Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2017 Jan;2(1):28–37. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.09.006

Vulnerability to Depression in Youth: Advances from Affective Neuroscience

Autumn Kujawa a, Katie L Burkhouse b
PMCID: PMC5421558  NIHMSID: NIHMS820012  PMID: 28497126

Abstract

Vulnerability models of depression posit that individual differences in trait-like vulnerabilities emerge early in life and increase risk for the later development of depression. In this review, we summarize advances from affective neuroscience using neural measures to assess vulnerabilities in youth at high risk for depression due to parental history of depression or temperament style, as well as prospective designs evaluating the predictive validity of these vulnerabilities for symptoms and diagnoses of depression across development. Evidence from multiple levels of analysis indicates that healthy youth at high risk for depression exhibit abnormalities in components of the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) positive valence systems, including blunted activation in the striatum during reward anticipation and feedback, and that some of these measures can be used to predict later symptoms. In addition, alterations in components of RDoC’s negative valence systems, including neural processing of sadness, loss, and threat, have been observed in risk for depression, though effects appear to be more task and method dependent. Within the social processes domain, preliminary evidence indicates that neural processing of social feedback, including heightened reactivity to exclusion and blunted response to social reward, may be related to depression vulnerability. These studies indicate that affective neuroscience can inform understanding of developmental pathways to depression and identify altered emotional processing among youth at high risk. We provide an integrated summary of consistent findings from this literature, along with recommendations for future directions and implications for early intervention.

Keywords: depression, risk, developmental psychopathology, affective neuroscience, EEG/ERP, fMRI


Depression is a common psychiatric disorder and the second leading cause of disability worldwide (1; 2), highlighting the importance of early intervention for youth at high risk (HR). Although rates of depression increase dramatically in adolescence and young adulthood (35), at least some trait-like vulnerabilities emerge earlier in life and increase risk for later depression (6; 7). For example, at the behavioral and self-report level, maladaptive cognitive and affective styles, including hopelessness, rumination, negative attributions, and attentional biases for sad faces have been observed among HR youth and/or shown to prospectively predict the development of depressive symptoms or diagnoses (6; 5; 8; 9). More recent work has begun to apply affective neuroscience methods to identify early vulnerabilities for depression.

The Potential of Affective Neuroscience for Informing Vulnerability Research

Neural and psychophysiological measures can provide relatively objective measures of emotional processing, offer insight into the brain circuitry underlying vulnerability, and add to levels of analysis of constructs of the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC; 10). There is also evidence that along with clinical and behavioral measures, neural measures account for additional variance in predicting future behavior, including clinical status and response to treatment (11), raising the possibility that these measures may aid in identifying youth at greatest risk. The goal of the current review is to synthesize initial efforts to apply affective neuroscience in understanding vulnerability to depression in children and adolescents. We highlight the potential of this work for informing early intervention work, as well as challenges in the field and needs for further research.

To rule out the possibility that altered emotional processing is a correlate of symptoms or consequence of a past depressive episode, we focus on studies of youth prior to the onset of the disorder, including both cross-sectional analyses of HR youth compared to those at lower risk (LR), and prospective studies examining predictors of future depressive symptoms or diagnoses, which are the most direct approach to examining precursors. With regard to HR designs, a common approach is studying offspring of parents with a history of depression, who are at an increased risk of developing the disorder themselves (12; 13). These designs are complemented by studies of child temperament. Specifically, high negative emotionality (NE), characterized by sadness, irritability, and anxiety, and low positive emotionality (PE), characterized by positive affect, appetitive behavior and sociability (14), have been linked to depression and prospectively predict later symptoms (15; 16). Although NE may be a stronger predictor (hazard ratio = 1.31) than PE (hazard ratio = 0.96) of depression onset in adults (17), low PE in early childhood has been shown to prospectively predict increases in depressive symptoms (Cohen’s d = .58 to .85) (18), and the combination of low PE and high NE may be particularly predictive of depressive symptoms (18; 19). When possible, we describe studies that tested specificity of effects for parental depression as opposed to other psychopathology or for temperament styles that may be relatively unique to depression, such as PE (20). Such designs provide insight into whether these vulnerabilities are specific to risk for depression or psychopathology more broadly.

In terms of methods, we focus on neural measures (i.e., physiological and circuit levels of analysis) of responses to emotional stimuli, within the RDoC domains (10) of positive valence systems (PVS), negative valence systems (NVS), and social processes. Recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have provided insight into neural circuits involved in emotional processing in HR youth. This work is complemented by studies of event-related potentials (ERP) derived from electroencephalogram (EEG) responses to affective stimuli. ERP measures are economically measured across development, allowing for assessment of large samples of youth, and offer improved temporal resolution, which provides insight into multiple stages of stimulus processing (see Table 1 for a summary of measures). We first present a comprehensive review of this literature (Table 2), followed by summaries of consistent findings emerging across two or more studies (Table 3). Finally, we discuss potential for identifying youth at greatest risk and informing early intervention, as well as directions for future research.

Table 1.

Overview of neural measures used to examine positive and negative valence systems and social processes in depression vulnerability research.

Measure Affective Process Possible Neural Circuit
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI)

 Blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) Signal in Reward Systems Wanting and liking rewards, decision-making, reward learning (94; 95) Striatum, anterior insula, amygdala, thalamus, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), prefrontal cortex (PFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC; 94; 95)
 BOLD Signal in Threat/Negative Emotion Systems1 Identifying, appraising and regulating responses to threat and negative emotions (96; 97) Amygdala/hippocampus, insula, striatum, thalamus, PCC, ACC, supplementary motor area, PFC, occipitotemporal cortex (97; 98)

Event-Related Potentials (ERP)

 Reward Positivity (RewP) Processing reward outcomes, reinforcement learning (99) Striatum, medial PFC (25; 100)
 Late Positive Potential (LPP) Sustained attention towards salient stimuli, activation of motivational systems (37; 101) Occipitotemporal cortex, amygdala/hippocampus, insula, temporal pole, OFC, PFC (102; 103)
 Error-Related Negativity (ERN) Performance monitoring, cognitive control, sensitivity to errors/endogenous threat (55; 56; 104) ACC (104)
 P300 Stimulus evaluation and attentional allocation (105) Frontal and temporal/parietal regions; correlated with activation in insula and medial frontal gyrus (105107)
1

Note: We have separated reward and negative emotion systems to parallel the order of the review and distinct literatures on reward processing and negative mood/face processing in depression vulnerability; however, these systems overlap, with common neural circuitry involved in emotional processing regardless of valence (98)

Table 2.

Summary of studies included in the review

Study HR definition Measure Sample size (N) Age M(SD) Primary finding Estimated Cohen’s d
Positive Valence Systems
Bress et al., 2013 (30) Prospective prediction RewP to reward and loss feedback 68 (16 MDE at follow up) 17.72 (0.89) ↓ RewP predicted MDE onset and depressive symptoms .50
Foti et al., 2011 (27) Parental depression RewP to reward and loss feedback following negative mood induction 44 LR, 37 HR 16.04 (0.89) No main effect of risk on RewP; ↓ RewP in HR who reported greater sadness during mood induction
Forbes et al., 2010 (33) Low PE fMRI during anticipation and receipt of reward/loss 77 11.81 (0.64) ↓ striatum during reward anticipation and feedback among youth lower in PE .68–.77
Gotlib et al., 2010 (31) Maternal recurrent depression fMRI during anticipation and receipt of reward/loss 13 LR, 13 HR 12.45 (1.56) ↓ striatum to reward anticipation/feedback, ↓ left and ↑ right insula during reward anticipation in HR 1.27–1.51
Kessel et al., under review (41) Low PE LPP to emotional images 340 9.14 (0.35) Lower PE predicted ↓ LPP to pleasant images .22
Kujawa et al., 2012 (39) Maternal depression LPP to emotional faces 218 LR, 116 HR 6.12 (0.46) ↓ LPP to happy faces in HR .29
Kujawa et al., 2014 (26) Maternal depression RewP to reward and loss feedback 179 LR, 70 HR 9.18 (0.40) ↓ RewP in HR youth with maternal history of depression without comorbid anxiety .33
Kujawa et al., 2015 (28) Low PE RewP to reward and loss feedback 381 9.20 (0.40) Lower observed and self-reported PE predicted ↓ RewP .18–.22
Luking et al., 2016 (34) Maternal depression fMRI during anticipation and receipt of reward/loss 31 LR, 16 HR 9.17 (0.40) ↓ ventral striatum and insula during reward feedback in HR .84–1.26
Monk et al., 2008 (42) Parental depression fMRI to faces 22 LR, 17 HR 14.10 (2.30) ↓ ventral striatum to happy faces in HR .58–.60
Morgan et al., 2013 (36) Prospective prediction fMRI during anticipation and receipt of reward/loss 72 (40 mid-to-late pubertal, 32 boys) 11.94 (0.60) ↓ striatum to reward anticipation predicted symptoms in mid- to late-pubertal adolescents; ↑ ventromedial PFC to reward feedback predicted depressive symptoms in boys 1.06–1.40
Nelson et al., 2015 (38) Parental depression LPP to emotional images 422 LR, 107 HR 14.39 (0.63) ↓ LPP to pleasant images in HR .20
Nelson et al., 2016 (29) Prospective prediction RewP to reward and loss feedback 444 (40 depressive disorder at follow up) 14.39 (0.63) ↓ RewP predicted depressive disorder onset and symptoms .26
Olino et al., 2014 (32) Maternal depression fMRI during anticipation and receipt of reward/loss 12 LR, 14 HR 15.72 (2.82) ↓ striatum during reward anticipation in HR 1.50
Sharp et al., 2014 (35) Maternal recurrent depression fMRI during anticipation and receipt of reward/loss 19 LR, 19 HR 13.36 (1.89) ↓ striatum to reward feedback in HR 1.13
Speed et al., 2015 (40) Low PE LPP to pleasant images 523 14.39 (0.63) ↓ LPP to pleasant images among youth low in PE .28

Negative Valence Systems
Gotlib et al., 2010 (31) Maternal recurrent depression fMRI during anticipation and receipt of reward/loss 13 LR, 13 HR 12.45 (1.56) ↓ striatum and ↑ dorsal ACC activation in response to loss of reward in HR 1.14–1.69
Joormann et al., 2012 (50) Maternal recurrent depression fMRI during sad mood induction and mood repair 27 LR, 20 HR 11.82 (1.25) ↑ ventrolateral PFC and amygdala during mood induction in HR; ↑ amygdala, parahippocampus, OFC and ↓ dorsal ACC and dorsolateral PFC during mood repair in HR .69–1.29
Kujawa et al., 2012 (39) Maternal depression LPP to emotional faces 218 LR, 116 HR 6.12 (0.46) ↓ LPP to negative faces, including sad, angry, and fearful faces, in HR .29
Luking et al., 2016 (34) Maternal depression fMRI during anticipation and receipt of reward/loss 31 LR, 16 HR 9.17 (0.40) ↓ striatum, insula, and parahippocampus in response to loss of reward in HR .88–1.59
Mannie et al., 2011 (54) Parental depression fMRI to faces 28 LR, 28 HR 19.24 (1.42) ↓ dorsolateral PFC to fearful faces in HR .59
Meyer et al., 2016 (59) Maternal recurrent depression ERN 38 LR, 24 HR 12.93 (1.96) ↓ ERN in HR .77
Monk et al., 2008 (42) Parental depression fMRI to faces 22 LR, 17 HR 14.10 (2.30) ↑ amygdala and ventral striatum to fearful faces in HR .84–.95
Nelson et al., 2015 (38) Parental depression LPP to emotional images 422 LR, 107 HR 14.39 (0.63) ↓ LPP to unpleasant images in HR .20
Speed et al., 2015 (40) Low PE LPP to emotional images 523 14.39 (0.63) ↓ LPP to unpleasant images among youth low in PE .22
Speed et al., 2016 (52) Maternal depression LPP to emotional words during self-referential encoding task 92 LR, 29 HR 12.67 (1.64) ↑ LPP to negative words in HR .46
Swartz et al., 2015 (53) Family depression fMRI to faces 112 LR, 120 HR 13.58 (0.97) Amygdala activation to fearful faces ↑ across 2 years among HR .40
Torpey et al., 2013 (60) High NE; Parental depression ERN 326 6.14 (0.42) High NE predicted ↓ ERN; no effect of parental depression .25

Social Processes
Masten et al., 2011 (74) Prospective prediction fMRI to social exclusions 20 12.94 ↑ sgACC during social exclusion predicted depressive symptoms .84
Olino et al., 2015 (76) Maternal depression fMRI to peer acceptance 23 LR, 10 HR 12.98 (2.14) ↑ precuneus and IPL and ↓ ACC, insula, and caudate during social acceptance in HR .84–1.30
Pérez-Edgar et al., 2006 (75) Parental depression P300 during Posner task under threat of social evaluation 17 LR, 16 HR 7.80 (0.80) ↑ P300 during Posner task under threat of social evaluation in HR

Note: ACC = anterior cingulate cortex; ERN = error related negativity; fMRI = functional magnetic resonance imaging; IPL = inferior parietal lobule; LPP = late positive potential; LR = lower risk; HR = high risk; MDE = major depressive episode; NE = negative emotionality; PE = positive emotionality; PFC = prefrontal cortex; RewP = reward positivity; sgACC = subgenual anterior cingulate cortex.

Table 3.

Summary of consistent patterns (observed across two or more studies) of altered emotional processing associated with risk for depression in youth by type of stimulus and measurement.

BOLD fMRI ERP
Positive Valence Reward anticipation/feedback
↓ striatum
Reward feedback:
↓ RewP
Positive faces/scenes:
↓ LPP

Negative Valence Loss:
↓ striatum
Threatening faces:
↑ amygdala
Negative faces/scenes:
↓ LPP

BOLD fMRI = blood-oxygen-level-dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging; ERP = event-related potentials; LPP = late positive potential; RewP = reward positivity

Positive Valence Systems

Reward Processing

Within PVS, a relatively large literature has examined ERP and fMRI measures of reward processing as vulnerability for depression. The reward positivity (RewP) ERP component, also referred to as the feedback negativity, appears as a relative positivity approximately 250–300 ms following reward feedback compared to loss (21; 22). RewP can be reliably assessed across development (23) and correlates with self-report and behavioral measures of reward sensitivity (24), as well as activation in ventral striatum and medial prefrontal cortex (PFC; 25). There is growing evidence that reduced reward reactivity, as measured by RewP, may be a vulnerability for depression. That is, 9-year-old children with a maternal history of depression but not maternal anxiety exhibited a blunted RewP compared to LR children, even when controlling for subthreshold child symptoms (26). A similar pattern was observed in a smaller sample of HR adolescents who reported increases in sad mood following a mood induction (27). RewP may also be linked to temperamental risk, as low observed and self-reported PE predicted a more blunted RewP in a large sample of children (28). Finally, in two studies of adolescent girls, a reduced RewP prospectively predicted the first onset of depression when accounting for baseline symptoms (29; 30), and appeared to be a relatively specific vulnerability for depression rather than anxiety (29).

In addition, evidence from fMRI indicates that healthy youth with a parental history of depression or who are low in PE exhibit reduced activation in subcortical regions involved in reward processing, including ventral and dorsal striatum in anticipation of (3133) and following receipt of reward (34; 3133; 35) compared to LR youth. Furthermore, one study indicated that reduced activation in ventral striatum during reward anticipation prospectively predicted increases in depressive symptoms in middle to late puberty (36). Risk for depression has also been characterized by alterations in regions of the insula and PFC, which integrate information and regulate responses to feedback. Specifically, there is some evidence that HR youth exhibit reduced insula activation during reward anticipation or outcome compared to LR youth (34; 31), and one study found that increased ventromedial PFC activation to rewards prospectively predicted greater increases in depressive symptoms in adolescent boys (36).

Processing of Positive Images

There is also evidence of blunted neural responses to positive images, including happy faces and pleasant scenes, among HR youth. Several studies with large samples have measured the late positive potential (LPP), a positivity in the ERP wave beginning around 300 ms after stimulus onset that indexes sustained attention towards salient information and activation of motivational systems in the brain (37; Table 1). Youth at HR due to parental depression exhibited a blunted LPP in response to happy faces and pleasant images compared to LR youth (38; 39). A blunted LPP to pleasant images has also been observed in children and adolescents who are lower in PE (40; 41). Finally, in one small fMRI study of offspring of depressed parents, HR youth showed reduced activation of ventral striatum to happy faces compared to LR youth (42). Despite these effects of risk status on neural measures, HR youth did not appear to exhibit abnormalities in behavioral performance when responding to targets presented following emotional images (38; 40; 41).

Summary

Paralleling behavioral evidence that lower reward-seeking may be a vulnerability for later depression (43), consistent evidence from affective neuroscience indicates that altered processing of reward and positive images is observable among HR youth before the onset of the disorder. Similar to findings in depressed adolescents and adults (4446), youth at HR exhibit blunted ERPs to reward feedback and positive images, as well as reduced activation in the striatum during reward anticipation and feedback (e.g., 29; 31; 40).

Moreover, findings of blunted neural processing of reward and positive images are consistent with a large body of EEG asymmetry research indicating that HR youth show reduced relative left frontal activation at rest compared to LR youth, which is thought to reflect reductions in tendencies towards approach motivation (for reviews, 47; 48). Taken together, these studies suggest that HR youth exhibit broad deficits in approach-related motivation and reward processing. Interestingly, altered EEG asymmetry in depression risk is apparent even in infancy (47). As ERP and fMRI studies have typically focused on later childhood and adolescence, it remains unclear when these potential vulnerabilities emerge, but at least some of these measures appear to predict later development of depression (29).

Negative Valence Systems

Processing of Loss and Sadness

Compared to PVS, studies of NVS in depression risk are more mixed and task dependent. Although processing of sadness and loss may be particularly relevant (49), only a few small fMRI studies have examined these responses in HR youth. A recent study found that children of depressed mothers exhibited deactivation of striatum, anterior insula, and parahippocampus when losing a reward, possibly reflecting enhanced sensitivity to loss (34). An earlier small study of adolescent girls found a similar pattern, with HR youth exhibiting less activation in caudate and putamen in response to loss compared to LR youth, along with increased activation in dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; 31). With regard to sad mood, HR adolescent girls in one study exhibited greater activation in amygdala and ventrolateral PFC while viewing sad film clips and less activation in regions involved in emotion regulation (e.g., dorsolateral PFC) during mood repair compared to LR girls (50).

Processing of Threatening or Negative Stimuli

A larger literature has examined ERP and fMRI measures of reactivity to threatening or negative images in depression risk. ERP studies tend to indicate that depression risk is characterized by disengagement from both positive and negative emotional stimuli, consistent with evidence of emotion context insensitivity in adult depression (51). Specifically, several large studies have indicated that HR youth exhibit an attenuated LPP to negative faces and unpleasant images, possibly reflecting less activation of motivational systems in response to salient stimuli. Two studies indicated that offspring of depressed parents exhibited a decreased LPP to negative or threatening faces and scenes compared to LR youth, findings that were not accounted for by parental anxiety (38; 39), and in a third study, lower PE in adolescent girls predicted a blunted LPP to unpleasant images (40). Despite effects observed on the LPP, depression risk was unrelated to behavioral measures, including accuracy and reaction time when responding to targets presented following emotional images (38; 40). Recent evidence indicates that a blunted LPP response may be specific to emotional images, with distinct patterns emerging for self-referential processing. That is, when evaluating adjectives as self-referential, HR girls showed an enhanced LPP response to negative words compared to LR girls (52).

A few fMRI studies have examined threat reactivity in HR youth, with a focus on activation in amygdala and PFC regions involved in emotion regulation. A small study indicated that HR youth exhibited increased amygdala activation to threatening faces compared to LR youth, though groups did not differ in subjective ratings of fear (42). In a relatively large sample, a similar effect of depression risk on amygdala activation was shown to emerge across development. That is, HR youth exhibited an increase in amygdala activation to fearful faces over 2 years, while LR youth exhibited a decrease in amygdala activation over time (53). Finally, one study found that adolescents of depressed parents exhibited decreased dorsolateral PFC activation when processing fearful faces, compared to LR youth, indicating less engagement of regions involved in emotion regulation (54).

Error Monitoring

In addition to responses to threatening images, the error-related negativity (ERN) is an ERP component appearing around the time of commission of an error that is thought to reflect sensitivity to internal sources of threat and activation of cognitive control systems (55; 56). Although an enhanced ERN has been shown to predict the development of anxiety (57; 58), there is some evidence that the ERN may be blunted in risk for depression. A recent study found that despite comparable behavioral performance, 9- to 17-year-old offspring of mothers with recurrent depression exhibited a reduced ERN compared to LR youth, and this effect remained significant when accounting for maternal anxiety (59). A blunted ERN was also observed among 6-year-old children high in temperamental NE (60), though this study found a similar pattern among offspring of mothers with anxiety disorders and failed to find an effect of parental depression on the ERN in offspring. ERN effects among offspring of depressed parents may be specific to youth with greater exposure to maternal depression or emerge later in development. Additional research is needed to evaluate these possibilities and further explore effects among youth at risk for anxiety compared to depression.

Summary

HR youth exhibit alterations in components of NVS, though effects vary depending on the stimulus and measure. Few studies have examined fMRI or ERP responses specifically to loss and sadness, which may be particularly relevant to depression risk. Though there is some evidence that HR youth exhibit deactivation in regions of the striatum in response to losing rewards (31; 34), these findings rely on two studies with small samples. Additional work in this area may clarify the role of NVS in the development of depression. For example, there is evidence from pupillometry, a peripheral measure of neural reactivity indexing attentional capacity and emotional reactivity (61), that HR youth exhibit greater pupil dilation to sad, but not angry or happy, faces compared to LR (62). Moreover, when followed across two years, only responses to sad faces predicted trajectories of depression (63). Relatedly, in behavioral tasks, youth at HR have been shown to exhibit attentional biases when processing sad faces (6466); however, ERP and fMRI measures of responses to sad faces among HR youth have yet to be examined.

A larger literature has examined neural responses to threat and more generally negative stimuli. Consistent with observations of adults with depression (6769), HR youth exhibit a blunted LPP to negative images compared to LR youth, with some evidence that depression risk may also be characterized by a reduced ERN (3840; 59; 60). Although it has been suggested that the ERN may be a specific marker of anxiety rather than depression (70) and two studies of the ERN in offspring of depressed parents have yielded conflicting results, a blunted ERN in HR youth parallels some observations in depressed adolescents and adults (69; 71). Taken together, the existing ERP literature indicates that HR youth exhibit broad emotional disengagement, which may play a role in the later development of symptoms.

On the other hand, two fMRI studies provided evidence of hyperactivation of amygdala to threatening faces among HR youth (42; 53). There are several possible explanations for these seemingly distinct patterns. Given differences in temporal resolutions of fMRI and ERP, one possibility is that HR youth show blunted responses initially, but later difficulty disengaging from negative content. In addition, there is likely variability among HR youth, and distinct patterns of emotional processing may predict specific subtypes of depression (e.g., melancholic depression) or comorbidities (e.g, anxious-depression). In particular, heightened amygdala response to threat is a feature of anxiety disorders (72). Although there is evidence that effects of parental depression on ERP measures in offspring remain significant when accounting for parental anxiety disorders, the unique effects of parental depression and anxiety on amygdala activation in offspring have yet to be examined.

Social Processes

The importance of social factors in the development and treatment of depression has long been recognized (73), but this work has only recently been extended to affective neuroscience to explore how individual differences in neural processing of social interactions relates to risk. Only a few small studies to date have examined fMRI and ERP measures of social processes in depression risk. In one study, youth who exhibited greater subgenual ACC activation during peer exclusion, thought to reflect greater sensitivity to exclusion, exhibited greater increases in depressive symptoms one year later (74). In addition, one study indicated that HR children showed an enhanced P300, an ERP component appearing around 300 ms after a stimulus and reflecting enhanced attention, when completing a cognitive task under threat of social evaluation (i.e., instructed that poor performance would require them to give an embarrassing speech) (75). Finally, though only one study has examined social reward processing in the context of depression risk, preliminary results indicated that HR youth exhibited reduced responses to social acceptance in reward processing regions, including caudate, insula, and ACC (76).

Summary

Though this work is still in early stages, initial evidence indicates that depression risk may be characterized by increased reactivity to negative social feedback (74) and blunted responses to social reward (76). Novel ERP and fMRI paradigms for measuring neural responses during social interactions are in development (77; 78), and may further inform our understanding of trajectories to depression. Despite the majority of this work being focused on peer feedback, there is also growing interest in measuring physiological and neural responses during parent-child interactions (79), which may be particularly relevant for informing mechanisms involved in intergenerational transmission of depression.

Conclusions

The current review indicates that children and adolescents at risk for depression exhibit altered neural responses to both positive and negative emotional stimuli. Importantly, these vulnerabilities are apparent before the onset of the disorder, with evidence that some measures prospectively predict the development of depression into adolescence. Given the range of methods and samples included in these studies, we focus our conclusions on findings emerging across two or more studies (Table 3).

Across levels of analysis, consistent evidence indicates that depression risk in youth is characterized by deficits in approach motivation and PVS, including reduced ERP and striatal responses to reward (e.g., 26; 31; 52). Consistent with theories that reductions in positive reinforcement in the environment increase risk for the development of depression (80), reduced approach motivation and blunted reward responses may serve as vulnerabilities for depression that, in combination with life stress, increase tendencies to withdraw from the environment and failure to adjust behavior to increase positive reinforcement.

Depression risk is also characterized by alterations in NVS, though findings appear to be more dependent on the type of stimulus and measure. Although there is emerging evidence that HR youth exhibit deactivation of regions in the striatum when processing loss of rewards (31; 34), only a few small studies have evaluated responses to loss or sadness in HR youth. With regard to threat reactivity, several ERP studies have indicated that depression risk is characterized by blunted reactivity to negative images and faces, as measured by the LPP (3841), and consistent with patterns of emotional disengagement observed in depression (51; 81). On the other hand, when viewing threatening emotional faces, there is evidence that HR youth exhibit increased amygdala activation compared to LR youth (42; 53). Heterogeneity in outcomes among youth at risk may contribute to discrepant findings, raising the importance of accounting for risk for other forms of psychopathology when examining depression vulnerability. Understanding of the role of NVS in risk for depression could be clarified by comparing types of negative stimuli (e.g., dysphoric vs. threatening) and by multi-method approaches, such as combined EEG-fMRI or integration of neural measures with assessments of gaze patterns and pupil dilation, which could provide insight into the direction and time course of attentional allocation (82).

Lastly, recent work has begun to examine neural and physiological measures during social processes, including peer rejection/acceptance. Though this work is still in the very early stages and results have yet to be replicated or extended to larger samples and additional levels of analysis, this area of research has the potential to further inform understanding of pathways to depression.

Implications for Prevention and Intervention

The current review highlights several processes that may serve as promising targets for prevention and early intervention. Brain-based measures have the potential to inform prediction of outcomes (11), and identifying youth most likely to develop depression may lead to more targeted depression prevention strategies (83). Combining established risk factors (e.g., parental depression) and affective measures (e.g., blunted reward responses) may further improve efforts to identify youth at greatest risk, while providing a specific target and objective marker of outcome (e.g., increasing reward reactivity). We recently demonstrated that a more blunted RewP among adult patients with depression and anxiety predicted greater treatment gains with cognitive behavior therapy (84), supporting the possibility that affective neuroscience methods can be used to identify those most likely to benefit from intervention.

Future Directions

Despite the promise of this work, future research is needed to further clarify vulnerability to depression and to translate neuroscience into clinical practice. First, although some of the measures appear to assess alterations in emotional processing that may not be reflected in overt behavior, additional work is needed to evaluate the extent to which vulnerabilities identified through affective neuroscience correspond with behavioral and self-report observations of maladaptive cognitive and affective styles in depression risk (5; 6; 8; 9). It should also be noted that although effect sizes for neural measures assessed in smaller samples were large in magnitude, these may be overestimates of true effects (85; 86). Indeed, evidence from meta-analyses of FA (47; 48) and studies in large samples indicate that effect sizes for depression risk and neural measures in youth are likely more modest (Table 2), which may be partly attributed to lack of shared method variance between clinical and physiological measures (87). Moreover, many HR youth will not go on to develop the disorder, which may also contribute to modest effect sizes. An essential future direction is examining factors that moderate whether youth at HR develop vulnerabilities. For example, among children of depressed parents, low positive parenting in early childhood predicted a more blunted RewP later in childhood (88), suggesting that the early family environment may be particularly important in shaping the reward system among HR youth. In addition to environmental variables, future studies should consider the role of genetic factors (89), as well as sex differences in vulnerabilities to elucidate factors that make girls more vulnerable to depression (90; 91).

Developmental changes in neural systems involved in emotional processing may further inform understanding of vulnerability (53; 92), raising the need for additional longitudinal research. Further prospective work is also needed to evaluate whether potential vulnerabilities predict changes in symptoms into adolescence and adulthood, whether they mediate associations between early risk factors (e.g., parental depression) and later outcomes, and to identify factors moderate outcomes (e.g., peer relationships, life stress, puberty). Determining whether these processes are risk factors, which increase the probability of depression but do not explain how they influence the likelihood of the condition, or risk mechanisms, which explain the intervening paths that link the risk factor to the outcome, will be essential for identifying processes to target (93). Relatedly, while there is some evidence that blunted RewP and LPP may be relatively specific vulnerabilities for depression rather than anxiety (26; 29; 38; 39), the extent to which other measures reflect affective styles that predict risk for psychopathology more broadly or depression specifically must be further examined in future prospective work.

Finally, it will be imperative to examine psychometric properties of these measures in order to inform intervention. Large studies of diverse samples are needed to examine reliability and validity across development, and ultimately develop norms, including cut points, sensitivity/specificity, and positive and negative predictive power. Moreover, future work is needed to develop guidelines for scoring and interpreting these measures, including development of software packages that are accessible to practitioners. Similarly, it will be important to capitalize on tools that are economical and easily assessed in clinical or community settings, such as EEG/ERP, pupillometry, and autonomic measures.

The current review indicates that advances from affective neuroscience are contributing to our understanding of factors that shape vulnerability to depression across development. Though additional research is needed, several prospective studies support the possibility that these measures can be used to predict which children and adolescents will develop depression, with the potential for informing early intervention efforts and improving long-term outcomes for youth at risk for depression.

Acknowledgments

A.K. and K.L.B. were supported by National Institute of Mental Health T32MH067631 to Mark Rasenick.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Financial Disclosures

All authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

References

  • 1.Ferrari AJ, Charlson FJ, Norman RE, Patten SB, Freedman G, Murray CJL, et al. Burden of depressive disorders by country, sex, age, and year: Findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. PLoS Med. 2013;10 doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kessler RC, Chiu WT, Demler O, Walters EE, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:617–627. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Rudolph KD. Adolescent depression. In: Gotlib I, Hammen C, editors. Handbook of depression. Vol. 2. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2009. pp. 444–466. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Costello EJ, Mustillo S, Erkanli A, Keeler G, Angold A. Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:837–44. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Klein DN, Kujawa A, Black SR, Pennock AT. Depressive disorders. In: Beauchaine TP, Hinshaw SP, editors. Child Adolesc Psychopathol. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; 2013. pp. 543–575. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Garber J, Flynn C. Vulnerability to depression in childhood and adolescence. Vulnerability to psychopathology: Risk across the lifespan. 2001:175–225. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ingram RE, Luxton DD. Vulnerability-stress models. In: Hankin BL, Abela JRZ, editors. Dev Psychopathol A vulnerability-stress Perspect. Thousand Oaks, CA US: Sage Publications, Inc; 2005. pp. 32–46. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hankin BBL. Depression from childhood through adolescence: Risk mechanisms across multiple systems and levels of analysis. Curr Opin Psychol. 2015;4:13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.01.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Lakdawalla Z, Hankin BL, Mermelstein R. Cognitive theories of depression in children and adolescents: A conceptual and quantitative review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2007;10:1–24. doi: 10.1007/s10567-006-0013-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.National Institute of Mental Health. RDoC Matrix. 2016 Retrieved August 5, 2016, from http://www.nimh.nih.gov/research-priorities/rdoc/constructs/rdoc-matrix.shtml.
  • 11.Gabrieli JDE, Ghosh SS, Whitfield-gabrieli S. Prediction as a humanitarian and pragmatic contribution from human cognitive neuroscience. Neuron. 2015;85:11–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Weissman M, Wickramaratne P, Nomura Y, Warner V, Pilowsky D, Verdeli H. Offspring of depressed parents: 20 years later. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:1001–1008. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.6.1001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell AM, Broth MR, Hall CM, Heyward D. Maternal depression and child psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2011;14:1–27. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0080-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Shiner R, Caspi A. Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:2–32. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Klein DN, Dyson MW, Kujawa AJ, Kotov R. Temperament and internalizing disorders. In: Zentner M, Shiner RL, editors. Handbook of temperament. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2012. pp. 541–561. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D. Linking “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2010;136:768–821. doi: 10.1037/a0020327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Kendler KS, Gatz M, Gardner CO, Pederse NL. Personality and Major Depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:1113–1120. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.10.1113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Dougherty LR, Klein DN, Durbin CE, Hayden EP, Olino TM. Temperamental positive and negative emotionality and children’s depressive symptoms: a longitudinal prospective study from age three to age ten. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2010;29:462–488. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Joiner TE, Lonigan CJ. Tripartite model of depression and anxiety in youth psychiatric inpatients: relations with diagnostic status and future symptoms. J Clin Child Psychol. 2000;29:372–382. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP2903_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Clark LA, Watson D, Mineka S. Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103:103–116. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gehring WJ, Willoughby AR. The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science (80-) 2002;295:2279–2282. doi: 10.1126/science.1066893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Miltner WHR, Braun CH, Coles MGH. Event-related brain potentials following incorrect feedback in a time-estimation task: Evidence for a “generic” neural system for error detection. J Cogn Neurosci. 1997;9:788–798. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.6.788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bress JN, Meyer A, Proudfit GH. The stability of the feedback negativity and its relationship with depression during childhood and adolescence. Dev Psychopathol. 2015;17:1285–1294. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414001400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bress JN, Hajcak G. Self-report and behavioral measures of reward sensitivity predict the feedback negativity. Psychophysiology. 2013;50:610–616. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Carlson JM, Foti D, Mujica-Parodi LR, Harmon-Jones E, Hajcak G. Ventral striatal and medial prefrontal BOLD activation is correlated with reward-related electrocortical activity: A combined ERP and fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2011;57:1608–1616. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kujawa A, Proudfit GH, Klein DN. Neural reactivity to rewards and losses in offspring of mothers and fathers with histories of depressive and anxiety disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 2014;123:287–297. doi: 10.1037/a0036285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Foti D, Kotov R, Klein D, Hajcak G. Abnormal neural sensitivity to monetary gains versus losses among adolescents at risk for depression. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2011;39:913–924. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9503-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kujawa A, Proudfit GH, Kessel EM, Dyson M, Olino T, Klein DN. Neural reactivity to monetary rewards and losses in childhood: Longitudinal and concurrent associations with observed and self-reported positive emotionality. Biol Psychol. 2015;104:41–47. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Nelson B, Perlman G, Klein DN, Kotov R, Hajcak G. Blunted neural response to rewards prospectively predicts the development of depression in adolescent girls. Am J Psychiatry 2016 [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bress JN, Foti D, Kotov R, Klein DN, Hajcak G. Blunted neural response to rewards prospectively predicts depression in adolescent girls. Psychophysiology. 2013;50:74–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01485.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Gotlib IH, Hamilton JP, Cooney RE, Singh MK, Henry ML, Joormann J. Neural processing of reward and loss in girls at risk for major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:380–7. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Olino TM, McMakin DL, Morgan JK, Silk JS, Birmaher B, Axelson DA, et al. Reduced reward anticipation in youth at high-risk for unipolar depression: A preliminary study. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2014;8:55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Forbes EE, Ryan ND, Phillips ML, Manuck SB, Worthman CM, Moyles DL, et al. Healthy adolescents’ neural response to reward: Associations with puberty, positive affect, and depressive symptoms. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:162–172. doi: 10.1097/00004583-201002000-00010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Luking KR, Pagliaccio D, Luby JL, Barch DM. Depression risk predicts blunted neural responses to gains and enhanced responses to losses in healthy children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;55:328–337. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Sharp C, Kim S, Herman L, Pane H, Reuter T, Strathearn L. Major depression in mothers predicts reduced ventral striatum activation in adolescent female offspring with and without depression. J Abnorm Psychol. 2014;123:298–309. doi: 10.1037/a0036191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Morgan JK, Olino TM, McMakin DL, Ryan ND, Forbes EE. Neural response to reward as a predictor of increases in depressive symptoms in adolescence. Neurobiol Dis. 2013;52:66–74. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2012.03.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Cuthbert BN, Schupp HT, Bradley MM, Birbaumer N, Lang PJ. Brain potentials in affective picture processing: covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report. Biol Psychol. 2000;52:95–111. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(99)00044-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Nelson BD, Perlman G, Hajcak G, Klein DN, Kotov R. Familial risk for distress and fear disorders and emotional reactivity in adolescence: an event-related potential investigation. Psychol Med. 2015:1–12. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715000471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kujawa A, Hajcak G, Torpey D, Kim J, Klein DN. Electrocortical reactivity to emotional faces in young children and associations with maternal and paternal depression. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2012;53:207–215. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02461.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Speed BC, Nelson BD, Perlman G, Klein DN, Kotov R, Hajcak G. Personality and emotional processing: A relationship between extraversion and the late positive potential in adolescence. Psychophysiology. 2015;52:1039–1047. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kessel E, Kujawa A, Goldstein B, Hajcak G, Bufferd S, Dyson M, Klein DN. Behavioral observations of Positive and Negative Valence Systems in early childhood predict physiological measures of emotional processing three years later. n.d doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.10.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Monk CS, Klein RG, Telzer EH, Schroth EA, Mannuzza S, Moulton JL, III, et al. Amygdala and nucleus accumbens activation to emotional facial expressions in children and adolescents at risk for major depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:90–98. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06111917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Rawal A, Collishaw S, Thapar A, Rice F. “The risks of playing it safe”: A prospective longitudinal study of response to reward in the adolescent offspring of depressed parents. Psychol Med. 2012;1:1–12. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Silk JS, Davis S, Mcmakin DL, Dahl RE, Forbes EE. Why do anxious children become depressed teenagers? The role of social evaluative threat and reward processing. 2012;42:2095–2107. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712000207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Foti D, Hajcak G. Depression and reduced sensitivity to non-rewards versus rewards: Evidence from event-related potentials. Biol Psychol. 2009;81:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Weinberg A, Perlman G, Kotov R, Hajcak G. Depression and reduced neural response to emotional images: Distinction from anxiety and importance of symptom dimensions and age of onset. 2016;125:26–39. doi: 10.1037/abn0000118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Thibodeau R, Jorgensen RS, Kim S. Depression, anxiety, and resting frontal EEG asymmetry: a meta-analytic review. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006;115:715–729. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Peltola MJ, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Alink LRA, Huffmeijer R, Biro S, van Ijzendoorn MH. Resting frontal EEG asymmetry in children: Meta-analyses of the effects of psychosocial risk factors and associations with internalizing and externalizing behavior. Dev Psychobiol. 2014;56:1377–1389. doi: 10.1002/dev.21223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Woody ML, Gibb BE. Integrating NIMH Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) into depression research. Curr Opin Psychol. 2015;4:6–12. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Joormann J, Cooney RE, Henry ML, Gotlib IH. Neural correlates of automatic mood regulation in girls at high risk for depression. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121:61–72. doi: 10.1037/a0025294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bylsma LM, Morris BH, Rottenberg J. A meta-analysis of emotional reactivity in major depressive disorder. Clin Psychol Rev. 2008;28:676–691. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Speed BC, Nelson BD, Auerbach RP, Klein DN, Hajcak G. Depression risk and electrocortical reactivity during self-referential emotional processing in 8 to 14-year-old girls. J Abnorm Psychol. 2016 doi: 10.1037/abn0000173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Swartz JR, Williamson DE, Hariri AR. Developmental change in amygdala reactivity during adolescence: Effects of family history of depression and stressful life events. Am J Psychiatry. 2015;172:276–283. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14020195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Mannie ZN, Taylor MJ, Harmer CJ, Cowen PJ, Norbury R. Frontolimbic responses to emotional faces in young people at familial risk of depression. J Affect Disord. 2011;130:127–132. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.09.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Weinberg A, Meyer A, Hale-Rude E, Perlman G, Kotov R, Klein DN, Hajcak G. Error-related negativity and sustained threat: Conceptual framework and empirical evaluation in an adolescent sample. Psychophysiology. 2016;53:372–385. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12538. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Moser JS, Moran TP, Schroder HS, Donnellan MB, Yeung N. On the relationship between anxiety and error monitoring: a meta-analysis and conceptual framework. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:466. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Meyer A, Hajcak G, Torpey-Newman DC, Kujawa A, Klein DN. Enhanced error-related brain activity in children predicts the onset of anxiety disorders between the ages of 6 and 9. J Abnorm Psychol. 2015;124:266–274. doi: 10.1037/abn0000044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Lahat A, Lamm C, Chronis-Tuscano A, Pine DS, Henderson Ha, Fox Na. Early behavioral inhibition and increased error monitoring predict later social phobia symptoms in childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53:447–455. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.12.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Meyer A, Bress JN, Hajcak G, Gibb BE. Maternal depression is related to reduced error-related brain activity in child and adolescent offspring. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2016:1–12. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2016.1138405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Torpey DC, Hajcak G, Kim J, Kujawa AJ, Dyson MW, Olino TM, Klein DN. Error-related brain activity in young children: Associations with parental anxiety and child temperamental negative emotionality. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2013;54:854–862. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Laeng B, Sirois S, Gredebäck G. Pupillometry: A Window to the Preconscious? 2012;3 doi: 10.1177/1745691611427305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Burkhouse KL, Siegle GJ, Gibb BE. Pupillary reactivity to emotional stimuli in children of depressed and anxious mothers. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2014;55:1009–1016. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Burkhouse KL, Siegle GJ, Woody ML, Kudinova AY, Gibb BE. Pupillary reactivity to sad stimuli as a biomarker of depression risk: Evidence from a prospective study of children. J Abnorm Psychol. 2015;124:498–506. doi: 10.1037/abn0000072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Joormann J, Talbot L, Gotlib IH. Biased processing of emotional information in girls at risk for depression. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007;116:135–143. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Kujawa AJ, Torpey D, Kim J, Hajcak G, Rose S, Gotlib IH, Klein DN. Attentional biases for emotional faces in young children of mothers with chronic or recurrent depression. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2011;39:125–135. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9438-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Gibb BE, Benas JS, Grassia M, McGeary J. Children’s attentional biases and 5-HTTLPR genotype: Potential mechanisms linking mother and child depression. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2009;38:415–426. doi: 10.1080/15374410902851705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Foti D, Olvet DM, Klein DN, Hajcak G. Reduced electrocortical response to threatening faces in major depressive disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2010;27:813–820. doi: 10.1002/da.20712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.MacNamara A, Kotov R, Hajcak G. Diagnostic and symptom-based predictors of emotional processing in generalized anxiety disorder and major depressive disorder: An event-related potential study. Cognit Ther Res. 2015 doi: 10.1007/s10608-015-9717-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Weinberg A, Klein DN, Hajcak G. Increased error-related brain activity distinguishes generalized anxiety disorder with and without comorbid major depressive disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121:885–896. doi: 10.1037/a0028270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Bress JN, Meyer A, Hajcak G. Differentiating anxiety and depression in children and adolescents: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2013:1–12. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.814544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Ladouceur CD, Slifka JS, Dahl RE, Birmaher B, Axelson Da, Ryan ND. Altered error-related brain activity in youth with major depression. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2012;2:351–362. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2012.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Goodkind MS, Gyurak A, Etkin A. Functional neurocircuitry and neuroimaging studies of anxiety disorders. In: Charney DS, Sklar P, Buxbaum JD, Nestler EJ, editors. Neurobiol Ment Illn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2013. pp. 606–620. [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Gotlib IH, Hammen CL. Psychological aspects of depression: Toward a cognitive-interpersonal integration. Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Masten CL, Eisenberger NI, Borofsky LA, McNealy K, Pfeifer JH, Dapretto M. Subgenual anterior cingulate responses to peer rejection: A marker of adolescents’ risk for depression. Dev Psychopathol. 2011;23:283–292. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Pérez-Edgar K, Fox NA, Cohn JF, Kovacs M. Behavioral and electrophysiological markers of selective attention in children of parents with a history of depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:1131–1138. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.02.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Olino TM, Silk JS, Osterritter C, Forbes EE. Social reward in youth at risk for depression: A preliminary investigation of subjective and neural differences. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2015;25:711–721. doi: 10.1089/cap.2014.0165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Kujawa A, Arfer KB, Klein DN, Proudfit GH. Electrocortical reactivity to social feedback in youth: A pilot study of the Island Getaway task. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2014;10:140–147. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Silk JS, Siegle GJ, Lee KH, Nelson EE, Stroud LR, Dahl RE. Increased neural response to peer rejection associated with adolescent depression and pubertal development. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014;9:1798–1807. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Woody M, Feurer C, Sosoo EE, Hastings PD, Gibb BE. Synchrony of physiological activity during mother-child interaction: Moderation by maternal history of depression. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;53:1689–1699. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Dimidjian S, Barrera M, Martell C, Muñoz RF, Lewinsohn PM. The origins and current status of behavioral activation treatments for depression. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2011;7:1–38. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Hajcak Proudfit G, Bress JN, Foti D, Kujawa A, Klein DN. Depression and event-related potentials: emotional disengagement and reward insensitivity. Curr Opin Psychol. 2015;4:110–113. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.12.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Price RB, Rosen D, Siegle GJ, Ladouceur CD, Tang K, Allen KB, et al. From anxious youth to depressed adolescents: Prospective prediction of 2-year depression symptoms via attentional bias measures. J Abnorm Psychol. 2016;125:267–278. doi: 10.1037/abn0000127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Horowitz JL, Garber J. The prevention of depressive symptoms in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic review. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2006;74:401. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Burkhouse KL, Kujawa A, Kennedy AE, Shankman SA, Langenecker SA, Phan KL, Klumpp H. Neural reactivity to reward as a predictor of cognitive behavioral therapy response in anxiety and depression. Depress Anxiety. 2016;33:281–288. doi: 10.1002/da.22482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Leon AC, Davis LL, Kraemer HC. The role and interpretation of pilot studies in clinical research. J Psychiatr Res. 2011;45:626–629. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Button KS, Ioannidis JPa, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ESJ, Munafò MR. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14:365–76. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Patrick CJ, Venables NC, Yancey JR, Hicks BM, Nelson LD, Kramer MD. A construct-network approach to bridging diagnostic and physiological domains: Application to assessment of externalizing psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol. 2013;122:902. doi: 10.1037/a0032807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Kujawa A, Proudfit GH, Laptook R, Klein DN. Early parenting moderates the association between parental depression and neural reactivity to rewards and losses in offspring. Clin Psychol Sci. 2015;3:503–515. doi: 10.1177/2167702614542464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Hariri AR, Drabant EM, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS, Mattay VS, Egan MF, Weinberger DR. A susceptibility gene for affective disorders and the response of the human amygdala. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:146–152. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.2.146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Hankin BL, Abramson LY, Moffitt TE, Silva PA, McGee R, Angell KE. Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal study. J Abnorm Psychol. 1998;107:128–140. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.107.1.128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Nolen-Hoeksema S, Girgus JS. The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence. Psychol Bull. 1994;115:424–443. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Goldstein BL, Shankman SA, Kujawa A, Torpey-Newman DC, Olino TM, Klein DN. Developmental changes in electroencephalographic frontal asymmetry in young children at risk for depression. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016 doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Garber J. Depression in children and adolescents: Linking risk research and prevention. Am J Prev Med. 2006;31:104–125. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Liu X, Hairston J, Schrier M, Fan J. Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:1219–1236. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML. Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron. 2015;86:646–664. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage. 2002;16:331–48. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Fusar-Poli P, Placentino A, Carletti F, Landi P, Allen P, Surguladze S, et al. Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: A voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2009;34:418–432. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Lindquist KA, Satpute AB, Wager TD, Weber J, Barrett LF. The brain basis of positive and negative affect: Evidence from a meta-analysis of the human neuroimaging literature. Cereb cortex. 2016;26:1910–1922. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Proudfit GH. The reward positivity: From basic research on reward to a biomarker for depression. Psychophysiology. 2015;52:449–459. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Foti D, Weinberg A, Dien J, Hajcak G. Event-related potential activity in the basal ganglia differentiates rewards from nonrewards: Temporospatial principal components analysis and source localization of the feedback negativity. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32:2207–2216. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Hajcak G, MacNamara A, Olvet DM. Event-related potentials, emotion, and emotion regulation: an integrative review. Dev Neuropsychol. 2010;35:129–155. doi: 10.1080/87565640903526504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Liu Y, Huang H, McGinnis-Deweese M, Keil A, Ding M. Neural substrate of the late positive potential in emotional processing. J Neurosci. 2012;32:14563–14572. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3109-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Sabatinelli D, Keil A, Frank DW, Lang PJ. Emotional perception: Correspondence of early and late event-related potentials with cortical and subcortical functional MRI. Biol Psychol. 2013;92:513–519. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Miltner WHR, Lemke U, Weiss T, Holroyd C, Scheffers MK, Coles MGH. Implementation of error-processing in the human anterior cingulate cortex: A source analysis of the magnetic equivalent of the error-related negativity. Biol Psychol. 2003;64:157–166. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(03)00107-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Polich J. Neuropsychology of P300. Oxford handbook of event-related potentials. 2012:159–188. [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Warbrick T, Mobascher A, Brinkmeyer J, Musso F, Richter N, Stoecker T, et al. Single-trial P3 amplitude and latency informed event-related fMRI models yield different BOLD response patterns to a target detection task. Neuroimage. 2009;47:1532–1544. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Bengson JJA, Kelley T, Mangun GR. The neural correlates of volitional attention: A combined fMRI and ERP study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36:2443–2454. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES