Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2017 Mar 27;7:426. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-00521-0

Interspecific differences in how habitat degradation affects escape response

Mark I McCormick 1,, Bridie J M Allan 1
PMCID: PMC5428724  PMID: 28348362

Abstract

Degradation of habitats is widespread and a leading cause of extinctions. Our study determined whether the change in the chemical landscape associated with coral degradation affected the way three fish species use olfactory information to optimize their fast-start escape response. Water from degraded coral habitats affected the fast-start response of the three closely-related damselfishes, but its effect differed markedly among species. The Ward’s damselfish (Pomacentrus wardi) was most affected by water from degraded coral, and displayed shorter distances covered in the fast-start and slower escape speeds compared to fish in water from healthy coral. In the presence of alarm odours, which indicate an imminent threat, the Ambon damsel (P. amboinensis) displayed enhanced fast-start performance in water from healthy coral, but not when in water from degraded coral. In contrast, while the white-tailed damsel (P. chrysurus) was similarly primed by its alarm odour, the elevation of fast start performance was not altered by water from degraded coral. These species-specific responses to the chemistry of degraded water and alarm odours suggest differences in the way alarm odours interact with the chemical landscape, and differences in the way species balance information about threats, with likely impacts on the survival of affected species in degraded habitats.

Introduction

Habitat degradation is one of the foremost global causes of extinction13, but while the loss of organisms due to changes in the quality and characteristics of habitat is well documented4, 5, the mechanisms that underlie these changes are often unclear6. As a habitat patch degrades the structural complexity of the patch also changes as dominant habitat-forming organisms are lost. These structural changes can affect the utility of visual information, and changes in surface contours can disrupt the flow of air or water moving through a habitat patch, thereby altering the provision of olfactory or sound information7, 8, the cues that animals use to inform their behavioural choices9, 10.

The partial loss of one community and the colonization by organisms better suited to the new conditions also changes the landscape of chemicals against which the biologically significant odours are interpreted11. Alterations to the quality, type or balance of information sources on which to make decisions can lead to making the wrong choice and increase mortality1214. While these changes are becoming increasing commonplace to all habitats, whether terrestrial or aquatic, they currently manifest most dramatically within one of the world’s most biologically diverse and endangered ecosystems, coral reefs15.

Coral reefs around the world have been impacted by human induced environmental changes including elevated temperatures, increased frequency of severe storms, modified chemistry and elevated turbidity16, 17. These and other changes have seen many coral reefs change from coral dominated to algal dominated landscapes worldwide5, 18, 19. Recent studies of coral reef fishes have highlighted how the change from a hard coral to algal dominated landscape can lead to changes in the availability of information on which decisions are made and hence change the balance of information sources used to inform decisions10, 20, 21. For organisms that live under a constant threat of predation, how individuals judge risk is central to how they balance vigilance against other fitness promoting behaviours such as foraging, courtship and mating.

Juvenile marine fishes typically employ behavioural tactics to maximize growth22, and optimizing when they will respond to a potential predator is critical for maximizing long-term fitness, which is often size-dependent. A fish that waits to the last moment before responding to a predator maximizes its time spent on fitness-promoting activities, but must balance this against a predicted probability of escaping a strike. Until recently it was often assumed that prey escape responses were an all-or-nothing burst of activity that was largely autonomic, with performance maximised by strong predator selection and with little variation within individuals2325. Recent studies have broken up the fast-start escape sequence into parts that are under behavioural modification and those that are more autonomic26, 27. For instance, whether to respond and the time to react may be considered as being under some behavioural control, while acceleration and maximum speed are physiologically determined and may be more automatic26. For fishes, information to forewarn of the activity of predators in the vicinity often comes from the direct receipt of olfactory, visual or mechanical cues. A recent study found that fish can optimize their fast-start escape performance based on information available concerning the levels of threat posed by a predator28. Anything that alters the availability of public information or the perception/detection of the cues, will modify the balance of information used to inform a decision and may alter the ability of a prey to escape a predator strike.

This finding takes on even greater significance when paired with recent evidence showing that chemicals from degraded coral habitats negate the innate response of some fishes to damage-released odours from conspecifics29. Fishes of the same, closely-related or ecologically similar species elicit an innate antipredator response to these alarm odours because they represent a reliable indicator of an active predator in the vicinity30, 31. Research to date has shown that the ability to use alarm odours in degraded habitats is species specific, with one species no longer able to use alarm odours to inform risk (Ambon damselfish, Pomacentrus amboinensis), while another species (the neon damsel, P. coelestis) was not adversely affected29.

The present study aimed to determine whether a change in the chemical landscape associated with coral degradation affected the way three closely related fish species used olfactory information to optimize their fast-start response when startled. Specifically, we conducted a laboratory experiment that crossed fish species, which have different habitat associations (P. amboinensis, P. chrysurus, P. wardi), with background odour (live coral or degraded-dead coral habitats) and threat-forewarning odour (chemical alarm odour or seawater) in a 3 × 2 × 2 design. Fish within each treatment combination were exposed to a mechanized drop stimulus to elicit a startle response. Given our previous finding of a species-specific response to alarm odour in degraded habitats29, we predicted that water that had passed over degraded habitat may modify the alarm odour to nullify the forewarning effect in some species, while others may not be affected.

Methods

Ethics statement

Research was carried out under approval of the James Cook University animal ethics committee (permit: A2005, A2080) and according to the University’s animal ethics guidelines.

Study species

Three species of damselfish (Pomacentridae) were used, representing a range of habitat preferences. The Ambon damselfish, Pomacentrus amboinensis is a generalist found associated with variety of substrata from live coral through to rubble as a juvenile32, 33. The white-tail damselfish, Pomacentrus chrysurus, is a dead coral-rubble specialist34. The Ward’s damselfish, Pomacentrus wardi, is a habitat generalist with a particular affinity for soft coral35. The Ambon damselfish is an omnivore feeding on plankton and benthic algae33, while the other two species principally feed on benthic algae as adults34, 35. All fishes appear to have similar pelagic larval durations36.

All fishes were collected as newly metamorphosed juveniles at night using light traps37 moored more than 30 m from a reef edge in 10–16 m of water column (trap entrances ~1 m depth) around Lizard Island on the northern Great Barrier Reef (14°40′S, 145°27′E), Australia in October 2015. Fishes were taken from the traps and transported in 60 L tanks to the research station, where they were sorted to species. At testing fish were of similar sizes (standard length): Ambon damsel (range 10.9–14.4 mm, mean 12.5 ± 1.5 mm SE); white-tailed damsel (range 12.1–15.3 mm, mean 13.9 ± 1.2 mm); Ward’s damsel (range 11.9–15.1 mm, mean 13.2 ± 1.3 mm).

Conditioning treatment

Prior to fast-start trials commencing, fishes were randomly allocated to 8 tanks (4 tanks per seawater source) supplied by one of two aerated seawater sources for 48 to 72 h: seawater that had flowed through 35 L header tanks filled with an ~50 cm perimeter piece of live coral (Pocillopora damicornis), or seawater that had flowed over dead-degraded coral (dead hard coral and rubble with a similar topography to the live coral but which was covered in algae and some sessile invertebrates; see Fig. 1 McCormick & Lönnstedt29 for habitat image). Flow rates were ~1 L per min. Fishes were fed Artemia nauplii ad libitum and had access to ample shelter during the habitat conditioning period.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Comparison of the mean (±SE) fast-start performance of three damselfish species, the Ambon damsel (Pomacentrus amboinensis; ac), the whitetail damselfish (P. chrysurus; df) and the Ward’s damselfish (P. wardi; gi) that had been kept in either water that had passed over live coral or degraded-dead coral, and then exposed to either clean seawater (SW) or conspecific alarm odours (AO) prior to a repeatable startle stimulus. Fast-start variables shown are: response distance (a,d,g), response speed (b,e,h) and maximum speed (c,f,i). N = 17–23. Letters above the bars in panels a–c represent Tukey’s means comparison groupings (only tests on significant interactions between factors are shown, p < 0.05).

Fast-start arena and protocol

Following conditioning, fishes were transferred individually into a fast-start arena containing water from the appropriate source (i.e., seawater that had passed over live coral or degraded-dead coral) and allowed to acclimate for 5 min. A chemical odour was then introduced into the fast-start arena, and fish were given another 5 min prior to release of the startle stimulus. Fish were only tested once and were tested in water in accordance with the water source they had been conditioned within.

The testing arena consisted of a transparent circular acrylic arena (diameter 200 mm; height 70 mm), within a large opaque-sided plastic tank (585 × 420 × 330 mm; 60 L) with a transparent Perspex bottom to allow responses to be filmed from below as a silhouette (Fig. S1). The water level was low (60 mm) to reduce movements in the vertical plane. Fishes were not fed overnight (~16 h) and after the acclimation period, one of two treatments were applied to the arena: damage-released odours (alarm odours) from conspecifics, or a seawater control (injection control). Fifteen mL of either odour were slowly injected into the inner arena through a piece of clear tubing.

The fish were exposed to either odour for a period of 5 min before a fast-start response was elicited by the release of a weight with a tapered end into the testing arena. Fish were only startled when they moved to the middle portion of the tank, allowing an individual to move an equal distance in any direction and standardising for fish position relative to the stimulus. The weight was released from an electromagnet and was governed by a piece of fishing line that was long enough such that the tapered tip only just touched the surface of the water. To avoid a premature escape response associated with visual stimulation occurring, the weight was released from above into a 550 mm piece of 40 mm diameter PVC pipe with the bottom edge at a distance of 10 mm above the water level. The arena was filled with water from the appropriate source for the trial (i.e., seawater that had passed over live coral or degraded coral) and after each trial the arena was emptied and rinsed to avoid a build-up of alarm odours.

Prey escape variables were only measured when prey performed a C-start (commencement of fast-start that results in the individual forming a C-shape). Escape responses were recorded at 480 frames per second (Casio EX-ZR1000) as a silhouette from below obtained through pointing the camera through a small hole in a black plastic sheet, to avoid visual interference to the fish, at a mirror angled at 45° below the arena. A 1 cm line was drawn in the centre of the inner arena to enable calibration during video analysis.

Kinematic variables

Kinematic variables associated with the fast-start response were analysed using the image-analysis software Image-J, with a manual tracking plug-in. For analysis of the high-speed movements each fish was reduced to a single moving point. The point where each fish was tracked was standardised by following the same point on each fish (i.e., the position directly behind the eyes which corresponds to the widest part of the body). We chose to standardise tracking based on this point of the body as it the most stable and easiest to track owing to the small size of the juveniles. The following kinematic variables were measured:

  1. Response latency (s) was measured as the time interval between the stimulus touching the water surface and the first detectable movement of the fish.

  2. Response distance (m) is a measure of the total distance covered by the fish during the first two flips of the tail (the first two axial bends, i.e. stages 1 and 2 defined based on Domenici and Blake38, which is the period considered crucial for avoiding ambush predator attacks39.

  3. Response speed (m/s) was measured as the distance covered within a fixed time (24 ms). This fixed duration was based on the average duration (22.8 ms) of stage 1 and 2 (as defined above).

  4. Maximum response speed (m/s) was measured as the maximum speed achieved at any time during stage 1 and stage 2.

Production of alarm odours

Conspecific alarm odours were used fresh and obtained by making six vertical cuts on each ssof four freshly euthanized (using cold shock) conspecific fish and then rinsing the fish in 60 mL of seawater (of the appropriate source). We injected 15 mL of this alarm odour solution into the fast start arena, which gave a standardised concentration of 2 cuts/L once injected. This concentration has been shown to elicit strong antipredator responses in coral reef fishes40.

Statistical analyses

To determine whether water source (Habitat odour: live coral or dead-degraded coral water) or threat odour (Threat odour: SW or Alarm odour) affected the kinematics of the fast-start response two-factor MANOVA’s were undertaken incorporating all four kinematic variables for each of the three fish species. ANOVA’s were then used to determine the nature of significant differences found by MANOVA. When necessary, Tukey’s HSD post-hoc means comparisons were undertaken. Data were examined for the assumptions of homogeneity of variance and normality using residual analysis. Latency was non-normal and was normalized using a log10(x) transformation. Sample sizes for the four treatment combinations were between 17 and 23 independent fish.

Results

Seawater from the two sources (from live or dead-degraded coral) and the addition of two threat odours (SW or Alarm odour) influenced the way the three damselfish species reacted to a startle stimulus in different ways (Fig. 1, Sup Fig. S2, Table 1). Within each of the species all kinematic variables displayed the same patterns of significance when examined using ANOVA (Fig. 1, Table 2), with the exception of latency to respond, which was not affected by either factor (p > 0.05; Fig. S2).

Table 1.

Comparison of the fast-start response kinematics of three juvenile damselfishes that have been kept to seawater that has passed over live-healthy coral or dead-degraded coral (Habitat), and then either exposed to either clean seawater or conspecific alarm odours (Cues) within the burst arena just prior to being startled.

Source P. amboinensis P. chrysurus P. wardi
F4,64 p F4,68 p F4,73 p
Habitat 3.35 0.015 0.46 0.768 14.80 <0.001
Cue 2.19 0.080 5.52 <0.001 1.25 0.300
Habitat*Cue 2.59 0.045 1.01 0.411 1.62 0.180

Damselfish belong to the genus Pomacentrus. Results shown are the results of MANOVA’s on four kinematic variables (see text). The test statistic is Pillai’s Trace.

Table 2.

Comparison of the fast-start response kinematics of three juvenile damselfishes that have been kept in seawater that has passed over live-healthy coral or dead-degraded coral (Habitat), and then either exposed to either clean seawater or conspecific alarm odours (Cues) within the burst arena just prior to being startled.

