Skip to main content
Translational Psychiatry logoLink to Translational Psychiatry
. 2017 Mar 7;7(3):e1054. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.34

Effects of naltrexone are influenced by childhood adversity during negative emotional processing in addiction recovery

G Savulich 1,2, R Riccelli 2,3, L Passamonti 4, M Correia 5, J F W Deakin 6, R Elliott 6, R S A Flechais 7, A R Lingford-Hughes 7, J McGonigle 7, A Murphy 6, D J Nutt 7, C Orban 7, L M Paterson 7, L J Reed 7, D G Smith 1,2, J Suckling 1,2, R Tait 1,2, E M Taylor 6, B J Sahakian 1,2, T W Robbins 2,8, K D Ersche 1,2,*; ICCAM Platform
PMCID: PMC5416677  PMID: 28267152

Abstract

Naltrexone is an opioid receptor antagonist used in the management of alcohol dependence. Although the endogenous opioid system has been implicated in emotion regulation, the effects of mu-opioid receptor blockade on brain systems underlying negative emotional processing are not clear in addiction. Individuals meeting criteria for alcohol dependence alone (n=18, alcohol) and in combination with cocaine and/or opioid dependence (n=21, alcohol/drugs) and healthy individuals without a history of alcohol or drug dependence (n=21) were recruited. Participants were alcohol and drug abstinent before entered into this double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized, crossover study. Functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to investigate brain response while viewing aversive and neutral images relative to baseline on 50 mg of naltrexone and placebo. We found that naltrexone modulated task-related activation in the medial prefrontal cortex and functional connectivity between the anterior cingulate cortex and the hippocampus as a function of childhood adversity (for aversive versus neutral images) in all groups. Furthermore, there was a group-by-treatment-by-condition interaction in the right amygdala, which was mainly driven by a normalization of response for aversive relative to neutral images under naltrexone in the alcohol/drugs group. We conclude that early childhood adversity is one environmental factor that influences pharmacological response to naltrexone. Pharmacotherapy with naltrexone may also have some ameliorative effects on negative emotional processing in combined alcohol and drug dependence, possibly due to alterations in endogenous opioid transmission or the kappa-opioid receptor antagonist actions of naltrexone.

Introduction

Emotions have a critical role in the development, maintenance and successful treatment of addiction.1, 2, 3 Suppression of negative affective states such as anxiety and withdrawal symptoms is one motivational pathway to support the consumption of alcohol.4, 5 As negatively reinforced drinking becomes more pronounced, negative affective states increase, thereby escalating alcohol intake and raising vulnerability to relapse after treatment.6, 7 Negative reinforcement is driven by activation of stress-induced neurocircuitry in what is widely referred to as the ‘dark side' view of addiction.8, 9 Negative emotion is thus a key affective process that could be targeted by treatment interventions for addiction.

Functional abnormalities during negative emotional processing have been found in limbic and cortical networks in substance dependence.1 Threat-related reactivity of the amygdala, for example, is strongly associated with negatively reinforced problem drinking.10 Connectivity between the amygdala and the hippocampus is associated with maladaptive emotional processing,11 and alterations in hippocampal network activation and connectivity have been shown to predict relapse.12 The prefrontal cortex has extensive connections with subcortical structures that regulate emotional processing, including the amygdala.13 Alcohol and drug exposure impairs emotion regulation in this region, with interconnected medial and cingulate networks showing enhanced reactivity to arousing stimuli and reduced capacity to suppress negative affect.14 The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) also act to appraise and regulate negative emotions.15 These cortical areas over-activate in response to substance-related and naturally evocative stimuli16 and, together with limbic regions associated with impaired emotional processing in addiction, are candidate loci for pharmacological intervention.

Naltrexone is one pharmacotherapy used in the management of alcohol dependence that works by modulating opioid control of dopaminergic cell firing in the ventral tegmental area, thus preventing an increase in dopaminergic activity.17 The endogenous opioid system has been implicated in emotion regulation.18 There is some evidence of naltrexone dampening responses to negative emotional stimuli in healthy adults,19 although this likely reflects stress-reducing effects of the potent kappa-opioid receptor (KOR) antagonist actions of naltrexone. mu-opioid receptor (MOR) antagonism is known to precipitate withdrawal symptoms in humans with current20 and previous21 opiate use. MOR antagonism also precipitates aversive consequences of withdrawal from chronic opioid exposure in animals.22, 23 Naltrexone increases negative emotions in response to stress- and drug-related images in individuals with opioid dependence24 and increases anxiety in response to drug-related films in individuals with alcohol and cocaine dependence.25 However, the efficacy of naltrexone treatment has been shown to be associated with greater naltrexone-induced aversion26 (that is, the more negative the aversive stimulus, the greater the treatment response). This may be due, in part, by activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis associated with all substances of abuse potential. Specifically, naltrexone-induced adrenocorticotropic hormone and cortisol levels are thought to reduce craving in individuals with alcohol dependence.27 Exposure to psychological trauma, another form of aversion, is also known to contribute to individual treatment responses to naltrexone.28 This is consistent with preclinical evidence showing that early environmental adversity is associated with better treatment effects of naltrexone,29 suggesting that adversity experienced in early stages of development upregulates endogenous opioid function.

Childhood adversity has long been known to be common in substance use disorders, with at least two-thirds of alcohol- or drug-dependent adults reporting a history of physical, sexual or emotional abuse.30 Childhood adversity leads to more illicit substance use and increases the risk of dependence in adulthood.31, 32, 33 Combined alcohol and drug dependence is particularly harmful, as individuals dependent on both consume more units of alcohol and have greater incidence and severity of psychiatric illness than individuals dependent on alcohol alone.34 The heterogeneity of alcohol use disorders, including concurrent non-alcohol drug use, necessitates development of more tailored treatment approaches.35, 36 However, patients are typically categorized according to their primary dependency or by the drug for which they seek treatment.37 Personalized assessment and specialized treatments addressing the effects of combined alcohol and drug-taking behavior are often omitted, thus reducing the potential for more successful recovery.6

We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate brain response to aversive and neutral images. We sought to determine the effects of naltrexone at standard dose (50 mg) during negative emotional processing between groups dependent on alcohol alone, dependent on alcohol and drugs (both in abstinence) and healthy control volunteers. On the basis of previous research showing altered activation in limbic and cortical networks during negative emotional processing in substance-dependent individuals,1, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15 including the processing of evocative16 and negative emotional images,38, 39 we hypothesized that the dependent groups would show increased activation in the amygdala, the mPFC and the hippocampus in response to aversive images. In light of preclinical evidence,29 we further hypothesized that these effects would be modulated by naltrexone depending on the degree of childhood adversity experienced.

Materials and methods

Participants

This was a double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized, crossover study involving three sites (Imperial College London, University of Cambridge, University of Manchester; ICCAM). Full details of the ICCAM platform are reported elsewhere.40, 41, 42 Briefly, inclusion criteria were fluency in English; age 20–64; meeting DSM-IV criteria43 for alcohol, cocaine, amphetamine or opiate dependence; and abstinence from alcohol or drugs for at least 4 weeks prior to the experimental medicine sessions. Control participants did not meet DSM-IV criteria for any disorder. Exclusion criteria for all participants were use of medication that could not be paused for the study duration; current primary Axis I or neurological diagnosis; current or past psychiatric history (excluding lifetime or secondary history of anxiety or depression); and MRI contraindications. All participants screened negative (using urine samples) for amphetamines, barbiturates, cocaine, opiates, cannabinoids and benzodiazepines.

Here, we compared the effects of naltrexone between individuals who were only dependent on alcohol from individuals who in addition to alcohol were also dependent on cocaine and/or opiates. This distinction was made due to evidence showing that individuals dependent on alcohol and drugs have more severe psychopathology,34 and differences in brain function44 and structure45 compared with individuals dependent on alcohol alone. Control participants with a history of heavy alcohol or drug use were also excluded due to known associated functional and structural abnormalities.46 Heavy alcohol use was defined as a score higher than eight on the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT).47 Heavy drug use was defined by clinicians of the ICCAM team using the following criteria: more than 300 pills (lifetime) for MDMA; daily/almost daily THC use for more than one year and more than 2 spliffs; and more than once a week for more than six months and more than 1 g for amphetamines. On the basis of these criteria, patients with drug dependence but without alcohol dependence (n=11) and control participants with a history of heavy alcohol or drug use (n=9) were excluded.

The final alcohol group (alcohol) comprised 18 individuals meeting criteria for alcohol dependence only. The alcohol and drugs group (alcohol/drugs) comprised 21 individuals meeting criteria for alcohol dependence and the following drug dependencies: cocaine (42.9%); cocaine and opiates (47.6%); amphetamines (4.8%); and opiates (4.8%). The control group comprised 21 individuals without a history of alcohol or drug dependence.

Baseline assessment

All participants completed the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR)48 to measure verbal intelligence and control volunteers also completed the AUDIT to screen for harmful drinking patterns. To investigate the effects of individual variations of perceived stress and adverse childhood experiences on emotional processing, participants completed the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-14)49 and the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ).50 For the PSS-14, we used the total score and for the CTQ we calculated an abuse composite score from three abuse subscales, that is, physical, sexual and emotional abuse.51 In light of prior research suggesting the effects of naltrexone might be modulated by variations in locus of control beliefs,52 participants completed Rotter's locus of control scale53 and a drug-related version.51

Procedure

This study received ethical approval from the West London and Gene Therapy Advisory Committee National Research Ethics Service committee (11/H0707/9). Participants provided consent, basic demographic information and baseline assessment measures. For the experimental medicine sessions, participants completed a urine screen and alcohol breath and pregnancy tests. Participants screening negative were dosed 2 h prior to the MRI scan. The experimental medicine sessions consisted of administering placebo or 50 mg of naltrexone in a counterbalanced order. The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II)54 and the Spielberger-State Anxiety Inventory (STAI)55 were administered to evaluate current mood on the day of testing. Smoking was permitted up to 1 h before scanning; caffeine was only permitted in the morning. Participants were given a snack on arrival but subsequent food intake was restricted to ensure full drug absorption. The experimental medicine sessions were separated by at least 1 week.

Evocative images task

The evocative images task probes negative arousal to aversive stimuli.40 Aversive images of threat or injury were contrasted with neutral images of human or inanimate objects selected from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS: https://csea.phhp.ufl.edu/media/iapsmessage.html). Any images with alcohol or drugs in them were not included. Similar tasks have been used to demonstrate functional activation in alcohol- and drug-dependent groups in response to negative IAPS images.38, 39 A total of 240 images (120 aversive, 120 neutral) were presented in a block design consisting of two runs. Each run contained eight blocks of six images presented for 5 s each, followed by a 400 ms inter-stimulus interval that consisted of a fixation cross. Participants were given the implicit task to press a response button when the next image appeared on the screen. Images were counterbalanced for valence and arousal between sessions and between blocks and were presented in a pseudorandomized order, with a neutral image always presented first. Each block was separated by a 15 s rest period to prevent habituation effects. Response latency (in ms) was recorded for each participant in response to all images presented.

Statistical analysis

Demographic, baseline assessment and behavioral measures

Demographic and baseline assessment measures were analyzed using univariate analysis of variance (ANOVAs) for continuous variables and chi-square for categorical variables. Post-hoc Tukey tests were used to identify pairwise differences for continuous variables. Mean response latencies during emotional processing were square-root transformed to stabilize variances relative to the mean (untransformed scores presented in Table 1). Repeated-measures ANOVAs were used with the two within-subject factors condition (neutral, aversive) and treatment (placebo, naltrexone) and the between-subject factor group (control, alcohol, alcohol/drugs) to examine response latencies.

Table 1. Evocative Images task mean group latencies (ms) and standard deviations in response to neutral and aversive images on placebo and naltrexone.
Condition Treatment Control group, n=21 Alcohol group, n=18 Alcohol/drugs group, n=21
Neutral images Placebo 691.14 (±294.94) 862.17 (±291.95) 740.62 (±313.31)
Neutral images Naltrexone 671.14 (±237) 893.50 (±354.28) 724.29 (±219.06)
Aversive images Placebo 684.48 (±261.16) 945.94 (±368.01) 855.71 (±457.75)
Aversive images Naltrexone 689.76 (±238.30) 970.39 (±478.30) 806.71 (±382.78)

Means are untransformed scores.

Repeated-measures ANOVAs were also used to evaluate changes in affective states between the experimental medicine visits. To determine the influence of naltrexone on task performance, we calculated a change score for each individual by subtracting mean latencies from each condition (aversive minus neutral) following placebo and naltrexone. We then used Pearson's r coefficients to test for associations between change scores and individual variations in affective states.

Neuroimaging data acquisition, processing and analysis

Volumetric MRI data was acquired with 3 T systems at three sites (Imanova, London; Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre, Cambridge; and the Translational Imaging Unit, Manchester). London and Cambridge operated nominally identical 3 T Siemens Tim Trio systems and Manchester operated a 3 T Philips Achieva.

fMRI data were preprocessed using SPM8. Mean echo-planar imaging (EPI) was first computed for each participant and visually inspected in the orbitofrontal cortex and temporal lobe to ensure that none showed excessive signal dropout. All EPIs were then realigned to the first scan by rigid body transformations to correct for head movements. Next, EPIs were normalized to the standard template in the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space using linear and nonlinear transformations and smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of full width at half maximum of 8 mm. Realignment parameters were then inspected for each subject to make sure that movements of translation and rotation were <2 mm and 2°, respectively.

To identify significant differences across groups in regional responses of brain areas, a general linear model (GLM) was employed. Subject-specific GLMs included three experimental factors (aversive images, neutral images and fixation cross) and six realignment parameters as effects of no interest to account for residual motion-related variance. Low-frequency signal drift was removed using a high-pass filter (cut-off 128 s). An autoregressive modeling of temporal autocorrelations was applied. The following contrasts were generated for the second-level analysis: (1) aversive images versus neutral images, (2) aversive images versus fixation cross and (3) neutral images versus fixation cross.

Group differences in brain responses were assessed via: (1) a full factorial analysis for the contrast ‘aversive versus neutral' including three groups (control, alcohol, alcohol/drugs) and two pharmacological treatments (placebo, naltrexone) as main factors and (2) a full factorial analysis including three groups (control, alcohol, alcohol/drugs), two pharmacological treatments (placebo, naltrexone) and two task conditions (aversive images versus fixation cross, neutral images versus fixation cross) as main factors. For each model, SPM-F-maps assessing the main effect of group, the main effect of treatment, the group-by-treatment interaction and the group-by-treatment-by-condition interaction were generated, respectively. An additional covariate of site was added to remove the effect of the different scan sites. In the first model, we also included the abuse composite score as a covariate-of-interest to test for associations between brain response and childhood adversity as a function of pharmacological treatment.

To threshold the second-level maps, we used a priori regions of interest (ROIs) based on a functional definition of the brain regions as previously recommended (P<0.05, family-wise error (FWE) correction for multiple comparisons after a small volume correction).56, 57, 58, 59 This commonly employed statistical procedure not only ensures a robust protection against type I errors but also prevents false negative results.

Here, the following anatomical ROIs were selected because of their functional role in negative emotional processing in substance use disorders:1, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 38, 39 the amygdala, the mPFC and the hippocampus. Each ROI was defined using a sphere centered on MNI coordinates obtained from previous studies.60, 61 Specifically, a sphere with a radius of 5 mm was centered on the right amygdala (x,y,z: 25,−3,−27), a sphere with a radius of 10 mm was centered on the right mPFC (x,y,z: 14,62,1) and a sphere with a radius of 8 mm was centered on the right hippocampus (x,y,z: 29,−14,−14). The different size of the spheres was chosen to reflect the actual size of the regions. Brain regions that were not predicted a priori but met a threshold of P<0.05, whole-brain corrected, were also reported.

Functional connectivity: psycho-physiological interaction analyses

Psycho-physiological interaction (PPI) represents the change in connectivity between a seed region and the rest of the brain that is induced by a specific psychological context. The ACC was chosen as a seed due to the high density of MORs in this region that potentially reflects higher binding potential associated with greater capacity to modulate negative emotional processing.62 We sought to identify brain regions that had a differential functional connectivity pattern with the seed region during the processing of aversive versus neutral images. For each participant, a 15 mm sphere was centered on the right anterior cingulate cortex (x,y,z: 6,60,4; extracted from the first model). The time series of the BOLD response for each participant was then computed using the first eigenvariate from all voxels' time series in the sphere.

The BOLD time series for each individual was deconvolved to estimate a neuronal time series for the source, using the PPIs deconvolution parameter defaults in SPM8.63 The PPI regressor was calculated as the element-by-element product of the seed neuronal time series and a vector coding for the main effect of task (1 for aversive images, −1 for neutral images). This product was reconvolved by the canonical haemodynamic response function. The statistical model also included the main effect of the task convolved by the haemodynamic response function, the seed neuronal time series and the six movement parameters as effects of no interest.

Subject-specific PPI models were run, and contrast images were generated such that the identified target regions were those that showed a change in connectivity with the ACC during the processing of aversive versus neutral images. Subject-specific PPI contrast images were entered into second-level GLMs to assess if change in connectivity between the ACC and other regions in the brain were associated with childhood adversity as a function of pharmacological treatment. The same statistical approaches previously described were employed to threshold the second-level PPI maps.

Results

Demographic information and baseline assessment

As shown in Table 2, the three groups did not differ with regard to gender, age, verbal intelligence, handedness, smoking status and locus of control beliefs (all P's>0.05). However, significant group differences emerged with regard to stress sensitivity (F2,57=5.71, P=0.005) and childhood adversity (F2,57=6.63, P=0.003), such that the alcohol/drugs group reported significantly higher sensitivity to stress compared with the control group (Tukey's P=0.004) and significantly higher levels of childhood adversity compared with the control (Tukey's P=0.005) and alcohol (Tukey's P=0.012) groups.

Table 2. Group demographic information and baseline assessment prior to the experimental medicine sessions.

  Control group, n=21 Alcohol group, n=18 Alcohol/drugs group, n=21 Statistic, P-value
Site (London: Cambridge: Manchester) 8 L: 12 C: 1 M 9 L: 6 C: 3M 12 L: 7 C: 2 M X2=4.04, P=0.40
Gender (male: female) 17 M: 4 F 14 M: 4 F 16 M: 5 F X2=0.15, P=0.93
Age (years) 41.52 (±10.05) 44.22 (±8.72) 40.57 (±7.43) F2,57=0.88, P=0.42
Verbal IQ (WTAR) 106.57 (±10.88) 105.78 (±8.31) 99.57 (±11.45) F2,57=2.80, P=0.07
Handedness (Edinburgh Inventory) 40.62 (±67.71) 58.39 (±66.72) 61.33 (±58.06) F2,57=0.63, P=0.54
Smoking (smoker: non-smoker) 9 S: 12 N-S 13 S: 5 N-S 16 S: 5 N-S X2=5.90, P=0.052
Stress sensitivity (PSS-14, total score) 15.10 (±6.36) 18 (±7.23) 21.95 (±6.27) F2,57=5.71, P=0.005
Childhood adversity (CTQ total abuse score) 21.90 (±10.07) 22.61 (±12.06) 34.24 (±16.50) F2,57=6.63, P=0.003
Locus of control (Rotter's I–E) 10.25 (±43.18) 10.76 (±43.39) 10.40 (±43.28) F2,57=0.05, P=0.95
Drug-related locus of control (DR-LOC recovery) 1.24 (±1.61) 1.50 (±1.58) 1.48 (±.98) F2,57=0.21, P=0.81

Abbreviations: CTQ, Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; DR-LOC, drug-related locus of control; PSS-14, Perceived Stress Scale; Rotter's I–E, Rotter's internal–external control scale; WTAR, Wechsler Test of Adult Reading.

The groups also differed with regard to affective states (BDI-II, STAI-State) on the day of testing, as assessed prior to scanning on placebo (F2,57=6.37, P=0.003; F2,57=8.42, P=0.001) and naltrexone (F2,57=5.04, P=0.010; F2,57=3.93, P=0.025). The alcohol/drugs group scored significantly higher than the control group on total scores for both measures (both Tukey's P=0.002). However, affective state did not significantly fluctuate between the two experimental medicine sessions, as reflected by non-significant group-by-session interactions (BDI-II: F2,57=.64, P=0.534; STAI-State: F2,57=2.13, P=0.129), and the main effects of treatment were not significant for either measure (BDI-II: F1,57=1.41, P=0.240; STAI-State: F1,57=1.72, P=0.194). Total scores of the BDI-II and STAI-State did not correlate significantly with behavioral change scores (all P's>0.05) and consequently were not used as covariates in subsequent behavioral analyses. As we did not identify differences in demographics between participants recruited at the three different sites (all P's>0.2), we did not include scanning site as a covariate in behavioral analyses.

Task-related performance during evocative image processing

As shown in Table 1, participants in both the alcohol and alcohol/drugs groups tended to respond more slowly in general, but there was considerable variability and differences between the groups failed to reach significance (F2,57=3.04, P=0.056). There was a main effect of condition (aversive versus neutral images; F1,57=11.27, P=0.001), such that response latency to aversive images (818.87 ms s.d. ±343.55) was significantly longer than response latency to neutral images (758.11 ms ±271.47) (t59=−3.36, P=0.001) across groups. However, there was no main effect of treatment (F1,57=0.006, P=0.941) and the group-by-treatment (F2,57=0.90, P=0.915) and the group-by-treatment-by-condition (F2,57=0.45, P=0.640) interactions were also not significant.

Task-related brain activation during evocative image processing

Significant correlations between the ROIs and childhood adversity were found during the processing of aversive versus neutral images as a function of pharmacological treatment. Figure 1a shows a positive association between activity in the right mPFC (x,y,z: 20,60,0) and childhood adversity (z-score=3.71, P=0.004 FWE for multiple comparisons after small volume correction) and, similarly, Figure 1b shows a positive association between functional connectivity between the right ACC and the right hippocampus (x,y,z: 22,−16,−10) and childhood adversity (z-score=3.11; P=0.002, FWE for multiple comparisons after small volume correction) in the placebo relative to the naltrexone session. This pattern of association was found irrespective of group.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Naltrexone modulated (a) task-related activation in the right medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC; MNI coordinates: x,y,z: 20,60,0; z-score=3.71, P=0.004) and (b) functional connectivity between the right anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; seed coordinates: x,y,z: −14,62,1) and the right hippocampus (MNI coordinates: x,y,z: 22,−16, −10; z-score=3.11; P=0.002) as a function of childhood adversity (Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) abuse score) in all groups.

As shown in Table 3, there was a main effect of group and a main effect of condition in temporal–occipital regions. There was a significant group-by-treatment-by-condition interaction (z-score=2.66; P=0.032 FWE for multiple comparisons after small volume correction) in the right amygdala (x,y,z: 26,2,−24; Figure 2) while viewing aversive and neutral images relative to baseline. Figure 2a shows that the alcohol and control groups had higher activation in the amygdala when processing aversive relative to neutral images on placebo, thus demonstrating increased activation to threat-related stimuli. However, activation in the amygdala increased to both aversive and neutral images in the alcohol/drugs group, irrespective of emotional valence.

Table 3. Significant whole-brain-corrected regions for the main effect of group, main effect of condition and group-by-treatment-by-condition interaction.

  Hemisphere P-value z-score Coordinates (x:y:z)
Main effect of group
 Fusiform gyrus Left 0.012 4.83 −26, −66, −16
 Inferior occipital gyrus Left 0.025 4.66 −26, −82, −6
 Precuneus Right 0.029 4.63 30, −60, 28
         
Main effect of condition
 Inferior temporal gyrus Right 0.001 5.27 46, −44, −18
 Superior temporal gyrus Left 0.023 4.66 −58, −12, 4
 Middle occipital gyrus Left 0.028 4.61 −44, −80, −2
         
Group-by-treatment-by-condition interaction
Amygdala Right 0.032 2.66 26, 2, −24

Figure 2.

Figure 2

(a) Significant group-by-treatment-by-condition interaction in the right amygdala (MNI coordinates: x,y,z: 26,2,−24; z-score=2.66; P=0.032). (b) On placebo, the control and alcohol groups showed increased activation to aversive relative to neutral images, whereas the alcohol/drugs groups showed increased activation to all visual images, irrespective of emotional valence. (c) Naltrexone normalized activation in the alcohol/drugs group, such that activation was higher to aversive relative to neutral images, but did not change the pattern of activation in the control or alcohol groups. MNI, Montreal Neurological Institute.

Figure 2b shows that the group-by-treatment-by-condition interaction was mainly driven by a normalization of response in the amygdala for aversive relative to neutral images under naltrexone in the alcohol/drugs group, whereas the pattern of activation did not change in the alcohol and control groups. There was no main effect of treatment and the group-by-treatment interaction was not significant.

Discussion

Influence of childhood adversity on naltrexone

Childhood adversity was highly prevalent in our alcohol/drugs group and significantly differed between the patient groups. This is in line with previous studies showing that individuals with a history of childhood abuse are more prone to combined alcohol and drug taking in adulthood.31, 32, 33 Sensitivity to stress was also high in our alcohol/drugs group compared with the control group. These differences support that aversive emotional states contribute to negatively reinforced drug seeking, consistent with the ‘dark side' view of addiction.8, 9 Negative reinforcement thus acts to increase more compulsive drug-seeking behavior, particularly during the withdrawal/negative affect stage of the addiction cycle.64

Similarities between the effects of childhood maltreatment, one form of early adversity, and impairments in emotion regulation systems underlying vulnerability to substance use disorders have been identified, but are poorly understood.65 In maltreated adolescents, increased activation in prefrontal regions has been found during effortful control of emotion regulation.66 However, surprisingly few fMRI studies have investigated brain response to emotional stimuli in substance-dependent groups abused in childhood. It has been shown that functional alterations in mPFC activity are associated with greater maltreatment severity in response to drug- and stress-related cues in individuals with cocaine dependence.67 Heighted mesolimbic response has also been found in response to drug-related and evocative cues in cocaine-dependent individuals reporting a history of emotional, physical and sexual abuse.68 Consistent with preclinical evidence demonstrating that the therapeutic efficacy of naltrexone is associated with early adverse experiences,29 we found that naltrexone modulated task-related activation in the mPFC, one key region associated with emotion dysregulation in both substance use disorders and adults with histories of childhood adversity, depending on the degree of abuse experienced.

Naltrexone also modulated ACC–hippocampal connectivity, two regions integral to mood regulation,69 depending on childhood adversity. In fMRI studies, hippocampal activation and network connectivity has been shown to predict cocaine relapse.12 Limbic connectivity between the hippocampus and the amygdala is also associated with reduced adaptive emotional processing in maltreated individuals with methamphetamine dependence.11 Interplay between the ACC and the hippocampus has been implicated in the reconsolidation and expression of fear memories,70 with alterations in ACC–hippocampal connectivity found in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder.71 These findings, together with our data, suggests that opioid receptors in the ACC and the hippocampus are important pharmacological targets in individuals with a history of abuse, which, although not specific to our substance-dependent groups, may be of relevance to other disorders characterized by childhood adversity, such as trauma-related or psychotic disorders.

The neurobiological effects of childhood adversity on emotional processing are under-investigated in addiction. Our data confirm that environmental factors, specifically early adverse experiences, influence variations in naltrexone response. It is possible that differences in childhood adversity might account for some of the variability in outcome in alcohol-dependent patients treated with naltrexone. Early life experiences have been shown to affect the endogenous opioid system in preclinical models,72, 73 and the present data offer convergent evidence that the effects of childhood adversity on endogenous opioids are an important indicator of responsiveness to pharmacological treatments acting on MOR transmission in humans. We suggest that behavioral interventions that promote emotion regulation strategies for traumatic experiences or pharmacological interventions that normalize cortical–limbic connectivity may be effective in groups with high levels of childhood adversity.

Effects of MOR blockade on different types of substance dependence

Previous fMRI studies have shown altered limbic activation in response to naturally evocative,16 negative emotional37, 38 and substance-related74, 75, 76, 77, 78 stimuli in substance-dependent groups. We observed greater task-related activation in the right amygdala during the processing of aversive relative to neutral images under placebo in individuals recovering from alcohol dependence. Heightened reactivity in this region is consistent with previous investigations measuring brain response to negative IAPS images in individuals with alcohol dependence.38 Similar to our alcohol group, participants from the above studies were characterized by only one drug dependency. The lack of discriminatory response between aversive and neutral images was specific to our alcohol/drugs group, possibly reflecting an emotional desensitization following prior exposure to adverse events, an attentional disengagement to aversive images (although we consider this unlikely as our task does not tap into attentional resources) or altered MOR availability/density associated with abstinence from repeated drug exposure (for example, cocaine;79, 80 opiates;81 alcohol82, 83) and/or individual differences in environment factors. Although speculative for the present study, these possibilities each deserve more detailed investigation in substance-dependent groups, including use of positron emission tomography to elucidate whether associations between MOR binding potential and limbic activation differ during negative emotional processing between alcohol-dependent patients with and without combined drug dependence.

Our data further showed that naltrexone normalized the pattern of activation in the amygdala for aversive relative to neutral images in our alcohol/drugs group (that is, there was only an effect of treatment at the group level when interacting with the content of the images presented). These data demonstrate that naltrexone-induced functional alterations during negative emotional processing are specific to groups characterized by distinct patterns of drug dependencies that can persist in prolonged abstinence. Activation of stress-induced neurocircuitry contributes to the ‘dark side' view of addiction, in which recruitment of brain ‘anti-reward' systems (for example, within motivational circuits of the extended amygdala), an opponent process to hyperactivation of the brain reward system, provides strong motivation for negatively reinforced drug seeking.64, 84 Increased activation in response to aversive images could also lend vulnerability to craving and relapse via stress-induced reinstatement of emotion-processing circuitry.8, 9, 84 However, a relative normalization of response by naltrexone in our alcohol/drugs group is likely due to a kappa-antagonist effect being more potent in individuals with increased dynorphin-dependent KOR activation. The latter is associated with negative emotional states, higher sensitivity to stress and/or more severe psychopathology as a consequence of combined drug taking.85 Stress-induced activation of the dynorphin/kappa-opioid system has dysphoric-like effects that are thought to mediate negative emotional states.86, 87 However, KOR blockade has shown consistent anxiolytic and antidepressant effects in both humans and animals, including attenuation of a stress response.88, 89 Evaluation of the potential therapeutic benefits of selective kappa-opioid antagonism in the treatment of addiction and comorbid stress-related mood disorders thus may form an important area of new research. Increasing activation of the HPA axis with mu-opioid antagonism (including adrenocorticotropic hormone, beta-endorphin and cortisol levels) is also a potential mechanism for reducing craving and withdrawal symptoms in alcohol addiction.

Treatment implications

Naltrexone significantly reduces return to heavy drinking (to 83% of the risk of placebo) and decreases drinking days by about 4%.90 In individuals with opioid dependence, the efficacy of naltrexone treatment is largely heterogeneous and most effective in subgroups that complete treatment.91 However, medication compliance and retention rates remain poor (28%), particularly during early stages of recovery.92 This is likely explained by unwanted side effects such as sedation (for example, daytime sleepiness) and gastrointestinal problems (for example, nausea, stomach pain, loss of appetite).90 Extended-release naltrexone has thus been developed and used, with success, to improve adherence over and above other pharmacotherapies used in the treatment of alcohol and opioid dependence (for example, acamprosate, buprenorphine).93

Encouragingly, cognitive behavioral therapy with adjunct naltrexone treatment has shown to reduce return to drinking in alcohol-dependent adults, thus suggesting beneficial, synergistic effects of concurrent therapies.94 Cognitive behavioral therapy also has the potential to improve functional abnormalities during emotional processing (in the medial prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices, for example), as shown in patients with depression.95, 96 Interventions that combine naltrexone and coping skills have shown to improve treatment outcome in alcohol and cocaine dependence.97, 98, 99 Coping skills therapies with a particular focus on relapse prevention may also help naltrexone-treated patients better manage their emotions after non-substance using strategies are implemented. As the current treatment length of naltrexone is recommended for 6 months,17 coping skills for childhood adversity should be addressed and evaluated as part of a longer-term therapeutic process for addiction recovery.

Limitations

The main limitation of our analyses is its use of a retrospective self-report measure of childhood adversity. Measurement of an emotional or traumatic event may be unreliable as recollection is likely to be influenced by several confounding factors including memory biases, memory repression and current mood; however, retrospective recall of physical and sexual abuse is associated with more false negatives than false positives,100 suggesting a greater tendency for adults to under- rather than to overestimate the occurrence of an abusive experience.

Secondly, our study only investigated the effects of naltrexone on the processing of aversive images compared with neutral images. Comparison with positive or rewarding non-alcohol and -drug-related images would have allowed us to investigate if the valence of emotional processing had a differential treatment effect, or if motivational salience had the same effect irrespective of emotionality.

Lastly, childhood adversity is a robust risk factor for other disorders in which emotional problems pervade. Major depressive disorder, for example, is associated with abnormal responses to negative emotional stimuli in similar brain regions observed here.95, 96 It is possible that variations in anxiety and depression could also drive abnormalities in negative emotional processing in substance use disorders, or that more severe comorbidity has a particularly potent effect for enhancing emotion-processing circuitry. Although we cannot preclude this possibility from the current data, levels of anxiety and depression were consistent in groups across the experimental medicine sessions (and therefore exerting the same influence on emotional processing at both time points), and sensitivity to stress did not differ between the patient groups at baseline.

Conclusions

Problems with emotion regulation may predate substance dependence, or may serve to exacerbate susceptibility to relapse during addiction recovery. Furthermore, loss of brain reward and recruitment of brain stress systems produces negatively reinforced motivation for compulsive drug seeking and addiction. The present study demonstrates that the effects of naltrexone on cortical activation and cortical–limbic connectivity are dependent on variations in childhood adversity (that is, the more abuse experienced, the greater the ameliorative effect of naltrexone). Pharmacotherapy with naltrexone may also have some ameliorative effects on negative emotional processing in the amygdala in individuals with combined alcohol and drug dependence, but not alcohol dependence alone, possibly because of alterations in endogenous opioid transmission or the KOR antagonist actions of naltrexone. Childhood adversity was higher in individuals with combined alcohol and drug dependence compared with individuals dependent on alcohol only and should thus be an important consideration by treatment strategies for addiction. We conclude that childhood adversity is one environmental factor that influences pharmacological response to naltrexone. More tailored treatment approaches are required that take into account such early experiential factors.

Acknowledgments

ICCAM platform collaborators: David Nutt, Anne Lingford-Hughes, Louise Paterson, John McGonigle, Remy Flechais, Csaba Orban, JF William Deakin, Rebecca Elliott, Anna Murphy, Eleanor Taylor, Trevor Robbins, Karen Ersche, John Suckling, Dana Smith, Laurence Reed, Filippo Passetti, Luca Faravelli, David Erritzoe, Inge Mick, Nicola Kalk, Adam Waldman, Liam Nestor, Shankar Kuchibatla, Venkataramana Boyapati, Antonio Metastasio, Yetunde Faluyi, Emilio Fernandez-Egea, Sanja Abbott, Barbara Sahakian, Valerie Voon and Ilan Rabiner. The research was supported by the NIHR CRF at Imperial College Healthcare NHS Trust, the NIHR/Wellcome Trust Cambridge Research Facility and Clinical Trials Unit at Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust, and is supported by the North West London, Eastern and Greater Manchester NIHR Clinical Research Networks. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the Medical Research Council, the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. We wish to thank research assistants Claire Whitelock, Heather Agyepong, Rania Christoforou and Natalie Cuzen for their help with data collection, MR physicist Rex Newbould and MR technician, Jonathan Howard for their assistance with MR acquisition and task set-up. We also wish to thank their recruitment partners; Imperial College Healthcare NHS Trust, Central and North West London NHS Trust, Camden and Islington NHS Trust, Cambridge University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Norfolk and Suffolk NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, South Staffordshire and Shropshire NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester Mental Health NHS and Social Care Trust, Greater Manchester West NHS Foundation Trust, Pennine Care NHS Foundation Trust, Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust, Addaction, Foundation 66 and CRI (Crime Reduction Initiative). The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship and/or publication of this article: this article presents independent research funded by the MRC as part of their addiction initiative (Grant Number G1000018). George Savulich was funded by a grant from the Wallitt Foundation.

Footnotes

JFWD currently advises or carries out research funded by Autifony, Sunovion, Lundbeck, AstraZeneca and Servier. All payment is to the University of Manchester. ARL-H has received honoraria from Lundbeck and research support from GSK for a PhD studentship. DJN is an advisor to British National Formulary, MRC, General Medical Council, Department of Health, is President of the European Brain Council, past President of the British Neuroscience Association and European College of Neuropsychopharmacology, chair of the Independent Scientific Committee on Drugs (UK), is a member of the International Centre for Science in Drug Policy, advisor to Swedish government on drug, alcohol and tobacco research, editor of the Journal of Psychopharmacology, sits on advisory Boards at Lundbeck, MSD, Nalpharm, Orexigen, Shire, has received speaking honoraria (in addition to above) from BMS/Otsuka, GSK, Lilly, Janssen, Servier, is a member of the Lundbeck International Neuroscience Foundation, has received grants or clinical trial payments from P1vital, MRC, NHS, Lundbeck, has share options with P1vital, has been expert witness in a number of legal cases relating to psychotropic drugs, and has edited/written 27 books, some purchased by pharmaceutical companies. BJS consults for Cambridge Cognition, Brainbow (Peak), Servier, Otsuka, and Lundbeck, holds a grant from Janssen/Johnson & Johnson, and has share options in Cambridge Cognition. Trevor Robbins has had research grants with Eli Lilly and Lundbeck, has received royalties from Cambridge Cognition (CANTAB), has received editorial honoraria from Springer Verlag, Elsevier, Society for Neuroscience; has performed educational lectures for Merck, Sharpe and Dohme and does consultancy work for Cambridge Cognition, Eli Lilly, Lundbeck, Teva and Shire Pharmaceuticals. The remaining authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Wilcox CE, Pommy JM, Adinoff B. Neural circuitry of impaired emotion regulation in substance use disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2016; 173: 344–361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aguilar de Arcos F, Verdejo-Garcia A, Peralta-Ramierez MI, Sanchez-Barrera M, Perez-Garcia M. Experience of emotions in substance abusers exposed to images containing neutral, positive, and negative affective stimuli. Drug Alcohol Depend 2005; 78: 159–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ersche KD, Hagan CC, Smith DG, Jones PS, Calder AJ, Williams GB. In the face of threat: neural and endocrine correlates of impaired facial emotion recognition in cocaine dependence. Transl Psychiatry 2015; 5: e570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Heinz A, Löber S, Georgi A, Wrase J, Hermann D, Rey E-R et al. Reward craving and withdrawal relief craving: assessment of different motivational pathways to alcohol intake. Alcohol Alcohol 2003; 38: 35–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Simons JS, Carey KB, Wills TA. Alcohol abuse and dependence symptoms: A multidimensional model of common and specific etiology. Psychol Addict Behav 2009; 23: 415–427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Heilig M, Goldman D, Berrettini W, O'Brien CP. Pharmacogenetic approaches to the treatment of alcohol addiction. Nat Rev Neurosci 2011; 12: 670–684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Heilig M, Thorsell A, Sommer WH, Hansson AC, Ramchandani VA, George DT et al. Translating the neuroscience of alcoholism into clinical treatments: From blocking the buzz to curing the blues. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2010; 35: 334–344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science 1997; 278: 52–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology 2001; 24: 97–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Nikolova YS, Knodt AR, Radtke SR, Hariri AR. Divergent responses of the amygdala and ventral striatum predict stress-related problem drinking in young adults: possible differential markers of affective and impulsive pathways of risk for alcohol use disorder. Mol Psych 2016; 2: 348–356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dean AC, Kohno M, Hellermann G, London ED. Childhood maltreatment and amygdala connectivity in methamphetamine dependence. Brain Behav 2014; 4: 867–876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Adinoff B, Gu H, Merrick C, McHugh N, Jeon-Slaughter H, Lu H et al. Basal hippocampal activity and its functional connectivity predicts cocaine relapse. Biol Psychiatry 2015; 78: 496–504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Arnsten AFT. Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009; 10: 410–422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat Rev Neurosci 2011; 12: 652–669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 2011; 15: 85–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Garavan H, Pankiewicz J, Bloom A, Cho JK, Sperry L, Ross TJ. Cue-induced cocaine craving: neuroanatomical specificity for drug users and drug stimuli. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 157: 1789–1798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Lingford-Hughes A, Welch S, Peters L, Nutt DJ. BAP update guidelines: evidence-based guidelines for the pharmacological management of substance abuse, harmful use, addiction and comorbidity: recommendations from BAP. J Psychopharmacol 2012; 26: 899–952. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Liberzon I, Zubieta JK, Fig LM, Luan Phan K, Koeppe RA, Taylor SF. mu-Opioid receptors and limbic responses to aversive emotional stimuli. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002; 99: 7084–7089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Wardle MC, Bershad AK, de Wit H. Naltrexone alters the processing of social and emotional stimuli in healthy adults. Soc Neurosci 2016; 22: 1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kirby KC, Stitzer ML, Heishman SJ. Acute opioid physical dependence in humans: effect of varying the morphine-naloxone interval II. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1990; 255: 730–737. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Heishman SJ, Stitzer ML, Bigelow GE, Liebson IA. Acute opioid physical dependence in postaddict humans: naloxone dose effects after brief morphine exposure. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1989; 248: 127–134. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Zhang Z, Schulteis G. Withdrawal from acute morphine dependence is accompanied by increased anxiety-like behaviour in the elevated plus maze. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2008; 89: 392–403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Schulteis G, Morse AC, Liu J. Repeated experience with naloxone facilitates acute morphine withdrawal: potential role for conditioning processes in acute opioid dependence. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2003; 76: 493–503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hyman SM, Fox H, Hong KI, Doebrick C, Sinha R. Stress and drug-cue-induced craving in opioid-dependent individuals in naltrexone treatment. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2007; 15: 134–143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Modesto-Lowe V, Burleson JA, Hersh D, Bauer LO, Kranzler HR. Effects of naltrexone on cue-elicited craving for alcohol and cocaine. Drug Alcohol Depend 1997; 49: 9–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Mitchell JM, Bergren LJ, Chen KS, Rowbotham MC, Fields HL. Naltrexone aversion and treatment efficacy are greatest in humans and rats that actively consume high levels of alcohol. Neurobiol Dis. 2009; 33: 72–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. O'Malley SS, Krishnan-Sarin, Farren C, Sinha R, Kreek MJ. Naltrexone decreases craving and alcohol self-administration in alcohol-dependent subjects and activates the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Psychopharmacology 2002; 160: 19–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Liberzon I, Taylor SF, Luan Phan K, Britton JC, Fig LM, Bueller JA et al. Altered central and μ-opioid receptor binding after psychological trauma. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61: 1030–1038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Daoura L, Nylander I. The response to naltrexone in ethanol-drinking rats depends on early environmental experiences. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2011; 99: 626–633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Cohen FS, Densen-Gerber J. A study of the relationship between child abuse and drug addiction in 178 patients: preliminary results. Child Abuse Negl 1982; 6: 383–387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Enoch MA. The role of early life stress as a predictor for alcohol and drug dependence. Psychopharmacology 2011; 214: 17–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Widom CS, Marmorstein NR, White HR. Childhood victimization and illicit drug use in middle adulthood. Psychol Addic Behav 2006; 20: 394–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kessler RC, McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber NA, Sampson AM, Zaslavsky S et al. Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Healthy Surveys. Br J Psychiatry 2010; 197: 378–385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Moss HB, Goldstein RB, Chen CM, Yi H. Patterns of use of other drugs among those with alcohol dependence: associations with drinking behaviour and psychopathology. Addict Behav 2015; 50: 192–198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Cloninger CR, Sigvardsson S, Gilligan SB, von Knorring AL, Reich T, Bohman M. Genetic heterogeneity and the classification of alcoholism. Adv Alcohol Subst Abuse 1988; 7: 3–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Litten RZ, Ryan ML, Falk DE, Reilly M, Fertig JB, Koob GF. Heterogeneity of alcohol use disorder: understanding mechanisms to advance personalized treatment. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2015; 39: 579–584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Brecht M, Huang D, Evans E, Hser Y. Polydrug use and implications for longitudinal research: ten-year trajectories for heroin, cocaine, and methamphetamine users. Drug Alcohol Depend 2008; 96: 193–201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kim Y, Song H, Seo J, Lee J, Lee J, Kwon D et al. The differences in neural network activity between methamphetamine abusers and healthy subjects performing an emotion-matching task: functional MRI study. NMR Biomed 2011; 24: 1392–1400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gilman JM, Hommer DW. Modulation of brain response to emotional images by alcohol cues in alcohol-dependent patients. Addict Biol 2008; 13: 423–434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Paterson LM, Flechais RSA, Murphy M, Reed LJ, Abbott S, Boyapati V et al. The Imperial College Cambridge Manchester (ICCAM) platform study: an experimental medicine platform for evaluating new drugs for relapse prevention in addiction. Part A: study description. J Psychopharmacol 2015; 29: 943–960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. McGonigle J, Murphy A, Paterson LM, Reed LJ, Nestor L, Nash J et al. The ICCAM platform study: an experimental platform study for evaluating new drugs for relapse prevention in addiction. Part B: fMRI description. J Psychopharmacol 2016; 31: 3–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Taylor EM, Murphy A, Boyapati V, Ersche KD, Flechais R, Kuchibatla S et al. Impulsivity in abstinent alcohol and polydrug dependence: a multidimensional approach. Psychopharmacology 2016; 233: 1487–1499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. American Psychiatric Association.Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  44. Nestor LJ, Murphy A, McGonigle J, Orban C, Reed L, Taylor E et al. Acute naltrexone does not remediate fronto-striatal disturbances in alcoholic and alcoholic polysubstance-dependent populations during a monetary incentive delay task. Addict Biol 2016.. [DOI] [PubMed]
  45. Mon A, Durazzo TC, Abe C, Gazdzinski S, Pennington D, Schmidt T et al. Structural brain differences in alcohol-dependent individuals with and without comorbid substance dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend 2014; 144: 170–177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Squeglia LM, Gray KM. Alcohol and drug use and the developing brain. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2016; 18: 46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Bohn MJ, Babor TF, Kranzler HR. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): validation of a screening instrument for use in medical settings. J Stud Alcohol 1995; 56: 423–432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Wechsler D. Welscher Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd edn. The Psychological Corportation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  49. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav 1983; 24: 385–396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Bernstein DP, Fink L. Childhood Trauma Questionnaire: A Retrospective Self-Report Manual. The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  51. Ersche KD, Turton AJ, Croudace T, Stochl J. Who do you think is in control in addiction? A pilot study on drug-related locus of control beliefs. Addict Disord Their Treat 2012; 11: 173–223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Altamirano LJ, Fields HL, D'Esposito M, Boettiger CA. Interaction between family history of alcoholism and locus of control in the opioid regulation of impulsive responding under the influence of alcohol. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2011; 35: 1905–1914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Rotter JB. Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychol Monogr 1966; 80: 1–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II, vol. 1. Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  55. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene PR, Vagg PR, Jacobs AG. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y). Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  56. Friston KJ. Testing for anatomically specified regional effects. Hum Brain Mapp 1997; 5: 133–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Poldrack RA. Region of interest anlysis for fMRI. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2007; 2: 67–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Poldrack RA, Mumford JA. Indepndence in ROI analyses: where is the voodoo? Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2009; 4: 208–213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Worsley KJ, Marrett S, Neelin P, Vandal AC, Friston KJ, Evans AC. A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerbral activation. Hum Brain Mapp 1996; 4: 58–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Becker B, Wagner D, Koester P, Tittgemeyer M, Mercer-Chalmers-Bender K, Hurlemann R et al. Smaller amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex predict escalating stimulant use. Brain 2015; 138: 2074–2086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Elsey J, Coates A, Lacadie CM, McCrory EJ, Sinha R, Mayes LC et al. Childhood trauma and neural responses to personalized stress, favorite-food and neutral-relaxing cues in adolescents. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015; 40: 1580–1589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Vogt BA, Wiley RG, Jensen EL. Localization of Mu and delta opioid receptors to anterior cingulate afferents and projection neurons and input/output model of Mu regulation. Exp Neurol 1995; 135: 83–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Gitelman DR, Penny WD, Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Modeling regional an and psychophysiologic interactions in fMRI: the importance of hemodynamic deconvolution. Neuroimage 2003; 19: 200–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Koob GF, LeMoal M. Plasticity of reward neurocicruitry and the ‘dark side' of drug addiction. Nat Neurosci 2005; 8: 1442–1444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Peutz VB, McCroy E. Exploring the relationship between childhood maltreatment and addiction: a review of the neurocognitive evidence. Curr Addict Rep 2015; 2: 318–325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. McLaughlin KA, Peverill M, Gold AL, Alves S, Sheridan MA. Child maltreatment and neural systems underlying emotion regulation. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2015; 54: 753–762. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Elton A, Smitherman S, Young J, Kilts CD. Effects of childhood maltreatment on the neural correlates of stress- and drug cue-induced cocaine craving. Addict Biol 2015; 20: 820–831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Regier PS, Monge ZA, Franklin TR, Wetherill RR, Teitelman A, Jagannathan K et al. Emotional, physical and sexual abuse are associated with a heightened limbic response to cocaine cues. Addict Biol 2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  69. Drevets WC. Neuroimaging studies of mood disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 48: 813–829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Einarsson EO, Pors J, Nader K. Systems reconsolidation reveals a selective role for the anterior cingulate cortex in generalized contextual fear memory expression. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015; 40: 480–487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Fani N, King TZ, Shin J, Srivasrava A, Brewster RC, Jovanovic T et al. Structural and functional connectivity in posttraumatic stress disorder: associations with FKBP5. Depress Anxiety 2016; 33: 300–307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Gustafsson L, Zhou Q, Nylander I. Ethanol-induced effects on opioid peptides in adult male Wistar rats are dependent on early environmental factors. Neuroscience 2007; 146: 1137–1149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Gustafsson L, Oreland S, Hoffmann P, Nylander I. The impact of postnatal environment on opioid peptides in young and adult male Wistar rats. Neuropeptides 2008; 42: 177–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. George MS, Anton RF, Bloomer C, Teneback C, Drobes DJ, Lorberbaum JP et al. Activation of prefrontal cortex and anterior thalamus in alcoholic subjects on exposure to alcohol-specific cues. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58: 345–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Grant S, London ED, Newlin DB, Villemagne VL, Liu X, Contoreggi C et al. Activation of memory circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996; 93: 12040–12045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Childress AR, Mozley PD, McElgin W, Fitzgerald J, Reivich M, O'Brien CP. Limbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156: 11–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Wrase J, Schlagenhauf F, Kienast T, Wustenberg T, Bermpohl F, Kahnt T et al. Dysfunction of reward processing correlates with alcohol craving in detoxified alcoholics. Neuroimage 2007; 35: 787–794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Wexler BE, Gottschalk CH, Fulbright RK, Prohovnik I, Lacadie CM, Rounsaville BJ et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of cocaine craving. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158: 86–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Gorelick DA, Kim YK, Bencherif B, Boyd SJ, Nelson R, Copersino M et al. Imaging brain mu-opioid receptors in abstinent cocaine users: time course and relation to cocaine craving. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57: 1573–1582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Zubieta J-K, Gorelick DA, Stauffer R, Ravert HT, Dannals RF, Frost JJ. Increased mu opioid receptor binding detected by PET in cocaine-dependent men associated with cocaine craving. Nat Med 1996; 2: 1225–1229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Williams TM, Daglish MR, Lingford-Hughes A, Taylor LG, Hammers A, Brooks DJ et al. Brain opioid receptor binding in early abstinence from opioid dependence: positron emission tomography. Br J Psychiatry 2007; 191: 63–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Weerts EM, Wand GS, Kuwabara H, Munro CA, Dannals RF, Hilton J et al. Positron emission tomography imaging of mu- and delta-opioid receptor binding in alcohol-dependent and healthy control subjects. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2011; 35: 2162–2173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Heinz A, Reimold M, Wrase J, Hermann D, Croissant B, Mundle G et al. Correlation of stable elevations in striatal mu-opioid receptor availability in detoxified alcoholic patients with alcohol craving: a positron emission tomography study using carbon 11-labeld carfentanil. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62: 57–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Koob GF. The dark side of emotion: the addiction perspective. Eur J Pharmacol 2015; 753: 73–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Sirohi S, Bakalkin G, Walker BM. Alcohol-induced plasticity in the dynorphin/kappa-opioid receptor system. Front Mol Neurosci 2012; 5: 95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Bruchas MR, Land BB, Chavkin C. The dynorphin/kappa opioid system as a modulator of stress-induced and pro-addictive behaviors. Brain Res 2010; 1314: 44–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Wee S, Koob GF. The role of the dynorphin-kappa opioid system in the reinforcing effects of drugs of abuse. Psychopharmacology 2010; 210: 121–135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Van't Veer A, Carlezon WA Jr. Role of kappa-opioid receptors in stress and anxiety-related behaviour. Psychopharmacology 2013; 229: 435–452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Chavkin C, Koob GF. Dynorphin, dysphoria, and dependence: the stress of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 2016; 41: 373–374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Rosner S, Hackl-Herrwerth A, Leucht S, Vecchi S, Srisuraparnont M, Soyka M. Opioid antagonists for alcohol dependence. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2010; 12: CD001867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Johansson BA, Berglund M, Lindgren A. Efficacy of maintenance treatment with naltrexone for opioid dependence: a meta-analytical review. Addiction 2006; 101: 491–503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Minozzi S, Armato L, Vecchi S, Davoli M, Kirchmayer U, Verster A. Oral naltrexone maintenance treatment for opioid dependence. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2011; 4: CD001333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Hartung DM, McCarty D, Fu R, Wiest K, Chalk M, Gastfriend DR. Extended-release naltrexone for alcohol and opioid dependence: a meta-analysis of healthcare utilization studies. J Subst Abuse Treat 2014; 47: 113–121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Jonas DE, Amick HR, Feltner C, Bobashev G, Thomas K, Wilnes R et al. Pharmacotherapy for adults with alcohol use disorders in outpatient settings: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA 2014; 311: 1889–1900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Ritchey M, Dolcos F, Eddingon KM, Strauman TJ, Cabeza R. Neural correlates of emotional processing in depression: changes with cognitive behavioral therapy and predictors of treatment response. J Psychiatr Res 2011; 45: 577–587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Yoshimura S, Okamoto Y, Onoda K, Matsunaga M, Okada G, Kunisato Y et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy for depression changes medial prefrontal and ventral anterior cingulate cortex activity associated with self-referential processing. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 9: 487–493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. O'Malley SS, Jaffe AJ, Chang G, Schotteneld RS, Meyer RE, Rounsaville B. Naltrexone and coping skills therapy for alcohol dependence. A controlled study. Arch Gen Psychiatey 1992; 49: 881–887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. O'Malley SS, Jaffe AJ, Chang G, Rode S, Schottenfeld R, Meyer RE et al. Six-month follow-up of naltrexone and psychotherapy for alcohol dependence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53: 217–224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Schmitz JM, Stotts AL, Rhoades HM, Grabowski J. Naltrexone and relapse prevention treatment for cocaine-dependent patients. Addict Behav 2001; 26: 167–180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Hardt J, Rutter M. Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: review of the evidence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004; 45: 260–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Translational Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES