Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2017 Apr 25;12(4):e0175737. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0175737

Chronic pain and sex-differences; women accept and move, while men feel blue

Graciela S Rovner 1,2,3, Katharina S Sunnerhagen 1,*, Ann Björkdahl 1,4, Björn Gerdle 5, Björn Börsbo 6,7, Fredrik Johansson 8, David Gillanders 9
Editor: JianJun Yang10
PMCID: PMC5404887  PMID: 28441403

Abstract

Purpose

The aim of this study is to explore differences between male and female patients entering a rehabilitation program at a pain clinic in order to gain a greater understanding of different approaches to be used in rehabilitation.

Method

1371 patients referred to a specialty pain rehabilitation clinic, completed sociodemographic and pain related questionnaires. They rated their pain acceptance (CPAQ-8), their kinesiophobia (TSK), the impact of pain in their life (MPI), anxiety and depression levels (HAD) and quality of life scales: the SF-36, LiSat-11, and the EQ-5D. Because of the large sample size of the study, the significance level was set at the p ≤.01.

Results

Analysis by t-test showed that when both sexes experience the same pain severity, women report significantly higher activity level, pain acceptance and social support while men report higher kinesiophobia, mood disturbances and lower activity level.

Conclusion

Pain acceptance (CPAQ-8) and kinesiophobia (TSK) showed the clearest differences between men and women. Pain acceptance and kinesiophobia are behaviorally defined and have the potential to be changed.

Introduction

It is well known that the prevalence of chronic musculoskeletal pain is higher among women, both in community and treatment seeking cohorts [15]. In addition, response to treatment differs between sexes [610]. In this article, the term ‘sex’ is chosen only because in this study the data inform about the patients’ biological sex, not about their gender [1113]. Basic research in laboratory environments show that women are more sensitive to pain [1416], report higher pain intensity and more often report widespread pain [17, 18, 19]. However, despite a large volume of laboratory-based research in this area, a consistent pattern of sex differences in pain sensitivity, expression and impact has not yet emerged [20]. What it is known from clinical studies is that women use more analgesic medication [21] and are more sensitive to both dosage and type of medication [2224], but other than that, basic research has not been successfully linked to meaningful differences that can influence the design of pain rehabilitation packages [25].

An inconsistency is also found regarding what extent there are sex differences with respect to different psychological factors [20, 26]. For example, some studies report higher anxiety levels among men [27], while others report higher anxiety among women [9, 28]. Depression is twice as common in women as in men [29]. Men, on the other hand, report lower physical and psychological quality of life [30] and tend to score higher than women on specific aspects of anxiety, such as kinesiophobia (movement avoidance) [26].

Clinical and rehabilitation relevant measures are those sensitive to sexes’ potentially differential rehabilitation needs. These differences may also support decision making, to assess and allocate patients into different rehabilitation packages, potentially designed for the different sexes’ needs [31].

The aim of this study was to explore differences in baseline outcome variables between male and female patients referred to a cognitive behaviorally based rehabilitation program at a multidisciplinary pain center.

Materials and methods

Participants

The participants in this study were 1371 patients with chronic musculoskeletal non-malignant pain consecutively referred to the Pain and Rehabilitation Centre, University Hospital, Linköping, Sweden between 2009 and April 2011. The clinic specializes in multidisciplinary pain rehabilitation and pain assessments and as such taking part in the Swedish National Quality Registry of Pain Rehabilitation (SQRP).

The Swedish National Quality Registry of Pain Rehabilitation (SQRP)

To ensure quality of pain care services and in order to develop evidence-based methods in pain rehabilitation, the SQRP has been implemented in around 40 specialty clinics in Sweden. The registry is used both for quality control in health-care management and for clinical research. The data collected in SQRP can be divided into two sections: 1) Sociodemographic data and 2) psychometric instruments.

The instruments included in the SQRP are completed before the first assessment (pre-rehab), after rehabilitation (post-rehab) and at a 1-year follow up. This study used the pre-rehab data to explore sex differences.

Ethics

The questionnaires are normally sent by mail to the participants who then send them back by post before the first visit, together with their informed written consent. Ethical approval was obtained from the Regional Ethics Board in Gothenburg (815–12).

Sociodemographic data

The SQRP includes the following socio-demographic data: education, work status, sick leave or insurance situation.

Psychometric instruments

Pain characteristics

The following pain related data were gathered a) pain severity (from the MPI, read below under MPI), b) the duration of pain and duration of persistent pain in years; c) quantity of regions with pain (between 0 and 36) and d) location of worst pain, or the alternative to mark widespread pain.

Hospital Anxiety and Depression scale

(HAD; [32]). The HADS rates the severity of depression and anxiety symptoms separately in two self-administrated subscales, which include 7 items. The scale is designed for non-psychiatric hospital settings and excludes items that might reflect somatic complaints. Each item has four Likert’s responses ranging from 0 (no complaints) to 3, yielding a maximum score of 21 for each component. A score of <7 in a component is taken as a normal result; a score of 8–10 indicates mild symptoms and complaints; >10 indicates moderate, and >14 severe symptoms [3335]. The Swedish translation has shown acceptable psychometric properties [36]. In the current study, the internal consistency of the anxiety sub-scale was .77 and.76 for the depression subscale.

The short form 36 survey questionnaire for chronic pain

(SF-36; [37]) The SF-36 measures Health Related Quality of Life by assessing the ability of the patient to function, the impact of emotions on daily functioning, and the impact of pain on daily functioning. It includes 36 questions that yield an 8-scale profile of functional health and well-being scores: Physical Functioning (PF), Role Physical (RP), Bodily Pain (BP), General Health (GH), Vitality (V), Social Functioning (SF), Role Emotional (RE) and Mental Health (MH) as well as two psychometrically-derived summary clusters, the Physical Component and the Mental Component Summary (PCS and MCS) [38]. The internal consistency of the eight scales of the SF-36 in the Swedish norm is between .79 to .93, comparable to US and UK scales [39] and in this study the Alpha were between .70 to.87.

EuroQuol, quality of life measure

(EQ-5D; [40, 4143]) is a generic measure of health-related quality of life, covering five dimensions: mobility, self-care, usual activities, pain/discomfort, and anxiety/depression. In addition, there is a global self-rating of QoL according to a 100-point scale, a thermometer-like visual analogue scale with defined end points (high values indicate good health and low values indicate poor health). The EQ-5D has been well-validated across a number of countries, settings and conditions. It has good psychometric properties and the Cronbach alpha for this study was .58. The EuroQol allows the calculation of a total index score, with higher values indicating a better health-related QoL.

Life satisfaction questionnaire (LiSat-11)

[44, 45] consists of 11 items relating to satisfaction with life, activities and participation (Likert scale 1–6) relating to health status, the vocational and financial situation as well as relationships. High scores reflect greater level of life satisfaction [46].

Multidimensional Pain Inventory (MPI)

The MPI has been validated and translated into several languages. The Swedish version (MPI-S), includes 61 items, has good internal consistency, with a Cronbach’s alpha of between 0.6 and 0.9 [4750]. Alpha in the current study was .79. The original MPI has three sections and 12 scales. Part 1 contain five scales: Pain severity; Pain Interference; Perceived Life Control; Affective Distress; and Social Support. Part 2 assesses the perception of responses from significant others to displays of pain and suffering and consists of three scales: Punishing Responses; Solicitous Responses; and Distracting Responses. Part 3 measures the extent to which patients engage in various activities and these four scales are combined in a composite scale labeled general activity index (MPI-GAI). In the Swedish validation, some of the scales has been excluded [51, 52], but in the SQRP these are still included, thus also reported in this study.

The Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire 8-items (CPAQ-8)

[53] measures acceptance behaviours and attitudes towards pain. This short form was extracted from the long form (20-items) (CPAQ; [54, 55]). The CPAQ-8 has demonstrated good reliability and validity both in English and Swedish[5658]. Pain acceptance is operationalized with two main classes of behavior (subscales): 1) ‘Activity Engagement’ (‘AE’, score range: 0–24), which involves continuing to engage in personally valued activity despite the presence of pain and 2) ‘Pain Willingness’ (‘PW’, score range: 0–24), the capacity to be open to pain without struggling against it, or unproductive efforts to control it. This willingness and openness to discomfort is functional when it is in the service of living a personally valued life. The eight items are rated on a scale from 0 (never true) to 6 (always true). In the current study, the internal consistency of the AE sub-scale was .88 and .74 for the PW.

The Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia (TSK)

[59] measures pain-related fear of movement [60]. The items are rated on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from ‘‘strongly disagree” to ‘‘strongly agree.” The total score has a range from 17–68 where scores higher than 36 for women and 38 for men indicate high pain-related fear [61]. Male patients score somewhat higher than female patients [26]. The TSK has proven to be a reliable assessment tool for chronic pain [60, 62, 63]; the factor structure has been demonstrated to be stable across pain diagnoses and nationalities [64].

Statistics

All analyses and procedures were carried out in the statistical software package IBM SPSS 19.0.0.1 for Mac. Background data is presented descriptively with means, SDs, and comparisons between the sexes (Tables 1 and 2). Descriptive statistics (means and SDs) of the outcome variables, the psychometric instruments, were computed for the sample and were grouped according to sex. Each instrument was first reported using descriptive statistics, and then comparisons were carried out between the sexes for each instrument using Chi Squared and t-tests (two-tailed).

Table 1. Percentages and differences between men and women in respect to their sociodemographic data and presence of widespread pain.

Variable (N) All
(%)
Women
(%)
Men
(%)
Sig.
p
Born in Sweden (1368) 79.6 83.0 72.2 < .001*
University education (1307) 17.1 18.0 15.3 .219
Widespread pain1 (1283) 31.0 35.1 21.8 < .001*
Sickness benefit 100% (1368) 13.9 13.8 14.1 .866
Time-limited sickness benefit 100% (1368) 5.6 6.0 4.6 .310
Working/studying 100% (1331) 25.3 23.6 29.0 .035
Working/studying to some extent (1331) 37.3 39.4 32.9 .030

*p ≤ .001

1 the patients reported that their pain was not localized in one area but spread over several body regions.

Table 2. Differences between men and women regarding their age, their pain severity, duration, persistency and location as well as regarding their sick-leave-time.

Variable (N) All Women Men Sig. Effect
M SD M SD M SD p Size1
Age (1368) 47 15 45 15 51 14 < .001* -0.41
Pain Severity MPI (range 0–6) (1217) 4.50 1.04 4.54 0.99 4.43 1.14 .123 0.11
Pain duration (days) (1145) 2953 3319 2939 3046 2982 3831 .850 -0.01
Persistent pain duration (days) (936) 2425 3048 2304 2559 2677 3866 .127 -0.12
N:o of pain locations (0–36) (1317) 13 8 14 8 10 8 < .001* 0.50
Days since last job (574) 2337 3200 2396 3480 2216 2542 .530 0.06

*p ≤ .001

1 Effect Sizes Cohen’s d. A negative value indicates that women’s values are lower.

Because of the large sample size of the study, stringent p-values were chosen, to reduce the likelihood of Type 1 error. In this study, results that were significant at the p ≤.01 were considered significant, and at the p ≤. 001 were considered "highly" significant. Significances between .05 and .01 were seen as ‘tendencies’ that if relevant could be discussed. Cohen’s Effect Sizes were calculated and a d>.8 was considered as large (8/10 of a standard deviation unit), a d between .5 and .7, as moderate and between .2 and .4 as small [65]. Cases with missing data were excluded on a pairwise basis and the n of each analysis is stated in the results.

Results

Background descriptive is presented in Tables 1 and 2 and show some significant differences between sexes. Of the 1371 patients, 68.4% were women and 35% of them suffered from widespread pain while the majority of men (78%) suffered from localized pain χ2 (one, n = 1283) = 22.63, p = .000 (Table 1). The patients have had pain for an average of 8 years, and they suffered of continuous pain for an average of 6 years. However, the variation among the patients was larger than the means (SD = 9 and 8.3 respectively) showing the heterogeneity of the total group. As seen in Table 2, women reported a greater number of pain locations than men t (1316) = 8.63, p = < .0001, d = .50. The magnitude of the differences in these means was large (mean difference = 4.032, 99% CI: 2.82 to 5.24).

Women were significantly younger than men (Age M = 45 vs. men’s M = 51) t (1367) = -6.52, p < .001, d = .41 (Table 2) and more were born in Sweden (Table 1).

A quarter of participants were working full time and one third were receiving full sickness benefit. Most of the men that worked, were working full time χ2 (1, n = 1331) = 7.012, p = .030, while most women were working to some extent χ2 (1, n = 1331) = 6.684, p = .035 (Table 1) and 41% (n = 574) had been unemployed for, on average, 6 years (Table 2).

Mental Health was measured with HAD, EQ-5D and SF-36. The majority of men (42%) and women (46%) scored low levels of anxiety (HAD scored under 8). Almost the same amount of men (38%) and women (34%) scored between 11 and 21, which is considered indicative of a case of anxiety. The remaining 20% of both sexes scored borderline levels (scores of 8 to 10), showing no statistically significant difference in anxiety according to HAD. The same is true for depression, when almost 48% of women and 41% of men scored low levels, around 24% of both sexes scored borderline levels. More men than women (34% vs 30%) scored at a level of depression. No differences were in the single item from EQ-5D item about Anxiety and depression. However, according to the Mental Health subscale of SF-36, men scored significantly lower than women, indicating that men were experiencing more problem with their mental health than women t(1109) = 2.78, p = .007, d = .18 (mean difference = 4.08, 99% CI: 0.18 to 7.98) (Table 3).

Table 3. Mean and SD for the whole population and separately for women and men for mood (HAD), quality of life (EQ-5D, SF-36 and LiSat-11), pain impact (MPI), pain acceptance (CPAQ) and in kinesiophobia (TSK).

The effects sizes and significance (p) are shown for differences between men and women.

Sub-scale (min-max) (N) All Women Men Sig. Effect
M SD M SD M SD p Sizes1
HAD Anxiety (0–21) (1203) 8.73 5.06 8.62 5.01 8.99 5.16 .245 -0.07
Depression (0–21) (1208) 8.42 4.66 8.25 4.63 8.80 4.71 .056 -0.12
MPI Pain Severity (0–6) (1217) 4.50 1.04 4.54 0.99 4.43 1.14 .123 0.11
Pain interference (0–6) (1216) 4.36 1.15 4.41 1.14 4.25 1.35 .043 0.13
Life control (0–6) (1216) 2.62 1.24 2.64 1.21 2.56 1.31 .296 0.06
Affective distress (0–6) (1216) 3.40 1.44 3.43 1.41 335 1.49 .359 0.06
Social support (0–6) (1216) 4.18 1.46 4.20 1.40 4.15 1.60 .594 0.03
Punishing responses (0–6) (1213) 1.61 1.41 1.61 1.43 1.63 1.37 .806 -0.01
Solicitous responses (0–6) (1213) 2.59 1.68 2.64 1.64 2.50 1.77 .192 0.08
Distracting responses (0–6) (1213) 2.17 1.42 2.19 1.39 2.14 1.49 .544 0.04
General Activity Level (0–6) (1213) 2.29 0.94 2.36 0.92 2.16 1.00 < .001* 0.21
EQ-5D 1. Mobility (1–3) (1205) 1.66 0.50 1.66 0.50 1.66 0.50 .966 0.00
2. Self-care (1–3) (1211) 1.24 0.46 1.23 0.45 1.26 0.49 .199 -0.06
3. Usual activities (1–3) (1209) 1.97 0.68 1.97 0.65 1.97 0.72 .966 0.00
4. Pain/discomfort (1–3) (1213) 2.64 0.50 2.62 0.50 2.67 0.50 .105 -0.10
5. Anxiety/depression (1–3) (1222) 1.95 0.63 1.94 0.63 1.96 0.63 .565 -0.03
EQ-VAS- Health (0–100) (1219) 39.84 20.94 40.11 20.78 39.30 21.29 .539 0.04
EQ-5D Index (max 1) (1083) 0.25 0.32 0.26 0.32 0.22 0.32 .143 0.13
SF-36 Physical functioning (0–100) (1115) 48.74 24.42 48.38 23.78 49.47 25.68 .010* -0.04
Role-Physical (0–100) (1080) 15.65 29.03 15.05 28.49 16.90 30.11 .162 -0.06
Bodily Pain (0–100) (1114) 23.16 15.24 23.25 15.13 22.98 15.47 .876 0.02
General Health (0–100) (1096) 39.37 21.03 39.74 21.36 38.62 20.35 .165 0.05
Vitality (0–100) (1111) 27.03 20.64 26.52 20.46 28.08 20.99 .785 -0.07
Social Functioning (0–100) (1120) 47.76 26.96 47.36 26.53 48.57 27.82 .293 -0.05
Role-Emotional (0–100) (1067) 40.29 43.63 40.19 43.72 40.49 43.51 .885 -0.01
Mental Health (0–100) (1110) 55.19 23.08 56.53 22.36 52.47 24.27 .007* 0.18
Physical Component (0–80) (1031) 28.02 8.71 27.73 8.54 28.63 9.03 .297 -0.10
Mental Health Comp. (0–80) (1031) 35.62 12.91 35.88 12.89 35.08 12.96 .914 0.06
LiSat-11-
Satis-faction with…
Life as a whole (1–6) (1239) 3.46 1.90 3.60 1.35 3.31 1.40 < .001* 0.21
Vocational situation (1–6) (1175) 2.88 2.47 2.87 1.61 2.88 1.65 .896 -0.01
Financial situations (1–6) (1241) 3.39 2.06 3.30 1.55 3.48 1.64 .057 -0.11
Spare time (1–6) (1233) 2.95 2.06 3.01 1.40 2.89 1.46 .155 0.08
Friends/acquaintances (1–6) (1246) 3.66 1.89 3.77 1.45 3.54 1.45 .010* 0.16
Sexual life (1–6) (1213) 2.89 2.47 3.05 1.62 2.73 1.58 < .001* 0.20
Personal ADL (1–6) (1248) 4.09 1.86 4.15 1.51 4.03 1.52 .202 0.08
Family life (1–6) (1191) 4.06 2.19 4.35 1.58 3.76 1.97 < .001* 0.35
Partner relationship (1–6) (1135) 3.70 2.76 3.86 2.17 3.54 2.31 .027 0.14
Somatic health (1–6) (1246) 2.31 2.02 2.31 1.29 2.30 1.26 .896 0.01
Psychological health (1–6) (1247) 3.52 1.93 3.56 1.46 3.47 1.54 .349 0.06
CPAQ-8 Activities Engagement (0–24) (1325) 12.83 9.49 13.46 9.76 11.45 8.73 < .001* 0.21
Pain Willingness (0–24) (1086) 9.51 5.35 10.08 5.27 8.36 5.34 < .001* 0.33
TSK Kinesiophobia (17–68) (869) 41.51 9.30 39.50 8.83 43.52 9.63 < .001* -0.44

Abbreviations: HAD: The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, MPI: The Multidimensional Pain Inventory, EQ-5D: EuroQuol, quality of life measure SF36, Medical Outcome Study Short Form 36; CPAQ-8: Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire- 8 items; LiSat-11: Life Satisfaction questionnaire; TSK: Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia

*p≤ .001

1 Effect Sizes Cohen’s d. A negative value indicates that women’s values are lower.

According to CPAQ, women scored significantly higher levels of Activity Engagement than men t (1323) = 3.59, p = < .001, d = .21 (mean difference = 2.01, 99% CI: .56 to 3.45), and Pain Willingness t (1084) = -5.03, p = < .001, d = .33 (mean difference = 1.72, 99% CI: .84 to 2.61) (Table 3).

Women scored less on physical function (SF-36 PF) than men, t (1114) = 12.04, p = < .001, d = .04 (mean difference = 0.20, 99% CI: 0.084 to 1.30) while men reported significantly higher levels of kinesiophobia (TSK) than women t(867) = -6.15, p = < .001, d = .44 (mean difference = -3.99, 99% CI: -5.71 to -2.26). That women scored higher in general activity level as reflected by the MPI, t (1212) = 3.31, p < .001, d = .21 (mean difference = 0.20, 99% CI: 0.04 to 0.35). The effect sizes of these differences are however low.

According to LiSat-11, women stated higher satisfaction with: life as a whole friends and acquaintances as well as greater satisfaction with their family life, and their sexual life (See LiSat-11 Table 3).

Discussion

The main results were that at the same pain severity, women report significantly higher activity level, pain acceptance and social support while men report higher kinesiophobia, mood disturbances and lower activity level.

In the present population, there were fewer men than women born in Sweden were as well as men being older. However, the total group of patients included in this study was representative when compared with a European epidemiological study [1] in terms of age and sex (middle-aged women), and pain duration (> 7 years) and with the pain population attending specialty pain care clinics in Sweden [66]. The overall poor quality of life of the patients in this study, according to the SF-36, was consistent with previously published pain studies [6769] but distinctively lower than the Swedish population norm [38].

Some of the differences already known between men and women with chronic pain were confirmed by this study. More women than men seek rehabilitation, they present with pain in more body areas, and or widespread pain, while men report having more often localized pain. Among the group of patients in this study, more men were born outside Sweden and they were older than women (Tables 1 and 2). These demographic differences may or may not be clinical significant, while differences in how they react and behave while in pain, might have a clinical impact in rehabilitation.

Women in this study reported higher pain acceptance levels and being less afraid of pain. They reported higher activity level (MPI) and activity engagement (CPAQ-8), despite the fact that they have the same level of pain, the same severity of symptoms, the same discomfort and the same somatic health as men. However, men scored having higher physical function (SF-36) but also struggling with psychological issues, such as mood disturbances (SF-36). Even though women had pain in several regions (widespread pain, see Table 2) they expressed being more satisfied with their social life (intimate, private, family and friends) and with life in general (Li-Sat11). This capacity for positivity has also been found in a previous study, where it has been linked to greater social support among women with chronic pain, compared to men [70].

Although initially developed for use with chronic lower back pain patients and later validated in other musculoskeletal pain populations, recent studies suggest that it is a valid measure of pain-related fear of movement in Swedish [63] and for heterogeneous chronic pain samples. It was interesting to find that women scored significantly lower in kinesiophobia than men. Women also scored having a higher level of activity even though they reported pain in multiple body areas (Table 2), which was consistent with prior studies [14, 18, 22]. Also consistent with prior investigation, men‘s statistically significant higher levels of kinesiophobia is also consistent with prior studies [60, 71, 72]. In this study both men and women score above the kinesiophobia cut off. But why men are significantly more avoidant to move than women can depend upon social norms, higher expectations or a deeper concern about losing work capacity or productivity as a result of re-injury, thus with a risk of having a sedentary life. It has been demonstrated that kinesiophobia has shown to be a strong predictor for pain disability and pain chronicity in the longer term [73] associated with higher rates of sick-leaves [74], pain vigilance and disability as well as decreased physical activity levels [7577].

One limitation to the current study is that the large sample size can lead to results that are overall small in magnitude, but nevertheless, highly statistically significant. These results must be treated with suitable caution. To counteract this, we made the value of p to be accepted as significant to be lower than convention, and we provided effect sizes as a guide to the overall magnitude of effect, uninfluenced by sample size. The effect size was in some cases low, which is an indication of that the significance found may not be a meaningful difference. Another limitation are the potential flaws that self-reports can contain. This study may have benefited from gathering observational measures, such as physical capacity, activity level, strength, sedentary-time, etc. in order to see how the self-reported activity level correlates with the factual one.

It is important to interpret the significances in a frame of studying a very heterogeneous group indicated by large SD in some areas such duration of pain and number of pain location. This heterogeneity may be the cause of the relatively low effect sizes despite some significant differences. If the females and males were further stratified or clustered by rehabilitation relevant indicators potentially other and more clinical relevant differences might be identified. A limitation is that this has not be done, since it would have required a larger sample. Another limitation in this this study is that due to its exploratory and cross-sectional design, no causal inferences can be made, nor can it be established that the found differences necessarily influence treatment response. However, if outcomes were potentially sex-dependent, treatment may be more effective by differentially targeting the processes supporting the different needs, expectations and coping mechanisms of each of the sexes. It could be a possibility in the future to use the results from questionnaires to allocate patients into different rehabilitation packages, depending on the needs. Is looks as if the needs of the males and female in general are different. Further research is needed in order to investigate to what extent the differences we found are due to disparities in assessment modalities, practitioners’ attitude or referral patterns, knowledge and/or stigmatization that the patient has met before [78] or after referral [4].

To conclude, this study indicate differences between men and women regarding symptomatology and their reaction or handling these. The results give some suggestions that the sexes differ in how they accept their pain, which could potentially be useful to consider while selecting patients for rehabilitation programs (or designing programs adapted to the sexes).

Acknowledgments

The present study was supported by grants from The Swedish Association for Survivors of Accident and Injury (RTP), Renée Eanders Foundation, the Swedish Association of Local Authorities and Regions (SALAR), the Vårdal Foundation, (RehSAM) and AFA insurance, Sweden.

Data Availability

The data underlying this study are third party data from the Swedish National Quality Registry for Pain Rehabilitation submitted by the Pain and Rehabilitation Centre in Linköping, Sweden. To request for access to these data, visit the registry’s webpage at http://www.ucr.uu.se/nrs/. Ethical approval from a Swedish ethical committee is required to access these data. Please contact Prof Björn Gerdle at Pain and Rehabilitation Centre in Linköping (bjorn.gerdle@liu.se) if you have any questions.

Funding Statement

The first author was part-time founded by The Swedish Association for Survivors of Accident and Injury (RTP) and from Renée Eanders Foundation related to the Swedish Association of Physiotherapists. The rest of her time was unfunded. Professor Björn Gerdle was funded by the Swedish Association of Local Authorities and Regions (SALAR), the Vårdal Foundation, (RehSAM) and AFA insurance, Sweden.

References

  • 1.Breivik H, Collett B, Ventafridda V, Cohen R, Gallacher D. Survey of chronic pain in Europe: prevalence, impact on daily life, and treatment. European journal of pain. 2006;10(4):287–333. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2005.06.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Fejer R, Kyvik KO, Hartvigsen J. The prevalence of neck pain in the world population: a systematic critical review of the literature. European spine journal. 2006;15(6):834–48. doi: 10.1007/s00586-004-0864-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Gerdle B, Björk J, Henriksson C, Bengtsson A. Prevalence of current and chronic pain and their influences upon work and healthcare-seeking: a population study. The Journal of rheumatology. 2004;31(7):1399–406. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Haukenes I, Hensing G, Stålnacke B-M, Hammarström A. Does pain severity guide selection to multimodal pain rehabilitation across gender? European Journal of Pain. 2015;19(6):826–33. doi: 10.1002/ejp.609 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Stubbs D, Krebs E, Bair M, Damush T, Wu J, Sutherland J, et al. Sex Differences in Pain and Pain‐Related Disability among Primary Care Patients with Chronic Musculoskeletal Pain. Pain Medicine. 2010;11(2):232–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4637.2009.00760.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ahlgren C, Fjellman-Wiklund A, Hamberg K, Johansson EE, Stålnacke B-M. The meanings given to gender in studies on multimodal rehabilitation for patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain–a literature review. Disability and rehabilitation. 2016;38(23):2255–70. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2015.1127435 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Dawson A, List T. Comparison of pain thresholds and pain tolerance levels between Middle Easterners and Swedes and between genders. Journal of oral rehabilitation. 2009;36(4):271–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2842.2009.01943.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Edwards RR, Doleys DM, Lowery D, Fillingim RB. Pain tolerance as a predictor of outcome following multidisciplinary treatment for chronic pain: differential effects as a function of sex. Pain. 2003;106(3):419–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Riley JL, Robinson ME, Wade JB, Myers CD, Price DD. Sex differences in negative emotional responses to chronic pain. The journal of Pain. 2001;2(6):354–9. doi: 10.1054/jpai.2001.27000 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wijnhoven HA, de Vet HC, Picavet HSJ. Explaining sex differences in chronic musculoskeletal pain in a general population. Pain. 2006;124(1):158–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.King BM. Point: a call for proper usage of “gender” and “sex” in biomedical publications. American Journal of Physiology-Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology. 2010;298(6):R1700–R1. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00694.2009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.American Psychological Association, APA. Publication manual of the American Psychological Association. 6th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2010. xviii, 272 p. p. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.King BM. Point: a call for proper usage of "gender" and "sex" in biomedical publications. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2010;298(6):R1700–1. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00694.2009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Berkley KJ. Sex differences in pain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1997;20(03):371–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Fillingim RB, Maixner W, Kincaid S, Silva S. Sex differences in temporal summation but not sensory-discriminative processing of thermal pain. Pain. 1998;75(1):121–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Riley JL III, Robinson ME, Wise EA, Myers CD, Fillingim RB. Sex differences in the perception of noxious experimental stimuli: a meta-analysis. Pain. 1998;74(2):181–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Popescu A, LeResche L, Truelove EL, Drangsholt MT. Gender differences in pain modulation by diffuse noxious inhibitory controls: a systematic review. Pain. 2010;150(2):309–18. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.05.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Unruh AM. Gender variations in clinical pain experience. Pain. 1996;65(2–3):123–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cbiederman JJ, Schefft BK. Behavioral, physiological, and self-evaluative effects of anxiety on the self-control of pain. Behavior modification. 1994;18(1):89–105. doi: 10.1177/01454455940181006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Racine M, Tousignant-Laflamme Y, Kloda LA, Dion D, Dupuis G, Choinière M. A systematic literature review of 10years of research on sex/gender and pain perception–Part 2: Do biopsychosocial factors alter pain sensitivity differently in women and men? Pain. 2012;153(3):619–35. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.11.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gatchel RJ, Turk DC. Psychosocial factors in pain: Critical perspectives: Guilford Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Fillingim RB, King CD, Ribeiro-Dasilva MC, Rahim-Williams B, Riley JL. Sex, gender, and pain: a review of recent clinical and experimental findings. The journal of pain. 2009;10(5):447–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Manson JE. Pain: sex differences and implications for treatment. Metabolism. 2010;59:S16–S20. doi: 10.1016/j.metabol.2010.07.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Pieh C, Altmeppen J, Neumeier S, Loew T, Angerer M, Lahmann C. Gender differences in outcomes of a multimodal pain management program. Pain. 2012;153(1):197–202. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.10.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Racine M, Tousignant-Laflamme Y, Kloda LA, Dion D, Dupuis G, Choinière M. A systematic literature review of 10years of research on sex/gender and experimental pain perception–Part 1: Are there really differences between women and men? Pain. 2012;153(3):602–18. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.11.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Bränström H, Fahlström M. Kinesiophobia in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain: differences between men and women. Journal of rehabilitation medicine. 2008;40(5):375–80. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Buckelew SP, Shutty MS, Hewett J, Landon T, Morrow K, Frank RG. Health locus of control, gender differences and adjustment to persistent pain. Pain. 1990;42(3):287–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Keogh E, McCracken LM, Eccleston C. Gender moderates the association between depression and disability in chronic pain patients. European Journal of Pain. 2006;10(5):413-. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2005.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Munce SE, Stewart DE. Gender differences in depression and chronic pain conditions in a national epidemiologic survey. Psychosomatics. 2007;48(5):394–9. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.48.5.394 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Rustøen T, Wahl AK, Hanestad BR, Lerdal A, Paul S, Miaskowski C. Gender differences in chronic pain—findings from a population-based study of Norwegian adults. Pain Management Nursing. 2004;5(3):105–17. doi: 10.1016/j.pmn.2004.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Williams ACdC, Eccleston C, Morley S. Psychological therapies for the management of chronic pain (excluding headache) in adults. The cochrane library; 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983;67(6):361–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Mykletun A, Stordal E, Dahl AA. Hospital Anxiety and Depression (HAD) scale: factor structure, item analyses and internal consistency in a large population. The British journal of psychiatry. 2001;179(6):540–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Health and quality of life outcomes. 2003;1(1):29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta psychiatrica scandinavica. 1983;67(6):361–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Lisspers J, Nygren A, Söderman E. Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HAD): some psychometric data for a Swedish sample. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 1997;96(4):281–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Ware JE, Sherbourne CD. The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36). I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Med Care. 1992;30(6):473–83. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sullivan M, Karlsson J, Ware JE. The Swedish SF-36 Health Survey—I. Evaluation of data quality, scaling assumptions, reliability and construct validity across general populations in Sweden. Social science & medicine. 1995;41(10):1349–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Sullivan M, Karlsson J, Taft C, Ware J. SF-36 H? lsoenk? t. Svensk Manual och Tolkningsguide(Swedish Manual and Interpretation Guide). 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Group EuroQol. EuroQol—a new facility for the measurement of health-related quality of life. The EuroQol Group. Health Policy. 1990;16(3):199–208. Epub 1990/11/05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Brooks R. EuroQol: the current state of play. Health Policy. 1996;37(1):53–72. Epub 1996/06/06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Brooks RG, Jendteg S, Lindgren B, Persson U, Bjork S. EuroQol: health-related quality of life measurement. Results of the Swedish questionnaire exercise. Health Policy. 1991;18(1):37–48. Epub 1991/05/09. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Sullivan M, Karlsson J, Taft C, Ware JE. SF-36 hälsoenkät: svensk manual och tolkningsguide (Swedish manual and interpretation guide). 2. uppl. ed. Göteborg: Sahlgrenska sjukhuset, Sektionen för vårdforskning; 2002. x, ca 200 s. med var. pag. p. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Fugl-Meyer AR, Fugl-Meyer KS. The coping process after traumatic brain injury. Scandinavian journal of rehabilitation medicine Supplement. 1987;17:51–3. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Fugl-Meyer AR, Fugl-Meyer KS. The coping process after traumatic brain injury. Scand J Rehabil Med Suppl. 1988;17:51–3. Epub 1988/01/01. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Fugl-Meyer AR, Melin R, Fugl-Meyer KS. Life satisfaction in 18-to 64-year-old Swedes: in relation to gender, age, partner and immigrant status. Journal of rehabilitation medicine. 2002;34(5):239–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Turk DC, Rudy TE. Towards a comprehensive assessment of chronic pain patients. Behaviour research and therapy. 1987;25(4):237–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Turk DC, Rudy TE. Toward an empirically derived taxonomy of chronic pain patients: integration of psychological assessment data. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 1988;56(2):233 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Turk DC, Rudy TE. Towards a comprehensive assessment of chronic pain patients. Behav Res Ther. 1987;25(4):237–49. Epub 1987/01/01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Turk DC, Rudy TE. Toward an empirically derived taxonomy of chronic pain patients: integration of psychological assessment data. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;56(2):233–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bergström G, Jensen IB, Bodin L, Linton SJ, Nygren ÅL, Carlsson SG. Reliability and factor structure of the Multidimensional Pain Inventory–Swedish Language version (MPI-S). Pain. 1998;75(1):101–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Bergström KG, Jensen IB, Linton SJ, Nygren ÅL. A psychometric evaluation of the Swedish version of the Multidimensional Pain Inventory (MPI-S): a gender differentiated evaluation. European Journal of Pain. 1999;3(3):261–73. doi: 10.1053/eujp.1999.0128 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Rovner GS, Årestedt K, Gerdle B, Börsbo B, McCracken LM. Psychometric properties of the 8-item Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (CPAQ-8) in a Swedish chronic pain cohort. Journal of rehabilitation medicine. 2014;46(1):73–80. doi: 10.2340/16501977-1227 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.McCracken LM, Vowles KE, Eccleston C. Acceptance of chronic pain: component analysis and a revised assessment method. Pain. 2004;107(1–2):159–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Wicksell RK, Olsson GL, Melin L. The Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (CPAQ)-further validation including a confirmatory factor analysis and a comparison with the Tampa Scale of Kinesiophobia. European Journal of Pain. 2008;13(7):760–8. Epub 2008/10/18. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2008.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Fish RA, Hogan MJ, Morrison TG, Stewart I, McGuire BE. Willing and Able: A Closer Look at Pain Willingness and Activity Engagement on the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (CPAQ-8). J Pain. 2013;14(3):233–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2012.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Fish RA, McGuire B, Hogan M, Morrison TG, Stewart I. Validation of the chronic pain acceptance questionnaire (CPAQ) in an Internet sample and development and preliminary validation of the CPAQ-8. Pain. 2010;149(3):435–43. Epub 2010/03/02. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.12.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Baranoff J, Hanrahan SJ, Kapur D, Connor JP. Validation of the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire-8 in an Australian Pain Clinic Sample. International journal of behavioral medicine. 2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Miller RP, Kori SH, Todd DD. The Tampa Scale: a Measure of Kinisophobia. The Clinical Journal of Pain. 1991;7(1):51. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Vlaeyen JW, Kole-Snijders AM, Boeren RG, Van Eek H. Fear of movement/(re) injury in chronic low back pain and its relation to behavioral performance. Pain. 1995;62(3):363–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Roelofs J, van Breukelen G, Sluiter J, Frings-Dresen MH, Goossens M, Thibault P, et al. Norming of the Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia across pain diagnoses and various countries. Pain. 2011;152(5):1090–5. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.01.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Crombez G, Vlaeyen JW, Heuts PH, Lysens R. Pain-related fear is more disabling than pain itself: evidence on the role of pain-related fear in chronic back pain disability. Pain. 1999;80(1):329–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Lundberg MK, Styf J, Carlsson SG. A psychometric evaluation of the Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia—from a physiotherapeutic perspective. Physiotherapy Theory and Practice. 2004;20(2):121–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Roelofs J, Sluiter JK, Frings-Dresen MH, Goossens M, Thibault P, Boersma K, et al. Fear of movement and (re) injury in chronic musculoskeletal pain: Evidence for an invariant two-factor model of the Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia across pain diagnoses and Dutch, Swedish, and Canadian samples. Pain. 2007;131(1):181–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; Hillsdale, NJ: 1988:20–6. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Nyberg V, Sanne H, Sjölund BH. Swedish quality registry for pain rehabilitation: purpose, design, implementation and characteristics of referred patients. Journal of rehabilitation medicine. 2011;43(1):50–7. doi: 10.2340/16501977-0631 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Laursen BS, Bajaj P, Olesen AS, Delmar C, Arendt‐Nielsen L. Health related quality of life and quantitative pain measurement in females with chronic non‐malignant pain. European journal of pain. 2005;9(3):267-. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.07.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Peolsson M, Gerdle B. Coping in patients with chronic whiplash-associated disorders: a descriptive study. Journal of rehabilitation medicine. 2004;36(1):28–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Wallin MK, Raak RI. Quality of life in subgroups of individuals with whiplash associated disorders. European Journal of Pain. 2008;12(7):842–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2007.12.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Löfgren M, Ekholm J, Öhman A. ‘A constant struggle’: successful strategies of women in work despite fibromyalgia. Disability and rehabilitation. 2006;28(7):447–55. doi: 10.1080/09638280500197891 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Pells J, Edwards CL, McDougald CS, Wood M, Barksdale C, Jonassaint J, et al. Fear of movement (kinesiophobia), pain, and psychopathology in patients with sickle cell disease. The Clinical journal of pain. 2007;23(8):707–13. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0b013e31814da3eb [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Swinkels-Meewisse IE, Roelofs J, Verbeek AL, Oostendorp RA, Vlaeyen JW. Fear of movement/(re) injury, disability and participation in acute low back pain. Pain. 2003;105(1):371–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Grotle M, Foster NE, Dunn KM, Croft P. Are prognostic indicators for poor outcome different for acute and chronic low back pain consulters in primary care? PAIN®. 2010;151(3):790–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Symonds TL, Burton AK, Tillotson KM, Main CJ. Absence resulting from low back trouble can be reduced by psychosocial intervention at the work place. Spine. 1995;20(24):2738–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Monticone M, Ambrosini E, Rocca B, Cazzaniga D, Liquori V, Foti C. Group‐based task‐oriented exercises aimed at managing kinesiophobia improved disability in chronic low back pain. European Journal of Pain. 2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Monticone M, Ambrosini E, Rocca B, Magni S, Brivio F, Ferrante S. A multidisciplinary rehabilitation programme improves disability, kinesiophobia and walking ability in subjects with chronic low back pain: results of a randomised controlled pilot study. European Spine Journal. 2014;23(10):2105–13. doi: 10.1007/s00586-014-3478-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Vlaeyen JW, de Jong J, Geilen M, Heuts PH, van Breukelen G. The treatment of fear of movement/(re) injury in chronic low back pain: further evidence on the effectiveness of exposure in vivo. The Clinical journal of pain. 2002;18(4):251–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Green CR, Hart-Johnson T. The adequacy of chronic pain management prior to presenting at a tertiary care pain center: the role of patient socio-demographic characteristics. The Journal of Pain. 2010;11(8):746–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The data underlying this study are third party data from the Swedish National Quality Registry for Pain Rehabilitation submitted by the Pain and Rehabilitation Centre in Linköping, Sweden. To request for access to these data, visit the registry’s webpage at http://www.ucr.uu.se/nrs/. Ethical approval from a Swedish ethical committee is required to access these data. Please contact Prof Björn Gerdle at Pain and Rehabilitation Centre in Linköping (bjorn.gerdle@liu.se) if you have any questions.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES