economic circumstances and the people they hang around with. More research is needed for us to get a comprehensive understanding of this phenomenon, but we would suggest as a working hypothesis that an ostensibly meritocratic society based on interpersonal competition and political campaigns that attack the dignity of those who are not highly successful may generate a culture in which interpersonal dignity attacks are frequent.
The attacks being studied are interpersonal ones, but the people who attack the different populations have some differences. Relatives, and particularly mothers, are major sources of dignity attacks for PWID (and relatives are also sources, to a lesser extent, for HRH and MSM.) Some of these attacks on their dignity probably arise out of (often self-defeating) criticisms or other processes of interaction between relatives and drug users, but this is probably worsened by the war on drugs and other forms of public attacks on the humanity and dignity of people who use drugs (33 – 36). Similar attacks on sex work and sex workers may also help explain these data, since about a quarter of each population report having exchanged sex for money or drugs in the last month (14). MSM have their dignity attacked in a wide range of impersonal relationships, including by neighbors, bosses, co-workers, police officers, store owners, health care providers and strangers (35, 37, 38), and any resulting internalized stigma can also lead to worse health outcomes (39). While these attacks do occur, it is important to note that kin or other people can sometimes play a supportive role which is related to positive health outcomes including support of others around HIV prevention (40, 41).
Respondents in all three groups report that they very often shrug off attacks on their dignity. People who inject drugs seem to take such attacks particularly hard: They report crying inside, using more drugs or trying to change their behavior. MSM, on the other hand, seem to accept their being MSM far more than PWID accept their drug use, as indicated by over three fifths of MSM reporting they never or rarely try to change themselves in reaction to dignity attacks. This self-acceptance is probably due to the self-confidence and pride that many gay and bisexual men have developed through their social movements and other victories (1, 6, 42.) In addition, gayness and perhaps bisexuality or queerness in some localities, including New York, are probably more unifying self-identities than injecting drugs, with much more organized political activity and community building and tangible gains (43). However, while there may be an appearance of unification within MSM, the intersection between these key populations and race may lead to multiple subordination. For example, MSM who are also racial/ethnic minorities are at significantly increased risk for social disadvantage and poor health outcomes (44 – 47). In addition, the relatively high attacks on dignity by “other gays” towards the majority-Black MSM respondents in this sample suggests that these could be attacks by other MSM in majority race categories. The double minority status of Black MSM and feelings of exclusion from dominant LGBT populations has led some to ask whether Black MSM are part of the rainbow (48).
Some members of all three groups, PWID, HRH and MSM, do report that dignity attacks lead them to increase their sexual, alcohol and/or drug behaviors. To the extent that they do so, this seems to be associated with high-risk behaviors with controls for confounding by race/ethnicity, age and where appropriate for sex—and thus perhaps with HIV transmission rates. We interpret this result as consistent with dignity attacks being a switching point that leads some people onto pathways of more alcohol, more drugs and/or more sex and this leads to increased probability of HIV risk. This tends to support our hypothesis that dignity attacks may be part of a pathway toward HIV vulnerability. If confirmed in future longitudinal studies (or trials of interventions to reduce dignity attacks), findings of this sort would strongly support the pathways approach to analyzing dignity attacks and would provide some support for the CHAT model of Big Events and macro-structural interventions as presented by Friedman et al in this journal (6).
Data on interpersonal differences in joblessness, homelessness, and very low income may not be the best proxy for the effects of macro-social (structural) interventions or of Big Events (since misfortune caused by these may be reacted to very differently by one’s social circle and by oneself than a similar kind of misfortune in the absence of such visible interventions or in normal times), but it is all we have available in this dataset. By and large, homelessness, joblessness and very low income seem to be associated with having one’s dignity attacked. This finding is consistent with our discussion of dignity attacks as fostered by class and status differences in a culture based on invidious comparisons. This pattern of findings tends to increase our support for the validity of the measures of dignity attacks, and also lends (indirect) support for the underlying theory that some macro-social interventions or Big Events may tend to increase dignity attacks (to the extent that they produce joblessness, homelessness and very low incomes). Further, these same social characteristics and engaging in the sex trade are to some extent associated with reacting to dignity attacks by using more drugs or alcohol.
Since dignity attacks and stigma are associated with a range of ill health effects and with less willingness to use health services (2 – 5, 15 – 20) (and particularly since these data suggest that sizeable proportions of these populations have their dignity attacked by service providers—and we suspect that they almost certainly tell their friends about this), this paper suggests that dignity attacks may be important to study. Furthermore, we need more research on how macro-social interventions and Big Events affect dignity attacks and whether this is a pathway that helps determine whether a given set of interventions or Events does or does not lead to increases in PWID, sex work, and/or HIV or other diseases.
Thus, we are calling for several types of research. We need qualitative research into dignity attacks as they take place in communities and at workplaces, into how they are related to processes of intersectional oppression or subordination, how they affect groups of people at high risk of becoming PWID, sex workers, or of engaging in high-risk sex, and relatedly how they affect the lives and HIV outcomes of PWID, high-risk heterosexuals, and MSM.
Prospective studies are also needed of macro-social interventions, Big Events and other social changes; of the conditions and pathways that affect whether they do or do not lead to changes in attacks by various groups of actors on the dignity of PWID, high-risk heterosexuals, sex workers and MSM; of if and how such attacks lead to changes in risk and transmission behaviors, in risk networks, in social networks, and in health outcomes like HIV transmission rates, involvement in the continuum of care, and morbidity and mortality. A related issue for such prospective studies is if and how macro-interventions or events and dignity attacks, along with other pathways (1, 10 – 12), lead some youth to enter into high risk networks and behaviors.
Such research should integrate issues of structural stigma and individual stigma (2 – 5, 13 – 26). These are related concepts, and combining the insights and measures developed in the stigma literature should improve what we learn from studies such as those just discussed.
A final area that needs research is how to prevent negative pathways from developing and/or how to intervene in those that do develop. Specifically, we need research on how to keep macro-social changes (whether based on interventions or not), Big Events, oppression, or exploitation from precipitating structural or individual attacks on dignity; and of how to mitigate the effects of such attacks as do occur.
Issues of dignity attacks and stigmatization are not merely matters for study, of course. They are also issues for action (1, 49). One approach to this might be to encourage and to engage with social movements to change police practices including attacks on dignity that have been important in the USA, as is evidenced by the current Black Lives Matter movement and by Stonewall and subsequent gay activism. People who use drugs and sex workers have also engaged in considerable activism around these issues (33). Another might be to find other ways to alter the social and cultural contexts of people’s lives that encourage some relatives or friends to attack others’ dignity.
Acknowledgments
Funding: This research was supported by NIH grants R01DA031597, P30DA011041, DP1DA034989 and R01MH100021. The International AIDS Society (IAS) and the National Institute on Drug Abuse supported the post-doc fellowship of GN. We also acknowledge support from the University of Buenos Aires grants UBACyT 20020130100790BA and UBACyT 20020100101021. The views presented in this paper represent only the authors and not the funding agencies.
Footnotes
Compliance with Ethical Standards:
Disclosure of potential conflicts of interest: The authors (Friedman, Samuel R; Pouget, Enrique R, Sandoval, Milagros; Rossi, Diana; Mateu-Gelabert, Pedro; Nikolopoulos, Georgios K; Schneider, John A; Smyrnov, Pavlo; Stall, Ron D) all declare that they have no competing interests.
Research involving human participants and/or animals:
This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committees and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent: Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study
References
- 1.Friedman S, Rossi D, Ralón G. Dignity denial and social conflicts. Rethinking Marxism. 2015 Jan 2;27(1):65–84. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Jacobson N. Dignity and health: A review. Social Science & Medicine. 2007;64(2):292–302. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.08.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Jacobson N. Dignity violation in health care. Qualitative Health Research. 2009;19(11):1536–47. doi: 10.1177/1049732309349809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Jacobson N. Dignity & health. Nashville: Vanderbilt University Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Jacobson N, Oliver V, Koch A. An urban geography of dignity. Health & Place. 2009;15(3):725–31. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2008.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Friedman SR, Sandoval M, Mateu-Gelabert P, Rossi D, Gwadz M, Dombrowski K, Smyrnov P, Vasylyeva T, Pouget ER, Perlman DC. Theory, Measurement and Hard Times: Some issues for HIV/AIDS research. AIDS & Behavior. 2013;6:1915–1925. doi: 10.1007/s10461-013-0475-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Fullilove MT. Root shock: how tearing up city neighborhoods hurts American and what we can do about it. 1. New York, NY: The Random House Publishing Group; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wallace D, Wallace R. A plague on your houses: How New York was burned down and national public health crumbled. New York City: Verso Books; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Nikolopoulos GK, Fotiou A, Kanavou E, Richardson C, Detsis M, Pharris A, Suk JE, Semenza JC, Costa-Storti C, Paraskevis D, Sypsa V. National income inequality and declining GDP growth rates are associated with increases in HIV diagnoses among people who inject drugs in Europe: a panel data analysis. PloS one. 2015 Apr 15;10(4):e0122367. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0122367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Friedman SR, Rossi D, Braine D. Theorizing “Big Events” as a potential risk environment for drug use, drug-related harm and HIV epidemic outbreaks. International Journal on Drug Policy. 2009;20:283–291. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2008.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Nikolopoulos GK, Sypsa V, Bonovas S, Paraskevis D, Malliori-Minerva M, Hatzakis A, Friedman SR. Big events in Greece and HIV infection among people who inject drugs. Substance use & misuse. 2015 Jun 7;50(7):825–38. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2015.978659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Pouget ER, Sandoval M, Nikolopoulos GK, Mateu-Gelabert P, Rossi D, Smyrnov P, Jones Y, Friedman SR. Developing Measures of Pathways that May Link Macro Social/Structural Changes with HIV Epidemiology. AIDS and Behavior. 2016 Jan;:1–3. doi: 10.1007/s10461-016-1291-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Goffman E. Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. Englewood Cliffs, N.J: Prentice Hall; 1963. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Simić M, Rhodes T. Violence, dignity and HIV vulnerability: street sex work in Serbia. Sociology of Health and Illness. 2009;31(1):1–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9566.2008.01112.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Link BG, Phelan JC. Conceptualizing stigma. Annual Review of Sociology. 2001;(27):363–85. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Risher K, Mayer KH, Beyrer C. HIV treatment cascade in MSM, people who inject drugs, and sex workers. Curr Opin HIV AIDS. 2015;10(6):420–429. doi: 10.1097/COH.0000000000000200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Mizuno Y, Purcell DW, Knowlton AR, Wilkinson JD, Gourevitch MN, Knight KR. Syndemic vulnerability, sexual and injection risk behaviors, and HIV continuum of care outcomes in HIV-positive injection drug users. AIDS Behav. 2015;19(4):684–693. doi: 10.1007/s10461-014-0890-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Baral S, Holland CE, Shannon K, et al. Enhancing benefits or increasing harms: community responses for HIV among men who have sex with men, transgender women, female sex workers, and people who inject drugs. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2014;66(Suppl 3):S319–328. doi: 10.1097/QAI.0000000000000233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Malta M, Ralil da Costa M, Bastos FI. The paradigm of universal access to HIV-treatment and human rights violation: how do we treat HIV-positive people who use drugs? Curr HIV/AIDS Rep. 2014;11(1):52–62. doi: 10.1007/s11904-013-0196-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Mateu-Gelabert P, Maslow C, Flom PL, Sandoval M, Bolyard M, Friedman SR. Keeping it together: stigma, response, and perception of risk in relationships between drug injectors and crack smokers, and other community residents. AIDS care. 2005;17(7):802–813. doi: 10.1080/09540120500100486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Social Science & Medicine. Special issue on Structural Stigma and Population Health. 2014 Feb;103 doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.12.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hatzenbuehler Mark L. Introduction to the special issue on structural stigma and health. Social Science & Medicine. 2014 Feb;103:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.12.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Hatzenbuehler ML, McLaughlin KA, Keyes KM, Hasin DS. The Impact of Institutional Discrimination on Psychiatric Disorders in Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Populations: A Prospective Study. American Journal of Public Health: March 2010. 100(3):452–459. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.168815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hatzenbuehler ML. The Social Environment and Suicide Attempts in Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Youth. Pediatrics. 2011 May;127(5):896–903. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-3020. Epub 2011 Apr 18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hatzenbuehler ML, O’Cleirigh C, Grasso C, Mayer K, Safren S, Bradford J. Effect of Same-Sex Marriage Laws on Health Care Use and Expenditures in Sexual Minority Men: A Quasi-Natural Experiment. American Journal of Public Health. 2012 Feb;102(2):285–291. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hatzenbuehler ML, Bellatorre A, Lee Y, Finch BK, Muennig P, Fiscella K. Structural stigma and all-cause mortality in sexual minority populations. Social Science & Medicine. 2014 Feb;103:33–41. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Sue DW, Bucceri J, Lin AI, Nadal KL, Gina C, Torino GC. Racial Microaggressions and the Asian American Experience. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2007;13(1):72–81. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.13.1.72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Swann G, Minshew R, Newcomb ME, Mustanski B. Validation of the Sexual Orientation Microaggression Inventory in Two Diverse Samples of LGBTQ Youth. Arch Sex Behav. 2016 Aug;45(6):1289–98. doi: 10.1007/s10508-016-0718-2. Epub 2016 Apr 11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Jacoby Ann. Felt versus enacted stigma: A concept revisited: Evidence from a study of people with epilepsy in remission. Social Science & Medicine. 1994 Jan;38(2):270. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)90396-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.USAID. CAN WE MEASURE HIV/AIDS-RELATED STIGMA AND DISCRIMINATION? CURRENT KNOWLEDGE ABOUT QUANTIFYING STIGMA IN DEVELOPING COUNTRIES. http://www.policyproject.com/pubs/generalreport/Measure%20HIV%20Stigma.pdf (Downloaded 8/14/2016)
- 31.Friedman Samuel R, Maslow Carey, Bolyard Melissa, Sandoval Milagros, Mateu-Gelabert Pedro, Neaigus Alan. Urging others to be healthy: “Intravention” by injection drug users as a community prevention goal. AIDS Education and Prevention. 2004;16:250–263. doi: 10.1521/aeap.16.3.250.35439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Agresti A. Categorical Data Analysis. 2. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2002. pp. 369–373. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Dziuban A, Friedman SR. A gap in science’s and the media images of people who use drugs and sex workers: research on organizations of the oppressed. Substance Use and Misuse, Substance Use & Misuse. 2015;50:508–511. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2015.978194. print/1532–2491 online. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Friedman SR. The political economy of drug-user scapegoating—and the philosophy and politics of resistance. Drugs: Education, Prevention, and Policy. 1998a;1:15–32. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Friedman SR. HIV-related politics in long-term perspective. AIDS Care. 1998b;10(Supplement 2):S93–S103. doi: 10.1080/09540129850124208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Tempalski B, Friedman R, Keem M, Cooper H, Friedman SR. NIMBY localism and national inequitable exclusion alliances: The case of syringe exchange programs in the United States. Geoforum. 2007;38(6):1250–1263. doi: 10.1016/j.geoforum.2007.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Dale SK, Bogart LM, Galvan FH, Wagner GJ, Pantalone DW, Klein DJ. Discrimination and Hate Crimes in the Context of Neighborhood Poverty and Stressors Among HIV-Positive African-American Men Who Have Sex with Men. J Community Health. 2015 Dec 22; doi: 10.1007/s10900-015-0132-z. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Marcus U, Hickson F, Weatherburn P, Furegato M, Breveglieri M, Berg RC, Schmidt AJ EMIS network. Antiretroviral therapy and reasons for not taking it among men having sex with men (MSM)--results from the European MSM Internet Survey (EMIS) PLoS One. 2015 Mar 20;10(3):e0121047. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121047. eCollection 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Daniel H, Butkus R Health and Public Policy Committee of the American College of Physicians. Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Transgender Health Disparities: Executive summary of a policy position paper from the American College of Physicians. Ann Intern Med. doi: 10.7326/M14-2482. Published online 12 May 2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Schneider JA, Cornwell B, Ostrow D, Michaels S, Schumm P, Laumann EO, Friedman SR. Network mixing and network influences most linked to HIV infection and risk behavior in a Black men who have sex with men HIV epidemic. American Journal of Public Health. 2012 Nov 15; doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.301003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Schneider JA, Michaels S, Bouris A. Family network proportion and HIV risk among Black men who have sex with men. JAIDS. 2012 Sep 22; doi: 10.1097/QAI.0b013e318270d3cb. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Drucker P. Warped: Gay Normality and Queer Anti-Capitalism. Chicago: Haymarket Books; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kail BL, Acosta KL, Wright ER. State-level marriage equality and the health of same-sex couples. Am J Public Health. 2015 Jun;105(6):1101–5. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2015.302589. Epub 2015 Apr 16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Gayman M, Barragan J. Multiple perceived reasons for major discrimination and depression. Soc Ment Health. 2013;3:203–20. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Mays VM, Cochran SD. Mental health correlates of perceived discrimination among lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults in the United States. Am J Public Health. 2001;91:1869–76. doi: 10.2105/ajph.91.11.1869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Seng JS, Lopez WD, Sperlich M, Hamama L, Reed Meldrum CD. Marginalized identities, discrimination burden, and mental health: empirical exploration of an interpersonal-level approach to modeling intersectionality. Soc Sci Med. 2012;75:2437–45. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Smedley BD, Stith SY, Nelson AR, editors. Unequal treatment: Confronting racial and ethnic disparities in health care. Washington, D.C: National Academies Press. Institute of Medicine; 2002. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Davis DA, McGlotten S, editors. Black genders and sexualities. Palgrave Macmillan; 2012. Dec 24, [Google Scholar]
- 49.The People Living with HIV Stigma Index. http://www.stigmaindex.org/