Abstract
Objective
We examined whether lifetime racial discrimination and stigma consciousness (expecting to be stigmatized) are associated with blood pressure in minority and white middle aged and older adult men.
Design
Participants were 1533 men (mean age=63.2 [SD=7.9, range = 37.4–89.2]; 12.4% black, 7.8% Hispanic, 2.0% other) diagnosed with clinically localized prostate cancer. We separately modeled associations between discrimination/stigma consciousness and blood pressure outcomes for minorities and whites controlling for education, income, employment status, age, marital status, BMI, and recruitment site.
Results
Minorities reported more racial discrimination and stigma consciousness than Whites (ps<.001). For minorities, having experienced more racial discrimination was associated with having higher diastolic blood pressure (B=0.15, p=.016) and having greater stigma consciousness was associated with greater odds of having hypertension (OR=1.04, p=.047). Greater stigma consciousness was associated with lower systolic blood pressure in Whites (B=−0.24, p=.012).
Conclusion
Discrimination and stigma consciousness are associated with common risk factors for chronic disease and premature death that disproportionately affect minorities. Findings for stigma consciousness suggest that anticipatory vigilance may be impacting minority health.
Keywords: discrimination, racism, race/ethnicity, hypertension, blood pressure, stigma consciousness
Introduction
Blacks have a higher incidence of hypertension and pre-hypertension than non-Hispanic Whites [1, 2]. Recent data comparing rates of hypertension and uncontrolled hypertension suggest that Mexican Americans also have higher rates than non-Hispanic Whites [3]. Unless managed, hypertension can lead to life-threatening conditions, including cardiovascular disease, kidney disease, and other morbidities [4–8]. Racial/ethnic discrimination has been identified as a potential source of chronic stress that could increase blood pressure over time and put minorities at risk for hypertension. Racial/ethnic discrimination includes unjust, unfair and prejudicial treatment of individuals grounded in their categorization into socially constructed racial/ethnic categories. Although stemming from cultural beliefs about essential biological differences between these groups, a theory of racial/ethnic discrimination does not itself purport that these biological differences are real.
There is some, although not universal support for the hypothesis that racial/ethnic discrimination is a risk factor for hypertension. In experimental studies, exposure to racism-related stimuli results in elevated cardiac responses, including temporary increases in blood pressure in Blacks [9]. In addition, a number of studies have demonstrated a relationship between everyday experiences of discrimination and higher ambulatory blood pressure in Blacks and Hispanics [10–13]. Cardiac reactivity to discrimination stressors is a plausible mechanism underlying hypertension, given evidence from prospective longitudinal studies that cardiac reactivity predicts risk for developing hypertension years, if not decades later [14, 15]. A number of studies have examined whether there is a correlation between frequency of, or lifetime discrimination and blood pressure or diagnosis of hypertension. Results have been mixed, with some studies having demonstrated positive relations for Blacks and Hispanics [16–18]. Also, in some studies, minorities reporting the lowest and highest discrimination have the highest blood pressure (u-shaped relationship) [19, 20]. The latter has typically been explained as capturing defensive processing or internalized racism. It is hypothesized that those who report the lowest discrimination are coping using denial or internalization which may be more physically harmful than acknowledging and actively coping with discrimination [21]. Finally, a number of studies have failed to find any relationship between discrimination and blood pressure, although in many instances study samples have been small (<200) [22, 23] or relatively young [20, 22, 23]. It is possible that the accumulative impact of racism may take decades to result in hypertension. In individuals aged 18 to 39, prevalence of hypertension range from 3–11% based on sex and race/ethnicity, increases to 22–49% in those aged 40–59 and jumps to 62–83% in individuals 60 years and older [24]. Samples that include primarily younger and middle-aged adults might not be well suited for detecting effects. Consistent with this notion, in a study of older adults, frequency of everyday discrimination was associated with diastolic BP in Blacks (and not Whites) [18]. The Jackson Heart Study (N = 4939) also detected an association between discrimination and hypertension. The mean ages of women and men in the study were 55 and 54 [16].
In the present study, we tested associations between lifetime discrimination and blood pressure and hypertension in a sample of middle aged and older adult men. We did so in men who had been diagnosed with clinically localized prostate cancer, and were recruited to take part in a study of prostate cancer treatment decision making. One in seven men will be diagnosed with prostate cancer and the risk for Blacks is greater, nearly one in five [25]. It is a common, non-emergent diagnosis that is unlikely to impact relationships between accumulated lifetime experiences of discrimination and blood pressure. This study overcomes the shortcomings of younger samples for which rates of hypertension are low.
We also expand on recent work that is beginning to explore multiple facets of racism. The psychophysiological impact of racism may not be restricted to the discriminatory events themselves, but may also include anticipatory vigilance for discrimination as well as subsequent appraisals or rumination about experienced discrimination [26]. Anticipating discriminatory interpersonal situations has been associated with higher risk for hypertension among Blacks [27] and Latinas [28]. Chronic, even preconscious [29] vigilance for unjust or otherwise discriminatory behavior has been associated with negative health outcomes, including cardiac reactivity [28] and depression [30]. In the present study, in addition to examining the role of experienced discrimination in hypertension, we examine the potential that anticipatory discrimination, assessed via expectations of being stereotyped in interactions with others based on a salient group identity, is associated with higher blood pressure.
Finally, the relationship between discrimination and blood pressure has been studied almost exclusively in Blacks and Whites and rarely in Hispanics. We included Hispanics and other minorities in our study. In sum, the present study helps clarify whether there is a relationship between racial/ethnic discrimination and elevated systolic and diastolic blood pressure and the likelihood of having been diagnosed with hypertension in middle-aged and older minority men.
Methods
Procedure
Data were from participants who had been diagnosed with clinically localized prostate cancer and were taking part in a larger study of prostate cancer treatment decision-making and survivorship in which they were assessed prior to, and every six months for two years following treatment. Participants were recruited from 2 academic cancer centers and 3 community practices in four metropolitan regions. Participants were recruited on average, 47.9 (SD= 62.7) days after diagnosis.. They were approached in-person when they came into the clinic for a post-biopsy or second opinion consultation, or less commonly, a follow-up visit. Participants completed a baseline questionnaire during this visit, or if they did not complete the survey in clinic they were permitted to complete it at home and return it by mail. All participants completed the baseline prior to being treated. Participants were included in the present analyses if they had completed the baseline questionnaire that included questions about experiences of racial discrimination and stigma consciousness, a measure of anticipatory discrimination, as well as demographic characteristics, and if clinical data had been abstracted from their medical records (diagnosis of hypertension, systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, weight and height). Study procedures were institutional review board-approved.
Participants
Between July 2010 and May 2014, we approached 3337 patients, of whom, 2476 were consented to the study (74%) and 2,008 completed the first questionnaire (81% of consented participants). Of these, 456 were excluded because they did not have chart-abstracted clinical data at the time the analyses were conducted. Nineteen participants were excluded because they did not self-report their race/ethnicity, yielding a final sample of 1,533 men. In this sample, the percentage of missing for other variables were 0.7% for education, 0.2% for employment status, 0.2% for marital status, 2.3% for BMI, 2.0% for stigma consciousness, and 2.0% for systolic and diastolic blood pressure. There were no missing values for age or discrimination.
Measures
Predictor variables
Lifetime Racial/Ethnic Discrimination was assessed with the Experiences of Discrimination scale (test-retest reliability = 0.70) [31]. It ascertains how often people have experienced discrimination in 9 situations (e.g., workplace, store, school, medical setting; never/once/two or three times/four or more times) and then asks people to attribute the experiences to one main cause (e.g., race/ethnicity, age, disability). For those who reported any discrimination and attributed it to their race/ethnicity, their score was the sum of the frequencies for all the situations in which they experienced discrimination. If a participant did not report discrimination or reported discrimination but attributed it to something other than race/ethnicity, he received a score of 0. Scores could range from 0 to 40. The measure also assesses how people coped with the discrimination; however, this second part of the questionnaire was not used in the present analyses.
Stigma consciousness was assessed with the 10-item Stigma Consciousness Questionnaire (α = 0.78) that is intended to assess individual differences in the degree to which people are chronically self-conscious of their stigmatized status [32], and is associated with vigilance for conscious and non-conscious cues that threaten social identity [29]. Example items include, “When interacting with people of races/ethnicities different from my own, I feel like they interpret all my behaviors in terms of my race/ethnicity.” Whereas in the original questionnaire, items are answered using a 7-point scale [32], we used a 5-point response option that was the same as the one used for several other questionnaire items (1 = strongly agree… 5 = strongly disagree). Scores were summed and could range from 0 to 40.
Blood pressure outcomes
Self-reported diagnosis of hypertension and clinic-assessed systolic blood pressure, and diastolic blood pressure were abstracted from participants’ clinic notes for the date on, or closest to the date that they were consented to the study. Blood pressure measures would always have been assessed prior to treatment. The information would typically have been recorded by a nurse in the clinic intake medical history. Participants were classified as hypertensive if they reported a diagnosis of hypertension or had a systolic blood pressure > 140 mm Hg, or a diastolic blood pressure > 90 mm Hg.
Demographic and clinical covariates
Participants self-reported years of education completed, household income (<$25,000 / $25,000–49,999 / $50,000–74,999 / ≥$75,000), marital status (married or cohabitating vs. single, divorced or widowed), employment status (employed vs. not employed), and date of birth from which we calculated age at diagnosis. Participants also self-reported their race/ethnicity (White, Black or African American, American Indian or Alaska Native, Asian, Middle Eastern (recoded as White) or Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander and Hispanic or Latino vs. not). Racial/ethnic categories except White were collapsed into a minority category due to small sample size in each group. All participants regardless of race who identified as Hispanic or Latino were also included in the minority category. For the sake of brevity, non-Hispanic White is hereafter referred to as White; non-Hispanic Black is referred to as Black, and Hispanic of any race is referred to as Hispanic. Height and weight were abstracted from participants’ medical records and body mass index (BMI) was calculated from height (in) and weight (lb) ((weight/(height2) × 703).
Statistical Analyses
The variable income had 16 % missing data. We imputed income using multiple imputation (30 imputations). Multiple imputation is considered one of the best procedures for addressing data that are missing at random [33].
We tested for differences in racial discrimination, stigma consciousness, and covariates between Whites and minorities as well as between Whites and each racial/ethnic group with t-tests and chi-square tests. For describing the sample (Table 1), years of education was recoded into four categories (<high school / high school / college />college); however, years of education, a continuous variable ranging from 4 to 20, was entered into multivariable analyses as a single continuous variable. We explored which covariates were associated with discrimination or stigma consciousness with multivariable linear regression models containing all covariates (Table 2). Multivariable analyses were conducted separately for minorities and Whites as we expected discrimination and stigma consciousness to only be associated with health outcomes in minorities. We examined linear relationships between lifetime frequency of racial discrimination and stigma consciousness with diagnosis of hypertension, systolic blood pressure and diastolic blood pressure. Given that a u-shaped relationship between discrimination and blood pressure has been reported in some previous studies, we also tested whether there were quadratic effects of discrimination and stigma consciousness on blood pressure outcomes. All analyses were conducted using logistic regression for hypertension (binary outcome) and multiple linear regression for continuous outcomes (blood pressure), and with robust standard errors. We also explored whether any associations varied across the minority groups (Black vs. Hispanic vs. other) by adding interaction terms for race/ethnicity × discrimination or stigma consciousness as well as testing the models in the Black and Hispanic subsamples. All models included years of education, income, employment status, age, marital status, BMI, and recruitment site as control variables. We used Stata (Version 13, StataCorp) to complete all analyses.
Table 1.
Characteristic | Whites (n = 1193) [N, % or Mean (SD)] |
Minorities (n = 340) [N, % or Mean (SD)] |
p-value | Total sample (n = 1533) [N, % or Mean (SD)] |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Race/Ethnicity | |||||||
White | NA | NA | NA | NA | NA | 1193 | 77.82 % |
Black | NA | NA | NA | NA | 190 | 12.39 % | |
Hispanic | NA | NA | NA | NA | 120 | 7.83 % | |
Other | NA | NA | NA | NA | 30 | 1.96 % | |
Mean Age | 1190 | 63.58 (7.77) | 340 | 61.85 (8.32) | <.001 | 1533 | 63.19 (7.92) |
Educationa | |||||||
<High school | 27 | 2.28 % | 32 | 9.50 % | <.001 | 59 | 3.87 % |
High school | 212 | 17.88 % | 72 | 21.36 % | 284 | 18.65 % | |
College | 537 | 45.28 % | 152 | 45.10 % | 689 | 45.24 % | |
>College | 410 | 34.57 % | 81 | 24.04 % | 491 | 32.24 % | |
Income | |||||||
<$25,000 | 51 | 5.06 % | 45 | 15.57 % | <.001 | 96 | 7.41 % |
$25,000–49,999 | 116 | 11.52 % | 64 | 22.15 % | 180 | 13.89 % | |
$50,000–74,999 | 149 | 14.80 % | 40 | 13.84 % | 189 | 14.58 % | |
≥$75,000 | 691 | 68.62 % | 140 | 48.44 % | 831 | 64.12 % | |
Employed | 714 | 59.95 % | 190 | 56.05 % | .20 | 904 | 59.08 % |
Married | 1013 | 84.98 % | 260 | 76.92 % | <.001 | 1530 | 83.20 % |
Hypertension | 747 | 62.77 % | 241 | 70.88 % | .01 | 1530 | 64.58 % |
Mean Systolic BPb | 1170 | 134.36 (15.84) | 333 | 134.12 (15.28) | .80 | 1503 | 134.31 (15.71) |
Mean Diastolic BPb |
1170 | 79.23 (9.83) | 333 | 80.41 (10.03) | .06 | 1503 | 79.49 (9.89) |
Mean BMIc | 1165 | 28.89 (4.67) | 333 | 29.64 (5.41) | .01 | 1498 | 29.06 (4.85) |
Racial/ethnic discrimination |
1193 | 0.60 (2.57) | 340 | 7.75 | <.001 | 1533 | 2.19 (5.83) |
Stigma consciousness |
1169 | 14.87 (5.03) | 334 | 18.18 (6.27) | <.001 | 1503 | 15.60 (5.50) |
Note. Cells may not sum to the total N for each column due to missing data; percentages are for available data.
For the purpose of describing the sample, we subdivided years of education into corresponding degree categories illustrated in this table; however, years of education was entered as a continuous variable in all multivariable linear regression analyses;
blood pressure;
body mass index
Table 2.
Demographic factor |
Discrimination B (95% CI) |
Stigma Consciousness B (95% CI) |
||
---|---|---|---|---|
Minority | White | Minority | White | |
Race/Ethnicity | ||||
Hispanic | −6.38 (−8.40, −4.36)*** | NA | −3.09 (−4.50, −1.69)*** | NA |
Other | −7.45 (−10.92, −3.98)*** | −3.40 (−5.82, −0.99)** | ||
Age | −0.00 (−0.12, 0.12) | −0.01 (−0.04, 0.01) | −0.08 (−0.16, 0.01) | −0.02 (−0.06, 0.03) |
Education | 0.43 (0.09, 0.77)* | 0.04 (−0.01, 0.10) | 0.10 (−0.13, 0.34) | −0.09 (−0.20, 0.03) |
Income | 0.58 (0.03, 1.13)* | 0.01 (−0.11, 0.13) | −0.01 (−0.38, 0.36) | −0.07 (−0.30, 0.16) |
Employed | −1.71 (−3.84, ,0.43) | −0.26 (−0.62, 0.11) | −0.13 (−1.60, 1.34) | −0.13 (−0.86, 0.60) |
Married | −4.16 (−6.57, −1.75)** | 0.17 (−0.27, 0.61) | −2.42 (−4.10, −0.74)* | −0.77, (−1.64, 0.11) |
p<.05,
p<.01,
p<.001;
Reference group for Race/Ethnicity was African American
Results
Sample Characteristics and Demographic Factors Associated with Discrimination and Stigma Consciousness
Sample characteristics for Whites, minorities, and the sample as a whole are found in Table 1. Minorities experienced more discrimination and stigma consciousness than Whites. Whites and minorities also varied on several covariates; on average, minorities were less educated, had lower incomes, were less likely to be married, were younger and had slightly higher BMIs. They also were more likely to have been diagnosed with hypertension; however, mean systolic and diastolic blood pressure did not differ across groups. Discrimination and stigma consciousness were correlated, albeit more strongly for minorities (r = 0.45, p <.001) than Whites (r = 0.16, p < .001).
Among minorities, higher income and education were associated with experiencing relatively more lifetime discrimination and greater stigma consciousness and being Hispanic or other compared to Black and being married were associated with experiencing less discrimination and having lower stigma consciousness (Table 2).
Multivariable Associations between Lifetime Racial Discrimination / Stigma Consciousness and Health Outcomes
Lifetime racial discrimination
For minorities, lifetime racial discrimination was positively associated with diastolic blood pressure (B = 0.15, 95% CI = 0.03, 0.27, p = .02) but not hypertension or systolic blood pressure. Racial discrimination was not associated with any outcomes in Whites (Table 3). There was a marginally significant quadratic association between discrimination and diastolic blood pressure in minorities (B = −0.01 (95% CI = −0.02, 0.00), p = .05. All other quadratic effects were non-significant, ps>.12).
Table 3.
Hypertensiona OR (95% CI) |
Systolic blood pressureb B (95% CI) |
Diastolic blood pressureb B (95% CI) |
|
---|---|---|---|
Minorities | |||
Discrimination | 1.01 (0.98, 1.04) | 0.02 (−0.15, 0.19) | 0.15* (0.03, 0.27) |
Stigma consciousness |
1.04* (1.00, 1.09) | −0.11 (−0.37, 0.15) | 0.14 (−0.03 0.32) |
Whites | |||
Discrimination | 0.99 (0.94, 1.04) | −0.20 (−0.51, 0.11) | 0.09 (−0.10, 0.28) |
Stigma consciousness |
0.98 (0.95, 1.00) | −0.24* (−0.42, −0.05) | −0.07 (−0.18, 0.04) |
Note.
Models tested with multivariable logistic regression;
Models tested with multivariable linear regression;
p< .05;
Models controlled for education, income, employment status, age, marital status, and recruitment site
Stigma consciousness
In minorities, stigma consciousness was associated with greater likelihood of having been diagnosed with hypertension (OR = 1.04, 95% CI = 1.00, 1.09, p = .05), but not associated with higher diastolic or systolic blood pressure. Greater stigma consciousness was associated with lower systolic blood pressure (B = −0.24, 95% CI = −0.4, −0.05, p = .01) in Whites, but was not related to other health outcomes in Whites (Table 3). There were no quadratic effects for stigma consciousness (results not shown; ps>.38).
Did effects vary by race/ethnicity?
Neither discrimination nor stigma consciousness interacted with minority race/ethnicity to predict blood pressure outcomes (ps > .11). When we analyzed Blacks and Hispanics separately, we found that associations between discrimination /stigma consciousness and hypertension and blood pressure were positive but did not reach statistical significance (ps = .08–.97).
Discussion
Experiencing more racial discrimination during one’s lifetime was associated with having higher diastolic blood pressure for minorities but not Whites. Minorities who reported more than 3.5 incidences of discrimination during their lifetimes, the median number for minorities, had, on average, a diastolic blood pressure 3 mm Hg above those who reported fewer incidences of discrimination. Discrimination was not associated with systolic blood pressure in minorities; Lewis and colleagues also found that in a sample of older Blacks, discrimination was associated with diastolic but not systolic blood pressure [18]. We know less about why a social stressor might have an effect on diastolic blood pressure but not systolic blood pressure [18] than its consequences. Elevated diastolic blood pressure is associated with excess morbidity and death due to a host of cardiovascular outcomes, including coronary and cerebrovascular events, angina, and aortic aneurysm [34].
We detected a relationship between racial discrimination and blood pressure, whereas some other studies have not. One reason for this may be how we measured discrimination. Lifetime discrimination, rather than current experiences of everyday discrimination, may be more strongly associated with blood pressure outcomes given the hypothesis that it is, in part, repeated exaggerated cardiac responses to stress over time that ultimately results in hypertension. Studies in which associations between recent and lifetime discrimination and health outcomes have been compared provide some support. Lifetime, but not everyday discrimination was associated with hypertension in the Jackson Heart Study [16]. Similarly, chronic, but not recent exposure to discrimination was associated with coronary artery calcification in Black women [35]. It bears mentioning that this pattern may not hold for outcomes more likely to be affected by recent exposures to racism such as health behavior, and exceptions have been noted [36].
An important novel finding is that stigma consciousness was associated with hypertension in the minority men in our study. This suggests that the chronic stress associated with discrimination might involve not only reactivity to unfair events, but also anticipating unfair treatment. Stigma consciousness may result in individuals detecting threats more frequently [37], and therefore creating more opportunities to elicit stress responses [38]. It may also be associated with the chronic stress of constant vigilance for social identity and exclusion threats. For example, it has been proposed that hypervigilance results in chronic activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal and ultimately, excess release of system-damaging stress hormones [39].
Why Whites with higher stigma consciousness might have lower systolic blood pressure is also an interesting question. The Stigma Consciousness Questionnaire is designed to assess chronically activated expectations of being stigmatized in those who belong to stigmatized groups. In the race version of the questionnaire, several of the items may assess race-consciousness when used in White samples (e.g., “Most people do not judge others on the basis of their race or ethnic group”; “My being a certain race/ethnicity does not influence how people act with me”). It is possible that Whites higher in race consciousness reap psychological benefits of privilege. There are examples of individuals experiencing a stereotype lift in situations where they can make downward comparisons with a derogated outgroup [40]. People with privileged identities who are especially conscious of race/ethnicity may chronically experience stereotype lift from downward social comparisons which dampen reactivity in stressful social or performance situations, ultimately protecting against chronic elevations in blood pressure. The present study is not the only one to demonstrate potential health advantages of privilege. A recent study examining rates of myocardial infarction in Blacks and Whites as a function of exposure to state levels of structural racism demonstrated that Whites can experience an advantage from structural racism against minorities [41].
Limitations and Strengths
Our study had a number of limitations, including that blood pressure was abstracted from medical records rather than assessed multiple times under controlled conditions. Also, we did not know whether participants were taking medication for hypertension. Medication status might be a useful stratification variable for future studies as it should be easier to detect relations between discrimination and blood pressure in an uncontrolled sample. All minorities were grouped together for the purpose of analysis and we were underpowered to detect interactions between minority groups and the predictors of interest. Even with 340 minorities in our sample we may have been underpowered to detect what are typically small effects of discrimination on blood pressure.
Our participants had been recently diagnosed with prostate cancer or would subsequently be diagnosed with prostate cancer. This is a non-emergent diagnosis that is unlikely to have affected lifetime discrimination, stigma consciousness, and hypertension status. Some men may have had elevated blood pressure due to anxiety or distress from the diagnosis; however, transient reactivity to the context is unlikely to have varied systematically as a function of lifetime discrimination and stigma consciousness. Elevated blood pressure at the time of assessment may have introduced noise into the data, but should not have affected the hypothesized relationships. One exception is that cardiac reactivity may have been higher in patients if they anticipated unfair treatment (or lower among Whites expecting respectful treatment) in the medical setting where their blood pressure was measured. It is always possible that part of the association between discrimination and diastolic blood pressure was due to this reactivity, however, this would not contradict the more general hypothesis that experiencing racial discrimination is associated with elevated blood pressure.
Strengths of the study include assessment of more than one dimension of racial discrimination. Also, our study included groups in whom the association between discrimination and blood pressure have been less studied: older adults, Hispanics, and men. Findings suggest that richer assessment of racism will help us better understand how racism impacts health over the lifecourse.
Conclusion
Findings support an association between racial discrimination and increases in blood pressure and likelihood of developing hypertension over one’s lifetime. As with previous studies that have shown similar effects, they were small in magnitude. This may be, in part, due to the difficulty of assessing the multifaceted experience of racism. Discrimination is complex [26]; whether one is targeted may be ambiguous, or may be processed defensively to protect self-esteem. Discrimination experiences can include anticipating unfair treatment, being targeted, and ruminating about past unfair treatment. Stigma may be internalized into one’s self-concept or experienced vicariously. New attention is being paid to the effects of microaggressions that are not necessarily captured by traditional measures of discrimination [42]. Most of these dimensions of discrimination have received little attention in the literature on discrimination and hypertension (for an exception see Hicken 2015). The present study, which demonstrates a relationship between stigma consciousness, or tendencies among members of denigrated groups to anticipate racial discrimination, and hypertension contributes to a more nuanced exploration of racial discrimination and health.
Acknowledgments
The Live Well Live Long! research group includes Integrated Medical Professionals, site-PI, Carl A. Olsson and CEO Deepak A. Kapoor; Memorial Sloan Kettering Cancer Institute, site-PI, Christian J. Nelson; Urology San Antonio, site-PI Juan A. Reyna; Houston Metro Urology, P.A., site-PI Zvi Schiffman, Roswell Park Cancer Institute, site-PI, Willie Underwood, III, and the University at Buffalo, site-PI, Heather Orom. We would like to acknowledge the cooperation and efforts of the staff and physicians at these sites for their significant contribution to participant recruitment. This work was supported by the National Cancer Institute under Grant R01#CA152425.
Footnotes
Conflicts of Interest: Chaman Sharma, Gregory Homish and D. Lynn Homish declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Ethical approval: All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent: informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
References
- 1.Wang Y, Wang Q. The prevalence of prehypertension and hypertension among US adults according to the new joint national committee guidelines: New challenges of the old problem. Arch. Intern. Med. 2004;164:2126–2134. doi: 10.1001/archinte.164.19.2126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Fryar CD, Hirsch R, Eberhardt MS, Yoon SS, JD W. Hypertension, high serum total cholesterol, and diabetes: Racial and ethnic prevalence differences in U.S. adults, 1999–2006. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics; 2010. NCHS data brief, no 36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Racial/Ethnic disparities in the awareness, treatment, and control of hypertension — United States, 2003–2010. MMWR. Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2013;62:351–355. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Evans K, Coresh J, Bash LD, Gary-Webb T, Köttgen A, Carson K, et al. Race differences in access to health care and disparities in incident chronic kidney disease in the US. Nephrol. Dial. Transplant. 2011;26:899–908. doi: 10.1093/ndt/gfq473. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Fiscella K, Holt K. Racial disparity in hypertension control: Tallying the death toll. Annals of Family Medicine. 2008;6:497–502. doi: 10.1370/afm.873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Selvin E, Coresh J, Brancati F. The burden and treatment of diabetes in elderly individuals in the US. Diabetes Care. 2006;29:2415. doi: 10.2337/dc06-1058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Saydah S, Cowie C, Eberhardt MS, De Rekeneire N, Narayan KMV. Race and ethnic differences in glycemic control among adults with diagnosed diabetes in the United States. Ethn. Dis. 2007;17:529–535. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Vital signs: Avoidable deaths from heart disease, stroke, and hypertensive disease — United States, 2001–2010. MMWR. Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2013:721–727. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Merritt MM, Bennett GG, Williams RB, Edwards CL, Sollers JJ. Perceived racism and cardiovascular reactivity and recovery to personally relevant stress. Health Psychol. 2006;25:364–369. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.3.364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Steffen PR, McNeilly M, Anderson N, Sherwood A. Effects of perceived racism and anger inhibition on ambulatory blood pressure in African Americans. Psychosom. Med. 2003;65:746–750. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000079380.95903.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Brondolo E, Love EE, Pencille M, Schoenthaler A, Ogedegbe G. Racism and hypertension: A review of the empirical evidence and implications for clinical practice. Am. J. Hypertens. 2011;24:518–529. doi: 10.1038/ajh.2011.9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hill LK, Kobayashi M, Hughes JW. Perceived racism and ambulatory blood pressure in African American college students. Journal of Black Psychology. 2007;33:404–421. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Brondolo E, Libby DJ, Denton E-g, Thompson S, Beatty DL, Schwartz J, et al. Racism and ambulatory blood pressure in a community sample. Psychosom. Med. 2008;70:49–56. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815ff3bd. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Treiber FA, Kamarck T, Schneiderman N, Sheffield D, Kapuku G, Taylor T. Cardiovascular reactivity and development of preclinical and clinical disease states. Psychosom. Med. 2003;65:46–62. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200301000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Panaite V, Salomon K, Jin A, Rottenberg J. Cardiovascular recovery from psychological and physiological challenge and risk for adverse cardiovascular outcomes and all-cause mortality. Psychosom. Med. 2015;77:215–226. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sims M, Diez-Roux AV, Dudley A, Gebreab S, Wyatt SB, Bruce MA, et al. Perceived discrimination and hypertension among African Americans in the Jackson Heart Study. Am. J. Public Health. 2012;102:S258–S265. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Salomon K, Jagusztyn NE. Resting cardiovascular levels and reactivity to interpersonal incivility among Black, Latina/o, and White individuals: The moderating role of ethnic discrimination. Health Psychol. 2008;27:473. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.4.473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Lewis TT, Barnes LL, Bienias JL, Lackland DT, Evans DA, de Leon CFM. Perceived discrimination and blood pressure in older African American and White adults. J Gerontol a-Biol. 2009;64:1002–1008. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glp062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ryan AM, Gee GC, Laflamme DF. The association between self-reported discrimination, physical health and blood pressure: Findings from African Americans, Black immigrants, and Latino immigrants in New Hampshire. J. Health Care Poor Underserved. 2006;17:116–132. doi: 10.1353/hpu.2006.0092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Krieger N, Sidney S. Racial discrimination and blood pressure: The CARDIA study of young black and white adults. Am. J. Public Health. 1996;86:1370–1378. doi: 10.2105/ajph.86.10.1370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Krieger N. Racial and gender discrimination: risk factors for high blood pressure? Soc. Sci. Med. 1990;30:1273–1281. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(90)90307-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Peters RM. Racism and hypertension among African Americans. West. J. Nurs. Res. 2004;26:612–631. doi: 10.1177/0193945904265816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Chae DH, Nuru-Jeter AM, Adler NE. Implicit racial bias as a moderator of the association between racial discrimination and hypertension: A study of Midlife African American men. Psychosom. Med. 2012;74:961–964. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182733665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ong KL, Cheung BMY, Man YB, Lau CP, Lam KSL. Prevalence, awareness, treatment, and control of hypertension among United States adults 1999–2004. Hypertension. 2007;49:69–75. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000252676.46043.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.National Cancer Institute (NCI) SEER Stat Fact Sheets: Prostate Cancer. [Accessed 11/01 2015];2015 http://seer.cancer.gov/statfacts/html/prost.html.
- 26.Williams DR, Mohammed SA. Discrimination and racial disparities in health: Evidence and needed research. J. Behav. Med. 2009;32:20–47. doi: 10.1007/s10865-008-9185-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hicken MT, Lee H, Morenoff J, House JS, Williams DR. Racial/Ethnic disparities in hypertension prevalence: reconsidering the role of chronic stress. Am. J. Public Health. 2014;104:117–123. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Sawyer PJ, Major B, Casad BJ, Townsend SS, Mendes WB. Discrimination and the stress response: psychological and physiological consequences of anticipating prejudice in interethnic interactions. Am. J. Public Health. 2012;102:1020–1026. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kaiser CR, Vick SB, Major B. Prejudice expectations moderate preconscious attention to cues that are threatening to social identity. Psychological Science. 2006;17:332–338. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01707.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Lewis RJ, Derlega VJ, Griffin JL, Krowinski AC. Stressors for gay men and lesbians: Life stress, gay-related stress, stigma consciousness, and depressive symptoms. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2003;22:716–729. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Krieger N, Smith K, Naishadham D, Harman C, Barbeau EM. Experiences of discrimination: validity and reliability of a self-report measure for population health research on racism and health. Soc. Sci. Med. 2005;61:1576–1596. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Pinel E. Stigma consciousness: The psychological legacy of social stereotypes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1999;76:114–128. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.76.1.114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Graham JW. Missing data analysis: Making it work in the real world. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2009;60:549–576. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rapsomaniki E, Timmis A, George J, Pujades-Rodriguez M, Shah AD, Denaxas S, et al. Blood pressure and incidence of twelve cardiovascular diseases: lifetime risks, healthy life-years lost, and age-specific associations in 1.25 million people. The Lancet. 383:1899–1911. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60685-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lewis TT, Everson-Rose SA, Powell LH, Matthews KA, Brown C, Karavolos K, et al. Chronic exposure to everyday discrimination and coronary artery calcification in African-American women: The SWAN heart study. Psychosom. Med. 2006;68:362–368. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000221360.94700.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Paradies Y. A systematic review of empirical research on self-reported racism and health. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2006;35:888–901. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyl056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Pinel EC. You're just saying that because I'm a woman: Stigma consciousness and attributions to discrimination. Self and Identity. 2004;3:39–51. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Townsend SSM, Major B, Gangi CE, Mendes WB. From “in the air” to “under the skin”: Cortisol responses to social identity threat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2011;37:151–164. doi: 10.1177/0146167210392384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Mays VM, Cochran SD, Barnes NW. Race, race-based discrimination, and health outcomes among African Americans. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007;58:201–225. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Walton GM, Cohen GL. Stereotype lift. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2003;39:456–467. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Lukachko A, Hatzenbuehler ML, Keyes KM. Structural racism and myocardial infarction in the United States. Soc. Sci. Med. 2014;103:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.07.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hall JM, Fields B. “It’s Killing Us!” Narratives of Black adults about microaggression experiences and related health stress. Global Qualitative Nursing Research. 2015;2:1–14. doi: 10.1177/2333393615591569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]