Species (df) Variable Habitat Cue Habitat*Cue
F p F p F p
P. amboinensis Distance 13.6 <0.001 4.5 0.01 1.8 0.18
1, 75 Speed 14.8 <0.001 6.4 0.01 2.2 0.14
Max speed 16.7 <0.001 3.2 0.08 6.5 0.01
P. chrysurus Distance 0.2 0.66 12.8 <0.001 0.04 0.85
1, 74 Speed 0.4 0.51 11.1 0.001 0.2 0.68
Max speed 1.3 0.27 18.4 <0.001 2.3 0.14
P. wardi Distance 44.5 <0.001 0.8 0.38 0.2 0.68
1, 77 Speed 27.5 <0.001 0.02 0.89 0.01 0.92
Max speed 30.3 <0.001 0.004 0.95 0.4 0.55

Damselfish belong to the genus Pomacentrus. Results show the outcome of two-factor ANOVA’s on three kinematic variables: response distance (m), speed (m/s) and maximum speed (m/s). Bold values are significant at 0.05.

When the Ambon damselfish was startled in the presence of an alarm odour it showed a marked increase in fast-start performance (Fig. 1a–c), but only in the presence of the alarm odour in water from live coral (MANOVA interaction: F4,64, p = 0.045, Table 1). The significant interactions in the multivariate analyses were supported by the Tukey’s comparisons for response distance, speed and maximum speed (Fig. 1a–c). There was no effect of alarm odour when fish had been living in water that had passed over degraded coral.

The whitetail damselfish was affected by the odour added to the water, with higher performance observed when a alarm odour was added to the arena (MANOVA Cue: F4,68 = 5.52, p < 0.001; Fig. 1d–f, Table 1). There was no effect of water source on their fast-start response, with a similar magnitude of increase in performance regardless of water source with the addition of the alarm odour in response distance, speed and maximum speed (Fig. 1d–f; Table 2).

In contrast, the Ward’s damselfish was not affected by which cue was added to the water (Fig. 1g–i), but water source had a marked effect (MANOVA Habitat: F4,73 = 14.80, p < 0.001, Table 1). Fish displayed higher performance in water that had been in contact with live coral compared to degraded coral and this was shown by response distance, speed and maximum speed (Fig. 1g–i, Table 2).

Discussion

While recent studies have shown that water that has passed over dead-degraded coral habitat reduces the efficacy of alarm odours in at least one species of coral reef fish29, 41, this is the first study to demonstrate that it alters the performance of the fast-start response, a behavior that is crucial for escape from predators. Water from degraded coral habitats affected the fast-start performance of three closely-related damselfishes, but its effect differed markedly among species. The Ward’s damselfish was most affected by water from degraded coral, and displayed shorter distances covered during the fast-start and slower escape speeds compared to fish in water from healthy coral. When the Ambon damselfish was exposed to chemical alarm odours, escape performance was enhanced in keeping with predictions, but only when in a healthy coral water source. Water that had passed over degraded coral canceled the apparent priming effect of the alarm odour. In comparison, while the white-tailed damselfish was similarly primed by its alarm odour, the elevation of fast start performance was not negated by water from degraded coral. These species-specific responses to the chemistry of degraded water and to chemical alarm odours are surprising and suggest differences in the way alarm odours interact with the chemical landscape, and differences in the way species balance information about threats.

Surprisingly, the fast-start performance of the Ward’s damselfish was reduced by ~25% when exposed to water that had passed over degraded coral, while the seawater source alone had no effect on the other two congenerics. Changes to the fast-start response of less than this magnitude have been observed in response to elevated CO2 and temperature and been shown to affect survival (e.g., refs 42 and 43). Why the Ward’s damselfish are so affected by water from degraded coral is unclear. Latency to react was not affected, suggesting that the water did not alter the triggering of the Mauthner neurons that initiate the rapid C-start escape response23. Clearly the three species have differential sensitivity to chemicals that are active within the water from degraded corals and at least one component of this chemical cocktail appears to affect the Ward’s damselfish.

The provision of chemical alarm odours in the arena prior to the startle stimulus increased the efficacy of the fast-start response in the Ambon and white-tailed damselfish. This forewarning effect may occur because the chemical alarm odours represent a reliable indicator of a threat nearby leading to a priming of the escape response. In general, exposure to a stressor increases circulating catecholamines, such as norepinephrine and epinephrine (in seconds to minutes; ref. 44) and elevates levels of blood cortisol (in minutes to hours). While catecholamines, such as norepinephrine are clear candidates for this priming action given their rapid mobilization and known role in alertness, arousal, and readiness for action45, there is currently no data to support their role in fast-start priming46, 47. However, studies of freshwater fishes have shown that being exposed to alarm odours triggers a stress response, leading to a relatively rapid increase in cortisol4649, though this is not always the case50. Cortisol stimulates gluconeogenesis for the rapid mobilization of glucose into the blood stream. Elevated cortisol primes the body for a rapid response and also allows the body to recover rapidly from the fast start response, which is anerobically fueled and energetically costly51. Barreto et al.52 found that intraperitoneal cortisol implants into the frillfin goby increased its antipredator response to chemical alarm odours, suggesting that cortisol took on a priming function. Moreover, Lastein et al.53 found that treatment of crucian carp with corticotrophin-releasing factor antagonist before exposure to conspecific skin extracts suppressed the alarm reaction, supporting the hypothesis that cortisol plays a role in the antipredator response displayed to alarm odours. Since cortisol has been shown to heighten neurological activity and speed decisions on simple or well-rehearsed tasks5456, moderate levels of cortisol should enhance vigilance52 and the speed of the fast-start response. What is puzzling is that research has previously shown that the Ward’s damselfish displays a typical antipredator response to alarm odours (e.g., ref. 57), yet this alarm odour does not lead to a forewarning effect on the escape response. Obviously further research is necessary to determine the species-specific nature of threat-forewarning for fast-start performance and the physiology that underlies the response.

While the forewarning effect of chemical alarm odours on the performance of fast-starts was seen in two damselfish species, water that had passed over degraded corals prevented the forewarning effect of alarm odours only in one of the species (the Ambon damselfish). In a previous experiment, Lönnstedt et al.20 rinsed a lacerated alarm odour donor with water from dead-degraded coral and found that odour no longer elicited an antipredator response in the Ambon damselfish, suggesting that the water somehow modified the active components of the alarm odour in this species. However, at least one other damselfish species (the neon damsel, P. coelestis) has alarm cues that are not affected29 by water from dead-degraded coral. It is currently unknown whether the alarm odours of the Ward’s damselfish still elicit an antipredator response in waters from degraded coral, but the evidence in the current study suggests that it is likely that the alarm odours from the white-tailed damsel are still active in water from degraded coral.

The mechanism by which perception of the alarm odour is disrupted for the Ambon damselfish is poorly understood. Very little is known of the molecular composition of alarm odours. Recently Mathuru et al.58 found that while zebrafish (Danio rerio) respond to a previously suggested alarm odour candidate H3NO59 with a low intensity antipredator response, a detailed chemical analysis of the alarm odour also found that the components that caused the greatest antipredator response comprised of a mixture of glycosaminoglycans with different molecular weights. Clearly, if this finding is general, then it suggests that the chemicals associated with alarm odours are complex mixtures of compounds that enable them to be species-specific and carry a large amount of information concerning species identity and other information, such as body size and condition (e.g., ref. 60). This complexity argues against a simple olfactory perception model of a physicochemically driven, hard-wired process with specific receptors for alarm odour molecules as suggested by Wilson and Stevenson61. Behavioural studies have shown that some fish species will respond with an antipredator response to alarm odours from closely related species, with the intensity of response correlated to their phylogenetic relatedness62. This suggests that there is some commonality to the active parts of the alarm odour molecule, but there is sufficient complexity for species-specific identification. Moreover, recent data shows that the Ambon damselfish will respond to the alarm odours of a congeneric P. nagasakiensis in water from dead-degraded coral63, suggesting that the olfactory receptor sheet is probably not altered by the chemicals from degraded corals. Clearly, the alarm odours of some species, like the Ambon damsel, are adversely affected by degraded water while others have found ways of avoiding this problem. Determining the active ingredients within the degraded reef chemistry and intraspecific variability in the response of sensitive species will be key to determining the extent to which sensitive species may be able to adjust to the prevalence of degraded-dead coral that may surround remnant patches of live coral.

Alarm odours are central to an important cognitive mechanism for learning threats31, which is particularly important for aquatic organisms as they transition between habitats and encounter novel predators64. Our study has shown that they are also important in that, at least for some species (e.g., Ambon and white-tail damselfish), they play an important role in forewarning species of nearby threats thereby allowing individuals to optimize their fast-start escape response. Interestingly, simply the passage of water past a degraded-dead coral habitat was sufficient to alter the alarm odour of the Ambon damselfish so it no-longer fulfilled a forewarning function, and it also reduced the efficacy of the fast-start response in the Ward’s damsel. This highlights the important role that the olfactory landscape of the environment plays in mediating behavioural responses that may have fitness consequences, such as effective predator avoidance. Determining how species are affected by alterations in the olfactory landscape as coral reefs change from hard coral to algal dominated environments will be important to understanding the dynamics of change in the fish community and what characteristics promote resilience of a species to this dramatic change.

Data availability

Data is available from: doi:10.4225/28/589a9cbd422bb.

Electronic supplementary material

41598_2017_521_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (148.1KB, pdf)

Figures showing fast-start apparatus and results of effect of treatments on latency for three damselfishes

Acknowledgements

This research was funded through the ARC Centre of Excellence for Coral Reef Studies.

Author Contributions

M.I.M. and B.M.J.A. contributed to the initial idea and sampling design. B.J.M.A. undertook the fast-start assays and analysed the videos. M.I.M. analysed the data and wrote the first draft of the paper. B.J.M.A. contributed substantially to subsequent drafts.

Competing Interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at doi:10.1038/s41598-017-00521-0

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Lambin EF, Geist HJ, Lepers E. Dynamics of land-use and land-cover change in tropical regions. Annual Review of Environment and Resources. 2003;28:205–241. doi: 10.1146/annurev.energy.28.050302.105459. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lotze HK, et al. Depletion, degradation, and recovery potential of estuaries and coastal seas. Science. 2006;312:1806–1809. doi: 10.1126/science.1128035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Spalding MD, Brown BE. Warm-water coral reefs and climate change. Science. 2015;350:769–771. doi: 10.1126/science.aad0349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Williams SE, Bolitho EE, Fox S. Climate change in Australian tropical rainforests: an impending environmental catastrophe. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 2003;270:1887–1892. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Jones GP, McCormick MI, Srinivasan M, Eagle JV. Coral decline threatens fish biodiversity in marine reserves. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA. 2004;101:8251–8253. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0401277101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hoey A, et al. Recent advances in understanding the effects of climate change on coral reefs. Diversity. 2016;8:12. doi: 10.3390/d8020012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Weissburg MJ, Zimmerfaust RK. Life and death in moving fluids - hydrodynamic effects on chemosensory-mediated predation. Ecology. 1993;74:1428–1443. doi: 10.2307/1940072. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Nedelec SL, et al. Soundscapes and living communities in coral reefs: temporal and spatial variation. Marine Ecology Progress Series. 2015;524:125–135. doi: 10.3354/meps11175. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Creel S, Christianson D. Relationships between direct predation and risk effects. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2007;23:194–201. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2007.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.McCormick MI, Lönnstedt OM. Degrading habitats and the effect of topographic complexity on risk assessment. Ecology and Evolution. 2013;3:4221–4229. doi: 10.1002/ece3.793. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Dixson DL, Abrego D, Hay ME. Chemically mediated behavior of recruiting corals and fishes: A tipping point that may limit reef recovery. Science. 2014;345:892. doi: 10.1126/science.1255057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar] [Retracted]
  • 12.Robertson BA, Rehage JS, Sih A. Ecological novelty and the emergence of evolutionary traps. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2013;28:552–560. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2013.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Wong BB, Candolin U. Behavioral responses to changing environments. Behavioral Ecology. 2015;26:665–673. doi: 10.1093/beheco/aru183. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hale, R. & Swearer, S. E. Ecological traps: current evidence and future directions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences283, doi: 10.1098/rspb.2015.2647 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 15.Halpern BS, et al. A global map of human impact on marine ecosystems. Science. 2008;319:948–952. doi: 10.1126/science.1149345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hoegh-Guldberg O, Bruno JF. The impact of climate change on the world’s marine ecosystems. Science. 2010;328:1523–1528. doi: 10.1126/science.1189930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.De’ath G, Fabricius KE, Sweatman H, Puotinen M. The 27-year decline of coral cover on the Great Barrier Reef and its causes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA. 2012;109:17995–17999. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1208909109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hughes TP. Catastrophes, phase shifts, and large-scale degradation of a Caribbean coral reef. Science. 1994;265:1547–1551. doi: 10.1126/science.265.5178.1547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Depczynski M, et al. Bleaching, coral mortality and subsequent survivorship on a West Australian fringing reef. Coral Reefs. 2013;32:233–238. doi: 10.1007/s00338-012-0974-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lönnstedt OM, McCormick MI, Chivers DP. Degraded environments alter prey risk assessment. Ecology and Evolution. 2013;3:38–47. doi: 10.1002/ece3.388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lönnstedt OM, McCormick MI, Chivers DP, Ferrari MCO. Habitat degradation is threatening reef replenishment by making fish fearless. Journal of Animal Ecology. 2014;83:1178–1185. doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Choat, J. H. & Robertson, D. R. Age-based studies. In Coral Reef Fishes (ed. Sale, P. F.) 57-80 (Academic Press, 2002).
  • 23.Eaton, R. C. & Hackett, J. T. In Neural mechanisms of startle behavior (ed. Eaton, R. C.) 213–266 (Plenum Press, 1984).
  • 24.Langerhans RB, Layman CA, Shokrollahi AM, DeWitt T. Predator-driven phenotypic diversification in Gambusia affinis. Evolution. 2004;58:2305–2318. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2004.tb01605.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Weiss SA, Zottoli SJ, Do SC, Faber DS, Preuss T. Correlation of C-start behaviors with neural activity recorded from the hindbrain in free-swimming goldfish (Carassius auratus) Journal of Experimental Biology. 2006;209:4788–4801. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Marras S, Killen SS, Claireaux G, Domenici P, McKenzie DJ. Behavioural and kinematic components of the fast-start escape response in fish: individual variation and temporal repeatability. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2011;214:3102–3110. doi: 10.1242/jeb.056648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Allan BJM, Miller GM, McCormick MI, Domenici P, Munday PL. Parental effects improve escape performance of juvenile reef fish in a high CO2 world. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 2014;281:1777. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.2179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ramasamy, R. A., Allan, B. & McCormick, M. I. Plasticity of escape responses: prior predator experience enhances escape performance in a coral reef fish. PLoS One10(8), e0132790 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 29.McCormick MI, Lönnstedt OM. Disrupted learning: habitat degradation impairs crucial antipredator responses in naïve prey. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 2016;283:20160441. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2016.0441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ferrari MCO, Wisenden BD, Chivers DP. Chemical ecology of predator-prey interactions in aquatic ecosystems: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 2010;88:698–724. doi: 10.1139/Z10-029. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Chivers DP, Smith RJF. Chemical alarm signalling in aquatic predator-prey systems: a review and prospectus. Ecoscience. 1998;5:338–352. doi: 10.1080/11956860.1998.11682471. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.McCormick MI, Moore JAY, Munday PL. Influence of habitat degradation on fish replenishment. Coral Reefs. 2010;29:537–546. doi: 10.1007/s00338-010-0620-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.McCormick MI, Weaver C. It pays to be pushy: intracohort interference competition between two reef fishes. PLoS One. 2012;7:e42590. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Low RM. Interspecific territoriality in a Pomacentrid reef fish, Pomacentrus-flavicauda Whitley. Ecology. 1971;52:648–654. doi: 10.2307/1934153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ceccarelli DM, Emslie MJ, Lewis AR. Farming versatility by Pomacentrus wardi. Marine and Freshwater Research. 2013;64:558–561. doi: 10.1071/MF12343. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Brothers EB, Williams DM, Sale PF. Length of larval life in twelve families of fishes at “One Tree Lagoon”, Great Barrier Reef. Marine Biology. 1983;76:319–324. doi: 10.1007/BF00393035. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Meekan MG, Wilson SG, Halford A, Retzel A. A comparison of catches of fishes and invertebrates by two light trap designs, in tropical NW Australia. Marine Biology. 2001;139:373–381. doi: 10.1007/s002270100577. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Domenici P, Blake RW. The kinematics and performance of fish fast-start swimming. Journal of Experimental Biology. 1997;200:1165–1178. doi: 10.1242/jeb.200.8.1165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Webb PW. The effect of size on the fast-start performance of rainbow trout Salmo cairdneri, and a consideration of piscivorous predator-prey interactions. Journal of Experimental Biology. 1976;65:157–177. doi: 10.1242/jeb.65.1.157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ferrari MCO, et al. Living in a risky world: the onset and ontogeny of an integrated antipredator phenotype in a coral reef fish. Scientific Reports. 2015;5:15537. doi: 10.1038/srep15537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.McCormick, M. I., Chivers, D. P., Allan, B. J. M. & Ferrari, M. C. O. Habitat degradation disrupts neophobia in juvenile coral reef fish. Global Change Biology doi: 10.1111/gcb.13393 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 42.Allan BJM, Domenici P, McCormick MI, Munday PL. Elevated CO2 affects predator-prey interactions through altered performance. PLoS One. 2013;8:e58520. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Allan, B. J. M., Domenici, P., Watson, S., Munday, P. L. & McCormick, M. I. Differential effects of CO2 and warming on predator-prey interactions in coral reef fish. Scientific Reports (in press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 44.Barton BA, Bollig H, Hauskins BL, Jansen CR. Juvenile pallid (Scaphirhynchus albus) and hybrid pallid × shovelnose (S. albus × platorynchus) sturgeons exhibit low physiological responses to acute handling and severe confinement. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A. 2000;126:125–134. doi: 10.1016/S1095-6433(00)00192-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sara SJ, Bouret S. Orienting and reorienting: the locus coeruleus mediates cognition through arousal. Neuron. 2012;76:130–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Rehnberg BG, Schreck CB. Chemosensory detection of predators by coho salmon (Oncorhynchus kisutch): behavioural reaction and the physiological stress response. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 1987;65(3):481–485. doi: 10.1139/z87-074. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hare AJ, Waheed A, Hare JF, Anderson WG. Cortisol and catecholamine responses to social context and a chemical alarm signal in juvenile lake sturgeon. Acipenser fulvescens. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 2015;93(8):605–613. doi: 10.1139/cjz-2015-0045. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Soares MC, Bshary R, Cardoso SC, Cote IM, Oliveira RF. Face your fears: cleaning gobies inspect predators despite being stressed by them. PLoS One. 2012;7:e39781. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Sanches FHC, Miyai CA, Pinho-Neto CF, Barreto RE. Stress responses to chemical alarm cues in Nile tilapia. Physiology & Behavior. 2015;149:8–13. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Barcellos LJG, Ritter F, Kreutz LC, Cericato L. Can zebrafish Danio rerio learn about predation risk? The effect of a previous experience on the cortisol response in subsequent encounters with a predator. Journal of Fish Biology. 2010;76:1032–1038. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2010.02542.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Moyes, C. D., Schulte, P. M. & Andwest, T. G. Burst exercise recovery metabolism in fish white muscle. In Surviving hypoxia: mechanisms of control and adaptation (ed. Hochachka, P. W.) pp. 527–539, Florida, CRC Press, Inc (1993).
  • 52.Barreto RE, Barbosa-Júnior A, Urbinati EC, Hoffman A. Cortisol influences the antipredator behavior induced by chemical alarm cues in the Frillfin goby. Hormones and Behavior. 2014;65:394–400. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lastein S, Hamdani EH, Doving KB. Single unit responses to skin odorants from conspecifics and heterospecifics in the olfactory bulb of crucian carp Carassius carassius. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2008;211:3529–3535. doi: 10.1242/jeb.018739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Mendl M. Performing under pressure: stress and cognitive function. Applied Animal Behavioural Sciences. 1999;65:221–244. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00088-X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Akinola M, Mendes WB. Stress-induced cortisol facilitates threat-related decision making among police officers. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2012;126:167–174. doi: 10.1037/a0026657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Sandi C. Stress and cognition. WIREs Cognitive Science. 2013;4:245–261. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Manassa RP, McCormick MI. Social learning improves survivorship at a life history transition. Oecologia. 2013;171(4):845–852. doi: 10.1007/s00442-012-2458-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Mathuru AjayS, et al. Chondroitin fragments are odorants that trigger fear behavior in fish. Current Biology. 2012;22:538–544. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Brown GE, Adrian JC, Jr, Smyth E, Leet H, Brennan S. Ostariophysan alarm pheromones: laboratory and field tests of the functional significance of nitrogen oxides. Journal of Chemical Ecology. 2000;26(1):139–154. doi: 10.1023/A:1005445629144. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Lönnstedt OM, McCormick MI, Chivers DP. Well-informed prey stealing: damage released chemical cues of injured prey signal quality and size to predators. Oecologia. 2012;168:651–658. doi: 10.1007/s00442-011-2116-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Wilson, D. A. & Stevenson, R. J. Learning to smell: olfactory perception from neurobiology to behavior (Baltimore, USA, J.H.U. Press, 2006).
  • 62.Mitchell MD, McCormick MI, Ferrari MCO, Chivers DP. Generalization of learned predator recognition in coral reef ecosystems: how cautious are damselfish? Functional Ecology. 2013;27:299–304. doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.12043. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Ferrari, M. C. O., McCormick, M. I., Allan, B. J. M. & Chivers, D. P. Not equal in the face of habitat change: closely related fishes differ in their ability to use predation-related information in degraded coral. Proceedings of the Royal Society B (submitted). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 64.Lönnstedt OM, McCormick MI, Meekan MG, Ferrari MCO, Chivers DP. Learn and live: the role of predator experience in influencing prey behaviour and survival. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2012;279:2091–2098. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.2516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

41598_2017_521_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (148.1KB, pdf)

Figures showing fast-start apparatus and results of effect of treatments on latency for three damselfishes

Data Availability Statement

Data is available from: doi:10.4225/28/589a9cbd422bb.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES