Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2016 Nov 15;11(2):333–345. doi: 10.1007/s11682-016-9649-8

Midlife managerial experience is linked to late life hippocampal morphology and function

C Suo 1,2,3,4, N Gates 1,2,3, M Fiatarone Singh 5,6,7, N Saigal 8, G C Wilson 8, J Meiklejohn 8, P Sachdev 3,9, H Brodaty 1,3,10, W Wen 3,9, N Singh 8, B T Baune 11, M Baker 5,12, N Foroughi 13, Y Wang 6,7,14, Michael J Valenzuela 1,2,3,15,
PMCID: PMC5408055  PMID: 27848149

Abstract

An active cognitive lifestyle has been suggested to have a protective role in the long-term maintenance of cognition. Amongst healthy older adults, more managerial or supervisory experiences in midlife are linked to a slower hippocampal atrophy rate in late life. Yet whether similar links exist in individuals with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) is not known, nor whether these differences have any functional implications. 68 volunteers from the Sydney SMART Trial, diagnosed with non-amnestic MCI, were divided into high and low managerial experience (HME/LME) during their working life. All participants underwent neuropsychological testing, structural and resting-state functional MRI. Group comparisons were performed on hippocampal volume, morphology, hippocampal seed-based functional connectivity, memory and executive function and self-ratings of memory proficiency. HME was linked to better memory function (p = 0.024), mediated by larger hippocampal volume (p = 0.025). More specifically, deformation analysis found HME had relatively more volume in the CA1 sub-region of the hippocampus (p < 0.05). Paradoxically, this group rated their memory proficiency worse (p = 0.004), a result correlated with diminished functional connectivity between the right hippocampus and right prefrontal cortex (p < 0.001). Finally, hierarchical regression modelling substantiated this double dissociation.

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s11682-016-9649-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Keywords: Cognitive lifestyle, Managerial experience, Hippocampal volume, Hippocampal morphometry, Hippocampal functional connectivity, Memory

Background

Cognitive lifestyle refers to lifetime patterns of participation in educational, occupational, social and leisure activities and is increasingly recognised as a protective and modifiable risk factor in the development of dementia. An enriched cognitive lifestyle has been linked to a lower risk for incident dementia (M. J. Valenzuela and Sachdev 2006) (Valenzuela et al. 2011), less prospective cognitive decline (Marioni et al. 2012b), higher chances of recovery from mild impairment to cognitive normalcy (Marioni et al. 2012a), a slower rate of hippocampal atrophy (Valenzuela et al. 2008; Suo et al. 2012) and greater neuronal density in the frontal lobe (Valenzuela et al. 2012).

Previously, in a population-based study of healthy older adults we showed that the link between active cognitive lifestyle and protection from hippocampal atrophy was primarily driven by high-level managerial and supervisory experience in midlife – ex-high level managers or supervisors had significantly larger hippocampal volume and slower hippocampal atrophy rate than those who had never been in charge of others in their working life (Suo et al. 2012). This is particularly interesting because a fast rate of hippocampal atrophy or smaller hippocampal volume is a sensitive and specific predictor of dementia (Jack et al. 2000) (Risacher et al. 2010) (Frankó et al. 2013) (den Heijer et al. 2006). Yet whether the same protective association applies to individuals with dementia precursor, Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI), is unknown.

Prediction of the relationship between occupational complexity and hippocampal structure and function in MCI from results obtained in healthy older adults is challenging. On the one hand, the neuroprotective association we previously reported (Suo et al. 2012) may continue into the preclinical phase. On the other hand, a compensatory effect can result in individuals with borderline impairment and greater exposure to complex cognitive lifestyles exhibiting more signs of disease burden (Stern et al. 1995) (Brayne et al. 2010) (Bennett et al. 2014). Indeed, previous studies have found contrasting correlations between cognitive lifestyle-related variables and brain structure/function measures in different cohorts, i.e., healthy elderly versus AD dementia (Arenaza-Urquijo et al. 2013; Sole-Padulles et al. 2009) (Seo et al. 2011) (Kidron et al. 1997) (Coffey et al. 1999). Further complicating matters, MCI has two major subtypes, amnestic MCI (aMCI) that is commonly linked to AD pathology (Albert et al. 2011), and non-amnestic MCI (naMCI) that has multiple aetiology (Petersen 2004) including both Alzheimer’s disease and cerebrovascular disease (Sudo et al. 2012). Irrespective of MCI sub-classification, diagnosis relies on both subjective and objective cognitive criteria (Petersen 2004). Cognition in one or more domains must be objectively impaired on neuropsychological tests accompanied by a subjective concern or complaint about one’s own cognitive proficiency. It is the latter that typically motivates a person to present to a medical professional and ultimately receive a diagnosis (Wong et al. 2006).

Our first objective was therefore to determine in older individuals with naMCI whether occupational supervision was related to late-life hippocampal volume and morphology based on cross-sectional data. Secondly, because links between cognition and hippocampal functional networks have been revealed by resting-state functional Magnetic Resonance Image (fMRI) (Liu et al. 2008; Wang et al. 2011), we also examined potential long-term functional implications of occupational managerial experience. Specifically, we tested whether managerial experience was linked to two of the core criteria for MCI, objective and subjective cognitive function, and further explored whether putative structural differences in the medial temporal lobe translate to subtle changes to resting-state fMRI networks. We hypothesized that like in our healthy elderly sample there would be a protective link between midlife managerial experience and hippocampal structure in late life. We also expected a difference in functional brain networks between individuals with high and low levels of managerial experience, but the direction of the difference was difficult to predict and so this was an exploratory question. Similarly, we tried to integrate hippocampal structure, function, objective memory and subjective concerns into one statistical model.

Methods

Participants

Participants were drawn from the Sydney SMART trial (Study of Mental Activity and Regular Training) (Gates et al. 2011b) (Suo et al. 2016), a randomised, double-blind, active-controlled longitudinal trial of resistance training and computer-based cognitive training in individuals who met ‘core criteria’ for MCI (Albert et al. 2011). All results reported here are derived from baseline data before randomization or the onset of any intervention. Participants (N = 100) were community-dwelling persons aged over 55, with diagnosis of MCI (self-reported memory complaint; objective cognitive deficit based on a Mini-Mental Status Examination (MMSE) score of 23-28/30 or 29/30 if error in memory registration; and no dementia (Clinical Dementia Rating of 0.5 or below). Primary exclusion criteria were clinical depression and unstable medical conditions or other neurological diseases. Full inclusion and exclusion details can be found in our published protocol (Gates et al. 2011a).

For this study further exclusion criteria were applied: 1) non-retired participants were excluded (n = 11) because of our specific interest on the long term correlates of managerial experience in working life on brain health after retirement (Valenzuela and Sachdev 2007); 2) Amnestic MCI based on an age-scaled score on the Logical Memory II WMS test of less than 5 (i.e., <=5th percentile) because aMCI was only a small proportion of our SMART cohort (n = 8) and exclusion therefore produced to a more homogenous naMCI sample; 3) Missed or refused MRI (n = 12) or severe artefacts in MRI data (n = 1). Our final sample with complete data comprised 68 participants. Because each neuroimaging modality is susceptible to different types of artefacts, a variable number of participants were omitted from different specific analyses as numerated below. Informed consent was obtained from all participants, in accordance with the guideline of the human research ethics committee of both the University of New South Wales and Sydney University.

Lifetime of experience questionnaire (LEQ)

LEQ data were collected using an online system (rng.org.au/leq) in a dedicated computer room at Sydney University. During data collection, a psychologist was on hand to provide assistance and supervision. All the sub-scores and total scores were automatically calculated using the same rules as paper-based LEQ (Valenzuela and Sachdev 2007). Managerial experience is based on asking “How many people have you been in charge of?” during each five year epoch from 30 to retirement. The answer has four response options: none, 1–5 people, 6–9 people and 10 or more people. The maximum answer of any five-year period is defined as midlife managerial experience. We dichotomised the four response options into: high managerial experience (6 or more people) and low managerial experience group (less than 6 people) based on our previous observation of a non-continuous step-change in hippocampal volume in both men and women (Suo et al. 2012) and also for practical purposes based on the number of participants in these four categories. To more specifically estimate the engagement in late life, we separated out all social related questions in the late life part of LEQ. As in our previous report (Suo et al. 2012), we included non-specific questions 2–6 and social related options in non-specific question 7 and 8, and generated a late-life total social engagement subscale.

Socio-demographic, health status, cognitive and psychological assessment

All socio-demographic and health status data were obtained by self-report using structured interviews. All cognitive and psychological data were acquired by an experienced neuropsychologist. For details of full neuropsychological battery and psychological test instruments see SMART protocol (Gates et al. 2011b). Main psychological measures are A) Self-rated Memory Appraisal, combining Subjective Memory Complaint (SMC) and Memory Awareness Rating Scale – Memory Function Scale (MARS-MFS) (Clare et al. 2002); B) Executive domain objective cognition; C) Memory domain objective cognition. Details provided in Supplementary.

MRI

MRI data were acquired on a Philip 3 T Scanner. Details provided in Supplementary.

Structural MRI

Left and right hippocampi were segmented using the Oxford Centre for Functional MRI of the Brian (FMRIB)’s Integrated Registration and Segmentation Tool (FIRST), according to the processing protocol of Erickson et al. (Erickson et al. 2011). Mean bilateral hippocampal volume and vertex/mesh model for each individual was calculated automatically, followed by quality checking for segmentation error, using the protocol established by the ENIGMA (Enhancing Neuro-Imaging Genetics Through Meta-Analysis) consortium. Nine (2 in HME, 7 in LME) participants were excluded from this analysis because segmentation failed due to abnormal brain structure or movement artefacts, leaving N = 59 for this analysis. Two group t-tests were then performed based on vertex-to-vertex analyses on the both left and right hippocampus. We used sex as a covariate, because our previous population-based study showed a significant difference of sex proportion between high and low managerial groups (Suo et al. 2012). Results were FDR corrected for multiple comparison errors. Further details provided in Supplementary.

Resting state functional MRI

Four fMRI data had to be removed due to the artefacts (one participant has shunt, three discarded due to movement criteria: displacement >3 mm in any direction or rotation >5 degree along any axis) (Johnstone et al. 2006) (Kim et al. 1999), leaving N = 64 for fMRI analysis. We pre-processed the fMRI data and generated hippocampus seed functional connectivity (FC) map bilaterally, using SPM8 (Statistical Parametric Mapping) based tool-box (Chao-Gan and Yu-Feng 2010). Since individuals with MCI exhibited significant smaller hippocampi (Shi et al. 2009), the original Anatomical Automatic Labeling (AAL) hippocampus seed was eroded internally by 2 mm, resulting in a core hippocampal template less likely to be sensitive to partial volume effects on the BOLD signal (Figs. 1, 2 and 3a). Two group t-test of hippocampal functional connectivity maps was performed using SPM8, to test the topological difference between HME group and LME group, controlling for age, sex and education years. Clusters with p < 0.05 (cluster-level FDR correction) were consider significant, and mean functional connectivity index of these clusters were extracted for further statistical tests. Further details can be found in Supplementary.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Comparison of high and low managerial experience (HME, LME) on self-rated memory appraisal, and memory domain and executive domain function. All graphs displayed adjusted means/values ± SEM. a HME participants had significantly more subjective memory complaints (SMC, p = 0.016, controlling for sex, age and education years) and tendency for lower expectations about their future memory proficiency (measured by MARS-MFS, p = 0.328, controlling for sex, age and education years; p = 0.10 unadjusted). b HME group showed a significantly lower overall self-rated memory domain score (p = 0.004, controlling for sex, age and education years). c HME group performed significantly better on memory tests (p = 0.024, after adjustment for sex, age and education years). d HME participants also had a trend towards better performance on tests of executive function (p = 0.181, after adjustment for sex, age and education years; p = 0.016 unadjusted)

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Hippocampal volume differences and vertex analysis between HME and LME groups. a showed visible hippocampal differences comparing a HME (top) and LME participant (bottom) at the same coronal slice. b HME group had larger average hippocampal volume, after adjustment for intracranial volume, sex, age and education year (p = 0.006). Adjusted values are estimated marginal means ± SEM after controlling for these covariances. Significance was robust to exclusion of the outliner in LME group. Vertex analysis c showed two main areas of morphological differences for the right hippocampus between managerial experience groups. Warm colors indicated increasing significance level where vertices with p < 0.026 survive FDR correction controlling for sex. Arrows indicated the direction of inter-group differences at each vertex, and point from the mean surface of the LME group to the HME group, indicative of relative volume inflation (i.e., larger volume in HME group at this region). A: anterior, P: posterior. See Supplementary for a 3D-rotating movie

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Hippocampal seed resting-state functional connectivity differences between LME and HME group. All graphs display values after adjustment for sex, age and education ± SEM. a Erosion of the original AAL hippocampus template (red) resulted in an atrophy-like template (blue) for use as the FC seed. b FC connectivity of right hippocampus, across whole sample (voxel-level p(FWE-corr) = 0.000005). c LME group showed greater right hippocampal FC with the right middle frontal cortical gyrus (rPFC) than the HME group (k = 237; cluster-level p(FDR-corr) = 0.022; controlling for sex, age and education years). d The rHIP-rPFC connectivity in the LME group was significantly higher than that in HME group, after controlling for sex, age and education years (F = 26.8, df = 59, p < 0.001). e rHIP-rPFC connectivity was negatively correlated with hippocampal volume, after controlling for age, sex and education year. Unfilled dots indicate HME participants. f Self-rated memory appraisal was positively correlated with rHIP-rPFC connectivity, after controlling for age, sex and education year. Unfilled dots indicate HME participants. NB: Right side of the image indicated the right side of brain

Statistical analysis and modelling

Zero-order structural models were first built and simplified to a final model of independent relationships using a series hierarchical linear regression models based on backwards elimination linear regressions. Where appropriate, we explicitly tested for mediation effects using the Sobel test (Preacher and Hayes 2004). More details are in Supplementary.

A p-value of <0.05 was considered indicative of statistical significance. Also, bar/scatter plots in Figs. 1, 2 and 3 displayed residual values from the regression analyses, indicating the adjusted means/values after controlling for covariates listed in legends.

Results

Sociodemographic profile and cognitive lifestyle

Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics are presented in Table 1. The total sample (N = 68) was divided into participants with low managerial experience (LME) group (N = 52) or high managerial experience (HME) (N = 16). In general, participants had mild cognitive deficits as indicated by the majority of ADAS-COG scores in the MCI range of 8–9 (Pyo et al. 2006), and negligible depressive symptoms, consistent with SMART entry criteria. Also, their overall cognitive lifestyle (LEQ = 88.4) was similar to that reported for the population-based Sydney Memory & Ageing Study (healthy aged average 93.4 (Valenzuela et al. 2013)). There were no significant differences between managerial groups based on age, MMSE, CDR, retirement year, Geriatric Depression Scale, Physical Activities Scale, total LEQ score, or any LEQ lifestage subtotal. Further, no significant group differences were found in frequency of male sex, hypertension, diabetes type II, hypercholesterolemia, income group or job profile during midlife using Chi-Square analysis. Based on our previous finding of distinct late life socialization patterns between managerial experience groups (Suo et al. 2012), we tested this again in this sample and found a significantly higher level of social engagement for the HME group compared to LME, that reduced to trend after controlling for covariates (unadjusted p = 0.031, F = 1.94; adjusted for age, sex and education p = 0.075, Table 1).

Table 1.

Sociodemographic and clinical variables. Values represent averages ± SD unless otherwise stated

Low Managerial Experience N = 52 High Managerial Experience N = 16 p-value*
Age 71.3 ± 6.0 68.6 ± 6.3 0.126
Gender (M/F count)* 13/39 7/9 0.210
Education (years) 12.60 ± 3.8 14.69 ± 3.2 0.052
MMSE 27.5 ± 1.4 27.9 ± 1.2 0.293
ADAS-COG 8.1 ± 3.4 6.6 ± 3.1 0.114
Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) 0.23 ± 0.32 0.15 ± 0.40 0.429
Physical Activities Score 9.3 ± 4.0 10.7 ± 3.7 0.235
Geriatric Depression Scale 1.1 ± 1.3 1.6 ± 1.6 0.211
Hypertension (%)* 44.2 18.8 0.083
Diabetes Type II (%)* 7.7 6.3 1.000
Hypercholesterolemia (%)* 28.8 25.0 1.000
Young Adulthood Subtotal 29.3 ± 9.6 32.9 ± 7.5 0.180
Mid Life Subtotal 32.5 ± 10.3 38.6 ± 10.4 0.056
Late Life Subtotal 24.7 ± 6.2 26.3 ± 4.5 0.412
Late Life Social Engagement 7.08 ± 1.35 7.98 ± 0.86 0.031^
Total LEQ 86.2 ± 20.9 96.8 ± 16.7 0.109
Annual Income*
 Hi (>$30 K) 38.8 % 33.3 % 0.867
 Mid ($15 k ~ $30 k) 26.5 % 33.3 %
 Low (<$15 K) 34.7 % 33.3 %
Job Classification*
 Managers and Administrators 1.9 % 12.5 % 0.251
 Professionals 38.5 % 62.5 %
 Tradespersons and related works 9.6 % 6.3 %
 advanced clerical and service workers 3.9 % 6.3 %
 clerical, sales and service workers 30.8 % 12.5 %
 elementary clerical, sales & service workers 9.6 % 0 %
 Labourers and related workers 5.8 % 0 %

* Fisher’s Exact Chi-Square Test for non-parametric comparisons, otherwise T-test procedure applied

^ p < 0.05

Self-rated memory proficiency

As shown in Fig. 1, participants in the HME group had significantly more memory complaints (df = 66, F = 9.3, p = 0.003), a trend towards lower MARS-MFS scores indicating worse expectations about future memory proficiency (df = 66, F = 2.6, p = 0.10), and an overall significantly lower domain score for self-rated memory appraisal (df = 66, F = 12.9, p = 0.001). After controlling for age, gender and education years, significant differences remained for subjective memory complaints (df = 63, F = 6.16, p = 0.016) and overall self-rated memory appraisal (df = 63, F = 9.0, p = 0.004).

Objective cognitive performance

Executive function was generally superior in the HME group compared to LME, inclusive of tests of matrix reasoning (df = 66, F = 3.9, p = 0.05) and COWAT (df = 66, F = 6.7, p = 0.012) and non-significant trends were observed for faster task completion in TMTA (df = 66, F = 1.8, p = 0.179) and TMTB (df = 66, F = 2.1, p = 0.153). Without covariate adjustment, the total executive domain score for HME was significantly higher than LME (df = 66, F = 6.1, p = 0.016); after controlling for age, gender and education years, this comparison became non-significant (df = 63, F = 1.8, p = 0.181, Fig. 1). Memory function was significantly higher in the HME group than LME before (df = 66, F = 8.5, p = 0.005) and remained so after inclusion of the same covariate control (df = 63, F = 5.4, p = 0.024, Fig. 1)

Hippocampus volume and morphology

Hippocampus volume was significantly larger in the HME as compared to the LME group, on both the left (df = 54, F = 4.8, p = 0.032) and right side (df = 54, F = 5.1, p = 0.027), reflected by a greater bilateral average hippocampi volume (df = 54, F = 8.1, p = 0.006, after adjustment for age, sex, education year and total intracranial volume Fig. 2). This finding was still significant after additionally controlling for hypertension status, occupational status, ADAS-Cog, CDR, physical activity and diabetes status (Supplementary).

Computational vertex analysis found that the morphology of the hippocampal surface was also significantly different between managerial groups. After controlling for sex and correction for multiple comparisons, those in the HME group had significantly larger volume (more volume of HME denoted by outwards inflation) at the lateral aspect of the anterior hippocampus (consistent with CA1 hippocampal subdivision) and the posterior tail (Fig. 2). The same regions were observed after additionally controlling for age and education years (Figure S1 in Supplementary).

Hippocampal functional connectivity

Figure 3a shows the results of external erosion of the standard AAL hippocampal template in order to base our functional connectivity (FC) analysis on a hippocampal seed that takes into account atrophy of this structure. We separately tested right and left hippocampal-FC between HME and LME groups. For the right hippocampus seed, a one sample t-test illustrates the hippocampal FC map across the whole cohort (Fig. 3b, voxel-level p(FWE-corr) = 0.000005, k > 200). Using a more rigid threshold, we found this FC map included biliateral hippocampi, parahippcampus, thalamus, putamen, precunues and middle cingulate cortex (Table S2 and Figure S2 in Supplementary). A two group t-test with sex, age and education as covariates found there was a significant difference at the right Prefrontal Cortex (rPFC, Brodman Area 10) after whole-brain correction (cluster-level p(FDR-corr) = 0.022; k = 237; T = 3.99; [30 66 2]; Fig. 3c). LME had significantly higher FC between the right hippocampus and rPFC compared to the HME group controlling for age, sex and education years (F = 26.8, df = 59, p < 0.001, Fig. 3d). This finding was still significant after additionally controlling for hypertension status, occupational status, ADAS-Cog, CDR, physical activity and diabetes status (Supplementary). There was no difference for left hippocampal-FC after multiple comparison correction, though an identical left hippocampus and rPFC connection was observed using a lower threshold (Table S3 and Figure S3 in Supplementary). In general, right hippocampal-rPFC connectivity was negatively correlated with hippocampal volume (r = −0.330, p = 0.017, corrected for age, sex and education years Fig. 3e). Furthermore, this specific type of functional connectivity was correlated with the Self-rated Memory Appraisal domain (r = 0.420, p = 0.002, controlling for age, sex and education years Fig. 3f).

A model of competing brain-behavior relationships

We built an initial model based upon significant unadjusted bivariate correlations, depicted by two-way arrows among the variables of self memory appraisal, executive domain and memory domain scores, hippocampal volume, functional connectivity between right hippocampus and right prefrontal cortex (rHP-rPFC), age and education years (p < 0.05, Supplementary, Figure S4). Table S4 (Supplementary) shows the zero-order (unadjusted) correlations between managerial experience and these variables of interest. In our subsequent models, directionality from hippocampal volume to memory performance was preset because structural brain changes evolve over a relatively long time periods (Zhang et al. 2011; Zheng et al. 2012). Directionality of other inter-variable associations was not possible to assign and are depicted by two-way arrows.

Inter-relationships between the variables were next explored through a series of hierarchical multiple regression analyses designed to develop the most parsimonious model. For all models, demographic information (i.e., sex, age and education years) as well as managerial experience were entered into the model as a first step with a backwards elimination criterion, and then all four remaining independent variables (IVs) were entered in a second backwards elimination step (Table S5, statistic details provided in Supplementary).

Overall, the model suggested by these analyses is summarized in Fig. 4. Two major counter-veiling relationships were observed for midlife managerial experience. On the one hand, HME was related to better objectively-measured executive and memory performance, the latter mediated by protection of hippocampal volume. Formal mediator testing was completed using the Sobel test (Preacher and Hayes 2004). This test indicated that hippocampus volume positively mediates the relationship between managerial experience and objective memory performance (Mean = 0.751, 95%CI = [0.0067,0.1915]). On the other hand, HME also had a negative association with subjective memory appraisals, correlated with less hippocampal connectivity with mid-frontal cortex. We also tested a second formal mediating effect and found rHIP-rPFC functional connectivity positively mediates the link between managerial experience and self-rated memory appraisals (Mean = −0.2339, 95%CI = [−0.4296, −0.0552]). Age was negatively correlated with memory and executive performance, whereas education years were positively correlated with executive performance, but negatively related to self-rated memory appraisals. Multicollinearity was not a major issue in these models given a maximal variance inflation factor of 2.04 between managerial experience and memory performance.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Simplified model of midlife managerial experience and independent links with age and late life hippocampal volume, cognition, self-rated memory appraisals and functional connectivity. The arrows pointed from independent variable to dependent variable, and the value was the Beta value in the regression model. See Supplementary for details of structural equation analyses used to build this model

Comments

Managerial experience in midlife may exhibit powerful, competing and complex relationships with brain structure and function in late life. In a non-amnestic MCI cohort, we demonstrate for the first time a positive relationship between managerial experience and hippocampal volume. Preservation of hippocampal volume in HME group mediated their tendency to display superior memory function. Moreover, supervising workers was linked to differences in hippocampal morphology, reserving hippocampal volume in those subregions specifically vulnerable to neurodegeneration. These same individuals rated their own memory prospects worse, and these poor self-ratings were linked to lower hippocampal-prefrontal connectivity, a network implicated in self-perception. Management of large groups of employees during one’s working life may therefore benefit hippocampal structure and memory prowess in late life, but may link to poor subjective memory appraisal in late life and desynchronisation of related hippocampal networks.

Neuropathology and neuroimaging studies identify the hippocampus as one of the first brain regions affected in MCI and AD (Jack et al. 2010) (Braak and Braak 1991). For example, low hippocampal volume in non-demented elders independently predicts a 3-fold greater risk of incident dementia five years later (den Heijer et al. 2006). We found that the neuroprotective association between greater managerial experience and reduced hippocampal volume seen in healthy older adults (Suo et al. 2012) also appears to extend to those individuals with non-amnestic MCI. Similar cognitive lifestyle-related neuroprotective mechanism was recently reported on healthy (Arenaza-Urquijo et al. 2013) and AD cohorts (Shpanskaya et al. 2014) In contrast, the compensatory mechanism was supported by the reports of more global gray matter loss in elders with higher estimated cognitive reserve in amnestic MCI (Sole-Padulles et al. 2009), and AD patients (Seo et al. 2011) Further research is therefore required that contrasts cognitive lifestyle-related neuroprotective versus compensatory mechanisms in non-amnestic, amnestic MCI and AD.

Importantly, these neuroprotective findings appeared to be functionally relevant. In our hierarchical linear model, preserved hippocampal volume in HME group mediated a relative proficiency in memory tests. This is consistent with the functional role of this medial temporal lobe structure in memory networks (Ranganath and Ritchey 2012). Moreover, being in charge of a large group of workers was not only linked to gross volumetric advantages, but also subtle differences in morphometric patterns on the hippocampal surface. Participants with high-level managerial experience exhibited larger volume of the right CA1 hippocampal subregion, indicated by diminished involution of the lateral anterior body and hippocampal tail (Gerardin et al. 2009). These are the same regions specifically vulnerable to neurodegeneration in MCI and AD (Patenaude et al. 2011). In conjunction with our previous work (Suo et al. 2012), these findings suggest that employee supervision in midlife may help protect AD-susceptible hippocampal areas from neurodegeneration in later life, an effect that mediates a relative sparing of memory function in the setting of MCI. Multiple mechanisms, including both neuroprotective effects in the CA1 hippocampal subregion and cortical compensatory processes (Valenzuela et al. 2012) may therefore be implicated for explaining the strong and independent epidemiological link between work-related managerial experience and a 42 % lower risk for dementia (Schmand et al. 1997). Further, as explored in our previous work (Suo et al. 2012), managerial work is heavily reliant on cognitive processes such as linguistic competency, verbal comprehension and verbal memory (Boatsman et al. 1983; Christoffels et al. 2006) (Penley et al. 1991) (Bolton and Dewatripont 1994), processes that together may trigger long term neuroplastic and neuroprotective changes in the hippocampus. In addition, we previously found that managerial experience during working life tends to bias a person towards seeking out more social contacts in late life (Suo et al. 2012), a behavioral trait that is linked to lower dementia risk (Fratiglioni et al. 2000) and may protect the hippocampus (Fratiglioni et al. 2004). A similar result was again seen in this sample, and hence may represent a possible behavioral explanation for these long term associations.

Given these objective benefits, it was somewhat unexpected that occupational supervision was also linked to negative self-appraisal. Retired high-level managers exhibited consistently worse self-rated memory appraisals compared to the LME group. These ratings were assessed using the MARS-MFS, a sensitive instrument that asks individuals to predict their own future proficiency on day-to-day memory challenges, for example remembering a person’s name, being able to recount news stories, and following instructions of a short route. Whilst superior to their peers in the LME group on several memory tests, these individuals rated themselves 2.7-fold less capable. One possible technical explanation is that that MARS’ questions about predicted prospective memory focused on a memory domain that was simply not evaluated by our traditional objective memory tests, a question for future studies. Alternatively, individuals in HME may have dropped further from a higher premorbid level yet are still outperforming LME participants on objective tests, in effect a long term change that has triggered their concerns. Longitudinal data will be required to confirm this hypothesis. From a clinical perspective, since subjective memory complaint is a core criterion for MCI (irrespective of subtype), and is itself a significant independent predictor for incident dementia (Waldorff et al. 2012; Geerlings et al. 1999), ex-high level managers may be at higher risk for MCI by virtue of inaccurately low self-appraisals of their memory ability rather than memory impairment per se.

Interestingly, low self-appraisals in the HME group were associated with absent resting state functional connectivity between right hippocampus and right PFC, in contrast to positive connectivity and self-appraisals in LME group. There are a number of plausible reasons for why rHP-rPFC connectivity may potentially mediate these inter-relationships, suggested by the formal Sobel test of mediation. Firstly, right PFC (specifically BA10) has been heavily implicated in prospective memory paradigms (Burgess et al. 2007), which require a shift of perception from the immediate environment to an alternate imaged future environment inclusive of oneself (Buckner and Carroll 2007). The lateral part of BA10 has been specifically associated with delayed intentions and self-generated and self-maintained thought processes (Burgess et al. 2007). Secondly, the hippocampus and BA10 are both involved in decision-making tasks that require self appraisal (Schmitz and Johnson 2006). Thirdly, the structure of the default mode network at rest is similar to during self-projection processes such as prospection (the act of thinking about the future) (Buckner and Carroll 2007). The spontaneous right PFC -hippocampal connectivity we identified may potentially participate in a network related to mnemonic self-perception or prospective function. This hypothesis is consistent with our observation of an independent correlation between the strength of any such connectivity and better self-rated perceptions about future memory proficiency. Interestingly, Wang et al. (Wang et al. 2011) found decreased functional connectivity between the right hippocampus and prefrontal areas (BA10 and BA11) when comparing MCI participants to age-matched healthy participants. As such, deterioration of this functional network may be an early non-cognitive biomarker of dementia risk, linked in our study to low memory-related perceptions. Clearly, this hypothesis requires further research.

The most striking aspect of our findings was the independent and contrary relationship of midlife managerial experience on objective memory function and hippocampal structure on the one hand, and worse memory self-ratings and hippocampal-PFC connectivity on the other. These results are germane to the longstanding controversy over the relative importance of subjective versus objective memory in the older adult (Jorm et al. 1997) (Rabbitt and Abson 1991) (Perlmutter 1978). In the specific context of occupational history, a longitudinal study of retired managers noted that memory self-ratings were not correlated with age-related changes in objective memory performances (Poitrenaud et al. 1989). A large epidemiological study further suggests that the discrepancy between subjective and objective cognitive impairment was influenced by a person’s social environment – a wider social environment may lead to more opportunities in which cognitive function is ‘tested’ against peers and hence personally perceived (Trouton et al. 2006). Other explanations are possible, including ex-high level managers developing higher standards of self-assessment, a type of ‘cognitive hyper-vigilance’, or alternatively personality traits may interact with vocational choice and subsequent self-appraisals in the context of age-related change. Based on our data we can, however, exclude a number of potential explanations given there were no managerial group differences in depressive symptoms, general physical activity, socioeconomic status, retirement status, general cognitive lifestyle activity or presence of different cardiovascular risk factors.

This study also had some limitations. There was for example an imbalance in sub-group size in our cohort in favour of LME. This may of course be related to the protective effect of HME being linked to lower risk for dementia (Schmand et al. 1997), but our cross-sectional study cannot clarify the issue. Similarly, our model tested for cross-sectional associations between variables, and so the possibility of reverse causation or residual confounds cannot be excluded. For example, factors outside of our models (e.g., personality, early life activities or stress) may affect brain structure or function throughout the lifespan and may also be associated with managerial experience. Longitudinal data are needed to identify possible causal relationships. Also, our participants were non-amnestic MCI. Considering the aetiology of naMCI is even less well understood than aMCI and is likely to reflect both Alzheimer and non-Alzheimer pathology, the generalizability of these findings to amnestic MCI is not clear.

In conclusion, we found a clear double dissociation related to midlife managerial experience. On average, ex-high level managers had a larger and less AD-like hippocampus compared to individuals with low managerial experience, a difference that formally mediated superior mnemonic function. By contrast, they also made more pessimistic predictions about their own future memory proficiency, correlated to diminished hippocampal-prefrontal connectivity at rest. Hierarchical regression clarified these complex relationships, suggesting both positive and negative long term links between midlife managerial experiences and brain structure and function in late life.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

ESM 1 (1.2MB, doc)

(DOC 1238 kb)

ESM 2 (454.3KB, gif)

(GIF 454 kb)

Acknowledgments

This work fulfilled a portion of the degree requirements for PhD for Nicola Gates and Chao Suo. We would also like to thank Brooke McMahon for assistance with recruitment and training, Dominique Williamson, Dr. Shelley Kaye, and Dr. Angela Wright for assistance with assessments, Brad Lloyd, and Dr. Angela Wright for their assistance with Manual of Procedure preparation, Kate Bradshaw for her assistance with recruitment, Dr. John Crawford for his assistance with statistical analyses. We would also like to thank the participants and their informants who devoted their time to this project.

Compliance with ethical standards

Funding

This study was funded by a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) of Australia Dementia Research Grant, project grant ID No. 512672 from 2008–2011. Additional funding for a research assistant position was sourced from the NHMRC Program Grant ID No. 568969, and the project was supported by the University of Sydney and University of New South Wales. MV was supported by a University of New South Wales Vice Chancellor’s Fellowship and a NHMRC Clinical Career Development Fellowship (1004156).

Conflict of interest

The authors have no competing interests.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

References

  1. Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, Dubois B, Feldman HH, Fox NC, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Landeau B, La Joie R, Mevel K, Mezenge F, Perrotin A, et al. Relationships between years of education and gray matter volume, metabolism and functional connectivity in healthy elders. NeuroImage. 2013;83:450–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bennett DA, Arnold SE, Valenzuela MJ, Brayne C, Schneider JA. Cognitive and social lifestyle: links with neuropathology and cognition in late life. Acta Neuropathologica. 2014;127(1):137–150. doi: 10.1007/s00401-013-1226-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Boatsman JR, Hansen DR, Kimbrell JI. Negotiating corporate combinations - a theory and evidence. Journal of Management. 1983;9(2):127–143. doi: 10.1177/014920638300900205. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bolton P, Dewatripont M. The firm as a communication network. The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 1994;109(4):809–839. doi: 10.2307/2118349. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathologica. 1991;82(4):239–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brayne C, Ince PG, Keage HA, McKeith IG, Matthews FE, Polvikoski T, et al. Education, the brain and dementia: neuroprotection or compensation? Brain. 2010;133(Pt 8):2210–2216. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Buckner RL, Carroll DC. Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007;11(2):49–57. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Burgess PW, Gilbert SJ, Dumontheil I. Function and localization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences. 2007;362(1481):887–899. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chao-Gan Y, Yu-Feng Z. DPARSF: a MATLAB toolbox for “pipeline” data analysis of resting-state fMRI. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2010;4:13. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Christoffels IK, de Groot AMB, Kroll JF. Memory and language skills in simultaneous interpreters: the role of expertise and language proficiency. Journal of Memory and Language. 2006;54(3):324–345. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.12.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Clare L, Wilson BA, Carter G, Roth I, Hodges JR. Assessing awareness in early-stage Alzheimer’s disease: development and piloting of the memory awareness rating scale. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 2002;12(4):341–362. doi: 10.1080/09602010244000129. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Coffey CE, Saxton JA, Ratcliff G, Bryan RN, Lucke JF. Relation of education to brain size in normal aging: implications for the reserve hypothesis. Neurology. 1999;53(1):189–196. doi: 10.1212/WNL.53.1.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. den Heijer T, Geerlings MI, Hoebeek FE, Hofman A, Koudstaal PJ, Breteler MM. Use of hippocampal and amygdalar volumes on magnetic resonance imaging to predict dementia in cognitively intact elderly people. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63(1):57–62. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.1.57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108(7):3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Frankó E, Joly O, for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging, I. Evaluating Alzheimer’s disease progression using rate of regional hippocampal atrophy. PLoS One. 2013;8(8):e71354. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Fratiglioni L, Wang HX, Ericsson K, Maytan M, Winblad B. Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: a community -based longitudinal study. Lancet. 2000;355:1315–1319. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02113-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fratiglioni L, Paillard-Borg S, Winblad B. An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurology. 2004;3(6):343–353. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00767-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gates NJ, Sachdev PS, Fiatarone Singh MA, Valenzuela M. Cognitive and memory training in adults at risk of dementia: a systematic review. BMC Geriatrics. 2011;11:55. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-11-55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gates NJ, Valenzuela M, Sachdev PS, Singh NA, Baune BT, Brodaty H, et al. Study of Mental Activity and Regular Training (SMART) in at risk individuals: a randomised double blind, sham controlled, longitudinal trial. BMC Geriatrics. 2011;11:19. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-11-19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Geerlings MI, Jonker C, Bouter LM, Ader HJ, Schmand B. Association between memory complaints and incident Alzheimer’s disease in elderly people with normal baseline cognition. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156(4):531–537. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.4.531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gerardin E, Chetelat G, Chupin M, Cuingnet R, Desgranges B, Kim HS, et al. Multidimensional classification of hippocampal shape features discriminates Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment from normal aging. NeuroImage. 2009;47(4):1476–1486. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jack CR, Jr, Petersen RC, Xu Y, O’Brien PC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, et al. Rates of hippocampal atrophy correlate with change in clinical status in aging and AD. Neurology. 2000;55(4):484–489. doi: 10.1212/WNL.55.4.484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Jack CR, Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, Shaw LM, Aisen PS, Weiner MW, et al. Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer’s pathological cascade. Lancet Neurology. 2010;9(1):119–128. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70299-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Johnstone T, Ores Walsh KS, Greischar LL, Alexander AL, Fox AS, Davidson RJ, et al. Motion correction and the use of motion covariates in multiple-subject fMRI analysis. Human Brain Mapping. 2006;27(10):779–788. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Jorm AF, Christensen H, Korten AE, Henderson AS, Jacomb PA, Mackinnon A. Do cognitive complaints either predict future cognitive decline or reflect past cognitive decline? A longitudinal study of an elderly community sample. Psychological Medicine. 1997;27(1):91–98. doi: 10.1017/S0033291796003923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kidron D, Black SE, Stanchev P, Buck B, Szalai JP, Parker J, et al. Quantitative MR volumetry in Alzheimer’s disease. Topographic markers and the effects of sex and education. Neurology. 1997;49(6):1504–1512. doi: 10.1212/WNL.49.6.1504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kim B, Boes JL, Bland PH, Chenevert TL, Meyer CR. Motion correction in fMRI via registration of individual slices into an anatomical volume. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 1999;41(5):964–972. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1522-2594(199905)41:5&#x0003c;964::AID-MRM16&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-D. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Liu Y, Wang K, Yu C, He Y, Zhou Y, Liang M, et al. Regional homogeneity, functional connectivity and imaging markers of Alzheimer’s disease: a review of resting-state fMRI studies. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(6):1648–1656. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Marioni RE, Valenzuela MJ, van den Hout A, Brayne C, Matthews FE, Function MRCC, et al. Active cognitive lifestyle is associated with positive cognitive health transitions and compression of morbidity from age sixty-five. PLoS One. 2012;7(12):e50940. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Marioni RE, van den Hout A, Valenzuela MJ, Brayne C, Matthews FE. Active cognitive lifestyle associates with cognitive recovery and a reduced risk of cognitive decline. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2012;28(1):223–230. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-110377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Patenaude B, Smith SM, Kennedy DN, Jenkinson M. A Bayesian model of shape and appearance for subcortical brain segmentation. NeuroImage. 2011;56(3):907–922. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Penley LE, Alexander ER, Jernigan IE, Henwood CI. Communication abilities of managers - the relationship to performance. Journal of Management. 1991;17(1):57–76. doi: 10.1177/014920639101700105. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Perlmutter M. What is memory aging aging of. Developmental Psychology. 1978;14(4):330–345. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.14.4.330. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine. 2004;256(3):183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Poitrenaud J, Malbezin M, Guez D. Self-rating and psychometric assessment of age-related-changes in memory among young-elderly managers. Developmental Neuropsychology. 1989;5(4):285–294. doi: 10.1080/87565648909540439. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 2004;36(4):717–731. doi: 10.3758/BF03206553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Pyo G, Elble RJ, Ala T, Markwell SJ. The characteristics of patients with uncertain/mild cognitive impairment on the Alzheimer disease assessment scale-cognitive subscale. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 2006;20(1):16–22. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000201846.22213.76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Rabbitt P, Abson V. Do older people know how good they are? British Journal of Psychology. 1991;82(Pt 2):137–151. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1991.tb02389.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ranganath C, Ritchey M. Two cortical systems for memory-guided behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13(10):713–726. doi: 10.1038/nrn3338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Risacher SL, Shen L, West JD, Kim S, McDonald BC, Beckett LA, et al. Longitudinal MRI atrophy biomarkers: relationship to conversion in the ADNI cohort. Neurobiology of Aging. 2010;31(8):1401–1418. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.04.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Schmand B, Smit JH, Geerlings MI, Lindeboom J. The effects of intelligence and education on the development of dementia. A test of the brain reserve hypothesis. Psychological Medicine. 1997;27(6):1337–1344. doi: 10.1017/S0033291797005461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Schmitz TW, Johnson SC. Self-appraisal decisions evoke dissociated dorsal-ventral aMPFC networks. NeuroImage. 2006;30(3):1050–1058. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Seo SW, Im K, Lee JM, Kim ST, Ahn HJ, Go SM, et al. Effects of demographic factors on cortical thickness in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging. 2011;32(2):200–209. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Shi F, Liu B, Zhou Y, Yu C, Jiang T. Hippocampal volume and asymmetry in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: Meta-analyses of MRI studies. Hippocampus. 2009;19(11):1055–1064. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Shpanskaya KS, Choudhury KR, Hostage C, Jr, Murphy KR, Petrella JR, Doraiswamy PM. Educational attainment and hippocampal atrophy in the Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative cohort. Journal of Neuroradiology. 2014;41(5):350–357. doi: 10.1016/j.neurad.2013.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sole-Padulles C, Bartres-Faz D, Junque C, Vendrell P, Rami L, Clemente IC, et al. Brain structure and function related to cognitive reserve variables in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging. 2009;30(7):1114–1124. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Stern Y, Alexander G, Stricks L, Link B, Mayeux R. Relationship between lifetime occupation and parietal flow: implications for a reserve against Alzheimer’s disease pathology. Neurology. 1995;45:55–60. doi: 10.1212/WNL.45.1.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sudo F, Oliveira Alves CE, Sousa Alves GS, Ericeira-Valente L, Tiel C, Moreira D, et al. Dysexecutive syndrome and cerebrovascular disease in non-amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review of the literature. Dement Neuropsychol. 2012;6(3):145–151. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642012DN06030006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Suo C, Leon I, Brodaty H, Trollor J, Wen W, Sachdev P, et al. Supervisory experience at work is linked to low rate of hippocampal atrophy in late life. NeuroImage. 2012;63(3):1542–1551. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Suo C, Singh MF, Gates N, Wen W, Sachdev P, Brodaty H, et al. Therapeutically relevant structural and functional mechanisms triggered by physical and cognitive exercise. Molecular Psychiatry. 2016 doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Trouton A, Stewart R, Prince M. Does social activity influence the accuracy of subjective memory deficit? Findings from a British community survey. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2006;54(7):1108–1113. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2006.00800.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Valenzuela MJ, Sachdev P. Brain reserve and dementia: a systematic review. Psychological Medicine. 2006;36(4):441–454. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Valenzuela MJ, Sachdev P. Assessment of complex mental activity across the lifespan: development of the Lifetime of Experiences Questionnaire (LEQ) Psychological Medicine. 2007;37(7):1015–1025. doi: 10.1017/S003329170600938X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Valenzuela MJ, Sachdev P, Wen W, Chen X, Brodaty H. Lifespan mental activity predicts diminished rate of hippocampal atrophy. PLoS One. 2008;3(7):e2598. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Valenzuela M, Brayne C, Sachdev P, Wilcock G, Matthews F. Cognitive lifestyle and long-term risk of dementia and survival after diagnosis in a multicenter population-based cohort. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2011;173(9):1004–1012. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwq476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Valenzuela MJ, Matthews FE, Brayne C, Ince P, Halliday G, Kril JJ, et al. Multiple biological pathways link cognitive lifestyle to protection from dementia. Biological Psychiatry. 2012;71(9):783–791. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.07.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Valenzuela MJ, Leon I, Suo C, Piamba DM, Kochan N, Brodaty H, et al. Cognitive lifestyle in older persons: the population-based sydney memory and ageing study. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2013;36(1):87–97. doi: 10.3233/JAD-130143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Waldorff FB, Siersma V, Vogel A, Waldemar G. Subjective memory complaints in general practice predicts future dementia: a 4-year follow-up study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2012;27(11):1180–1188. doi: 10.1002/gps.3765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wang Z, Liang P, Jia X, Qi Z, Yu L, Yang Y, et al. Baseline and longitudinal patterns of hippocampal connectivity in mild cognitive impairment: evidence from resting state fMRI. Journal of Neurological Sciences. 2011;309(1–2):79–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2011.07.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Wong CH, Lam LC, Lui VW, Chiu HF, Chan SS, Tam CW. Subjective complaints and self-evaluation of memory test performance in Questionable dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2006;21(10):937–944. doi: 10.1002/gps.1586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Zhang H, Sachdev PS, Wen W, Kochan NA, Zhu W, Crawford JD, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive performance in late life. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2011;32(3):216–226. doi: 10.1159/000333372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Zheng JJ, Delbaere K, Close JC, Sachdev PS, Wen W, Lord SR. White matter hyperintensities and impaired choice stepping reaction time in older people. Neurobiology of Aging. 2012;33(7):1177–1185. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

ESM 1 (1.2MB, doc)

(DOC 1238 kb)

ESM 2 (454.3KB, gif)

(GIF 454 kb)


Articles from Brain Imaging and Behavior are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES