Abstract
We sought to identify predictors of symptomatic recovery in vestibular neuritis. Forty VN patients were prospectively studied in the acute phase (median = 2 days) and 32 in the recovery phase (median = 10 weeks) with vestibulo‐ocular reflex, vestibular‐perceptual, and visual dependence tests and psychological questionnaires. Clinical outcome was Dizziness Handicap Inventory score at recovery phase. Acute visual dependency and autonomic arousal predicted outcome. Worse recovery was associated with a combination of increased visual dependence, autonomic arousal, anxiety/depression, and fear of bodily sensations, but not with vestibular variables. Findings highlight the importance of early identification of abnormal visual dependency and concurrent anxiety.
Introduction
Acute vertigo due to vestibular neuritis (VN), resolves over a matter of days but 30–50% of patients develop disabling chronic dizziness.1 Identifying predictors of the “acute‐to‐chronic” dizziness transition would allow patients at high risk of chronic dizziness to be targeted with focused therapies. Two possible predictors have been identified; follow‐up studies2, 3 have shown that psycho‐pathological features facilitate long‐term dizziness, however, cross‐sectional studies indicate that psycho‐physical estimates of how much an individual relies on vision for spatial orientation (“visual dependence”) is also associated with chronic dizziness.4, 5 As psychological questionnaires and psycho‐physical estimates of visuo‐vestibular interaction assess different functional domains, we now report a prospective study of VN patients examining visual dependence (rod‐and‐disk test), psychological features, as well as vestibulo‐reflex and vestibulo‐perceptual function. The aim is to establish how these variables interact to predict clinical outcome in VN.
Method
Forty patients (mean age 50 years, range 22–79, 18 females) were studied prospectively in the acute phase of VN (1–5 days after onset, median = 2 days) and 32 patients in the recovery phase (median = 10 weeks). Twenty‐six of these patients were also seen in a long‐term recovery stage (median = 10 months) to validate acute and recovery stage findings. Acute clinical examination revealed unidirectional horizontal nystagmus with a torsional component, a positive horizontal head impulse test,6 unilateral caloric canal paresis, lateropulsion, and no hearing impairment or symptoms/signs of CNS disorder.7 Of the 24 patients who were administered prochloperazine, only three received these on the day of testing, but the drug has been shown to have no effect upon vestibulo‐reflex or vestibulo‐perceptual function.8 MRI brain scans were not routinely performed, but when done on hospital arrival (n = 3), no abnormalities were detected. No patient received corticosteroids. Patients were strongly advised to remain physically active and were explained the benefits of doing so but, in the acute phase, none were referred for formal physiotherapy.
In light of the aim of this study to assess how psychological variables interact with vestibulo‐reflex, vestibulo‐perceptual measures and visual dependency, at each stage patients underwent bithermal caloric testing (30–44°C) and the following test battery.
Vestibular perceptual tasks
The threshold vestibular task (details in9) measures vestibular‐perceptual thresholds for detection of angular motion. The test comprised three rightward and three leftward rotations, with an initial acceleration of 0.5 deg/sec2, increasing by 0.5 deg/sec2 every 3 sec. Patients sat on a rotating chair with a hand‐held device with two buttons and were asked to press the button corresponding to their perceived direction of rotation (leftward/rightward) during each rotation. Vestibular‐perceptual thresholds were measured by the time taken from chair acceleration onset to button press.
The suprathreshold vestibular task (details in9) measures vestibulo‐perceptual responses to eight +/−90 deg/sec velocity steps lasting 60 sec with acceleration phase of 1 sec. Perceptual responses were recorded by patients turning a chair‐fixed tachometer wheel to indicate their perceived rotational velocity during the four rotational and four postrotational periods (starting‐stopping Barany test). The tachometer output follows an approximately exponential decay allowing measurement of the time constant of decay and the duration of the perceptual response.9 In this study, we only used the latter to reduce the number of variables statistically analyzed.
Rotations were performed using a vibration‐free motorized rotating chair (Contraves, USA; fitted with chin and head rests) in the dark with sound masking to eliminate nonvestibular cues.
Rod‐and‐disk task10
Visual dependence was measured with the rod‐and‐disk task on a laptop computer (Fig. 1A and B5; available at: http://www.imperial.ac.uk/medicine/dizzinessandvertigo ). Patients sat in front of the screen with the head held against an attached viewing cone to block extraneous visual cues. The stimulus consisted of a luminous white 6 cm rod against a black background filled with randomly distributed white dots. Patients had to align the rod to their perceived vertical (subjective visual vertical) with a roller mouse, from initial random rod settings ±40° from vertical, during four trials in three conditions: background dots stationary and dots rotating at 30 deg/sec clockwise and counterclockwise. Visually induced rod tilt was calculated as a measure of visual dependence for each subject. First, static tilt was calculated as the mean rod tilt in the four trials with background dots stationary. Then, visually induced rod tilt was calculated as the mean of the absolute values of the rod tilt from each trial with dots rotating minus the static rod tilt. Mean absolute static tilt was used as a measure of otolith function.
Questionnaire measures
The Dizziness Handicap Inventory (DHI),11 measured the perceived handicapping effects of dizziness. A normalized score (0–4) was calculated by dividing total score by number of questions answered and used as an overall measure of recovery (0–1.3 = nil to mild handicap, 1.4–2.6 = moderate handicap and 2.7–4 = severe handicap). A normalized score was used as patients in the acute stage were not able to answer all questions, for example “Does walking down the aisle of a supermarket increase your problem?” The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS)12 measured state anxiety and depression. For each scale, scores ranged from 0 to 21 (high level of anxiety/depression). The Body Sensations Questionnaire (BSQ)13 measured the intensity of fear relating to body sensations, with scores ranging from 0 to 5 (extremely fearful). The Vertigo Symptom Scale_arousal (VSS_A)14 measured autonomic arousal components (e.g., heart pounding, excessive sweating; score 0–64).
Analysis
The primary measure of outcome was symptomatic recovery as assessed with the Dizziness Handicap Inventory (DHI). Variables influencing symptoms at recovery (10 week_DHI) and long‐term recovery (10 month_DHI) stages were investigated using correlational (Pearsons ‘r’) analysis. We used stepwise multiple linear regression to predict outcome from baseline (acute) variables, whereas we used exploratory factor analysis15 to look for associations between significant variables and to assess whether these patterns of associations could be due to a small number of underlying factors (sometimes called unobserved or latent variables). Larzelere and Mulaik adjusted Bonferroni correction16 was used. Informed consent was obtained from all patients as approved by Charing Cross Hospital Ethics Committee.
Results
Symptoms improved drastically from acute to recovery stages in all patients, with considerable individual variability (Fig. 1C). Average symptom load (Normalized DHI, score 0–4) decreased from 2.13SD1.02 acutely to 0.63SD0.95 at recovery stage (10 week). Results for all measures are summarized in Table S1.
Predicting clinical recovery from the acute stage (Table 1)
Table 1.
Acute | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Suprathreshold | Threshold | |||||||||||
Age (years) | Canal Paresis (%) | Perception mean | Perception asymmetry (%) | Perception mean | Perception asymmetry (%) | Visual dependency | Static rod tilt | HADS | BSQ | VSS_A | ||
DHI Recovery (10 Week) |
Pearson Correlation | 0.233 | −0.239 | −0.059 | −0.172 | 0.066 | 0.057 | 0.504 | 0.252 | 0.176 | 0.351 a | 0.529 |
Sig. (2‐tailed) | 0.199 | 0.195 | 0.762 | 0.39 | 0.727 | 0.771 | 0.006 | 0.187 | 0.336 | 0.049 | 0.002 |
BSQ, body sensations questionnaire; HADS, hospital anxiety and depression scale. VSS_A, vertigo symptom scale_arousal
Not significant after Larzelere and Mulaik adjusted Bonferroni test. 16
Bold indicates significance at the P>0.05 level.
DHI score at recovery stage (10 week _DHI) was significantly correlated with acute autonomic arousal (r = 0.53, P = 0.002), acute visual dependency (r = 0.5, P = 0.006), and acute fear of bodily sensations (r = 0.35, P = 0.049, not significant after Larzelere and Mulaik adjusted Bonferroni correction16). Stepwise multiple linear regression to predict DHI at recovery (10 week) stage, entering all the baseline acute variables is shown in Table 1 as predictors, produced a significant model (adjusted R 2=0.562, ANOVA F = 13.8, df 2,18, P < 0.001) in which the two significant predictors were acute autonomic arousal (β = 0.47 P = 0.02) and acute visual dependency (β = 0.41 P = 0.038).
Associations between variables at the recovery stage
Table 2 shows all bivariate correlations between symptoms at the recovery stage (10 week) and psychophysical, visual dependency, and psychological variables also at 10 weeks. Clinical outcome (10 week _DHI) correlated with vestibulo‐perceptual thresholds (r = 0.52, P = 0.003), visual dependency (r = 0.56, P = 0.001) and, less significantly, with canal paresis (r = 0.38, P = 0.045, not significant after Larzelere and Mulaik adjusted Bonferroni correction16), all measured at 10 weeks. In addition, the following questionnaire data correlated with 10 Week_DHI: anxiety and depression (HADS, r = 0.71, P ≤ 0.001), autonomic arousal (VSS_A, r = 0.71, P < 0.001), and fear of bodily sensations (BSQ, r = 0.58, P = 0.001).
Table 2.
Recovery (10 week) | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Suprathreshold | Threshold | |||||||||||
Age (years) | Canal Paresis (%) | Perception mean | Perception asymmetry (%) | Perception mean | Perception asymmetry (%) | Visual dependency | Static rod tilt | HADS | BSQ | VSS_A | ||
DHI Recovery (10 week) |
Pearson Correlation | 0.233 | 0.376 a | 0.209 | −0.093 | 0.518 | −0.12 | 0.556 | −0.115 | 0.706 | 0.583 | 0.698 |
Sig. (2‐tailed) | 0.199 | 0.045 | 0.268 | 0.626 | 0.003 | 0.519 | 0.001 | 0.532 | <0.001 | 0.001 | <0.001 |
BSQ, body sensations questionnaire; HADS, hospital anxiety and depression scale. VSS_A, vertigo symptom scale_arousal
Not significant after Larzelere and Mulaik adjusted Bonferroni test. 16
Bold indicates significance at the P>0.05 level.
Factor Analysis (Fig. 2) was used to further explore the significant correlations outlined above and describe the underlying pattern of associations between variables. The first statistical component identified by Factor Analysis accounted for 59% of the variance and, critically, loaded 10 week_DHI, our outcome variable. This first component also loaded visual dependency, autonomic arousal, fear of body sensations (BSQ, acute and recovery stages), and anxiety‐depression scores (HADS, recovery stage). A second component was identified, accounting for just 12% of variance and loaded canal paresis and vestibular perceptual thresholds but, notably, did not include clinical outcome.
Long‐term prognosis
Additional correlational analysis was carried out to assess whether factors determining symptomatic recovery outlined above, continue to predict symptom load at long‐term recovery (10 month_DHI). Comparisons between recovery (10 week) and long‐term recovery (10 month) stages showed no significant change in symptom load (DHI) (Fig. 1C), confirming that most recovery takes place by week 10. Bivariate correlational analysis showed acute autonomic arousal (VSS_A; r=0.78, P ≤ 0.001), visual dependency (r = 0.67, P = 0.001), and fear of bodily sensations (r = 0.46, P = 0.02) continue to predict long‐term outcome (10 month_DHI).
There was no significant difference in the baseline measures of autonomic arousal, visual dependency, or fear of bodily sensations between participants who returned at 10 weeks or 10 months and those who did not, suggesting drop‐outs did not systematically distort results.
Discussion
We investigated how vestibular‐reflex (caloric), vestibulo‐perceptual, visual dependence (rod‐and‐disk), and psychological measures intertwine to predict clinical outcome in VN patients. Correlation and regression analyses showed that the main predictors of clinical recovery were increased levels of autonomic arousal (VSS_A) and visual dependence in the acute phase. Parameters in the recovery phase associated with clinical outcome were, again, visual dependency, anxiety/depression (HADS), autonomic arousal, and fear of bodily sensations. Vestibulo‐perceptual thresholds and, marginally, canal paresis at 10 weeks were also correlated with recovery. Critically, however, factor analysis revealed that visual dependency and questionnaire data loaded as a single factor, including the clinical outcome variable (10 week_DHI) and explained 59% of the variance. In contrast, the peripheral vestibular variables (caloric and threshold data) only accounted for 12% of the variance but, notably, did not include clinical outcome (Fig. 2).
Visual motion sensitivity and dizziness brought on by complex or moving visual surroundings are common in cross‐sectional studies of chronically symptomatic vestibular patients.4 Our prospective study shows that if too much weighting is placed on vision acutely (visual dependence), or if sensory integration mechanisms are unable to down‐regulate the visual contribution to the central compensation process, patients recover poorly. While prior studies have shown that anxiety, depression, and fear of body sensations are significantly associated with symptom recovery,2, 3 the novel finding is that it is the combination of psychological factors and visual dependence that best predicts clinical outcome. In agreement with previous studies, the degree of peripheral vestibular recovery (caloric, head‐impulse test or VEMPs) bears little influence on global clinical outcome.17, 18
Do autonomic arousal and psychological factors develop in response to heightened visual dependency, or vice‐versa, or are they coexisting independent parameters? The latter seems less likely given that compensation after a unilateral vestibular lesion relies upon multi‐sensory (visuo‐vestibular) reweighting, and central mechanisms subserving such functions are affected by psychological states.19 A mechanistic link between visuo‐vestibular compensation and psychological factors is underpinned by the presence of neuroanatomical networks processing visual, vestibular, and emotional inputs.20, 21. Moreover, fMRI data during simulated vertigo suggest an association between psychological traits and functional connectivity patterns within visuo‐vestibular and anxiety‐related cortical networks22, but the directionality of this association remains unclear. Our findings highlight (1) the importance of early identification of abnormal visual dependency and concurrent anxiety in VN and (2) the potential for early treatments to improve long‐term outcome by reducing visual dependency (sensory reweighting strategies23, 24) and combining pharmacotherapy and cognitive therapies to reduce anxiety and autonomic arousal. Further work should characterize the mechanism by which visual dependency is up‐regulated in such patients, in relation to increased anxiety, to allow more targeted therapies at the early phase of a vestibular injury.
Author Contributions
SC was responsible for recruitment of participants and conducting testing, data analysis, statistical analysis, interpretation of data, manuscript development, and revisions. DK recruited participants, conducted testing and was involved with interpretation of data, manuscript development and revisions. NC recruited participants and conducted testing and was involved with interpretation of data and manuscript revisions. QA conducted participant testing and was involved with interpretation of data. HA recruited participants and conducted testing. MAG was involved in study concept and design, interpretation of data and study supervision. BMS recruited participants, and was involved in interpretation of data and critical revision of manuscript. JG conducted statistical analysis and was involved in revision of manuscript. AMB was responsible for study concept and design, interpretation of data, and was involved in all revisions of manuscript.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
The study was supported by the Medical Research Council, award Program Number G0600183.
References
- 1. Halmagyi GM, Weber KP, Curthoys IS. Vestibular function after acute vestibular neuritis. Restor Neurol Neurosci 2010;28:37–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Godemann F, Koffroth C, Neu P, Heuser I. Why does vertigo become chronic after neuropathia vestibularis? Psychosom Med 2004;66:783–787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Best C, Eckhardt‐Henn A, Tschan R, Dieterich M. Why do subjective vertigo and dizziness persist over one year after a vestibular vertigo syndrome? Ann NY Acad Sci 2009;1164:334–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Guerraz M, Yardley L, Bertholon P, et al. Visual vertigo: symptom assessment, spatial orientation and postural control. Brain 2001;124(Pt 8):1646–1656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Cousins S, Cutfield NJ, Kaski D, et al. Visual dependency and dizziness after vestibular neuritis. PLoS ONE 2014;9:e105426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Halmagyi GM, Curthoys IS. A clinical sign of canal paresis. Arch Neurol 1988;45:737–739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Seemungal BM, Bronstein AM. A practical approach to acute vertigo. Pract Neurol 2008;8:211–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Patel M, Nigmatullina Y, Seemungal BM, et al. Effects of prochlorperazine on normal vestibular ocular and perceptual responses: a randomised, double‐blind, crossover, placebo‐controlled study. Audiol Neurootol 2014;19:91–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Cousins S, Kaski D, Cutfield N, et al. Vestibular perception following acute unilateral vestibular lesions. PLoS ONE 2013;8:e61862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Dichgans J, Held R, Young LR, Brandt T. Moving visual scenes influence the apparent direction of gravity. Science 1972;178:1217–1219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Jacobson GP, Newman CW. The development of the dizziness handicap inventory. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 1990;116:424–427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983;67:361–370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Chambless DL, Caputo GC, Bright P, Gallagher R. Assessment of fear of fear in agoraphobics: the body sensations questionnaire and the agoraphobic cognitions questionnaire. J Consult Clin Psychol 1984;52:1090–1097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Yardley L, Masson E, Verschuur C, et al. Symptoms, anxiety and handicap in dizzy patients: development of the vertigo symptom scale. J Psychosom Res 1992;36:731–741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. De Winter JC, Dodou D, Wieringa PA. Exploratory factor analysis with small sample sizes. Multivariate Behav. Res.. 2009;44:147–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Howell D. Statistical methods for psychology, (3rd Edition). Boston: PWS‐Kent, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 17. Patel M, Arshad Q, Roberts RE, et al. Chronic symptoms after vestibular neuritis and the high‐velocity vestibulo‐ocular reflex. Otol Neurotol 2016;37:179–184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Adamec I, Krbot Skoric M, Ozretic D, Habek M. Predictors of development of chronic vestibular insufficiency after vestibular neuritis. J Neurol Sci 2014;347(1–2):224–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Yardley L, Redfern MS. Psychological factors influencing recovery from balance disorders. J. Anxiety Dis. 2001;15:119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Balaban CD. Projections from the parabrachial nucleus to the vestibular nuclei: potential substrates for autonomic and limbic influences on vestibular responses. Brain Res 2004;996:126–137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Gorman JM, Kent JM, Sullivan GM, Coplan JD. Neuroanatomical hypothesis of panic disorder, revised. Am J Psychiatry 2000;157:493–505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Riccelli R, Indovina I, Staab JP, et al. Neuroticism modulates brain visuo‐vestibular and anxiety systems during a virtual rollercoaster task. Hum Brain Mapp 2016; Advance online publication. Doi: 10.1002/hbm.23411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Pavlou M, Lingeswaran A, Davies RA, et al. Simulator based rehabilitation in refractory dizziness. J Neurol 2004;251:983–995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Hall CD, Herdman SJ, Whitney SL, et al. Vestibular rehabilitation for peripheral vestibular hypofunction: an evidence‐based clinical practice guideline: from the American physical therapy association neurology section. J Neurol Phys Ther 2016;40:124–155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Hair JF, Anderson RE, Tatham RL, Black WC. Multivariate data analysis 5th ed Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1998. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.