Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2017 Apr-Jun;31(2):135–140. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000174

Willingness to be a brain donor: A survey of research volunteers from four racial/ethnic groups

Linda Boise a,*, Ladson Hinton b, Howard J Rosen c, Mary Ruhl a, Hiroko Dodge a, Nora Mattek a, Marilyn Albert d, Andrea Denny e, Joshua D Grill f,1, Travonia Hughes g, Jennifer H Lingler h, Darby Morhardt i, Francine Parfitt j, Susan Peterson-Hazan k, Viorela Pop l, Tara Rose m, Raj C Shah n
PMCID: PMC5403614  NIHMSID: NIHMS814955  PMID: 27779492

Abstract

Introduction

Racial and ethnic groups are under-represented among research subjects who assent to brain donation in Alzheimer’s disease research studies. There has been little research on this important topic. While there are some studies that have investigated the barriers to brain donation among African American study volunteers, there is no known research on the factors that influence whether or not Asians or Latinos are willing to donate their brains for research.

Methods

African American, Caucasian, Asian, and Latino research volunteers were surveyed at fifteen Alzheimer’s Disease Centers to identify predictors of willingness to assent to brain donation.

Results

Positive predictors included older age, Latino ethnicity, understanding of how the brain is used by researchers, and understanding of what participants need to do to ensure that their brain will be donated. Negative predictors included African/African American race, belief that the body should remain whole at burial, and concern that researchers might not be respectful of the body during autopsy.

Discussion

The predictive factors identified in this study may be useful for researchers seeking to increase participation of diverse ethnic groups in brain donation.

Keywords: brain, Alzheimer’s, research, autopsy, subjects, ethnicity

1. Introduction

Postmortem study of the human brain is a critical component of longitudinal research on neurodegenerative conditions such as Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias. For patients and research subjects who have been followed over an extended period while living, brain autopsy provides valuable data for studying underlying causes of dementia, for establishing and verifying prevalence estimates of clinically diagnosed disease, and for uncovering risk factors or associations with disease states.1 Despite its importance, only a portion of the brains of study volunteers in Alzheimer’s disease research programs come to autopsy when subjects die. Data from the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC), the repository for data for NIH-funded Alzheimer’s Research Centers, showed that for the 4552 recorded deaths among the 32 research centers that participated in the Uniform Data System (UDS) of the NACC between September 2005 and June 2013, only 62.8% percent had autopsies completed.2 The proportion of autopsies for non-white subjects has been even lower3, thus reducing the opportunities to study the unique neuropathological aspects of brain function among ethnic groups.

Since the 1990’s, researchers have investigated factors associated with brain donation for volunteers in Alzheimer’s disease research programs. Other studies, mostly involving African Americans have identified concerns about the effects of brain removal on the physical appearance of the body4,5, religious beliefs5,6, attitudes of family members4,6, and the lack of understanding about the rationale for brain autopsy4,6 as barriers to assenting to brain donation. While no prior studies have examined attitudes among Hispanic or Asian Americans towards brain donation, studies of attitudes towards research in general suggest that culturally-shaped values and beliefs may influence willingness to participate in Alzheimer’s research or to be a brain donor. For example, in one study Chinese Americans expressed concerns that participating in research might be harmful for a cognitively impaired person.7 It has also been reported that in Chinese culture, it is believed that when a person dies, one’s body should remain whole, presumably creating a barrier to brain donation.8 Studies have also found that Asians and Latinos often hold family-centered values (“familismo”) around medical decision-making.911 The dispersal of decision-making among multiple family members may hinder a study volunteer’s consideration about donating his or her brain.

The aim of this study is to identify predictors of willingness to assent to brain donation for research volunteers from four racial/ethnic groups, African American, Caucasian, Asian, and Latino. The survey was carried out in fifteen NIA-funded Alzheimer’s Disease Centers (ADCs). We hypothesized from our prior qualitative research12 and from a review of the literature that factors in four domains would predict a research subject’s willingness to assent to brain donation. These factors include the subject’s personal characteristics; religious beliefs and practices; family attitudes and support for brain donation; and experience, knowledge and concerns of study volunteers about participating in research.

2. Methods

2.1. Sample

The sample for this study included non-demented research volunteers in one or more studies at fifteen NIH-funded Alzheimer’s Disease Centers (ADCs). Only subjects participating in studies that did not require assent to brain donation as a condition of participation were surveyed. Participants were recruited by each collaborating center without regard to whether they had or had not signed assent to serve as a brain donor. A variety of recruitment methods were used in order to capture the greatest number of respondents. These included having the volunteer complete the survey in writing as part of their regular research assessment visit, mailing the survey to eligible research volunteers with phone follow-up as needed, and completing the survey through face-to-face interviews. By design, minorities were oversampled. The recruitment and implementation process was approved by the OHSU Institutional Review Board and the Institutional Review Board at the institution where subjects were recruited.

2.2 Survey development

The survey developed for this study includes questions on a range of factors developed through a multi-step process by co-investigators and research staff of four collaborating Alzheimer’s Disease Centers. It was designed to address the themes and concerns identified from focus groups conducted with African American, Chinese, Latino and White donor and non-donor research subjects and their family members from these centers to explore their beliefs and attitudes about brain donation.12 The themes identified in the focus groups included: family participation in decision making and their support for brain donation; religious practice, beliefs, and funeral arrangements; and factors related to study volunteers’ experience, knowledge and concerns about participating in research and brain donation.

Factors related to family included the family member research subjects would most likely discuss brain donation with and whether that person was supportive, whether family members would have difficulty carrying out their wishes for brain donation (if they agreed to brain donation), whether the respondent believes donating one’s brain is mostly an individual or family decision. Religious characteristics included primary religious affiliation, frequency of attendance at religious services, and the extent to which religion influenced the respondent’s thinking about brain donation. To measure respondents’ religiosity, they were asked to locate themselves on a continuum from 1, “strongly agree”, to 7, “strongly disagree” (reverse coded for analysis) on the following statement, “I consider myself to be a religious person.” The same response options were used to rate their spirituality. Questions were also asked about beliefs and plans related to funeral arrangements, including whether the respondent had made arrangements for burial or cremation; whether they expected to have an open or closed casket, a memorial service only, or no service at all; and if they were concerned that a brain autopsy might affect the appearance of their body.

Respondents’ experience, knowledge and concerns about participating in Alzheimer’s disease research were assessed using a series of questions that asked whether the respondent was concerned that researchers would not be respectful of their body, the extent to which they or their family members trust medical researchers, whether it would be helpful to have a clerical representative come to the center to discuss brain donation, and whether having more information about brain donation would be helpful for making a decision about brain donation. To assess understanding about the rationale for and process of brain donation, respondents were asked five questions (“yes” or “no”) of whether or not they understood “…how study of the brain is important for research on Alzheimer’s”, “…what researchers do with the brain”, “…how the researchers will remove the brain”, “…what one needs to do ahead of time to make sure the brain is donated after a person dies” and “…the difference between brain donation for research and donation of other organs for patients in need.”

The survey also gathered data on personal characteristics including age, gender, preferred language (English, Spanish, Chinese or other), country of birth, living arrangement, household finances (“have enough financial resources to do the things we want to do”, “mostly have enough financial resources to do the things we want to do” and “barely have enough money to take care of our needs”) and race/ethnicity (Latino, Hispanic; Chinese or other Asian; African or African American; Non-Hispanic White). An additional data collection form was created to collect information from the center’s data base where the respondent was a research subject. These data included years of education, cognitive status (normal, mild cognitive impairment (MCI), or other non-dementia condition affecting cognition), Clinical Dementia Rating13 score (0 or 0.5), and whether the subject had signed assent to brain donation.

The dependent variable used for analysis was the following question asked in the survey: “Whether or not you have been asked, how likely are you to donate your brain for research when you die.” Respondents were asked to pick one of five options: “Definitely not likely to donate my brain”, “Probably will not donate my brain”, “possibly will donate my brain,” “probably will donate my brain”, and “definitely will or already have agreed to donate my brain.”

The survey questions were written, reviewed, and refined by the study co-investigators and their research assistants and associates. The survey draft was reviewed by an expert in multicultural survey development (C. Nicolaidis, MD, January & March 2012) who advised the team on the organization, response options, and wording of survey questions to improve their appropriateness for persons from diverse ethnic groups. The survey was then pretested with four research subjects (two African American and two white), two Latino research assistants, and a Chinese research assistant. The survey was then refined to ensure language appropriateness and clarity, and the number of questions was reduced to minimize the time required for completion.

After the survey was finalized in English, it was translated into Mandarin Chinese and Spanish with consultation from C. Nicolaidis (March 2012) on best practices for translation. The Chinese version of the survey was translated by a professional translator who worked in a community service organization serving Chinese immigrants and back-translated to identify semantic discrepancies between the translated and the original English versions. Discrepancies were then reconciled. The Spanish version of the survey was translated by a native Spanish speaker from a professional translation service. Education level and cultural origin were expected to be varied for this group, so the translator used language that would be suited to a broad audience. Back translation was provided by a second professional translator at the same translation service. Discrepancies between the English survey and the back translated version were reviewed and reconciled by a Spanish instructor at the university of this study’s principal investigator. The survey was also reviewed by bilingual research staff of the collaborating center for language appropriateness for the population from which the Latino study subjects was drawn.

2.3. Analysis

Descriptive data (means and standard deviations and frequencies) are provided for personal characteristics. Chi-square and t-tests were used to compare those likely to donate their brain with those not likely to donate. To define “willingness to donate one’s brain”, a dichotomized dependent variable defined donors (scored 1) as those who checked that they “definitely will or already have agreed to donate my brain” or “probably will donate my brain” and non-donors (scored 0) as those who responded that they “possibly will donate my brain”, “probably will not donate my brain”, and “definitely will not donate my brain.” The factors considered in bivariate analysis are grouped into four categories: a) personal characteristics, b) family communication, agreement and support, c) religion, funeral arrangements, and beliefs related to handling of the body at death, and d) experience with research including length of study participation, preferences for discussing brain donation with researchers or staff, and knowledge and concerns about brain donation. Most of the independent variables were dichotomized into binary variables. Student’s t-tests or Wilcoxon Rank Sum Test were used as appropriate to compare ordinal and continuous variables with multiple response options.

Significant variables in the bivariate analysis were then used in logistic regression to evaluate five models for predicting willingness to donate one’s brain for research: Model 1) baseline personal characteristics including ethnic group measured as dichotomous variables with white as the reference, Model 2) baseline variables plus variables representing religious factors, Model 3) baseline variables plus factors related to family involvement in decision-making about brain donation, and Model 4) baseline variables plus factors related to the respondents’ experience in, attitudes, and understanding about brain donation. The final model included baseline variables and those that were significant in Models 2 through 4. All models showed adequate fit using Hosmer & Lemeshow goodness-of-fit test statistic. Analyses were performed using SAS software 9.3®.

3. Results

Data from 479 respondents were included in the analysis. Of these, 185 were non-Hispanic whites, 169 were African Americans, 50 were Asian (all but two were Chinese), and 61 were Hispanic. Thirty-one (51%) of the Hispanic respondents completed the survey in Spanish and 16 (32%) of the Asian participants completed the survey in Chinese. Other personal characteristics are reported in Table 1. Most of the survey respondents were likely brain donors: 49% reported that they definitely would donate their brains for research when they die or already had agreed to do so. An additional 14% reported that they “probably will” donate their brain bringing the “donor” group to 63%. Thirty-seven percent were classified as non-donors. This group included 19% of the sample who reported that they “possibly will” donate their brain, 10% reported that they “probably will not”, and the remainder, 8%, reported that they definitely were not likely to donate their brain.

Table 1.

Demographic and clinical characteristics of survey participants (n=479). Data from continuous variables is presented as mean ± standard deviation and data from categorical variables is presented as number (%).

Variable Mean ± SD or Number (%)
Age (yrs) 74.7 ± 8.8
Female 323 (69%)
Education (yrs) 15.2 ± 3.6
Race/Ethnicity (n=465)
  Non-Hispanic white 185 (40%)
  African American 169 (36%)
  Latino, Hispanic 61 (13%)
  Chinese, Asiana 50 (11%)
Duration of follow-up (yrs.) 6.2 ± 5.1
Cognitively Impaired (MCI) 134 (29%)
Living Alone 173 (37%)
Religious affiliation (n=464)
  Christian - Catholic 92 (20%)
  Christian - Protestant 221 (48%)
  Christian - Unknown 42 (6%)
  Jewish 29 (7%)
  Other Religion 7 (2%)
  No Affiliation 73 (16%)
Household finances: Have enough
resources to do the things we want to do
231 (50%)
a

All but two Asian respondents were Chinese

3.1. Comparison of donors and non-donors

Table 2 reports the bivariate analyses comparing donors and non-donors. Most of the factors analyzed were significantly different for donors and non-donors. Of particular note are the differences among the four ethnic groups, especially with respect to white and African/African American in their receptivity to brain donation. Religious affiliation and factors related to funeral plans were also highly significant. The family’s involvement in and supportiveness of brain donation were also strongly significant as were respondents’ understanding about and concerns related to the research purpose and process of brain donation.

Table 2.

Likelihood to donate brain according to participant characteristics and survey responses. Continuous variables are compared using t-tests and categorical variables using chi-square tests.

Not likely to donate
brain (n=178)
Likely to donate
brain (n=301)
p-value

Variables Mean ± SD or
Number (%)
Mean (SD) or
Number (%)
Participant characteristics
Age, yrs (Range: 48 – 99) 73.3 ± 8.9 75.5 ± 8.6 <0.01**
Female 129 (75%) 194 (66%) 0.047*
Education (yrs) 15.0 ± 3.4 15.3 ± 3.7 0.37
Living Alone 78 (45%) 95 (32%) <0.01**
Household finances (Enough
resources)
76 (45%) 155 (53%) 0.08
Cognitive impairment 53 (30%) 81 (28%) 0.59
Race/ethnicity
  White 41 (24%) 144 (49%) <0.0001***
  Black 96 (55%) 73 (25%)
  Hispanic 14 (8%) 47 (16%)
  Asian 22 (13%) 28 (10%)
Family
Most likely to discuss with spouse 53 (30%) 153 (51%) <0.0001***
Most likely to discuss with child 86 (48%) 117 (39%) 0.04*
Person most likely to talk to is
supportive of decision
45 (30%) 213 (75%) <0.0001***
Donation is mostly a personal
decision vs. family decision
132 (76%) 262 (88%) <0.001**
Family will not have difficulty
carrying out your wishes after death
44 (34%) 224 (77%) <0.0001***
Family members trusts medical
researchers
65 (38%) 150 (52%) <0.01**
Religion
Religious affiliation
  Christian- Catholic 22 (13%) 70 (24%) <0.0001***
  Christian - Protestant 115 (66%) 148 (51%)
  Jewish 14 (8%) 15 (5%)
  Other Religion 6 (3%) 1 (<1%)
  No Affiliation 16 (9%) 57 (20%)
Church attendance
at least weekly
91 (58%) 116 (49%) 0.09
Religiosity (Scale 1 – 7) 5.3 ± 2.1 4.3 ± 2.5 <0.0001***
Spirituality (Scale 1 – 7) 5.7 ± 1.9 5.3 ± 2.1 0.04*
Arranged for cremation vs. not 32 (19%) 103 (36%) <0.001**
Body should remain whole 58 (33%) 18 (6%) <0.0001***
Plans for open casket vs. none 62 (35%) 49 (16%) <0.0001***
Research experience, knowledge
& concerns
Duration of follow-up, yrs 5.1 ± 4.3 6.9 ± 5.4 <0.0001***
Asked by staff to donate brain 105 (63%) 254 (87%) <0.0001***
Heard a presentation on brain
donation
28 (16%) 83 (29%) <0.01**
Primary reason for research
participation is to help future
generations
95 (55%) 210 (70%) <0.01**
Prefer a researcher/doctor ask me
about donating vs. an RA
97 (85%) 192 (83%) 0.70
It makes a difference who asks me
about donating
49 (30%) 54 (21%) 0.03*
Do you understand…
  ‥how study of brain is important
    for research on AD?
145 (88%) 273 (93%) 0.14
  ‥what researchers will do with
    brain?
50 (31%) 143 (55%) <0.0001***
  ‥how researchers will remove
    brain?
37 (22%) 98 (35%) <0.01**
  ‥what one needs to do ahead of
    time?
57 (35%) 185 (65%) <0.0001***
  ‥difference between brain
    donation for research and
    donation of other organs for
    patients in need?
66 (42%) 191 (68%) <0.0001***
More info would be helpful 84 (49%) 93 (32%) <0.001**
Inviting clergy go talk about brain
donation would be helpful
21 (12%) 32 (11%) 0.71
Concern about body's appearance
after autopsy
42 (25%) 16 (5%) <0.0001***
Concern researchers will not be
respectful
70 (41%) 41 (14%) <0.0001***
Trust medical researchers 125 (71%) 259 (88%) <0.0001***

3.2. Multivariate analyses

Variables included in the logistic regression models (Table 3) were selected from among significant variables in the bivariate analyses that best represented the model-specific domains. The baseline model, including racial/ethnic group, is reported in Model 1. Older age was a significant predictor of willingness to donate. Being African or African American (compared with being white), Asian, and living alone were negative predictors of willingness to donate. In Model 2, we fit the baseline model and religion variables. Significant variables included plans for cremation, which was a positive predictor of willingness to donate one’s brain, and belief that the body should remain whole, which was a negative predictor. Of the family-related variables considered in Model 3, discussing brain donation with a spouse was not significant but having a supportive confidant regardless of the relationship was a strong predictor of willingness to donate one’s brain. Also, belief that brain donation is a personal rather than a family decision was a predictor of willingness to donate one’s brain. Of the research-related variables om Model 4, a significant negative factor was concern that researchers would not be respectful. Significant positive factors were reported understanding of what researchers will do with the brain and what to do ahead of time to make sure the brain is donated after they die. A desire to have more information about brain donation was not a predictor of willingness to donate.

Table 3.

Multivariate logistic regression models relating predictors to willingness to donate brain. Results are presented as odds ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI).

Model 1
Demographics
only
Model 2
Demographics
+ Religion
Model 3
Demographics
+ Family
Model 4
Demographics +
Research
knowledge
& concerns
Model 5
Demographics
+ significant
covariates
Variables OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95%
CI)
OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)
Demographics
Age, yrs 1.03 (1.01 –
1.06)
1.06 (1.02 –
1.10)
1.03 (1.00 –
1.07)
1.03 (0.99 – 1.07) 1.06 (1.02 –
1.11)
Female vs.
Male
0.98 (0.61 –
1.59)
0.87 (0.44 –
1.74)
0.66 (0.35 –
1.25)
0.70 (0.38 – 1.30) 0.50 (0.23 –
1.06)
Living alone vs.
not alone
0.62 (0.40 –
0.98)
0.53 (0.29 –
0.99)
0.80 (0.41 –
1.56)
0.56 (0.31 – 0.98) 0.51 (0.25 –
1.04)
Race/ethnicity
  White reference reference reference reference Reference
  Black 0.25 (0.15 –
0.40)
0.28 (0.14 –
0.56)
0.34 (0.18 –
0.62)
0.41 (0.21 – 0.80) 0.41 (0.19 –
0.85)
  Hispanic 0.96 (0.48 –
1.94)
1.01 (0.36 –
2.84)
1.87 (0.73 –
4.83)
1.73 (0.75 – 4.01) 3.38 (1.12 –
10.21)
  Asian 0.39 (0.20 –
0.77)
0.68 (0.25 –
1.82)
0.60 (0.26 –
1.40)
0.85 (0.34 – 2.09) 0.77 (0.29 –
2.05)
Religion
  Catholic
Religion
  vs. other
-- 1.68 (0.73 –
3.91)
-- -- --
  Arranged for
cremation vs.
not
-- 2.32 (1.13 –
4.73)
-- -- 1.53 (0.73 –
3.21)
  Body should
remain whole
-- 0.12 (0.06 –
0.27)
-- -- 0.24 (0.10 –
0.60)
  Plans for
open casket vs.
none
-- 1.06 (0.54 –
2.07)
-- -- --
Religiosity -- 0.91 (0.80 –
1.03)
-- -- --
Family
Most likely to
discuss with
spouse vs. not
-- -- 1.23 (0.60 –
2.52)
-- --
Supportive
person
vs. no
-- -- 6.60 (3.83 –
11.35)
-- 5.70 (3.00 –
10.85)
Donation is
mostly a
personal
decision
vs. family
decision
-- -- 1.90 (1.04 –
3.50)
-- 1.05 (0.48 –
2.26)
Family trusts
medical
researchers vs.
no
-- -- 0.91 (0.53 –
1.56)
-- --
Research
knowledge &
concerns
Years followed
at ADC
-- -- -- 1.04 (0.98 – 1.11) --
Primary reason
for participating
in research:
To help future
generations
-- -- -- 1.68 (0.99 – 2.85) --
Concern about
body's
appearance
-- -- -- 0.48 (0.20 – 1.13) --
Concern
researchers will
not be
respectful
-- -- -- 0.36 (0.19 – 0.70) 0.27 (0.14 –
0.54)
Trust medical
researchers
-- -- -- 1.52 (0.79 – 2.92) --
Understand
what
researchers will
do with brain
-- -- -- 2.01 (1.12 – 3.62) 2.20 (1.14 –
4.24)
Understand
what to do
ahead of time
-- -- -- 2.22 (1.25 – 3.96) 2.19 (1.13 –
4.24)
More info
would be
helpful
-- -- -- 0.94 (0.55 – 1.62) --

We entered into the final model all baseline variables, including race/ethnicity, and those variables in each of the four additional hypothesized domains that were significant predictors in the prior models. As with all prior models, African American ethnicity was a significant negative predictor of willingness to donate one’s brain. Although not significant in the four prior models, Latino ethnicity was a positive predictor of willingness to donate in the final model. Older age remained a significant predictor. In the religion domain, the only significant variable was the belief that the body should remain whole which was a negative predictor. For the family related variables, having a supportive confidant was a strong predictor of willingness to donate. Concern that researchers will not be respectful was a strong negative predictor of willingness to donate while understanding about what researchers will do with the brain and what the subject needs to do ahead of time were positive predictors of willingness to donate one’s brain.

4. Discussion

This study fills a critical gap in understanding of factors that influence research subjects from diverse ethnic groups to assent to brain donation. Our findings support our hypotheses that factors from four domains predict willingness to be a brain donor. However, while many of the factors investigated were significantly associated with willingness to donate one’s brain in the bivariate analysis, when entered into multivariate logistic regression, the number of factors that remained significant was reduced. In the final model, significant positive predictors were age, Latino ethnicity, understanding of how the brain is used by researchers and what subjects need to do to ensure that their brain will be donated. The relationship between older age and willingness to donate one’s brain may be due to the association of age with the length of time respondents had participated in research. This is suggested by the non-significance of age in Model 4 which included years followed, allowing for shared variance between these two variables. An alternative interpretation is that older respondents may have had the opportunity for greater reflection about the ending of their lives, may be more comfortable considering the ultimate disposition of their bodies, and/or may be more decisive about and open to brain donation. The first interpretation (along with the correlation of age and length of duration as a study volunteer) is supported by Schnieders et al.14 who found that longer term enrollment as a study volunteers was associated with greater likelihood of agreeing to brain donation. The reason behind the significance of Latino ethnicity as a predictor of willingness to donate one’s brain is unclear, especially since this relationship was not significant in the baseline model or the subsequent domain-specific models. One possible explanation is suggested by a study of Hispanics and African Americans living in Los Angeles, which found that health and healthcare information were important motivators for research participation for Hispanics.15 Whether the Centers in this study offered unique education and/or informational programs for their Hispanic research volunteers in ways that gained their openness to brain donation is unclear but worthy of further exploration. Information and education about the essential role of brain autopsy in longitudinal Alzheimer’s research may well support the goal of increasing the number of brain donors especially for Latinos but for other ethnic groups as well.

Negative predictors were African/African American ethnicity, belief that the body should remain whole, and concern that researchers might not be respectful of the body when performing autopsy. Of particular note is the significance of African/African American ethnicity as a predictor of unwillingness to donate one’s brain in all five regression models. This finding is consistent with prior research that has found that African Americans often express high degrees of mistrust in researchers6,16,17 and are acutely aware of the unethical treatment afforded members of their race in past research.18 In contrast to our findings for African/African Americans, we found an unstable relationship between Asian ethnicity and willingness to donate one’s brain. Although being Asian was associated with being a non-donor in the baseline logistic regression (Model 1), this relationship was not sustained when variables in the hypothesized domains were included in the models nor in the final model. This suggests that factors related to religious beliefs, family dynamics, and research knowledge and concerns moderate the relationship between being Asian and willingness to be a brain donor. This moderating effect may be related to our findings in prior qualitative research in which we found that Chinese research volunteers and their family members often expressed the desire for the body to remain whole after one dies and the high value the Chinese participants placed on family decision-making around brain donation.14 In contrast to our findings with respect to Asian ethnicity, we found that Hispanic ethnicity was only associated with willingness to be a brain donor in the final model and this association was positive. Further research is needed to more fully explicate the factors that influence the receptivity of these and other ethnic groups to brain donation.

Our findings provide clues to strategies for increasing brain donation of enrolled or potential study volunteers. While some factors, such as the belief that one’s body remain whole may not be feasible and/or ethically appropriate to attempt to modify, other factors such as uncomfortable attitudes of close family members and concerns that researchers might not be respectful may be amenable to change through education and other communications. Providing opportunities to learn about the research being conducted and to meet researchers in informal settings may be valuable strategies for improving autopsy rates.

There are several important limitations to this study. First, the respondents represented subjects already participating in clinical Alzheimer’s disease research; thus the results cannot be generalized to the larger community of potential research volunteers. Second, respondents in this study were not drawn from a random sample. Bias may have been introduced in several ways: first, recruitment processes of subjects varied with some centers inviting completion of the survey when they came into the clinic for a regularly scheduled study visit and others mailing the surveys to eligible research volunteers; second, we asked some centers to recruit only from specific ethnic groups in order to increase the number of respondents from these groups; third, eligibility criteria for studies within centers varied, introducing further potential for bias. Thus, it was not possible to determine a response rate for survey respondents. Additionally, willingness to complete the survey may have been biased towards subjects who held more or less positive views on brain donation. Despite its limitations, this study offers valuable insights into the factors that influence research volunteers to agree or not to agree to brain donation particularly among Asian, Latino and Caucasian subjects for which there has been little prior research. Further exploration will increase our understanding of how these factors differentially influence study volunteers and may yield ideas for more effective approaches to working with dementia research volunteers from diverse racial and ethnic groups.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the national Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center as a NACC Collaborative Project including Oregon Health & Science University; Boston University; University of Californian, Davis; and University of California, San Francisco. The dissemination phase of this study could not have been carried without the dedicated support and cooperation of faculty and staff of the fifteen participating Alzheimer’s Disease Centers, including Education Core leaders, Clinical Core leaders, research assistants, and others. We especially want to acknowledge the dedication and computer savvy of Cindy Tran, MPH, research assistant at University of California, Davis, and Allen (Lee) Kwok and Marian Tse at University of California, San Francisco, for their participation in the research team and assistance in reviewing and refining the survey drafts and recruiting subjects. We also would like to thank Dr. Christina Nicolaidis, professor and senior scholar in social determinants of health, Portland State University, and Department of Family Medicine, OHSU, for her insightful guidance on survey design aimed at gathering data from diverse groups and in the survey translation process.

The NACC database is funded by NIA/NIH Grant U01 AG016976. NACC data are contributed by the NIA funded ADCs: P30 AG019610 (PI Eric Reiman, MD), P30 AG013846 (PI Neil Kowall, MD), P50 AG008702 (PI Scott Small, MD), P50 AG025688 (PI Allan Levey, MD, PhD), P50 AG047266 (PI Todd Golde, MD, PhD), P30 AG010133 (PI Andrew Saykin, PsyD), P50 AG005146 (PI Marilyn Albert, PhD), P50 AG005134 (PI Bradley Hyman, MD, PhD), P50 AG016574 (PI Ronald Petersen, MD, PhD), P50 AG005138 (PI Mary Sano, PhD), P30 AG008051 (PI Steven Ferris, PhD), P30 AG013854 (PI M. Marsel Mesulam, MD), P30 AG008017 (PI Jeffrey Kaye, MD), P30 AG010161 (PI David Bennett, MD), P50 AG047366 (PI Victor Henderson, MD, MS), P30 AG010129 (PI Charles DeCarli, MD), P50 AG016573 (PI Frank LaFerla, PhD), P50 AG016570 (PI Marie-Francoise Chesselet, MD, PhD), P50 AG005131 (PI Douglas Galasko, MD), P50 AG023501 (PI Bruce Miller, MD), P30 AG035982 (PI Russell Swerdlow, MD), P30 AG028383 (PI Linda Van Eldik, PhD), P30 AG010124 (PI John Trojanowski, MD, PhD), P50 AG005133 (PI Oscar Lopez, MD), P50 AG005142 (PI Helena Chui, MD), P30 AG012300 (PI Roger Rosenberg, MD), P50 AG005136 (PI Thomas Montine, MD, PhD), P50 AG033514 (PI Sanjay Asthana, MD, FRCP), P50 AG005681 (PI John Morris, MD), and P50 AG047270 (PI Stephen Strittmatter, MD, PhD).

Footnotes

Competing financial interests: The authors declare no competing financial interests.

References

  • 1.Kaye JA, Dame A, Lehman S, Sexton G. Factors associated with brain donation among optimally health elderly people. Jl Ger: Med Sci. 1999;54A:M1–M5. doi: 10.1093/gerona/54.11.m560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center, report on Autopsy performance. 2013 Jul [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Fillenbaum GG, Huber MS, Beekley D, Henderson VW, Mortimer J, Morris JC, Harrell LE. The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1996;46(1):142–145. doi: 10.1212/wnl.46.1.142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bonner GJ, Darkwa OK, Gorelick PB. Autopsy Recruitment Programs for African Americans. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2000;14(4):202–208. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200010000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Connell CM, Avey H, Holmes SB. Attitudes about autopsy: Implications for educational interventions. Gerontologist. 1994;34(5):665–673. doi: 10.1093/geront/34.5.665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Jefferson A, Lambe S, Cook E, Pimontel M, Palmisano J, Chaisson C. Factors associated with African American and White Elders’ participation in a brain donation program. Alz Dis & Assoc Dis. 2011;25(1):11–16. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181f3e059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hinton L, Guo Z, Hillygus J, Levkoff S. Working with culture: A qualitative analysis of barriers to the recruitment of Chinese-American family caregivers for dementia research. Jl of Cross-cultural Gerontology. 2000;15:119–137. doi: 10.1023/a:1006798316654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Barry C. Cultural taboos surrounding organ donation. 2013 Message posted to http://www.livercancergenomics.com/cultural-taboos-surround-organ-donation/ [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Blackhall LJ, Murphy ST, Frank G, Michel V, Azen S. Ethnicity and attitudes toward patient autonomy. 1995;274:820–825. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gallagher D, Solano N, Coon D, et al. Recruitment and retention of Latino dementia family caregivers in intervention research: Issues to face, lessons to learn. Gerontologist. 2003;43(1):45–51. doi: 10.1093/geront/43.1.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Brugge D, Kole A, Lu W, et al. Susceptibility of elder Asian immigrants to persuasion with respect to participation in research. Jl Immigr Health. 2005;7(2):93–101. doi: 10.1007/s10903-005-2642-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Boise L, et al. Will my soul go to heaven if they take my brain? Beliefs and worries about brain donation. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnv683. (In press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): Current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43:2412–2414. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.11.2412-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Schnieders T, Danner DD, McQuire C, Reynolds F, Abner E. Incentives and barriers to research participation and brain donation among African Americans. American Jl of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias. 2013;28(5):485–490. doi: 10.1177/1533317513488922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Calderon JL, Baker RS, Fabrega H, et al. An ethno-medical perspective on research participation: A qualitative pilot study. Medscape General Medicine (CHECK RESOURCE FOR ABBREVIATIONS) 2006;8(2) PMCID: 1785211. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Braunstein JB, Sherber NS, Schulman SP, King EL, Powe NR. Race, medical researcher distrust, perceived harm, and willingness to participate in cardiovascular prevention trials. Medicine. 2008;87(1) doi: 10.1097/MD.0b013e3181625d78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lambe, Cantwell, Islam, Horvath, Jefferson initials? Perceptions, Knowledge, Incentives, and Barriers of brain donation among African American elders enrolled in and Alzheimer’s Research Program. Gerontologist. 2011;51(1):28–38. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnq063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Katz RV, Kegeles SS, Kressin NR, et al. Awareness of the Tuskegee Syphilis study and the US Presidential apology and their influence on minority participation in biomedical research. AJPH. 2008;98(6):1137–1142. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.100131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES