Abstract
Combining transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) with electroencephalography (EEG) allows for the assessment of various neurophysiological processes in the human cortex. One of these paradigms, short-latency afferent inhibition (SAI), is thought to be a sensitive measure of cholinergic activity. In a previous study, we demonstrated the temporal pattern of this paradigm from both the motor (M1) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) using simultaneous TMS–EEG recording. The SAI paradigm led to marked modulations at N100. In this study, we aimed to investigate the age-related effects on TMS-evoked potentials (TEPs) with the SAI from M1 and the DLPFC in younger (18–59 years old) and older (≥60 years old) participants. Older participants showed significantly lower N100 modulation in M1–SAI as well as DLPFC–SAI compared to the younger participants. Furthermore, the modulation of N100 by DLPFC–SAI in the older participants correlated with executive function as measured with the Trail making test. This paradigm has the potential to non-invasively identify cholinergic changes in cortical regions related to cognition in older participants.
Keywords: TMS-EEG, short-latency afferent inhibition, age-related changes, cognition, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
Introduction
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) allows for the non-invasive measurement of various inhibitory and excitatory processes in the human brain (Hallett, 2011). Different TMS paradigms produce distinct inhibitory responses in cortical activity, and TMS paradigms administered sequentially produce unique neurophysiological interactions (Chen, 2004; Ni et al., 2011). One such paradigm involves median nerve stimulation (MNS) followed by a single TMS pulse to the motor cortex (M1) approximately 20 ms later. The MNS followed by the inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 20 ms produces a marked reduction of motor-evoked potentials (MEP) from a single TMS pulse (Classen et al., 2000; Tokimura et al., 2000), and has been termed “short-latency afferent inhibition” (SAI) (Sailer et al., 2003).
SAI can be modulated by acetylcholine agonists and anticholinergic agents, implying a direct role of cholinergic activity in the SAI cortical response (Di Lazzaro et al., 2000, 2002, 2005b). The muscarinic acetylcholine receptor antagonist scopolamine decreases SAI in healthy participants (Di Lazzaro et al., 2000). Moreover, lorazepam reduces (Di Lazzaro et al., 2005a) and diazepam slightly increases (Di Lazzaro et al., 2005b) SAI, suggesting that gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA)A receptor-mediated inhibitory circuits are also involved in this neurophysiological response.
SAI in M1 is thought to involve contributions from S1 through an indirect cortico-cortical projection (Cash et al., 2015), as well as a direct thalamocortical projection to M1. Clinically it is impossible to distinguish the effects of both contributions (Ferreri et al., 2012). Similarly, SAI in the DLPFC is also speculated to involve the contributions from S1 and thalamocortical projection to DLPFC. Indeed, the inter-stimulus interval of M1-SAI has been shown to occur as early as N20+0 ms (Tokimura et al., 2000; Fischer and Orth, 2011), while the optimal interstimulus interval of DLPFC-SAI is approximately N20+4 ms (Noda et al., 2016). Therefore, we can generalize that SAI in M1 and DLPFC are significantly mediated by both of cortico-cortical and thalamocortical projections. This is strongly supported by the finding that they display a shared electrophysiological signature in both domains, indicating that they are mediated by similar mechanisms.
The role of cholinergic activity in cognitive function has been well established (Kopelman, 1986; Everitt and Robbins, 1997; Erskine et al., 2004). The presence of a relationship between cognitive deficits and reduced SAI response supports the notion that SAI is a direct measure of cholinergic activity. Previous studies have demonstrated that SAI is significantly diminished in patients with central cholinergic deficit such as Alzheimer's disease (Di Lazzaro et al., 2002). Furthermore, these SAI impairments were restored by the administration of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors rivastigmine (Di Lazzaro et al., 2002, 2005b) and donepezil (Nardone et al., 2012). Additionally, the amount of SAI decreases with age (Young-Bernier et al., 2012). Moreover, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) has been reported to be closely involved in the cholinergic-mediated cognitive function in monkey studies (Croxson et al., 2011; Yang et al., 2013). Thus, taken together, cholinergic function varies with age and brain region, supporting the role of cholinergic function in cognitive changes that occur with aging and neurodegeneration.
Previous TMS–electroencephalography (EEG) studies have demonstrated that the SAI paradigm at M1 induces significant modulation of N100 of TMS-evoked potential (TEP) component in the midline central area (Bikmullina et al., 2009; Ferreri et al., 2012), and showed the N100 TEP component also correlated with attenuation of motor–evoked potentials (MEP) in SAI (Bikmullina et al., 2009). TMS can be combined with EEG to study cortical regions outside M1. Our previous study demonstrated that the SAI protocol in M1 induced significant TEP increases at N45, N100, and P180 at an ISI of N20+2 ms over the left central region of interest (ROI), whereas SAI in the DLPFC induced significant attenuation of P60 TEP and increase of N100 TEP at an ISI of N20+4 ms over the left frontal ROI (Noda et al., 2016). Additionally, we replicated the correlation between N100 TEP and SAI–MEP changes at M1 (Bikmullina et al., 2009). Our results demonstrated that the SAI response exists in the DLPFC, and may share common mechanisms of M1–SAI (Noda et al., 2016).
Prior studies have investigated the effect of SAI on MEP in older subjects compared with younger subjects (Degardin et al., 2011; Young-Bernier et al., 2012, 2014, 2015). These studies other than one (Degardin et al., 2011) demonstrated that SAI is reduced in older subjects (Hedges's g = 0.778, p < 0.0001) (Bhandari et al., 2016). However, there have been no studies that have investigated the SAI effects on TMS-evoked potentials (TEPs) in M1 or the DLPFC in older adults. Thus, the present study aimed to investigate the modulatory effects of SAI paradigm on TEP in both M1 and the DLPFC in older adults compared to younger adults. Based on previous work (Young-Bernier et al., 2012; Noda et al., 2016), we anticipated that N100 TEP component would be significantly altered with SAI administered to M1 and the DLPFC in the older compared to the younger participants. We also sought to examine whether there are any associations between the modulations of SAI–TEP and cognitive measures in the older participants.
Methods
Participants
Twelve younger (6 female, mean age ± SD; 39 ± 12 years, 22–57 years) and 12 older (6 female, mean age ± SD; 72 ± 9 years, 64–92 years) individuals participated in the present study. All participants were right-handed. In addition, there was no significant age difference (t22 = −0.270, p = 0.789) between female (54 ± 21 years) and male (56 ± 19 years) participants for all participants. Participants over 18 years were eligible to participate in this study if they met the following criteria: (i) no history of neurological disorders including seizure or stroke, (ii) no history of neuropsychiatric disorders, (iii) normal cognitive function, (iv) no history of alcohol or other drug abuse/dependence, and (v) did not smoke, use recreational substances or prescription medications. All participants were screened with either the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders or the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Sheehan et al., 1998) prior to study participation to exclude a history of psychiatric illness. The study was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health with written informed consent from all subjects, and was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of “Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord, roots and peripheral nerves: Basic principles and procedures for routine clinical and research application. An updated report from an International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology Committee.” All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The same procedures and analyses in the younger participants were applied in the older participants in this study.
TMS procedure and EMG measure
Monophasic TMS pulses were administered to M1 on the left hemisphere using a 70 mm figure–of–eight coil, and two Magstim 200 stimulators (Magstim Company Ltd., UK) connected via a Bistim module. MEP data were collected using the commercially available software, Signal (Cambridge Electronics, UK). Participants were seated in a chair and instructed to relax and keep their eyes open. Surface electromyography (EMG) was recorded from Ag/AgCl electrodes placed over the belly of the first dorsal interosseous muscle in the right hand.
SAI procedure
The SAI paradigm was administered using standard methods from the literature (Tokimura et al., 2000; Noda et al., 2016). Specifically, the median nerve was stimulated at the right wrist using a standard bar electrode, with a cathode positioned proximally using a constant current stimulator (Digitimer model DS7A, Digitimer Ltd., UK). It is noted that we chose to stimulate the median nerve since there is a relatively low specificity of peripheral nerve stimulation in terms of the strength of SAI (Cash et al., 2015). The conditioning MNS intensity (pulse width 200 μs) was adjusted to three times the sensory threshold. For the SAI paradigm, TMS was performed over the motor hotspot of the first dorsal interosseous muscle at an intensity that evoked a 1mV response in MEP amplitude peak–to–peak. SAI was delivered at the MNS–TMS ISIs relative to the somatosensory evoked potential (SSEP) at N20 (Fischer and Orth, 2011; Ferreri et al., 2012; Noda et al., 2016). To obtain the individual N20, SSEP were recorded before starting the SAI protocol (200 stimuli delivered at 3 Hz and 3 times sensory threshold). SSEP N20 was calculated using Neuroscan software (Compumedics Neuroscan) based on previously published method (Noda et al., 2016). Following to our previous method, we applied the ISI of N20+2 ms for the M1–SAI paradigm while we used the ISI of N20+4 ms for the DLPFC–SAI paradigm (Noda et al., 2016). TMS was applied to F5 electrode site in the DLPFC–SAI following previously published methodology (Rusjan et al., 2010). The inter-stimulus interval of the SAI protocol was 5 s and the number of TMS–EEG trials for each condition was 100 times (i.e., 200 trials in total for both M1 and DLPFC–SAI, respectively). Furthermore, the order of single test pulse (TS) or conditioned-test pulse (SAI), as well as the order of M1 or DLPFC stimulation site was randomized to avoid potential order and cumulative effects on MEP and TEP (Pellicciari et al., 2016). EMG measures of SAI (i.e., MEP–SAI) were calculated as a ratio of conditioned MEP amplitude (i.e., SAI) divided by the 1 mV peak–to–peak MEP amplitude (i.e., TS); that is, [conditioned MEP (SAI)]/[unconditioned MEP (TS)].
In the present study, we analyzed TEP modulations by SAI paradigm primarily with a ratio of TEP amplitude change (i.e., SAI/TS), and secondary with a TEP amplitude subtraction method (i.e., SAI – TS). The results analyzed in a ratio method are presented in figures and results analyzed in a subtraction method are demonstrated in Supplementary Figures.
EEG recording and pre-processing
A Synamps 2/RT 64-channel EEG system and a Quik-Cap Electrode Placement System (Compumedics Neuroscan, Australia) were used to record cortical activity. All electrodes were referenced to an electrode placed on the vertex positioned electrode. EEG signals were recorded at DC at 20 kHz sampling rate and with a low pass filter of 200 Hz. EEG data were processed offline using MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA). All data were down–sampled to 1,000 Hz for analyses.
SAI–TEP data analysis
SAI–TEP data were analyzed based on the method in our previous study (Noda et al., 2016). The continuous EEG time series were sectioned to include data from −1,000 ms before to 2,000 ms after the TMS pulse and then baseline-corrected with respect to the pre-stimulus interval −500 to −110 ms. TMS artifact 10 ms after the TMS pulse was removed during data cleaning. The data was visually inspected, and trials and channels that were highly contaminated with noise (muscle activity, electrode artifacts) were subsequently removed from analysis. More than 80% of trials and 95% of channels remained after artifact removal. Before applying the independent component analysis cleaning method, each condition of EEG data was concatenated to avoid the bias of the independent component analysis (ICA) cleaning process. Then, the ICA (an automated version of the infomax ICA algorithm in the EEGLAB toolbox) (Bell and Sejnowski, 1995; Makeig et al., 1997; Delorme and Makeig, 2004) was applied to remove eye–related artifacts (blinks and eye movements), the remaining muscle artifacts, as well as the TMS-related decay artifacts immediately after the TMS pulse. Following the ICA, the Butterworth, zero-phase shift 1–55 Hz band pass filter (24 dB/Oct) and notch filter were applied. Finally, data was re-referenced to the average for further analyses. Of note, SAI–TEP values were obtained after subtracting the SSEP trace from the TEP individually for each participant, in accordance with our previous study evaluating SAI with TMS–EEG (Noda et al., 2016). Furthermore, we detected TEP values by identifying the peaks (i.e., P30, P60, and P180) and troughs (i.e., N45 and N100) individually. In addition, to evaluate the SAI effect on TEPs, we calculated the ratio for each TEP component in the M1–SAI and DLPFC–SAI, which represents the degree of modulation by SAI paradigm, as follows: Modulation of TEP = conditioned TEP (SAI) /unconditioned TEP (TS).
Cognitive assessment
The Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) (Holdnack, 2001), the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) (Howieson et al., 2004), the Stroop test (Howieson et al., 2004), and the Trail Making Test (TMT) Parts A & B (Bowie and Harvey, 2006) were performed in the older participants. The WTAR estimates a degree of intellectual functioning prior to the onset of neuropsychiatric or neurological disorder (Holdnack, 2001). The RBANS assesses five domains of cognition as follows: immediate memory, visuospatial and constructional memory, language, attention, and delayed memory (Howieson et al., 2004). The Stroop test measures some aspects of executive functioning such as selective attention, cognitive flexibility, inhibition, self-regulation capacity, and processing speed (Howieson et al., 2004). The TMT evaluates general executive functioning such as visual search speed, scanning, task switching, processing speed, and mental flexibility (Bowie and Harvey, 2006).
Statistical analysis
SPSS version 19.0 was used for statistical analysis. Based on our previous findings (Noda et al., 2016), we have focused on the left central area as a ROI for M1–SAI analysis and the left frontal area as a ROI for the DLPFC–SAI analysis in the present study (see Supplementary Figure 1).
First, to examine the degree of modulation of TEPs (i.e., P30, N45, P60, N100, and P180) by the M1– and DLPFC–SAI in the older participants, paired t-tests with Bonferroni correction were applied between TS and SAI (i.e., ISI of N20+2 for M1–SAI; ISI of N20+4 for the DLPFC–SAI).
Next, to compare the degree of modulations between the young and older participants cross-sectionally, we performed independent t-tests with Bonferroni correction. Further, to elucidate age-related differences in the M1–SAI and DLPFC–SAI paradigms, we performed Pearson's correlation analyses between age and the following variables: the modulations of MEP–SAI and SAI–TEP in all healthy participants. Moreover, we examined the relationship between modulations of SAI–TEP and cognitive measures in the older participants in M1–SAI or the DLPFC–SAI paradigm. Additionally, we explored the relationship between MEP–SAI modulation and cognitive measures.
In addition, to explore the gender differences for all participants on MEP–SAI and TEP modulations by the M1–SAI, as well as DLPFC–SAI paradigms, independent t-tests were performed between female and male participants.
Results
MEP–SAI between the young and old healthy participants
Mean intensity (±SE) to induce 1 mV peak-to-peak MEP amplitude of younger and older participants were 80.3 ± 11.5% and 82.8 ± 9.7%, respectively. In the M1–SAI paradigm, the MEP was significantly attenuated compared to TS alone, in the young and older participants by 41.2 ± 8.0% (t11 = 4.364, p = 0.001) and 32.2 ± 5.9% (t11 = 4.977, p = 0.0004). However, there was no significant difference between younger and older participants (t22 = −0.577, p = 0.570) or gender difference (t22 = −0.633, p = 0.534) in the MEP–SAI.
Modulation of TEPs by M1–SAI
Averaged TEP traces and EEG topographical plots by M1–SAI for the older participants are shown in Figures 1A,B (refer to Supplementary Figure 2). For older participants compared to young, independent t-tests indicated significantly smaller modulation of N45 TEP (t22 = 5.485, p < 0.0001; young > old; α–level: 0.01) and N100 TEP (t22 = 3.627, p = 0.001; young > old) at the left central ROI (Figure 1C). Furthermore, averaged TEP traces without SSEP subtraction in the M1-SAI are shown in a Supplementary Figure 3A. Furthermore, we demonstrated the results of cross-sectional comparisons of SAI–TEP amplitude modulations in M1 between younger and older participants using an amplitude subtraction method in a Supplementary Figure 4A. In this subtraction based analysis, older participants demonstrated a significant increase of N100 TEP amplitude as in the ratio based analysis, but did not show a significant change of N45 TEP.
In addition, within older participants, the M1–SAI paradigm induced a significant increase amplitude on N45 TEP (t11 = 4.062, p = 0.002; α–level: 0.05/5 = 0.01) at the left central ROI (Figure 1D). Our previously published data contain details on the TEP analyses of the young participants (Noda et al., 2016). Further, there was no gender difference on the modulation of TEPs by M1-SAI paradigm.
Modulation of TEPs by the DLPFC–SAI
Averaged TEP traces and EEG topographical plots by DLPFC–SAI for the older participants are shown in Figures 2A,B (refer to Supplementary Figure 2). Older individuals showed less modulation in N100 TEP compared to the younger group (t22 = 2.921, p = 0.008) (Figure 2C). Further, averaged TEP traces without SSEP subtraction in the DLPFC-SAI are shown in a Supplementary Figure 3B. In addition, the results of cross-sectional comparisons of SAI–TEP amplitude modulations in the DLPFC using an amplitude subtraction method are shown in a Supplementary Figure 4B.
Further, within older participants, at the left frontal ROI, P30 (t11 = 3.204, p = 0.008; α–level: 0.01), P60 (t11 = 3.165, p = 0.009), and N100 (t11 = −4.871, p < 0.0001) TEP components were significantly attenuated by the DLPFC–SAI paradigm (Figure 2D). In addition, no gender difference was observed in the modulation of TEPs by DLPFC–SAI paradigm.
Age-related correlations in M1–SAI and the DLPFC–SAI paradigm
Pearson's correlation analyses of all participants demonstrated that increasing age was associated with reduced modulation of N45 TEP (r = −0.618, p = 0.001, N = 24) and N100 TEP (r = −0.548, p = 0.006, N = 24) for SAI at the left central ROI (Figure 3). Further, there was a significant negative correlation between age and the modulation of N100 at the left frontal ROI (r = −0.529, p = 0.008, N = 24) (Figure 3). In addition, the analyses based on a TEP amplitude subtraction method are shown in Supplementary Figure 5.
Correlation analyses between SAI–TEP modulations and cognitive outcomes in the old healthy participants
In the DLPFC–SAI paradigm, we observed significant correlations: the WTAR standardized score was negatively correlated with the modulation of P60 TEP (r = −0.632, p = 0.028, N = 12), the RBANS total score was negatively correlated with the modulation of P60 TEP (r = −0.639, p = 0.025, N = 12), and the ratio of TMT part B to part A was negatively correlated with the modulation of N100 TEP (r = −0.727, p = 0.007, N = 12) at the left frontal ROI (Figure 4). Importantly, we did not observe any relationships between cognition and the modulation of TEPs in the M1–SAI paradigm. Further, the analyses based on a subtraction method are shown in a Supplementary Figure 6.
Correlation analyses between modulations of MEP–SAI and M1–SAI TEPs and DLPFC–SAI TEPs
No significant relationship was observed among these indices in both ratio and subtraction based analyses.
Discussion
The present study, investigating the SAI effect on TEPs in M1 and the DLPFC for the first time, generated several important findings. First, cross-sectional comparisons between young and older participants in the M1–SAI paradigm revealed that N45 and N100 TEPs were significantly less modulated in older participants compared with younger participants. Second, DLPFC–SAI induced a significant N100 decrease in older participants, compared to younger participants, and further the degree of N100 modulation correlated with age. Third, we observed significant correlations between cognitive function and the modulation of TMS–EEG related markers of SAI (i.e., DLPFC–SAI) in older participants.
The findings of M1–SAI showing lower modulations of N45 and N100 in older participants may demonstrate an age-mediated decline of modulation in both early and late phase of TEPs at M1 Thus, it is possible that GABAA receptor-mediated inhibition of N45 (Ferreri et al., 2011; Premoli et al., 2014) and cholinergic mediated modulation of N100 (Ferreri et al., 2012; Noda et al., 2016) may be declined in an age-dependent manner. More specifically, in the M1–SAI experiment, the modulations of N45 and N100 TEPs were significantly lower in older participants, compared to younger participants, which may indicate that older participants had lower GABAA ergic and cholinergic function. Previous studies have reported that GABAA receptor-mediated short-interval intracortical inhibition is reduced with age (Peinemann et al., 2001; Marneweck et al., 2011; Heise et al., 2013). Another study demonstrated that GABABreceptor-mediated inhibition was also reduced with age, as measured with the long-interval intracortical inhibition paradigm (Opie and Semmler, 2014). However, a recent meta-analysis has demonstrated that the effect of aging on MEP from M1-TMS, reflecting GABA receptor-mediated inhibition, are highly variable (Bhandari et al., 2016). Collectively, it seems that central cholinergic activity decays reliably with age (Bartus et al., 1982; Newhouse et al., 1994; Mitsis et al., 2009), whereas the findings of age-related reduction in GABAA receptor-mediated inhibition may be less reliable (Bhandari et al., 2016). Thus, our findings may indicate that M1–SAI paradigm detects cholinergic mediated modulation in a more age-sensitive manner than it detects GABAA receptor-mediated inhibition. In other words, while M1–SAI may induce a similar mechanism of cholinergic mediated modulation on N100 TEP to the DLPFC–SAI, it may not induce the similar pattern of GABAA receptor-mediated effect on the early TEP components compared to the DLPFC–SAI.
The DLPFC–SAI paradigm for both young and older participants demonstrated a significant decrease of P60 TEP at the left frontal ROI. A prior TMS–EEG study utilizing the SAI paradigm in M1 demonstrated attenuation of P60 and N100 TEP amplitudes (Ferreri et al., 2012). Ferreri et al. suggested that this finding may be related to cortico–cortical activation of GABA receptor-mediated inhibition onto the corticospinal neurons (Ferreri et al., 2012). Furthermore, in our previous TMS–EEG study investigating short-interval intracortical inhibition and intracortical facilitation, we observed that an amplitude of P60 was significantly attenuated in the short-interval intra-cortical inhibition paradigm, whereas it was significantly increased in the intracortical facilitation paradigm (Cash et al., 2016), suggesting that P60 may be a useful neurophysiological marker of neural excitability in the DLPFC. Further, our results revealed N100 TEP in the DLPFC–SAI induced robust changes similar to that of the M1–SAI paradigm. However, the direction of the modulation of N100 TEP was opposite between the young and older participants, suggesting that the modulation of N100 by the DLPFC–SAI paradigm may be more sensitive to age compared to M1–SAI. Furthermore, taken together with the finding of age-dependent decline of N100 modulation by DLPFC–SAI, it is possible that N100 modulation by the SAI paradigm is associated with the cholinergic tone as well as GABA receptor-mediated activity in the prefrontal cortex (Young-Bernier et al., 2012; Opie and Semmler, 2014). Moreover, in older participants, DLPFC–SAI induced significantly lower modulations of P30, P60, and N100 TEPs (Figure 2D). Similar to the results of TEP modulations in M1–SAI paradigm, we speculate that partial GABAA receptor-mediated and principally cholinergic mediated modulation may be involved in the DLPFC–SAI neurophysiological responses. However, the modulation on the early TEP components (i.e., N45) by DLPFC–SAI seems to be different in M1–SAI (Figures 1C,2C). This may occur because M1–SAI paradigm can induce both GABAA receptor-mediated and cholinergic mediated functions to some extent, whereas the DLPFC–SAI may detect cholinergic activity in a more specific manner.
We also observed correlations between cognitive measures and inhibitory responses evoked by the DLPFC–SAI paradigm, but not from M1–SAI paradigm. The DLPFC is involved in cognition (Croxson et al., 2011; Yang et al., 2013), and cognitive processes involve cholinergic neurotransmission (Kopelman, 1986; Everitt and Robbins, 1997; Erskine et al., 2004). Therefore, the SAI paradigm in the DLPFC may be associated with cholinergic function in a brain region involved in cognition. Pharmacological studies must be conducted to confirm that DLPFC-SAI is indeed mediated by cholinergic function. More specifically, given the strong correlation between the TMT B/A ratio and N100 modulation, SAI in the DLPFC may be related to executive function.
There are limitations in the present study. First, it is known that the TMS “click” generates an auditory evoked potential, however, we did not use masking noise in this study. This is because the MNS input does not generate an auditory evoked potential—i.e., SAI and TS are matched. Second, in this study, we did not use a magnetic resonance imaging-guided neuronavigation system to localize the DLPFC for each participant. While neuronavigation is more precise in identifying the individual targeting region, the method of approximation using the F5 electrode DLPFC loci has been effective in previous studies (Fitzgerald et al., 2009; Rusjan et al., 2010; Rogasch et al., 2015). To minimize this technical limitation, we applied the ROI-based analysis for TEP data by clustering several electrodes to capture the representative characteristics for both M1 and the DLPFC. Third, cognitive assessment was lacking for the younger participants in this study. Future studies should include the cognitive assessment of younger participants. Fourth, in the absence of a specific pharmacological intervention such as acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, it is not able to demonstrate a causal relationship between cholinergic function and TEPs in the DLPFC–SAI. Thus, the pharmacological intervention study will be needed to confirm the results of DLPFC–SAI. Fifth, in the present study, we analyzed the degree of TEP amplitude modulations by each SAI paradigm in both a ratio and subtraction based approach to confirm the validity of our SAI–TEP analyses. The results demonstrate that there can be somewhat discrepant results depending on the analytical approach taken and caution should be exercised in interpreting TEP modulation data. Notwithstanding this limitation, the main findings persisted across both analyses and suggest that N100 modulation may be the most robust marker of the SAI effect. Finally, the sample size of 12 subjects is relatively small and requires replication in a larger sample
Major neurocognitive disorders such as Alzheimer's disease have been related, at least in part, to loss of cortical cholinergic innervation (Coyle et al., 1983). Modulation of SAI in M1 has been shown to predict long-term response to a cholinesterase inhibitor in Alzheimer's disease (Di Lazzaro et al., 2005b) and have been shown to distinguish between non-amnestic patients and amnestic patients with mild cognitive impairment (Nardone et al., 2012). Thus, the present findings extend the use of the SAI paradigm to the DLPFC in older participants and may be a useful tool to enhance our understanding of the neurophysiological basis of late-life neuropsychiatric disorders.
The present findings, which extend the SAI paradigm in both M1 and the DLPFC to the older participants, have significant potential for future clinical research. In summary, SAI appears reduced in older compared to young individuals. Further, DLPFC–SAI in older participants correlated with executive function. This finding is preliminary and requires further replication and verification. If confirmed in future studies, SAI in the DLPFC may be a potential relevant marker of cholinergic tone. The results of the present study warrant further investigations in populations with cognitive disorders such as Alzheimer's disease and those at risk for Alzheimer's disease such as patients with mild cognitive impairment and late-life depression.
Author contributions
DB, YN, and ZD were involved in conception and design of the study; YN performed experiments; YN and RZ analyzed data; YN, RFHC, RC, ZD, and DB interpreted results of experiments; YN prepared figures; YN and FB drafted the manuscript; MB, TR, DB, ZD, FB, and RC edited and revised the manuscript; all authors approved final version of manuscript.
Funding
YN receives postdoctoral fellowship from the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health (CAMH) Foundation. MB receives research support from the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (Formerly NARSAD) Young Investigator Grant and Schizophrenia Junior Faculty Grant from the CAMH Foundation. RFHC was supported by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR)—Dystonia Medical Research Foundation Fellowship award. TR received research support from Brain Canada, Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, Canada Foundation for Innovation, the CIHR, Ontario Ministry of Health and Long-Term Care, Ontario Ministry of Research and Innovation, the US National Institute of Health (NIH), and the W. Garfield Weston Foundation. RC received research support from the CIHR, the Catherine Manson Chair in Movement Disorders, Medtronic Inc. and Merz Pharma. ZD has received research support from the Ontario Mental Health (OMH) Foundation, the CIHR, the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (Formerly NARSAD), and the Temerty family and Grant family through the CAMH Foundation and the Campbell Institute. ZD received research and equipment in kind support for an investigator–initiated study through Brainsway Inc., and a travel allowance through Merck. ZD has also received speaker funding through Sepracor Inc., and AstraZeneca, served on advisory boards for Hoffmann–La Roche Limited and Merck, and received speaker support from Eli Lilly. DB has received research support from the CIHR, NIH, Brain Canada and the Temerty Family through the CAMH Foundation and the Campbell Research Institute. He receives research support and in-kind equipment support for an investigator-initiated study from Brainsway Ltd. and he is the site principal investigator for three sponsor-initiated studies for Brainsway Ltd. He receives in-kind equipment support from Magventure for an investigator-initiated study. He receives medication supplies for an investigator-initiated trial from Indivior.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by the Temerty Centre for Therapeutic Brain Intervention, the Campbell Family Research Institute through the CAMH Foundation, and Canada Foundation for Innovation.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00119/full#supplementary-material
References
- Bartus R. T., Dean R. L., III., Beer B., Lippa A. S. (1982). The cholinergic hypothesis of geriatric memory dysfunction. Science 217, 408–414. 10.1126/science.7046051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell A. J., Sejnowski T. J. (1995). An information-maximization approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution. Neural Comput. 7, 1129–1159. 10.1162/neco.1995.7.6.1129 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bhandari A., Radhu N., Farzan F., Mulsant B. H., Rajji T. K., Daskalakis Z. J., et al. (2016). A meta-analysis of the effects of aging on motor cortex neurophysiology assessed by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 2834–2845. 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.05.363 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bikmullina R., Kicic D., Carlson S., Nikulin V. V. (2009). Electrophysiological correlates of short-latency afferent inhibition: a combined EEG and TMS study. Exp. Brain Res. 194, 517–526. 10.1007/s00221-009-1723-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowie C. R., Harvey P. D. (2006). Administration and interpretation of the Trail Making Test. Nat. Protoc. 1, 2277–2281. 10.1038/nprot.2006.390 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cash R. F., Isayama R., Gunraj C. A., Ni Z., Chen R. (2015). The influence of sensory afferent input on local motor cortical excitatory circuitry in humans. J. Physiol. (Lond). 593, 1667–1684. 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.286245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cash R. F., Noda Y., Zomorrodi R., Radhu N., Farzan F., Rajji T. K., et al. (2016). Characterisation of glutamatergic and GABAA mediated neurotransmission in motor and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex using paired-pulse TMS-EEG. Neuropsychopharmacology 42, 502–511. 10.1038/npp.2016.133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen R. (2004). Interactions between inhibitory and excitatory circuits in the human motor cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 154, 1–10. 10.1007/s00221-003-1684-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Classen J., Steinfelder B., Liepert J., Stefan K., Celnik P., Cohen L. G., et al. (2000). Cutaneomotor integration in humans is somatotopically organized at various levels of the nervous system and is task dependent. Exp. Brain Res. 130, 48–59. 10.1007/s002210050005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coyle J. T., Price D. L., Delong M. R. (1983). Alzheimer's disease: a disorder of cortical cholinergic innervation. Science 219, 1184–1190. 10.1126/science.6338589 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Croxson P. L., Kyriazis D. A., Baxter M. G. (2011). Cholinergic modulation of a specific memory function of prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1510–1512. 10.1038/nn.2971 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Degardin A., Devos D., Cassim F., Bourriez J. L., Defebvre L., Derambure P., et al. (2011). Deficit of sensorimotor integration in normal aging. Neurosci. Lett. 498, 208–212. 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.05.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme A., Makeig S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Lazzaro V., Oliviero A., Pilato F., Saturno E., Dileone M., Marra C., et al. (2005b). Neurophysiological predictors of long term response to AChE inhibitors in AD patients. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 76, 1064–1069. 10.1136/jnnp.2004.051334 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Lazzaro V., Oliviero A., Profice P., Pennisi M. A., Di Giovanni S., Zito G., et al. (2000). Muscarinic receptor blockade has differential effects on the excitability of intracortical circuits in the human motor cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 135, 455–461. 10.1007/s002210000543 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Lazzaro V., Oliviero A., Saturno E., Dileone M., Pilato F., Nardone R., et al. (2005a). Effects of lorazepam on short latency afferent inhibition and short latency intracortical inhibition in humans. J. Physiol. 564, 661–668. 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.061747 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Lazzaro V., Oliviero A., Tonali P. A., Marra C., Daniele A., Profice P., et al. (2002). Noninvasive in vivo assessment of cholinergic cortical circuits in AD using transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 59, 392–397. 10.1212/WNL.59.3.392 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erskine F. F., Ellis J. R., Ellis K. A., Stuber E., Hogan K., Miller V., et al. (2004). Evidence for synergistic modulation of early information processing by nicotinic and muscarinic receptors in humans. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 19, 503–509. 10.1002/hup.613 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt B. J., Robbins T. W. (1997). Central cholinergic systems and cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 48, 649–684. 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.649 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferreri F., Pasqualetti P., Määttä S., Ponzo D., Ferrarelli F., Tononi G., et al. (2011). Human brain connectivity during single and paired pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuroimage 54, 90–102. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferreri F., Ponzo D., Hukkanen T., Mervaala E., Könönen M., Pasqualetti P., et al. (2012). Human brain cortical correlates of short-latency afferent inhibition: a combined EEG-TMS study. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 314–323. 10.1152/jn.00796.2011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischer M., Orth M. (2011). Short-latency sensory afferent inhibition: conditioning stimulus intensity, recording site, and effects of 1 Hz repetitive TMS. Brain Stimul. 4, 202–209. 10.1016/j.brs.2010.10.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgerald P. B., Maller J. J., Hoy K. E., Thomson R., Daskalakis Z. J. (2009). Exploring the optimal site for the localization of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in brain stimulation experiments. Brain Stimul. 2, 234–237. 10.1016/j.brs.2009.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hallett M. (2011). Neurophysiology of dystonia: the role of inhibition. Neurobiol. Dis. 42, 177–184. 10.1016/j.nbd.2010.08.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heise K. F., Zimerman M., Hoppe J., Gerloff C., Wegscheider K., Hummel F. C. (2013). The aging motor system as a model for plastic changes of GABA-mediated intracortical inhibition and their behavioral relevance. J. Neurosci. 33, 9039–9049. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4094-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holdnack H. A. (2001). Wechsler Test of Adult Reading: WTAR. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Howieson D. B., Lezak M. D., Loring D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kopelman M. D. (1986). The cholinergic neurotransmitter system in human memory and dementia: a review. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 38, 535–573. 10.1080/14640748608401614 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makeig S., Jung T. P., Bell A. J., Ghahremani D., Sejnowski T. J. (1997). Blind separation of auditory event-related brain responses into independent components. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 94, 10979–10984. 10.1073/pnas.94.20.10979 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marneweck M., Loftus A., Hammond G. (2011). Short-interval intracortical inhibition and manual dexterity in healthy aging. Neurosci. Res. 70, 408–414. 10.1016/j.neures.2011.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitsis E. M., Cosgrove K. P., Staley J. K., Bois F., Frohlich E. B., Tamagnan G. D., et al. (2009). Age-related decline in nicotinic receptor availability with [(123)I]5-IA-85380 SPECT. Neurobiol. Aging 30, 1490–1497. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.12.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nardone R., Bergmann J., Christova M., Caleri F., Tezzon F., Ladurner G., et al. (2012). Short latency afferent inhibition differs among the subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. J. Neural Transm. 119, 463–471. 10.1007/s00702-011-0725-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newhouse P. A., Potter A., Corwin J., Lenox R. (1994). Age-related effects of the nicotinic antagonist mecamylamine on cognition and behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology 10, 93–107. 10.1038/npp.1994.11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ni Z., Gunraj C., Wagle-Shukla A., Udupa K., Mazzella F., Lozano A. M., et al. (2011). Direct demonstration of inhibitory interactions between long interval intracortical inhibition and short interval intracortical inhibition. J. Physiol. 589, 2955–2962. 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.207928 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noda Y., Cash R. F., Zomorrodi R., Dominguez L. G., Farzan F., Rajji T. K., et al. (2016). A combined TMS-EEG study of short-latency afferent inhibition in the motor and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 116, 938–948. 10.1152/jn.00260.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Opie G. M., Semmler J. G. (2014). Age-related differences in short- and long-interval intracortical inhibition in a human hand muscle. Brain Stimul. 7, 665–672. 10.1016/j.brs.2014.06.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peinemann A., Lehner C., Conrad B., Siebner H. R. (2001). Age-related decrease in paired-pulse intracortical inhibition in the human primary motor cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 313, 33–36. 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02239-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellicciari M. C., Miniussi C., Ferrari C., Koch G., Bortoletto M. (2016). Ongoing cumulative effects of single TMS pulses on corticospinal excitability: an intra- and inter-block investigation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 621–628. 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Premoli I., Castellanos N., Rivolta D., Belardinelli P., Bajo R., Zipser C., et al. (2014). TMS-EEG signatures of GABAergic neurotransmission in the human cortex. J. Neurosci. 34, 5603–5612. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5089-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogasch N. C., Daskalakis Z. J., Fitzgerald P. B. (2015). Cortical inhibition of distinct mechanisms in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is related to working memory performance: a TMS-EEG study. Cortex 64, 68–77. 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rusjan P. M., Barr M. S., Farzan F., Arenovich T., Maller J. J., Fitzgerald P. B., et al. (2010). Optimal transcranial magnetic stimulation coil placement for targeting the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex using novel magnetic resonance image-guided neuronavigation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 1643–1652. 10.1002/hbm.20964 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sailer A., Molnar G. F., Paradiso G., Gunraj C. A., Lang A. E., Chen R. (2003). Short and long latency afferent inhibition in Parkinson's disease. Brain 126, 1883–1894. 10.1093/brain/awg183 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan D. V., Lecrubier Y., Sheehan K. H., Amorim P., Janavs J., Weiller E., et al. (1998). The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59(Suppl. 20), 22–33. quiz: 34–57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tokimura H., Di Lazzaro V., Tokimura Y., Oliviero A., Profice P., Insola A., et al. (2000). Short latency inhibition of human hand motor cortex by somatosensory input from the hand. J. Physiol. 523(Pt 2), 503–513. 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00503.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang Y., Paspalas C. D., Jin L. E., Picciotto M. R., Arnsten A. F., Wang M. (2013). Nicotinic alpha7 receptors enhance NMDA cognitive circuits in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 12078–12083. 10.1073/pnas.1307849110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young-Bernier M., Kamil Y., Tremblay F., Davidson P. S. (2012). Associations between a neurophysiological marker of central cholinergic activity and cognitive functions in young and older adults. Behav. Brain Funct. 8:17. 10.1186/1744-9081-8-17 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young-Bernier M., Tanguay A. N., Davidson P. S., Tremblay F. (2014). Short-latency afferent inhibition is a poor predictor of individual susceptibility to rTMS-induced plasticity in the motor cortex of young and older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:182. 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00182 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young-Bernier M., Tanguay A. N., Tremblay F., Davidson P. S. (2015). Age differences in reaction times and a neurophysiological marker of cholinergic activity. Can. J. Aging 34, 471–480. 10.1017/S0714980815000409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.