Abstract
OBJECTIVE
To identify whether child and mother characteristics in early childhood predict TV exposure and engagement during mealtime in middle childhood.
METHODS
A total of 220 low-income mother-child dyads participated. Children were 4.26 years old (SD = 0.51) at baseline and 5.94 years (SD = 0.68) at two-year follow-up. Mothers completed baseline measures of child negative emotionality and parenting practices. Family mealtimes were video-recorded and coded for background TV exposure and child TV engagement. Multinomial logistic regression tested whether child emotionality and parenting practices during early childhood predicted risk of child TV exposure or engagement during mealtime, relative to no TV use, two years later.
RESULTS
Children with greater negative emotionality in early childhood were more likely to engage with TV during mealtime than to have no TV. Similarly, early parenting disciplinary practices characterized by over-reactivity and laxness increased the risk for child TV engagement versus no TV during mealtime approximately two years later.
CONCLUSIONS
We identified two factors that associated with an increased risk for TV viewing during meals. Helping parents manage child negative emotionality using positive parenting strategies may reduce later child TV engagement and improve the quality of family mealtimes.
Keywords: mealtime, screen time, television, emotion regulation, parenting, pediatric obesity, longitudinal studies
INTRODUCTION
Family mealtime is a salient context for promoting child and family health. Shared family mealtimes have been linked to better child health outcomes, including higher consumption of healthy foods, lower consumption of unhealthy foods, and healthier eating patterns.1 Recent longitudinal research has demonstrated that frequency of family meals during childhood predicts reduced risk for obesity in adulthood.2 Indeed, recommending that families share meals together each week is a key child health promotion message.
Despite the well-documented importance of family mealtimes, television (TV) and other electronic media use during mealtime may compromise its potential benefits. For example, mealtime TV and other electronic media use is associated with poorer overall dietary quality of foods served at meals3 and overall dietary intake.4 To prevent unfavorable mealtime practices in childhood, it is essential that pediatricians identify, early in child development, which families may be at greater risk for TV use during mealtime. Identifying these early risk factors will enable tailoring of obesity prevention messages provided during well child visits.
Characteristics of both children and their parents may play a role in family mealtime practices. Children with more challenging dispositions, for example those prone to negative emotions and lability, may be more likely to be pacified with media during meals. Likewise, parents lacking effective discipline strategies may use TV to reduce conflict, or simply be more permissive with TV use during mealtime. Although prior studies have examined child temperament and/or maternal factors as predictors of screen time in infants and young children,5–7 it is not yet known whether child negative emotionality or poorer parenting practices predict later mealtime TV use in middle childhood. Another gap in the literature is that prior studies have used parent reports of screen time, whereas observational methods may provide a more nuanced picture of children’s meal time TV exposure. The current study seeks to fill these gaps by examining whether early childhood factors predict observed child TV exposure and TV engagement during mealtime in middle childhood.
METHODS
Participants
Participants in this study were part of a longitudinal cohort of mother-child dyads recruited from Head Start locations to participate in a study of child eating behavior between 2009 and 2011 (see also Goulding et al., 20148). This study was approved by the authors’ IRB; primary caregivers provided informed consent to participate. Inclusion criteria were: child aged 3 or 4 years at enrollment; birth without peri- or neonatal complications; no current medical or developmental problems; both mother and child fluent in English; and child not currently in foster care. Child emotion regulation measures were completed at baseline and parenting practices were completed, on average, 0.63 year (SD = 0.48 year) later. Mothers were contacted approximately two years afterward (2011–2013) for participation in a follow-up study measuring family mealtime practices, during which time family meals were video-recorded. The final sample at follow-up consisted of 301 mother-child dyads. Of these dyads, 251 (83.4%) had video recordings codable for TV exposure; 220 of these 251 families had no missing data for the baseline measures of suboptimal discipline practices and child emotion regulation at baseline (73.1%). Thus, the final analytical sample consisted of 220 parent-child dyads.
Procedure
Three typical dinnertime meals in the home were video-recorded by mothers over a one-week time span. Mothers were provided with a camera and instructions on how to do these recordings. Instructions were to record the entire meal and the child’s upper body, and that the child’s plate and drink should be in view throughout the video.
Observational Coding
The second dinnertime meal recorded was selected for coding. Trained research assistants coded each video in 10 second intervals indicating whether the TV was audible (“background TV exposure”) and whether the child attended to the TV screen or not (i.e., indicated whether eye gaze was directed towards television; “TV engagement”). The research assistants were trained to reach a reliability criterion of a least Cohen’s κ >.70 (κ for background TV exposure = .88; κ for child TV engagement = .85). After achieving reliability (reached after coding twenty mealtime observations), the coders independently coded the remaining videos and double coded 20% of videos to protect against coder drift. These interval codes were summed across the meal and collapsed into three categories: (1) no TV exposure, (2) background TV exposure (i.e., TV audible but no instances of child TV engagement), and (3) TV engagement (i.e., child attended to TV at least once).
Measures
Suboptimal discipline practices were measured using the Parenting Scale.9 This scale measures lax and harsh parenting practices in the context of managing child behaviors. Items include, “When my child does something I don’t like… I often let it go,” “When I’m upset or under stress…I am picky and on my child’s back,” and “When my child misbehaves…I get so frustrated or angry that my child can see I'm upset.” To get an overall score of dysfunctional parenting practices, we took the mean of all items (22 items; alpha = .82). Response options ranged from 1 to 7, with higher scores reflecting poorer parenting practices.
Child negative emotionality was measured using the Emotion Regulation Checklist Lability/Negativity subscale,10 which was also completed by the mother. Items on this scale include: “is prone to angry outbursts, tantrums easily” and “responds angrily to limit setting by adults.” Response options ranged from 1 (Rarely/Never) to 4 (Almost Always). The mean of the items on the Lability/Negativity subscale was calculated, with higher scores indicating greater negative emotionality. This scale evidenced good reliability in the current study (16 items; alpha = .85) and correlated with suboptimal discipline practices at r = .13 (p < .05).
Data Analysis
To test whether early childhood negative emotionality and discipline practices predicted relative risk for (1) background TV exposure and (2) child TV engagement during mealtime compared to no TV exposure (reference category), we conducted a multinomial logistic regression, adjusting for child sex (coded as male = 1; female = 2), child age, child race/ethnicity, and maternal education level (0 ≤ high school degree/GED and 1 > high school degree/GED). Negative emotionality and suboptimal discipline practices were entered in the same model as the independent variables. We chose the reference category of no TV exposure given that research has indicated that both background TV and TV engagement may have negative effects on child health.11 As such, we were interested in whether these two separate TV use variables were predicted by early childhood factors.
RESULTS
Sample Characteristics
See Table 1 for demographic characteristics of sample. Half of the children in the sample did not have TV on during mealtime. Approximately 21% of children had background TV exposure. The remaining proportion of children had TV engagement (28.6%); on average, this proportion of children engaged with TV during 43% of their coded mealtime intervals (SD = 33.7%). No significant differences in demographic variables or baseline measures of interest were found between participants with baseline only data versus participants with family mealtime observations at the second wave.
Table 1.
Demographic variable | Mean (SD) or % (n) |
---|---|
Child age at baseline (years) | 4.26 (0.51) |
Child age at follow up (years) | 5.94 (0.68) |
Child sex (female) | 46.4% (102) |
Child race/ethnicity | |
White, non-Hispanic | 51.4% (113) |
Black, non-Hispanic | 17.7% (39) |
Hispanic | 12.7% (28) |
Other, non-Hispanic | 18.2% (40) |
Mother race/ethnicity | |
White, non-Hispanic | 65.9% (145) |
Black, non-Hispanic | 17.3% (38) |
Hispanic | 8.6% (19) |
Other, non-Hispanic | 8.2% (18) |
Mother education level | |
High school diploma/GED or less | 48.6% (107) |
More than a high school diploma | 51.4% (113) |
Child TV Exposure/Engagement Level | |
No TV Exposure | 50.0% (110) |
Background TV Exposure | 21.4% (47) |
Child TV Engagement | 28.6% (63) |
Predicting TV Exposure and Engagement
The multinomial logistic regression examined whether negative emotionality and suboptimal discipline practices were associated with increased risk for TV exposure during mealtime, relative to the reference category of no TV exposure. Neither factor predicted an increased risk for background TV exposure. However, both child negative emotionality and suboptimal discipline practices independently predicted a greater risk for child to engage with TV during mealtime compared to no TV exposure (see Table 2). Children higher in negative emotionality at preschool were more likely to engage with TV (i.e., direct their attention to TV) than to have no TV exposure during mealtime later in childhood. A one unit increase in negative emotionality resulted in 2.11 times increase in risk of engaging with TV versus no TV during mealtime later in childhood. Children of mothers who utilized suboptimal discipline practices when their child was younger (e.g., being too lax and overreactive), had a greater risk of engaging with TV during mealtime relative to no TV exposure during mealtime (RRR=1.6; 95% CI = (1.02,2.51)). There was no interaction between suboptimal parenting and child negative emotionality on risk of TV engagement versus no TV exposure.
Table 2.
Background TV Exposure | TV Engagement | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
RRR | 95% CI | Wald | RRR | 95% CI | Wald | |
Child sex | 1.76 | 0.84, 3.67 | 2.26 | 1.97* | 1.03, 3.78 | 4.17 |
Child age | 1.06* | 1.01, 1.11 | 5.73 | 1.00 | 0.96, 1.04 | 0.01 |
Maternal education | 0.89 | 0.43, 1.87 | 0.09 | 0.74 | 0.39, 1.43 | 0.79 |
Suboptimal discipline practices | 1.45 | 0.88, 2.40 | 2.10 | 1.60* | 1.02, 2.51 | 4.12 |
Negative emotionality | 2.05 | 0.91, 4.61 | 3.01 | 2.11* | 1.04, 4.30 | 4.27 |
Reference category is No TV exposure.
p < .05.
Note. Adjusted for child race/ethnicity. Child sex was coded as male = 1; female = 2; maternal education level was coded as 0 ≤ high school degree/GED and 1 > high school degree/GED.
DISCUSSION
The objective of this study was to identify early childhood predictors of TV exposure during mealtime—an important context for pediatric obesity prevention. We found that both child and parent characteristics predicted child TV engagement during mealtime. Children with greater negative emotionality in early childhood were more likely to engage with TV during mealtime than to have no TV. Similarly, early parenting disciplinary practices characterized by over-reactivity and laxness increased the risk for child TV engagement versus no TV during mealtime approximately two years later.
Children who have mood instability and are prone to negative emotions (e.g., anger, irritability), may be challenging to parent in certain contexts, such as mealtime. Organizing and managing family mealtimes can be stressful;12 attempting to feed or engage with a child with mood instability could amplify the stress of mealtime. Indeed, one challenge reported by parents in carrying out family mealtimes is parent-child conflict and power struggles between the parent and child.13 Thus, it is plausible that families of children with poorer emotion regulation may attempt strategies such as allowing television viewing during mealtime in order to prevent conflict and achieve mealtime goals.
Suboptimal discipline practices during early childhood also predicted increase risk for child TV engagement during mealtime two years later. There are several potential mechanisms by which an overreactive and lax parenting style could promote TV engagement during mealtime. First, parents who over-react to child behaviors may experience mealtimes as particularly overwhelming and stressful. For families already experiencing multiple stressors, such as the low-income families in our study, TV during mealtime may be a means by which to create a quiet/calm atmosphere (and potentially decrease the possibility of parent-child interactions that tax the caregiver). It is also possible that with a lax or permissive parenting style, rules about TV exposure during mealtime may not be enforced or implemented. Indeed, this has been found in another study wherein lax maternal parenting style was associated with greater amounts of child TV time.14
It may also be that harsh parenting early in childhood may engender a stressful emotional climate in the home and/or foster less communicative parent-child interactions over time. As has been suggested by others,15 harsh/over-reactive parenting style may be stressful for some children. Thus, in the context of mealtime, children may attend to TV instead of engaging in potentially stressful or unrewarding interactions with their caregivers. This may also explain why we found that dysfunctional parenting practices only predicted child TV engagement and not simply whether the TV was on and the child did not watch it.
It is important to distinguish that these early childhood characteristics did not predict background TV exposure, relative to no TV, during mealtime—only whether the child attended to TV during mealtime. This could be explained by the fact that background television or “constant television households” are more common among low-income families.16 Half of the families in the current study had audible television programming during the family meal. However, our results show that of the families with TV on, approximately 43% of children did not attend to the screen at all during their meal. It is possible for these families, TV noise is a normative background sound in their environments.
Although the observational methods are a strength of this study, there is a possibility for the Hawthorne effect, wherein some parents may have not allowed TV to be on during the mealtime because of the video recording. These results are limited to TV exposure during mealtime but it is possible these early predictors of mealtime TV exposure may also apply to new mobile technology. With the recent trends in children’s mobile device ownership,17 it will be important for future research to also assess children’s use of other screen media, as it is possible that these parent and child risk factors may also be precursors to mobile device use during mealtime. It should also be acknowledged that other caregivers responsible for child feeding were not assessed in the current study. Results are specific to the primary caregiver (in our study, mothers) but future research should assess whether parenting practices of the other caregivers may also associate with mealtime TV use. It is important to acknowledge that these results are based on low-income families, the majority of whom are non-Hispanic White. Thus, our findings may not generalize to families from higher income brackets or of different racial/ethnic backgrounds. However, given that obesity disproportionally affects children from low-income backgrounds, our findings still have important implications for those most in need of obesity prevention strategies. When treating children with emotion regulation difficulties, pediatricians could elicit from parents how they manage mealtimes and provide alternatives to TV use to manage child difficult behavior. Likewise, messages about no TV use during mealtime may be especially important to parents who have over-reactive and lax parenting styles. In terms of obesity prevention interventions, improving early childhood emotion regulation and the emotional climate of family mealtime may mitigate the risk of unfavorable mealtime practices, such as child TV engagement. Future research should examine whether improving parenting practices and child emotion regulation reduces children’s TV use during mealtime and lowers obesity risk.
CONCLUSIONS
Both child emotion regulation and dysfunctional parenting practices in early childhood predicted an increase in risk for child TV engagement during dinner time two years later. Low-income families with these risk factors could be identified early to prevent obesity-promoting mealtime practices. Interventions may target general parenting style and child emotion regulation as one means by which to reduce mealtime TV use.
WHAT’S NEW?
Both child negative emotionality and dysfunctional parenting practices (i.e., over-reactivity, laxness) in early childhood predicted an increased risk for child TV engagement during mealtime (relative to no TV during mealtime) two years later. Families with these risk factors could be identified early to prevent unfavorable mealtime practices.
Acknowledgments
Funding support: This work was supported by the NICHD Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award (F32HD085684) to Dr. Domoff. This work was also supported by grants R01 HD061356 and RC DK086376 to Dr. Lumeng, American Heart Association 10GRNT4460043 to Dr. Miller and R21DK090718 to Drs. Lumeng and Miller. The funders of this work did not contribute to, participate in or influence the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of data, writing of the manuscript or the decision to submit this manuscript for publication.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Conflicts of interest: The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.
Contributor Information
Sarah E. Domoff, Department of Psychology, Central Michigan University and Research Faculty Affiliate, Center for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan. 206 Sloan Hall, Central Michigan University, Mt. Pleasant, MI, 48859. Phone: (989) 774-1072, fax: (989) 774-1072, domof1se@cmich.edu.
Julie C. Lumeng, Medical School, University of Michigan. Professor of Nutrition Sciences, School of Public Health, University of Michigan. Research Professor, Center for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, 300 N. Ingalls Street, Ann Arbor MI, 48109-5406. Phone: (734) 647-1102, fax: (734) 936-9288, jlumeng@umich.edu.
Niko Kaciroti, Department of Biostatistics, School of Public Health, University of Michigan. Center for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, 300 N. Ingalls Street, Ann Arbor, MI, 48109-5406. Phone: (734) 763-9714, fax: (734) 763-2215, nicola@umich.edu.
Alison L. Miller, Department of Health Behavior and Health Education, School of Public Health, University of Michigan, Research Associate Professor, Center for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor MI, 48109. Phone: (734) 615-7459, fax: (734) 936-9288, alimill@umich.edu.
References
- 1.Hammons AJ, Fiese BH. Is frequency of shared family meals related to the nutritional health of children and adolescents? Pediatrics. 2011;127(6):1565–1574. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Berge JM, Wall M, Hsueh TF, Fulkerson JA, Larson N, Neumark-Sztainer D. The protective role of family meals for youth obesity: 10-year longitudinal associations. J Pediatr. 2015;166(2):296–301. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2014.08.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Fulkerson JA, Loth K, Bruening M, Berge J, Eisenberg ME, Neumark-Sztainer D. Time 2 tlk 2 nite: Use of electronic media by adolescents during family meals and associations with demographic characteristics, family characteristics, and foods served. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2014;114(7):1053–1058. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2013.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Feldman S, Eisenberg ME, Neumark-Sztainer D, Story M. Associations between watching TV during family meals and dietary intake among adolescents. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2007;39(5):257–263. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2007.04.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Radesky JS, Silverstein M, Zuckerman B, Christakis DA. Infant self-regulation and early childhood media exposure. Pediatrics. 2014;133(5):1172–1178. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-2367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Thompson AL, Adair LS, Bentley ME. Maternal characteristics and perception of temperament associated with infant TV exposure. Pediatrics. 2013;131(2):390–397. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-1224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Ansari A, Crosnoe R. Children's hyperactivity, television viewing, and the potential for child effects. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2016;61:135–140. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2015.12.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Goulding AN, Rosenblum KL, Miller AL, et al. Associations between maternal depressive symptoms and child feeding practices in a cross-sectional study of low-income mothers and their young children. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2014;11(1):75–86. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-11-75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Arnold DS, O'Leary SG, Wolff LS, Acker MM. The Parenting Scale: a measure of dysfunctional parenting in discipline situations. Psychol Assess. 1993;5(2):137–144. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Shields A, Cicchetti D. Reactive aggression among maltreated children: The contributions of attention and emotion dysregulation. J Clin Child Psychol. 1998;27(4):381–395. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2704_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Trofholz AC, Tate AD, Miner MH, Berge JM. Associations between TV viewing at family meals and the emotional atmosphere of the meal, meal healthfulness, child dietary intake, and child weight status. Appetite. 2017;108:361–366. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Rhee K. Childhood overweight and the relationship between parent behaviors, parenting style, and family functioning. Ann Am Acad Polit Soc Sci. 2008;615(1):11–37. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Fulkerson JA, Story M, Neumark-Sztainer D, Rydell S. Family meals: Perceptions of benefits and challenges among parents of 8-to 10-year-old children. J Am Diet Assoc. 2008;108(4):706–709. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2008.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Jago R, Davison KK, Thompson JL, Page AS, Brockman R, Fox KR. Parental sedentary restriction, maternal parenting style, and television viewing among 10-to 11-year-olds. Pediatrics. 2011;128(3):572–578. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-3664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Rhee KE, Lumeng JC, Appugliese DP, Kaciroti N, Bradley RH. Parenting styles and overweight status in first grade. Pediatrics. 2006;117(6):2047–2054. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-2259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Roberts DF. Kids and Media at the New Millennium. Collingdale, PA: Diane Publishing; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kabali HK, Irigoyen MM, Nunez-Davis R, et al. Exposure and use of mobile media devices by young children. Pediatrics. 2015;136(6):1044–1050. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-2151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]