Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2017 May 10;6:e23789. doi: 10.7554/eLife.23789

Reciprocal synapses between mushroom body and dopamine neurons form a positive feedback loop required for learning

Isaac Cervantes-Sandoval 1,*, Anna Phan 1, Molee Chakraborty 1, Ronald L Davis 1,*
Editor: K VijayRaghavan2
PMCID: PMC5425253  PMID: 28489528

Abstract

Current thought envisions dopamine neurons conveying the reinforcing effect of the unconditioned stimulus during associative learning to the axons of Drosophila mushroom body Kenyon cells for normal olfactory learning. Here, we show using functional GFP reconstitution experiments that Kenyon cells and dopamine neurons from axoaxonic reciprocal synapses. The dopamine neurons receive cholinergic input via nicotinic acetylcholine receptors from the Kenyon cells; knocking down these receptors impairs olfactory learning revealing the importance of these receptors at the synapse. Blocking the synaptic output of Kenyon cells during olfactory conditioning reduces presynaptic calcium transients in dopamine neurons, a finding consistent with reciprocal communication. Moreover, silencing Kenyon cells decreases the normal chronic activity of the dopamine neurons. Our results reveal a new and critical role for positive feedback onto dopamine neurons through reciprocal connections with Kenyon cells for normal olfactory learning.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23789.001

Research Organism: D. melanogaster

Introduction

Molecular, cellular and systems neuroscience studies using Drosophila have uncovered several major tenets that reveal the logic by which olfactory memories are formed and stored. The first is that modulation of mushroom body (MB) Kenyon cell (KC) synaptic activity underlies associative learning and memory (Davis, 1993, Davis, 2011; Heisenberg, 2003; Busto et al., 2010). KCs are connected and respond to multiple primary sensory centers including those for olfaction, vision and gustation (Yagi et al., 2016). The major olfactory input occurs in the MB calyx, with the KC dendrites receiving input in this neuropil region from approximately 180 olfactory projection neurons. The representation of different odorants is highly sparse (Honegger et al., 2011) across the three major types of KC – the αβ, α’β’ and γ – being generated, in part, by a negative feedback loop between KC and the GABAergic APL neuron (Liu and Davis, 2009; Lin et al., 2014a).

A second tenet is that the modulation of Kenyon cell function for olfactory learning employs intracellular cAMP signaling. Multiple learning mutants that alter cAMP synthesis, degradation, or effector function have been characterized and mapped through expression or functional studies to the Kenyon cells (Davis, 1993; 2005). More specifically, inputs representing an odor-conditioned stimulus (CS) along with an unconditioned stimulus (US) to the KC lead to the mobilization of the cAMP signaling cascade (Davis, 1993) employing the Rutabaga-encoded adenylyl cyclase as a coincidence detector (Tomchik and Davis, 2009) to induce changes in the output of the KC.

A third tenet is that the mushroom body output neurons (MBOn) receive input from the KC and their activation influences approach or avoidance behavior (Davis, 1993; Heisenberg, 2003; Aso et al., 2014b; Owald et al., 2015). There are 21 classes of MBOn whose dendrites tile the lobes containing the axons of the KC in 15 different compartments. Recent studies have shown that learning alters odor drive to specific MBOn (Séjourné et al., 2011; Pai et al., 2013; Plaçais et al., 2013; Bouzaiane et al., 2015; Hige et al., 2015; Owald et al., 2015). Interestingly, reward learning appears to reduce drive to output pathways that direct avoidance, whereas aversive learning increases drive to avoidance pathways while reducing drive to approach pathways.

A fourth tenet is that dopamine neurons (DAn) are activated by aversive or rewarding stimuli to provide the US input for olfactory classical conditioning (Riemensperger et al., 2005; Mao and Davis, 2009; Liu et al., 2012). There exist several lines of evidence in support of this model. First, blocking DAn during training with temperature sensitive dynamin (shibire) impairs memory acquisition (Schwaerzel et al., 2003). Second, mutants for the dopamine receptor dDA1 impair memory acquisition that can be rescued by expressing this receptor in KC (Kim et al., 2007; Qin et al., 2012). Third, the artificial activation of DAn using thermo- or optogenetic approaches along with odor CS stimulation leads to conditioned behavior (Schroll et al., 2006; Claridge-Chang et al., 2009; Aso et al., 2010, 2012; Burke et al., 2012; Liu et al., 2012). Thus, artificial stimulation of the DAn is sufficient to signal the reinforcing effect of the US. The innervation by DAn occurs primarily on the axonal processes of the KC in the MB lobes, across the same 15 compartments occupied by the dendrites of the MBOn (Mao and Davis, 2009; Pech et al., 2013; Aso et al., 2014a). These findings have suggested that DAn stimulation, driven by a US, changes the odor-specific output weight of KC synapses onto the corresponding MBOn, tilting the MBOn network to direct appropriate behavior. The dopaminergic inputs modulate KC synaptic transmission with precise spatial specificity, allowing KC to differentially convey olfactory signals to each of their postsynaptic MBOn (Cohn et al., 2015).

However, this model of multiple, independent plasticity/output channels is oversimplified given evidence of additional and poorly understood computations occurring within the KC neuropil. For instance, Perisse et al. (2016) provided evidence that plasticity in the MBOn-γ1pedc compartment initiates feed-forward inhibition that alters the flow of information in the MBOnβ’2mp and MBOnγ5β’2a compartments. In addition, different DAn drive synaptic changes between KC and MBOn with different rules (Hige et al., 2015). Activation of γ1pedc DAn induces both learning and odor-specific synaptic depression in MBOnγ1pedc when paired with only a 1 s odor presentation, whereas similar plasticity in the α2α’2 compartment requires 1 min odor presentation with DAn activation.

Here, we report additional complexity in the circuitry and connections that influence olfactory learning. We demonstrate that DAn are both pre- and postsynaptic to KC, forming axoaxonic reciprocal synapses that are required for optimal learning through a positive feedback mechanism. DAn input to KC feeds back onto the same DAn through KC>DAn cholinergic synapses amplifying the synaptic release of DA required for reinforcement.

Results and discussion

KC are both pre-and post-synaptic to DAn forming axoaxonic reciprocal connections

We previously showed that genetic insults in KC affect ongoing activity in DA PPL1 neurons, the set of DAn that innervate the vertical lobes, heel, and junction area of the MB (Figure 1A,B; compartments γ1–2, α1–3, α’1–3). Reducing the expression of the Scribble protein using the KC-specific R13F02-gal4 driver increases total ongoing activity measured in the axon terminals of the DAn that innervate γ2α’1 (Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2016). This result suggested the existence of a retrograde signal from the postsynaptic KC to the presynaptic DAn partners that influences their activity, or that KC might be both pre- and post-synaptic to DAn, perhaps forming axoaxonic reciprocal synapses in the complex MB lobe neuropil. We first used trans-synaptic GFP fluorescence reconstitution (Macpherson et al., 2015) to look for a possible physical and functional connection between KC axons and DAn axons. This system uses the expression of a split version of GFP tethered to presynaptic synaptobrevin (syb::spGFP1-10) in the hypothetical presynaptic partner and a membrane tethered version of the complementing split GFP (CD4::spGFP11) in the hypothetical postsynaptic counterpart (Figure 1C). Figure 1B shows KC co-labeled with DAn represented by the TH-LexA driver innervating the α’3, α3, α2α’2, γ1ped, γ1, γ2α’1 and β2 and β’2 compartments (Berry et al., 2015). As expected, expressing presynaptic split GFP (uas-syb::spGFP1-10) in DAn using TH-gal4 along with membrane tagged complementary split GFP (lexAop-CD4::spGFP11) in KC with R13F02-lexA reconstituted GFP fluorescence in all the aforementioned compartments (Figure 1D). Surprisingly, the reciprocal experiment, with uas-syb::spGFP1-10 expressed in the KC and lexAop-CD4::spGFP11 in the DAn, reconstituted GFP fluorescence in an identical pattern of MB lobe compartment expression (α’3, α3, α2α’2, γ1ped, γ1, and γ2α’1; minimal fluorescence also in PAM β2β’2a) (Figure 1E). Three-dimensional reconstruction of images obtained by structured illumination microscopy showed well-defined synaptic puncta in the α2α’2 domain (Video 1). No staining was observed when brains expressing either half of the GRASP components were imaged under the same conditions (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). These results strongly suggest the presence of reciprocal axoaxonic synaptic connectivity between KC and the DAn represented in the TH driver with the connectivity occurring essentially across the entire vertical MB lobe and the lateral aspect of the horizontal lobe.

Video 1. A three-dimensional structured illumination image of the α2α’2 MB lobe compartment showing pre-synaptic KC puncta with post-synaptic DAn specializations on their axons.

Download video file (3.3MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.23789.004

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23789.004

Figure 1. KC and DAn PPL1 neurons form axoaxonic reciprocal synapses.

(A) Diagram of 15 discrete compartments that divide the MB lobes according to the DAn pre- and MBOn postsynaptic innervation pattern. (B) Co-labeling of the MB lobes using R13F02-gal4 to drive uas-tdtomato expression and TH-lexA to drive lexAop-CD8::GFP expression. Lobe areas that remain primarily green receive less or no innervation from the PPL1 cluster of DAn compared to those that are white. (C) Diagram of trans-synaptic fluorescence reconstitution. Synaptic syb::spGFP1-10 is expressed in the presynaptic neuron along with spGFP11 in the putative postsynaptic counterpart. GFP reconstitution occurs only if synaptic connectivity exists. (D) GFP reconstitution when syb::spGFP1-10 is expressed in DAn. (E) GFP reconstitution when syb::spGFP1-10 is expressed in KC.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23789.002

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. No GFP signal was observed after staining brains that expressed only half of the GRASP system.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) No detectable GFP signal was observed when spGFP11 was expressed with R13F02-lexA. (B) No detectable GFP signal was observed when only syb::spGFP1-10 was expressed with TH-gal4. (C) Reconstitution of GFP was observed when spGFP was expressed in KC with R13F02-gal4 and syb::spGFP1-10 in DAn with TH-gal4 driver. The three images shown were obtained using the same image acquisition parameters.

To show functional connectivity between KC and DAn, we used the ATP/P2X2 system in ex vivo preparations to stimulate the KC and recorded cAMP and calcium influx in the DAn axon terminals using an Epac1 reporter and GCaMP3, respectively. We first verified that the ATP/P2X2 system was working properly by co-expressing P2X2 and the GCaMP3 reporter in DAn using TH-gal4 and recording stimulated calcium responses to bath applied ATP. A dose-response curve to ATP confirmed the efficacy of artificially activating DAn using P2X2/ATP (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). We then tested the pre-synaptic function of DAn and found that artificial stimulation of DAn contained in the TH-gal4 driver induced increasing elevations of cAMP in postsynaptic KC with increasing concentrations of ATP (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). We failed to observe calcium responses in the KC axons when DAn were stimulated with increasing concentrations of ATP (not shown). This is consistent with prior observations (Tomchik and Davis, 2009) and indicates that DA receptor engagement on KC produces the modulatory signal of stimulating cAMP accumulation without altering ion channel function to allow calcium influx. When we stimulated the KC with the ATP/P2X2 ion channel, we discovered potent cAMP and calcium responses in DAn (Figure 2A,B). Although not strictly comparable, the evoked cAMP responses in the DAn were roughly two fold larger than responses evoked in the KC with DAn stimulation (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). We discovered during the course of these experiments that our control genotype containing uas-P2X2 alone showed significant calcium responses with ATP application (Figure 2—figure supplement 3A). This suggested leaky expression of the P2X2 channel in PPL1 neurons despite reports that no leaky expression occurs in other types of neurons (Yao et al., 2012). Although responses in this control were significantly lower than the experimental group, we chose to use optogenetic tools to verify our results with the P2X2 system (Figure 2—figure supplement 3B–E). We first verified that we could stimulate KC using Chrimson. For this, we coexpressed Chrimson along with GCaMP6 in KC and recorded calcium responses to red light stimulation in living animals using two-photon microscopy. Strong calcium responses were observed in KC to red light stimulation. Control animals without Chrimson expression showed no response (Figure 2—figure supplement 3B). Similar to our results with ATP/P2X2, stimulating KC with red light produced potent calcium responses in PPL1 DAn (Figure 2—figure supplement 3C). Because the functional imaging responses and functional GRASP reconstitution experiment showed the largest responses in the α2α’2 DAn, we focused the remainder of our experiments on this particular neuron and the compartment of the MB lobes that it innervates.

Figure 2. KC and DAn PPL1 neurons axoaxonic reciprocal synapses are functional.

(A) Cyclic AMP responses in DAn (α3, α’3, α2α’2 and γ2α’1) to artificial stimulation of KC using ATP/P2X2 system. The solid line of each trace represents the mean and shaded area represents ± SEM at various concentrations of ATP used in the experiment. Cyclic AMP accumulation was observed in the axons of the DAn that innervate all of the MB lobe compartments that were recorded. N = 9–14. (B) Calcium responses in DAn (α3, α’3, α2α’2 and γ2α’1) to artificial stimulation of KC using ATP/P2X2 system. The solid line of each trace represents mean and shaded area represents ± SEM. Calcium responses were observed in the axons of the DAn that innervate all of the MB lobe compartments that were recorded. N = 12–16. (C) Diagram of the experimental setup. A micropipette was used to focally apply ACh to the calyx of the MB while imaging in both the calyx and MB lobes. (D) Calcium responses in both calyx and lobes of the KC to ACh application in the presence (red) or absence (black) of 1 µM TTX. TTX blocked the responses in the lobes but not the calyx, indicating that TTX was functional in blocking action potentials. N = 7–10. (E) Diagram of the experimental setup. A micropipette was used to focally apply 5 mM ATP to the α2α2’ compartment of MB while recording calcium responses in α2α’2 DAn. (F) Calcium responses in α2α’2 DAn in the presence (red) or absence (gray) of 1 µM TTX. TTX was without significant effect, indicating that the artificial activation of KC fibers were capable of evoking responses in DAn axon terminals through local, mono-synaptic transmission. N = 9–11. Data were analyzed using Mann-Whitney U non-parametric test. Bars represent the means ± SEM.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23789.005

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Testing the efficacy of the ATP/P2X2 system.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Diagram of the experimental setup. Flies co-expressing the P2X2 channel and GCaMP3 in DAn using TH-gal4 were stimulated by bath application of increasing concentrations of ATP. (B) Calcium responses obtained by stimulation with different concentrations of ATP and measured in all compartments shaded green in panel A. N = 12. The solid lines represent the means and the shaded area represents ± SEM.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. DAn are presynaptic to KC.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(A) Diagram of experiment setup. DAn were stimulated with increasing concentrations of ATP while recording cAMP responses in KC. (B) Cyclic AMP dose-response curve after artificial stimulation of DAn using the ATP/P2X2 system. N = 8. The solid lines represent the means and shaded areas represent ± SEM. Responses were recorded in all compartments shaded red in panel A.
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. KC and PPL1 DAn axoaxonic reciprocal synapses are functional.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

(A) Calcium responses in DAn (α3, α’3, α2α’2 and γ2α’1) to artificial stimulation of KC using the ATP/P2X2 system along with genetic controls. The uas-alone control (blue bars) showed significant responses to P2X2 stimulation, suggesting leaky expression of the channel. Bars represent the mean ± SEM at 2.5 mM ATP. Calcium responses were observed in the axons of the DAn that innervate all MB lobe compartments that were recorded. N = 7–12. Data were analyzed using Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA and Dunn’s multiple comparison test. Bars represent the means ± SEM. (B) Testing the efficacy of the light/Chrimson system. Left panel shows a diagram of the experimental setup. Flies co-expressing the Chrimson channel and GCaMP6f in KC using R13F02-gal4 were stimulated by red light and calcium responses recorded. N = 12. The solid lines represent the means with the shaded area ± SEM. (C) Calcium responses in DAn (α3, α’3, α2α’2 and γ2α’1) to artificial stimulation of KC using the light/Chrimson system. The solid line of each trace represents the mean and shaded area represents ± SEM. Calcium responses were observed in the axons of the DAn that innervate all MB lobe compartments that were recorded. N = 6–9. (D) Calcium responses in α2α’2 DAn in the presence (red) or absence (gray) of 1 µM TTX. TTX was without significant effect, indicating that the artificial activation of KC fibers were capable of evoking responses in DAn axon terminals through local, monosynaptic transmission. N = 14. Data were analyzed using a Mann-Whitney U non-parametric test. Bars represent the means ± SEM. (E) Connections between KC and DAn are cholinergic. Calcium responses in α2α’2 DAn to stimulation of KC using the light/Chrimson system were strongly inhibited in presence of nAChR antagonist mecamylamide (500 µM) (left). Vehicle was without effect (right). N = 16–18. Data were analyzed using a Mann-Whitney U non-parametric test. Bars represent the means for calcium response ± SEM.

To exclude the possibility that the evoked responses in the DAn were from polysynaptic innervation, we measured calcium responses in α2α’2 DAn to focal application of ATP through a glass micropipette using fast pressure injection in presence or absence of 1 µM tetradotoxin (TTX). TTX inhibits neuronal firing by binding to voltage-gated sodium channels and inhibiting action potentials driven from polysynaptic transmission. To ensure that TTX treatment was effective, we first measured calcium responses in both the KC calyx and lobes simultaneously after focal application of 10 mM ACh to the MB calyx in the presence or absence of TTX. As expected, robust calcium responses were observed in both calyx and lobes to ACh applied to the calyx. Nevertheless, the lobe but not calyx responses were completely abolished when TTX was present (Figure 2C–D), demonstrating that our procedures utilizing TTX effectively inhibited action potential propagation. Using this method, no difference in DAn calcium responses was observed after focal application of ATP in presence or absence of TTX (Figure 2E–F). Similar results were obtained using optogenetic stimulation (Figure 2—figure supplement 3D). These results, together with the functional trans-synaptic fluorescence reconstitution (Figure 1C–E) demonstrate that DAn are both pre- and post-synaptic KC, through axoaxonic reciprocal connections.

The connection between KC and DAn is cholinergic

Recent studies revealed that KCs are cholinergic and nAChR receptors in MBOn are required for memory retrieval (Barnstedt et al., 2016). To explore the chemical nature of KC>DAn synaptic connection, we recorded ex vivo, ATP/P2X2 evoked calcium responses in the α2α’2 DAn axon terminals in brains pre-incubated for 10 min with a battery of different inhibitors. We included inhibitors to nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (mecamylamide), muscarinic acetylcholine receptors (atropine), GABA receptors (CGP 35348), mGlu receptors (S-MCPG) and dopamine/serotonin receptors (pimozide). Only the nicotinic AChR inhibitor, mecamylamide (100 µM), significantly reduced ATP/P2X2 evoked calcium responses in DAn (Figure 3A). We then completed a full dose-inhibition curve to increasing concentrations of mecamylamide by comparing the responses in the presence of the inhibitor to a second response recorded ten minutes after wash out of the inhibitor. Up to sixty percent inhibition was obtained with 250 and 500 µM mecamylamide when measured in this manner (Figure 3B). We suggest that full inhibition was not achieved due to the leaky expression of P2X2 channel and perhaps incomplete washout. However, optogenetics experiments showed >85% inhibition with 500 µM mecamylaminde (Figure 2—figure supplement 3E). We then reasoned that if ACh is the neurotransmitter for the KC>DAn axoaxonic synapses, then DAn should respond directly to ACh. We therefore measured calcium responses in α2α’2 DAn axon terminals to focally applied ACh. As predicted, focal application of ACh robustly increased intracellular calcium in DAn projections, reaching maximum responses at 10 and 100 mM concentrations (Figure 3C–D). We subsequently recorded calcium responses to focally applied ACh in the presence of 1 µM TTX to rule out the possibility that the DAn responses resulted from indirect activation of other neurons including KC. The magnitude of ACh evoked calcium responses was no different in presence or absence of TTX, indicating that these neurons have ACh postsynaptic specializations in their axonal terminals located in the MB neuropil (Figure 3E).

Figure 3. Connections between KC and DAn are cholinergic.

(A) Calcium responses in α2α’2 DAn to stimulation of KC using the ATP/P2X2 system in presence of inhibitors to nACh receptors (100 µM mecamylamide), mACh receptors (atropine), GABA receptors (CGP 35348), mGlu receptors (S-MCPG) and dopamine/serotonin receptors (pimozide). N = 10–16. Data were analyzed with Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA and Dunn’s multiple comparison test. Bars represent the means for calcium response ± SEM. (B) Dose-response plots to increasing concentrations of mecamylamide comparing the responses in the presence of the inhibitor to a second response recorded after a ten-minute washout. N = 10–16. (C) Diagram of the experiment setup. A micropipette was used to focally apply ACh to α2α’2 DAn axonal terminals while recording calcium responses. (D) Calcium dose-response curve in α2α’2 DAn terminals to focal application of increasing concentrations of ACh. N = 6. Error bars represent SEM. (E) Calcium responses to focal application of 10 mM ACh to α2α’2 DAn axons in the presence (red) or absence (black) of 1 µM TTX. N = 6. Data were analyzed using Mann-Whitney U non-parametric test. Bars represent the mean ± SEM. (F) Three-minute aversive olfactory memory of flies expressing RNAi transgenes to different nAChRα subunits in DAn using TH-gal4. N = 12. Data were analyzed with Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA and Dunn’s multiple comparison test. *p<0.05. Bars represent the mean ± SEM. (G) Three-minute aversive olfactory memory of flies expressing an RNAi transgene to nAChRα1 RNAi compared to the parental controls, TH-gal4/+ and nAChRα1RNAi/+. N = 6. Data were analyzed with Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA and Dunn’s multiple comparison test. Bars represent the mean ± SEM. p<0.05. (H) Three-minute aversive olfactory memory of flies expressing an RNAi transgene to nAChRα4 compared to the parental controls, TH-gal4/+ and nAChRα4RNAi/+. N = 6. Data were analyzed with Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA and Dunn’s multiple comparison test. Bars represent the mean ± SEM. p<0.05. (I) Three-minute aversive olfactory memory of flies expressing an RNAi transgene to nAChRα5 compared to the parental controls, TH-gal4/+ and nAChRα5RNAi/+. N = 6. Data were analyzed with Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA and Dunn’s multiple comparison test. Bars represent the mean ± SEM. p<0.05. (J) Three-minute aversive olfactory memory of flies expressing an RNAi transgene to nAChRα4 in α2α’2 PPL1 neurons compared to the parental controls, MB058B-split-gal4/+ and nAChRα4RNAi/+. N = 6. Data were analyzed with Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA and Dunn’s multiple comparison test. Bars represent the mean ± SEM. p<0.05.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23789.009

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Knockdown of nAChR subunits using RNAi in DAn does not alter sleep patterns.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Sleep was monitored across three days. N = 24. Bars represent the mean ± SEM.

Cholinergic input to DAn is required for normal olfactory learning

As described above, the PPL1 DAn have been widely modeled as conveying US shock information to the KC during olfactory conditioning. Because of this, we probed the possible role of the KCDAn reciprocal connection for this behavior by reducing the ACh input to the DAn axons from the KC. To do so, we knocked down several nAChR subunits using multiple RNAi transgenes and tested the flies for performance after olfactory conditioning. Flies expressing RNAi to nAChRα1, nAChRα4, and nAChRα5 but not nAChRα6 using TH-gal4 modestly impaired aversive olfactory learning when compared to control flies. Control flies were constructed by crossing TH-gal4-containing flies to the RNAi parental line 36303, which contains an empty docking site, attp40 (Figure 3F). This attp40 site was used to insert RNAi constructs to obtain flies harboring the RNAi-transgenes. Similar results were obtained when flies expressing RNAi’s to nAChRα1, nAChRα4, and nAChRα5 were individually compared to their two parental genetic controls, TH-gal4/+ and uas-RNAi/+ (Figure 3G–I), suggesting that these three subunits are expressed on the axons of DAn. Similar results were observed when we used a Split-gal4 driver, MB058B, to restrict nAChRα4 KD in α’2 α’2 PPL1 neurons. No significant differences were observed in olfactory and electric shock avoidance for any of the genotypes used for behavioral experiments in our study (data not shown). Importantly, these results coupled with those showing KC axoaxonic synapses to DAn indicate that KC-driven cholinergic input to the distal axons of DAn is required for normal learning, possibly due to amplification of dopamine release at acquisition.

Since the DAn>KC circuit is known to be involved in sleep and sleep homeostasis, we also evaluated sleep profiles in flies expressing RNAi transgenes to nAChR subunits in DAn. We found no significant differences in sleep patterns compared to control flies (Figure 3—figure supplement 1).

KC input to DAn is required for normal DAn synaptic release during learning

If the cholinergic input from KC to DAn is required for full DA release during learning as suggested by our results above, then blocking MB output should impair DAn synaptic release during memory acquisition. To test this prediction, we silenced KC using Kir2.1 channel expression while recording presynaptic calcium responses using syt:GCaMP6s during a training session in the α2α’2 PPL1 DAn axon terminals. This reporter uses the genetically encoded calcium sensor GCaMP6s tethered to the C terminus of the vesicular synaptic protein synaptotagmin. Presynaptic calcium is a key regulator of neurotransmission and this sensor provides a sensitive readout of synaptic release (Cohn et al., 2015). Flies of the genotype R82C10-LexA/lexAop-sytGCaMP6s; R13F02-gal4/uas-kir2.1, tub-gal80ts were reared at 18°C and then switched at eclosion to 30°C for 16 hr to allow expression of kir2.1 in KC (Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2013) (Figure 4A). Expression of Kir 2.1, an inwardly rectifying potassium channel, silences neurons by inhibiting membrane depolarization. Presynaptic calcium responses were then recorded in flies subjected to aversive olfactory training under the microscope at room temperature. The responses were then compared to flies where kir2.1 expression was not induced by maintaining the flies at 18°C. A significant decrease in presynaptic calcium responses was observed when KC were silenced with kir2.1 compared to flies without kir2.1 expression (Figure 4B). The shape of α2α’2 PPL1 responses during CS/US conditioning suggested that they are bipartite, consisting of a rapidly adapting odor response with superimposed electric shock peaks. To determine which of the two components require KC input, we recorded responses in α2α’2 DAn with odor or shock delivery separately. Surprisingly, we found that silencing KC impaired both odor- and shock-induced presynaptic calcium responses in α2α’2 DAn (Figure 4C,D). As expected, control flies without the kir2.1 transgene (R82C10-LexA/lexAop-sytGCaMP6s; R13F02-gal4/tub-gal80ts) did not show significant differences in presynaptic calcium responses to conditioning, odor or shock presentation (Figure 4E–G). These results show that normal presynaptic calcium responses and dopamine release requires KC input to DAn during conditioning and with odor or shock stimuli presented alone. Most importantly, these results are consistent with a model of cholinergic input from KC representing a positive gain signal to DAn for subsequent DA release.

Figure 4. KC input is required for normal DAn synaptic release during learning.

Figure 4.

(A) Diagram of the experimental setup. Flies of the indicated genotype were reared at 18°C and then 1–2 day old adults were switched to 30°C to induce kir2.1 channel expression in KC for 16 hr. Flies were then returned to room temperature and prepared for functional imaging. Control flies remained at 18°C until removing them to room temperature for functional imaging. (B) Calcium responses in α2α’2 DAn during aversive olfactory training (odor+shock) in flies with (red) or without (black) kir2.1 expression in KC. N = 10–12. The solid line represents the mean and the shaded area represents ± SEM. Data were analyzed using Mann-Whitney U non-parametric test. Bars represent the mean ± SEM of the area under the curve during the one minute of conditioning (the 60–120 s imaging time window). (C) Calcium responses in α2α’2 DAn during odor delivery in flies with (red) or without (black) kir2.1 expression in KC. N = 13–14. The solid line represents the mean and the shaded area represents ± SEM. Data were analyzed using Mann-Whitney U non-parametric test. Bars represent the mean ± SEM of the area under the curve during the one minute of odor presentation (the 60–120 s imaging time windows). (D) Calcium responses in α2α’2 DAn during 12 shock pulses delivery in flies with (red) or without (black) kir2.1 expression in KC. N = 14. The solid line represents the mean and the shaded area represents ± SEM. Data were analyzed using Mann-Whitney U non-parametric test. Bars represent the mean ± SEM of the area under the curve during the one minute of shock pulses (the 60–120 s imaging time window) (E–G) Parallel data to panels B–D but using flies without the kir2.1 transgene. These control data show that the differential responses observed in panels B–D require the kir2.1 expression.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23789.011

KC input shapes DAn ongoing activity

DAn respond to a variety of external and internal stimuli (Riemensperger et al., 2005; Mao and Davis, 2009; Tomchik, 2013; Liu et al., 2012; Lin et al., 2014b; Lewis et al., 2015; Musso et al., 2015) and two recent studies have shown that ongoing dopaminergic activity is correlated with the behavioral state of the animal (Berry et al., 2015; Cohn et al., 2015). These observations suggest that the ongoing activity of the distributed DAn network represents the current external, internal and behavioral state of the animal providing for a moment-by-moment update of these elements in the fly (Berry et al., 2015; Cohn et al., 2015; Waddell, 2016). This hypothesis emphasizes the need to understand how the ongoing DAn activity is generated. Using the same tools as above, we tested the role for KC by recording ongoing calcium transients of α2α’2 PPL1 neurons for 10 min while silencing KC with expression of kir2.1 (Figure 5A). Ongoing activity recorded in the α2α’2 PPL1 neuron axon terminals with the syt:GCaMP6s presynaptic reporter showed robust calcium transients compared to the previously reported lack of activity recorded with GCaMP3 (Berry et al., 2012, Berry et al., 2015; Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2016) (Figure 5B). Silencing KC significantly reduced total ongoing activity in α2α’2 PPL1 neuron axon terminals compared to flies without kir2.1 expression (Figure 5B–C). Upon quantification, we found no change in the number of peaks counted between the experimental and control groups (Figure 5D). Nevertheless, a significant decrease was observed in the magnitude of these peaks when KCs were silenced with kir2.1 (Figure 5E). As expected, control flies without the kir2.1 transgene (R82C10-LexA/lexAop-sytGCaMP6s; R13F02-gal4/tub-gal80ts) did not show significant differences in total ongoing DAn activity, peak number or peak magnitude (Figure 5F–I), indicating that the differences observed with flies containing kir2.1 were not attributable to the temperature differences in the experimental protocol (Figure 5A). These results indicate that KC input to DAn shapes the magnitude of this chronic activity although it may not generate it. In addition, our results demonstrate that both phasic/evoked and chronic/ongoing activity of DAn is influenced by direct cholinergic input from KC through axoaxonic reciprocal synapses.

Figure 5. KC input shapes DAn ongoing activity.

Figure 5.

(A) Diagram of the experimental setup. Flies of the indicated genotype were reared at 18°C and 1–2 day old adults were then switched to 30°C to induce kir2.1 channel expression in KC for 16 hr. The flies were then returned to room temperature and prepared for functional imaging. Control flies remained at 18°C until removing them to room temperature for functional imaging. (B) Representative 10 min recording of calcium ongoing activity in α2α’2 DA axon terminals in flies with (red) or without (black) kir2.1 expression in the KC. (C) Mean of total activity per sec during 10 min of recording of ongoing activity in α2α’2 DAn axon terminals in flies with (red) or without (black) kir2.1 expression in the KC. N = 10–12. Data were analyzed using Mann-Whitney U non-parametric test. Bars represent the mean ± SEM. (D) Mean number of peaks of the same 10 min recordings of ongoing activity in α2α’2 DAn axon terminals in flies with (red) or without (black) kir2.1 expression in the KC. Data were analyzed using Mann-Whitney U non-parametric test. Bars represent the mean ± SEM. (E) Mean peak magnitude of ongoing activity in α2α’2 DAn in flies with (red) or without (black) kir2.1 expression in the KC. Data were analyzed using Mann-Whitney U non-parametric test. Bars represent the mean ± SEM. (F–I) Parallel data to panels B–E but using flies without the kir2.1 transgene. These control data show that the decrease in total activity and mean peak magnitude observed in panels B and E require the kir2.1 expression. N = 11–13. Data were analyzed using Mann-Whitney U non-parametric test.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23789.012

Concluding remarks

Our results demonstrate that DAn are both pre- and post-synaptic to KC through axoaxonic reciprocal connections, in contrast to current models which envision them as providing only pre-synaptic input. The DAn>KC half of the reciprocal synapse employs DA as neurotransmitter, although we cannot rule out the possibility that other neurotransmitters are co-released with DA. The KC>DAn half of the reciprocal synapse is cholinergic. However, the fact that we were able to observe both cAMP and calcium responses in DAn with KC stimulation suggests that there may be other mediators of this reciprocal connection. Blocking the cholinergic input to DAn attenuates aversive olfactory learning, providing evidence that its function is, at least in part, to provide an amplification signal for the initial DA release due to activating the US pathway. Consistent with this role we found that silencing KC impairs DAn presynaptic calcium responses to conditioning, odor and shock stimuli, presumably influencing dopamine release and explaining the learning phenotype. Overall, our results support the existence of a positive feedback loop (Riemensperger et al., 2005) required for optimal learning. We envision that DAn receive direct input from the US during conditioning which is conveyed to KC. The KCs also receive coincident olfactory input and this coincidence provides positive feedback onto the DAn through cholinergic synapses to further increase DAn activity.

Our results also show that KC input to DAn shapes their ongoing or chronic activity. It is plausible that ongoing activity in the DAn provides a moment-by-moment update of the external environment and internal states and the behavioral status of the fly that appropriately reconfigures the KC>MBOn flow of information (Berry et al., 2015; Cohn et al., 2015; Waddell, 2016). Thus, the DAn/KC/MBOn circuit may form a recurrent network that serves as the insect’s brain center for the rapid integration of sensory information and decision-making (Lewis et al., 2015). Local feedback loops, achieved by reciprocal connectivity like that described here, may provide computational benefits to fine tune and optimize the output. Behavioral flexibility may be achieved by passing information through local reentrant loops with constant updating from the external or internal state of the organism (Person and Khodakhah, 2016).

A caveat in this and other studies (Barnstedt et al., 2016) is that we cannot exclude that other, non-KC, cholinergic input to DAn contributes to memory acquisition. Massive efforts to generate ‘connectomes’ in multiple species (Bargmann and Marder, 2013) may offer resolution to this issue at some point in the future. Alternatively, they may continue to reveal additional connections and complexities that defy an immediate understanding. Studies like the present one, that reveal unexpected relationships between synaptic partners in difficult-to-untangle circuits, expose the need to advance beyond ‘connectomics’ and develop new tools that allow us to silence or activate specific channels and specific synaptic connections between neurons of interest without affecting other functions in the same cells (Bargmann and Marder, 2013).

Materials and methods

Drosophila husbandry

Flies were cultured on standard medium at room temperature. Crosses, unless otherwise stated, were kept at 25°C and 70% relative humidity with a 12 hr light-dark cycle. The Gal4 drivers used in this study include R13F02-gal4, R82C10-lexA (Jenett et al., 2012), TH-gal4 (Friggi-Grelin et al., 2003), and TH-lexA (Berry et al., 2015). The uas-RNAi transgene stocks were obtained from the TRiP collection library (Perkins et al., 2015) and included control line (36303), which contains the attP docking site without an RNAi insertion (hence same insertional modification as uas-RNAi lines), and the nAChRRNAi lines (28688, 31985, 25943, 25835). Additional transgene stocks included uas-dicer2, lexAop-GFP::CD8, uas-tdTomato (Pfeiffer et al., 2010), uas-syb::spGFP1-10, lexAop-CD4::spGFP11 (Macpherson et al., 2015), lexAop-epac1, uas-epac1, lexAop-P2X2, uas-P2X2, lexAop-GCamp3.0 (Lima and Miesenböck, 2005; Yao et al., 2012), uas-GCaMP3 (Tian et al., 2009), lexAop-syt::GCaMP6s (Cohn et al., 2015), uas-GCaMP6f (Chen et al., 2013), uas-CsChrimson::TdTomato, lexAop-CsChrimson::TdTomato (Hoopfer et al., 2015), uas-kir2.1 (Baines et al., 2001), and tub-gal80ts (McGuire et al., 2003).

Immunostaining

Whole brains were isolated and processed with minor modifications of those described (Jenett et al., 2012). Brains were first incubated with primary antibodies including: rabbit polyclonal anti-DsRed(1:1000, Clontech Labs, cat# 632496, RRID:AB_10013483) and mouse monoclonal anti-GFP (1:1000, Sigma, cat# G6539; anti-GRASP, RRID:AB_259941). Secondary antibodies included: anti-rabbit IgG conjugated to Alexa Fluor 488 (1:800, Life Technologies Cat# A11008, RRID: AB_143165) and anti-mouse IgG conjugated to Alexa Fluor 633 (1:400, Life Technologies Cat# A21052, RRID: AB_141459). Images were collected using a 10X objective with a Leica TCS SP5 II confocal microscope with 488 and 633 nm laser excitation.

For structured illumination microscopy (SIM), brains were processed as for standard immunostaining plus these additional steps: After the wash steps following secondary antibody incubation, brains were post-fixed in 1%PFA in PAT3 (0.5% BSA, 0.5% Triton, 1X PBS) for two hr at room temperature. The brains were then washed for 2 hr in 1 mL of PAT3 buffer two times, followed by a quick wash with 1X PBS and then one with dH2O. The brains were mounted onto high precision 18 × 18 mm coverslips previously coated with poly-lysine with the anterior face towards the coverslip for imaging the MB lobes. Excess moisture was removed from the coverslips, and these were then dehydrated by 5 min incubations in a series of ethanol dilutions (20%, 30%, 50%, 70%, 95%, 100%x2). The brains were cleared with an overnight incubation in methyl salicylate and then the brains with the coverslip still attached were sandwiched with a second coverslip between two pieces of plastic. The coverslips were then sealed with nail polish. SIM images were collected on Zeiss ELYRA PS1 and examined using ImageJ.

Functional imaging experiments

Bath applied experiments 

These experiments were performed as described previously with minor modifications (Tomchik and Davis, 2009). Briefly, brains were dissected at room temperature and maintained in continuous perfusion of saline solution (2 mL/min; 103 mM NaCl, 3 mM KCl, 5 mM HEPES, 1.5 mM CaCl2, MgCl2, 26 mM NaHCO3, 1 mM NaH2PO4, 10 mM trehalose, 7 mM sucrose, and 10 mM glucose [pH 7.2]) under a confocal microscope. Adenosine triphosphate (ATP) at different concentrations was applied for 30 s in perfusion solution and then subsequently washed out. Recordings were collected at 2 frames/sec during a 10 min recording session. Responses were plotted as the baseline-normalized change in GCaMP fluorescence (%∆F/Fo) or change in epac1-camps inverse FRET ratio (%∆R/Ro) within a defined region of interest.

Focal application experiments

For focal application of compounds, brains were dissected and treated for 30 s with 2 mg/mL collagenase and washed in saline for 1 min before initiating the recording. Acetylcholine or ATP was dissolved in saline with 5 mM Texas red dextran (to allow optical monitoring of the stimulus). The solutions were applied focally to the α2α’2 MB lobe compartment or calyx via pressure ejection from a glass micropipette (~5 µm tip diameter). The glass micropipette was previously coated with BSA-conjugated Texas Red (Life Technologies, Grand Island, NY) in order to visualize the micropipette tip position.

In vivo imaging 

For measuring calcium responses with conditioning, odor or shock delivery, we processed flies as previously described (Tomchik and Davis, 2009). Briefly, a single fly was aspirated without anesthesia into a narrow slot the width of a fly in a custom-designed recording chamber. The head was immobilized by gluing the eyes to the chamber using myristic acid and the proboscis fixed to reduce movements. A small, square section of dorsal cuticle was removed from the head to allow optical access to the brain. Fresh saline (103 mM NaCl, 3 mM KCl, 5 mM HEPES, 1.5 mM CaCl2, MgCl2, 26 mM NaHCO3, 1 mM NaH2PO4, 10 mM trehalose, 7 mM sucrose, and 10 mM glucose [pH 7.2]) was perfused immediately across the brain to prevent desiccation and ensure the health of the fly. Using a 25X water-immersion objective and a Leica TCS SP5 II confocal microscope with a 488 nm argon laser, we imaged the α2α’2 neuron for 3 min, during which stimuli (odor+shock, odor or shock) was delivered starting at the 2nd min. We used one PMT channel (510–550 nm) to detect syt::GCaMP6s fluorescence. We set the baseline for α2α’2 ongoing activity detected with syt::GCaMP6s as the mean of minimums across 10 s bins of the first 60 s of the recording. This baseline was then used to calculate %∆F/Fo for the complete recording. Total activity was quantified as the area under the curve during the 1 min corresponding to stimulus delivery (between 60 and −120 sec of the recording). Expression of kir2.1 was induced by transferring flies to 30°C for 16 hr. Flies were then returned to room temperature and imaged for the following 3 hr.

Ongoing activity recording

Ongoing activity of α2α’2 was recorded as previously reported (Berry et al., 2012). Briefly, flies processed as in the previous experiment were recorded at 2 frames/sec during a 10 min time window. The activity per sec was calculated for each recording using algorithms previously described (Berry et al., 2012). Peaks in these recordings were quantified using the Matlab (RRID:SCR_001622) algorithm peakfinder developed by Yoder, 2011.

Optogenetic stimulation and two-photon in vivo calcium imaging

Combined in vivo optogenetic and calcium imaging were conducted using a two-photon microscope to minimize non-specific optogenetic simulation during recording. Flies for these experiments were collected from crosses were kept in dark on standard corn meal supplemented with 0.2 mM all trans-retinal. After eclosion, adult flies were kept for 2–5 days in food supplemented with 0.5 mM all trans-retinal. Live flies were fixed to the recording chambers as in the previous experiments (above). For light stimulation, we used a high power 617 nm LED (Red-Orange Luxeon Rebel LED-122 lm) and a 0.5 mm diameter optic fiber (Edmund Optics) placed adjacent to the eyes of a fly in a recording chamber. The light power at the fly head was measured to be 1 mW/mm2. The LED was controlled using an arduino microcontroller (Arduino Uno). Light pulses were delivered at 40 Hz, with 10 ms duration for a total of 200 ms (Barnstedt et al., 2016).

Behavior

Two to six-day-old flies were used for all behavior experiments. Standard aversive olfactory conditioning experiments were performed as described (Beck et al., 2000). Briefly, a group of ~60 flies were loaded into a training tube where they received the following sequence of stimuli: 30 s of air, 1 min of an odor paired with 12 pulses of 90V electric shock (CS+), 30 s of air, 1 min of a second odor with no electric shock pulses (CS−), and finally 30 s of air. We used 3-octanol (OCT) and 4-methylcyclohexanol (MCH) as standard odorants. To measure memory, we transferred the flies into a T-maze where they were allowed 2 min to choose between the two odors used for training.

Monitoring sleep

Drosophila sleep and activity were measured using the Drosophila Activity Monitoring System (Trikinetcs) as described previously (Shaw et al., 2000). In summary, flies were placed into individual 65 mm tubes and the activity was continuously monitored. Locomotor activity was measured in 1 min bins and sleep was defined as periods of quiescence lasting at least 5 m. Sleep in min/hr was plotted as a function of circadian time (hr).

Statistical analyses

Statistics were performed using Prism 5 (Graphpad). All tests were two-tailed and confidence levels were set at α=0.05 The figure legends present the p values and comparisons made for each experiment. Unless otherwise stated, non-parametric tests were used for all imaging data and DAM system monitoring, while parametric tests were used for olfactory memory comparisons as PI values are normally distributed (Walkinshaw et al., 2015).

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by grants 4R37NS19904, 5R01NS052351 and 1R35NS097224 from the NINDS to RLD. We are grateful to the Iris and Junming Le Foundation for funds to purchase a super-resolution microscope. We thank Dr. Orie Shafer for sharing flies for ATP/P2X2 experiments. We thank Dr. Marco Gallio for sharing functional GRASP flies and Dr. Vanessa Ruta for sharing syt::GCaMP6s flies.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke 4R37NS19904 to Ronald L Davis.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke 5R01NS052351 to Ronald L Davis.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke 1R35NS097224 to Ronald L Davis.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

IC-S, Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

AP, Methodology, Writing—review and editing, Developed Super-resolution imaging for whole mount Drosophila brains.

MC, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, performed all immunostaining and curation/analysis of the corresponding data.

RLD, Resources, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

References

  1. Aso Y, Hattori D, Yu Y, Johnston RM, Iyer NA, Ngo TT, Dionne H, Abbott LF, Axel R, Tanimoto H, Rubin GM. The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning. eLife. 2014a;3:e04577. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aso Y, Herb A, Ogueta M, Siwanowicz I, Templier T, Friedrich AB, Ito K, Scholz H, Tanimoto H. Three dopamine pathways induce aversive odor memories with different stability. PLoS Genetics. 2012;8:e1002768. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aso Y, Sitaraman D, Ichinose T, Kaun KR, Vogt K, Belliart-Guérin G, Plaçais PY, Robie AA, Yamagata N, Schnaitmann C, Rowell WJ, Johnston RM, Ngo TT, Chen N, Korff W, Nitabach MN, Heberlein U, Preat T, Branson KM, Tanimoto H, Rubin GM. Mushroom body output neurons encode Valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila. eLife. 2014b;3:e04580. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aso Y, Siwanowicz I, Bräcker L, Ito K, Kitamoto T, Tanimoto H. Specific dopaminergic neurons for the formation of labile aversive memory. Current Biology. 2010;20:1445–1451. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baines RA, Uhler JP, Thompson A, Sweeney ST, Bate M. Altered electrical properties in Drosophila neurons developing without synaptic transmission. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:1523–1531. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-05-01523.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bargmann CI, Marder E. From the connectome to brain function. Nature Methods. 2013;10:483–490. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Barnstedt O, Owald D, Felsenberg J, Brain R, Moszynski JP, Talbot CB, Perrat PN, Waddell S. Memory-Relevant mushroom body output synapses are cholinergic. Neuron. 2016;89:1237–1247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Beck CD, Schroeder B, Davis RL. Learning performance of normal and mutant Drosophila after repeated conditioning trials with discrete stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:2944–2953. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-08-02944.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Berry JA, Cervantes-Sandoval I, Chakraborty M, Davis RL. Sleep facilitates memory by blocking dopamine neuron-mediated forgetting. Cell. 2015;161:1656–1667. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.05.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Berry JA, Cervantes-Sandoval I, Nicholas EP, Davis RL. Dopamine is required for learning and forgetting in Drosophila. Neuron. 2012;74:530–542. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bouzaiane E, Trannoy S, Scheunemann L, Plaçais PY, Preat T. Two independent mushroom body output circuits retrieve the six discrete components of Drosophila aversive memory. Cell Reports. 2015;11:1280–1292. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.04.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Burke CJ, Huetteroth W, Owald D, Perisse E, Krashes MJ, Das G, Gohl D, Silies M, Certel S, Waddell S. Layered reward signalling through octopamine and dopamine in Drosophila. Nature. 2012;492:433–437. doi: 10.1038/nature11614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Busto GU, Cervantes-Sandoval I, Davis RL. Olfactory learning in Drosophila. Physiology. 2010;25:338–346. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00026.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cervantes-Sandoval I, Chakraborty M, MacMullen C, Davis RL. Scribble scaffolds a signalosome for active forgetting. Neuron. 2016;90:1230–1242. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.05.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cervantes-Sandoval I, Martin-Peña A, Berry JA, Davis RL. System-like consolidation of olfactory memories in Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:9846–9854. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0451-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chen TW, Wardill TJ, Sun Y, Pulver SR, Renninger SL, Baohan A, Schreiter ER, Kerr RA, Orger MB, Jayaraman V, Looger LL, Svoboda K, Kim DS. Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature. 2013;499:295–300. doi: 10.1038/nature12354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Claridge-Chang A, Roorda RD, Vrontou E, Sjulson L, Li H, Hirsh J, Miesenböck G. Writing memories with light-addressable reinforcement circuitry. Cell. 2009;139:405–415. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.08.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cohn R, Morantte I, Ruta V. Coordinated and compartmentalized neuromodulation shapes sensory processing in Drosophila. Cell. 2015;163:1742–1755. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.11.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Davis RL. Mushroom bodies and Drosophila learning. Neuron. 1993;11:1–14. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(93)90266-T. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Davis RL. Olfactory memory formation in Drosophila: from molecular to systems neuroscience. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2005;28:275–302. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Davis RL. Traces of Drosophila memory. Neuron. 2011;70:8–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Friggi-Grelin F, Coulom H, Meller M, Gomez D, Hirsh J, Birman S. Targeted gene expression in Drosophila dopaminergic cells using regulatory sequences from tyrosine hydroxylase. Journal of Neurobiology. 2003;54:618–627. doi: 10.1002/neu.10185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Heisenberg M. Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2003;4:266–275. doi: 10.1038/nrn1074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hige T, Aso Y, Modi MN, Rubin GM, Turner GC. Heterosynaptic plasticity underlies aversive olfactory Learning in Drosophila. Neuron. 2015;88:985–998. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Honegger KS, Campbell RA, Turner GC. Cellular-resolution population imaging reveals robust sparse coding in the Drosophila mushroom body. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:11772–11785. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1099-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hoopfer ED, Jung Y, Inagaki HK, Rubin GM, Anderson DJ. P1 interneurons promote a persistent internal state that enhances inter-male aggression in Drosophila. eLife. 2015;4:e11346. doi: 10.7554/eLife.11346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Jenett A, Rubin GM, Ngo TT, Shepherd D, Murphy C, Dionne H, Pfeiffer BD, Cavallaro A, Hall D, Jeter J, Iyer N, Fetter D, Hausenfluck JH, Peng H, Trautman ET, Svirskas RR, Myers EW, Iwinski ZR, Aso Y, DePasquale GM, Enos A, Hulamm P, Lam SC, Li HH, Laverty TR, Long F, Qu L, Murphy SD, Rokicki K, Safford T, Shaw K, Simpson JH, Sowell A, Tae S, Yu Y, Zugates CT. A GAL4-driver line resource for Drosophila neurobiology. Cell Reports. 2012;2:991–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2012.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kim YC, Lee HG, Han KA. D1 dopamine receptor dDA1 is required in the mushroom body neurons for aversive and appetitive learning in Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:7640–7647. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1167-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lewis LP, Siju KP, Aso Y, Friedrich AB, Bulteel AJ, Rubin GM, Grunwald Kadow IC. A higher brain circuit for Immediate integration of conflicting sensory information in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2015;25:2203–2214. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lima SQ, Miesenböck G. Remote control of behavior through genetically targeted photostimulation of neurons. Cell. 2005;121:141–152. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2005.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lin AC, Bygrave AM, de Calignon A, Lee T, Miesenböck G. Sparse, decorrelated odor coding in the mushroom body enhances learned odor discrimination. Nature Neuroscience. 2014a;17:559–568. doi: 10.1038/nn.3660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lin S, Owald D, Chandra V, Talbot C, Huetteroth W, Waddell S. Neural correlates of water reward in thirsty Drosophila. Nature Neuroscience. 2014b;17:1536–1542. doi: 10.1038/nn.3827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Liu C, Plaçais PY, Yamagata N, Pfeiffer BD, Aso Y, Friedrich AB, Siwanowicz I, Rubin GM, Preat T, Tanimoto H. A subset of dopamine neurons signals reward for odour memory in Drosophila. Nature. 2012;488:512–516. doi: 10.1038/nature11304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Liu X, Davis RL. The GABAergic anterior paired lateral neuron suppresses and is suppressed by olfactory learning. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:53–59. doi: 10.1038/nn.2235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Macpherson LJ, Zaharieva EE, Kearney PJ, Alpert MH, Lin TY, Turan Z, Lee CH, Gallio M. Dynamic labelling of neural connections in multiple colours by trans-synaptic fluorescence complementation. Nature Communications. 2015;6:10024. doi: 10.1038/ncomms10024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mao Z, Davis RL. Eight different types of dopaminergic neurons innervate the Drosophila mushroom body neuropil: anatomical and physiological heterogeneity. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2009;3:5. doi: 10.3389/neuro.04.005.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. McGuire SE, Le PT, Osborn AJ, Matsumoto K, Davis RL. Spatiotemporal rescue of memory dysfunction in Drosophila. Science. 2003;302:1765–1768. doi: 10.1126/science.1089035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Musso PY, Tchenio P, Preat T. Delayed dopamine signaling of energy level builds appetitive long-term memory in Drosophila. Cell Reports. 2015;10:1023–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.01.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Owald D, Felsenberg J, Talbot CB, Das G, Perisse E, Huetteroth W, Waddell S. Activity of defined mushroom body output neurons underlies learned olfactory behavior in Drosophila. Neuron. 2015;86:417–427. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Pai TP, Chen CC, Lin HH, Chin AL, Lai JS, Lee PT, Tully T, Chiang AS. Drosophila ORB protein in two mushroom body output neurons is necessary for long-term memory formation. PNAS. 2013;110:7898–7903. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216336110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Pech U, Pooryasin A, Birman S, Fiala A. Localization of the contacts between Kenyon cells and aminergic neurons in the Drosophila melanogaster brain using SplitGFP reconstitution. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2013;521:3992–4026. doi: 10.1002/cne.23388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Perisse E, Owald D, Barnstedt O, Talbot CB, Huetteroth W, Waddell S. Aversive Learning and appetitive motivation toggle feed-forward inhibition in the Drosophila Mushroom body. Neuron. 2016;90:1086–1099. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.04.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Perkins LA, Holderbaum L, Tao R, Hu Y, Sopko R, McCall K, Yang-Zhou D, Flockhart I, Binari R, Shim HS, Miller A, Housden A, Foos M, Randkelv S, Kelley C, Namgyal P, Villalta C, Liu LP, Jiang X, Huan-Huan Q, Wang X, Fujiyama A, Toyoda A, Ayers K, Blum A, Czech B, Neumuller R, Yan D, Cavallaro A, Hibbard K, Hall D, Cooley L, Hannon GJ, Lehmann R, Parks A, Mohr SE, Ueda R, Kondo S, Ni JQ, Perrimon N. The transgenic RNAi project at Harvard Medical School: resources and validation. Genetics. 2015;201:843–852. doi: 10.1534/genetics.115.180208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Person AL, Khodakhah K. Recurrent feedback loops in associative learning. Neuron. 2016;89:427–430. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Pfeiffer BD, Ngo TT, Hibbard KL, Murphy C, Jenett A, Truman JW, Rubin GM. Refinement of tools for targeted gene expression in Drosophila. Genetics. 2010;186:735–755. doi: 10.1534/genetics.110.119917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Plaçais PY, Trannoy S, Friedrich AB, Tanimoto H, Preat T. Two pairs of mushroom body efferent neurons are required for appetitive long-term memory retrieval in Drosophila. Cell Reports. 2013;5:769–780. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2013.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Qin H, Cressy M, Li W, Coravos JS, Izzi SA, Dubnau J. Gamma neurons mediate dopaminergic input during aversive olfactory memory formation in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2012;22:608–614. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Riemensperger T, Völler T, Stock P, Buchner E, Fiala A. Punishment prediction by dopaminergic neurons in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2005;15:1953–1960. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.09.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Schroll C, Riemensperger T, Bucher D, Ehmer J, Völler T, Erbguth K, Gerber B, Hendel T, Nagel G, Buchner E, Fiala A. Light-induced activation of distinct modulatory neurons triggers appetitive or aversive learning in Drosophila larvae. Current Biology. 2006;16:1741–1747. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Schwaerzel M, Monastirioti M, Scholz H, Friggi-Grelin F, Birman S, Heisenberg M. Dopamine and octopamine differentiate between aversive and appetitive olfactory memories in Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:10495–10502. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-33-10495.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Shaw PJ, Cirelli C, Greenspan RJ, Tononi G. Correlates of sleep and waking in Drosophila melanogaster. Science. 2000;287:1834–1837. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5459.1834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Séjourné J, Plaçais PY, Aso Y, Siwanowicz I, Trannoy S, Thoma V, Tedjakumala SR, Rubin GM, Tchénio P, Ito K, Isabel G, Tanimoto H, Preat T. Mushroom body efferent neurons responsible for aversive olfactory memory retrieval in Drosophila. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:903–910. doi: 10.1038/nn.2846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Tian L, Hires SA, Mao T, Huber D, Chiappe ME, Chalasani SH, Petreanu L, Akerboom J, McKinney SA, Schreiter ER, Bargmann CI, Jayaraman V, Svoboda K, Looger LL. Imaging neural activity in worms, flies and mice with improved GCaMP calcium indicators. Nature Methods. 2009;6:875–881. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Tomchik SM, Davis RL. Dynamics of learning-related cAMP signaling and stimulus integration in the Drosophila olfactory pathway. Neuron. 2009;64:510–521. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Tomchik SM. Dopaminergic neurons encode a distributed, asymmetric representation of temperature in Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:2166–2176. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3933-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Waddell S. Neural plasticity: dopamine tunes the mushroom body output network. Current Biology. 2016;26:R109–R112. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Walkinshaw E, Gai Y, Farkas C, Richter D, Nicholas E, Keleman K, Davis RL. Identification of genes that promote or inhibit olfactory memory formation in Drosophila. Genetics. 2015;199:1173–1182. doi: 10.1534/genetics.114.173575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Yagi R, Mabuchi Y, Mizunami M, Tanaka NK. Convergence of multimodal sensory pathways to the mushroom body calyx in Drosophila melanogaster. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:29481. doi: 10.1038/srep29481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Yao Z, Macara AM, Lelito KR, Minosyan TY, Shafer OT. Analysis of functional neuronal connectivity in the Drosophila brain. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012;108:684–696. doi: 10.1152/jn.00110.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Yoder N. PeakFinder. http://www.mathworks.com/ matlabcentral/fileexchange/25500 2011
eLife. 2017 May 10;6:e23789. doi: 10.7554/eLife.23789.013

Decision letter

Editor: K VijayRaghavan1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Reciprocal synapses between mushroom body and dopamine neurons form a positive feedback loop required for learning" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by K VijayRaghavan as the Reviewing Editor and Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

Isaac Cervantes-Sandoval and colleagues report experiments that demonstrate reciprocal synapses between dopaminergic neurons and intrinsic mushroom body neurons (Kenyon cells) of the Drosophila brain. The entire study is of importance in the context of the dissection of neuronal circuits underlying associative learning, and it is written with this emphasis. Sensory stimuli, e.g., odors, are represented in the mushroom body in terms of sparsely activated ensembles of Kenyon cells. Dopaminergic neurons synapsing onto Kenyon cell axons have been shown to convey the reinforcing properties of salient, unconditioned stimuli. A current concept postulates that coincident activity of both modulates synaptic output from Kenyon cells onto mushroom body output neurons, ultimately influencing the animals' behavior.

Here, the authors report an additional and important finding, i.e., a reciprocal synaptic connection between dopaminergic neurons and Kenyon cells. They conclude from their results that synaptic output from Kenyon cells onto dopaminergic neurons is at least partially cholinergic, that this type of synaptic transmission is required for proper associative learning, and enhances acute, stimulus-evoked as well as continuous activity in dopaminergic neurons. They propose an excitatory feedback loop between dopamine neurons and Kenyon cell that constantly updates the efficiency of dopaminergic transmission onto Kenyon cells. They base their result by experiments using a number of state-of-the-art techniques, i.e., functional reconstitution of GFP, optogenetic activation of Kenyon cells together with functional cAMP and calcium imaging in dopaminergic neurons, pharmacological and genetic (RNAi) experiments to impair cholinergic transmission, and by reducing membrane depolarization in Kenyon cells together with stimulus-evoked calcium imaging in dopaminergic neurons.

Overall, we are enthusiastic about this report, and definitely recommend publication in eLife. It will certainly attract the attention of readers within the large community of scientists working on Drosophila neuroscience. Moreover, it widens our knowledge about fundamental principles underlying the neuronal computation of associative learning in general, which will be of interest for scientists working on vertebrate brains as well. The experimental procedures, the statistical analysis or the presentation of the results, all of which are accurate and sound. The manuscript is overall clearly written, and the illustrations and well understandable. There are some significant comments to be addressed, given below.

Essential revisions:

1) Subsection “Cholinergic input to DAn is required for normal olfactory learning”, first paragraph: the authors state that the functional results with TH-Gal4 and UAS-RNAi transgenes indicate that ACh receptors are expressed on the axons on DAn.

The authors should either show directly that ACh receptors are expressed in the expected sites or provide a reference that shows this directly.

The authors should acknowledge and discuss explicitly the possibility that the learning phenotype observed in Figures 3F-I is influenced by an effect of reduction in ACh receptor function elsewhere, since the TH-Gal4 driver shows a broad expression pattern in Figure 1B.

2) The labeling in Figure 1D is not strong. The authors should show control labeling levels observed when spGFP11 and spGFP1-10 are expressed singly.

Other comments:

1) The terminology is hard for the non-specialist to follow in some places. The use of Greek letters and prime designations in Figure 1 needs to be checked. Also, Figure 1 shows "gamma1" but not "gamma1ped". Where is PAMB2B'2a? The expression patterns of some of the drivers should be described more explicitly in the text (e.g. subsection “MBn are both pre- and post- synaptic to DAn forming axoaxonic reciprocal connections”, first paragraph).

2) Figure 1D legend: "GFP reconstitution occurs only if functional connectivity exists." This may be an overstatement; it would seem that GFP reconstitution could in principle occur if structural but not functional connectivity exists. A reference is needed if this statement is to be included.

3) Subsection “MBn are both pre- and post- synaptic to DAn forming axoaxonic reciprocal connections”, second paragraph: Please clarify why cAMP but not Ca2+ levels are elevated in MBn when DNa is stimulated.

4) It would be helpful to provide some general information about axonal-axonal reciprocal synapses in the Introduction, or at least to cite a reference, possibly a review article.

eLife. 2017 May 10;6:e23789. doi: 10.7554/eLife.23789.014

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) Subsection “Cholinergic input to DAn is required for normal olfactory learning”, first paragraph: the authors state that the functional results with TH-Gal4 and UAS-RNAi transgenes indicate that ACh receptors are expressed on the axons on DAn.

The authors should either show directly that ACh receptors are expressed in the expected sites or provide a reference that shows this directly.

Our conclusion is based on three lines of evidence. First, stimulation of cholingeric KC in the presence of TTX shows that the connectivity from KC to DAn is monosynaptic. This indicates that cholinergic receptors are expressed on the axons of DAn, since this is the only known point of direct contact between these two cell types. Second, focal application of ACh to the axons of DAn provoke strong calcium responses even in the presence of TTX, eliminating the possibility of inadvertent stimulation of other neurons. This experiment also indicates that nAChR are expressed on the axons of DAn. Third, RNAi knockdown of nACh receptor subunits impairs memory formation, using two different DAn drivers. This latter observation does not address the spatial specificity component of our conclusion but indicates that the nACh receptor is expressed in these neurons. Overall, these three lines of evidence together offer extremely strong support for our conclusion.

The reviewer’s request for us to demonstrate the presence of ACh receptors on the axons of the DAn is technically an extremely demanding one, so it is not surprising that no reference for this currently exists. It would require a tagged-ACh receptor fusion protein that one would be confident is trafficked in proper ways in an overexpression situation (Gal4 driven UAS), or high quality antibodies against the transmembrane receptor subunits for use in immunocytochemistry experiments. We did attempt the latter experiments but unfortunately all of the antibodies tested proved ineffective for immunostaining. Nevertheless, we argue that the three lines of evidence listed above offer extremely strong support for our conclusion. In the interest of a bit more caution, we changed our statement from “[…]indicating that ACh receptors are expressed on the axons on DAn” to “[…]suggesting that ACh receptors are expressed on the axons on DAn.”

The authors should acknowledge and discuss explicitly the possibility that the learning phenotype observed in Figures 3F-I is influenced by an effect of reduction in ACh receptor function elsewhere, since the TH-Gal4 driver shows a broad expression pattern in Figure 1B.

To exclude the possibility that the learning phenotype observed with TH-Gal4 is due to expression of iRNA in cells other than DAn we included a new experiment in which we knocked down nAChR alpha4 only in PPL1 α2α’2 neuron with the highly specific MB058B split-gal4.

2) The labeling in Figure 1D is not strong. The authors should show control labeling levels observed when spGFP11 and spGFP1-10 are expressed singly.

We show the level of labeling with spGFP11 and syb::spGFP1-10 when expressed singly in Figure 1—figure supplement 1. No fluorescence was observed by each half of the split GFP molecule when imaged under the same conditions that produced detectable labeling when both were expressed together. The monoclonal antibody used in these experiments can recognize spGFP1-10 and signal is observed if the gain is turned to maximum during image acquisition. In addition, we have found that naïve fluorescence is observed in the brain only when GFP is in the reconstituted state prior to antibody staining (data not shown).

Other comments:

1) The terminology is hard for the non-specialist to follow in some places.

We have tried to simplify terminology.

The use of Greek letters and prime designations in Figure 1 needs to be checked. Also, Figure 1 shows "gamma1" but not "gamma1ped". Where is PAMB2B'2a?

We have checked these. Differences in form of letters and superscripts may be a function of the use of specific word processors and image construction programs. We wanted to show in Figure 1A the 15 discrete compartments into which MB are divided rather than the innervation pattern of the DAn. This is why gamma1 ped is not shown. PAMB2B’2 is clearly observed in Figure 1D and 1E and we also added this in the text.

The expression patterns of some of the drivers should be described more explicitly in the text (e.g. subsection “MBn are both pre- and post- synaptic to DAn forming axoaxonic reciprocal connections”, first paragraph).

We added a statement to indicate that R13F02-gal4 is quite specific to KC. We did describe where the TH-lexA driver is expressed and we also included the reference to the paper that originally described the driver (Berry et al., 2015).

2) Figure 1D legend: "GFP reconstitution occurs only if functional connectivity exists." This may be an overstatement; it would seem that GFP reconstitution could in principle occur if structural but not functional connectivity exists. A reference is needed if this statement is to be included.

We altered this sentence to offering an overstatement.

3) Subsection “MBn are both pre- and post- synaptic to DAn forming axoaxonic reciprocal connections”, second paragraph: Please clarify why cAMP but not Ca2+ levels are elevated in MBn when DNa is stimulated.

It has been reported before (Tomchik et al., 2009) and we observed again here, that bath applying dopamine does not generate calcium transients in KC. Why is this? This is likely due to the fact that dopamine receptors in KC trigger signaling pathways (including cAMP signaling) that do not alter ion channel function. An alternative but less likely explanation is that GCamP3.0 used in this and other studies (Tomchik et al., 2009) lacks the sensitivity to detect such changes in KC.

4) It would be helpful to provide some general information about axonal-axonal reciprocal synapses in the Introduction, or at least to cite a reference, possibly a review article.

We have included in the concluding remarks some general information and speculation about reciprocal connections and recurrent loops, and their roles in information processing and associative learning.


Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

RESOURCES