Skip to main content
HAL-INSERM logoLink to HAL-INSERM
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 May 26.
Published in final edited form as: Occup Environ Med. 2016 May 17;73(10):640–647. doi: 10.1136/oemed-2015-103352

Domains of cognitive function in early old age: which ones are predicted by pre-retirement psychosocial work characteristics?

Erika Sabbath 1,*, Ross Andel 2, Marie Zins 3, Marcel Goldberg 3, Claudine Berr 4
PMCID: PMC5429340  PMID: 27188277

Abstract

Background

Psychosocial work characteristics may predict cognitive functioning after retirement. However, little research has explored specific cognitive domains associated with psychosocial work environments. Our study tested whether exposure to job demands, job control, and their combination during working life predicted post-retirement performance on eight cognitive tests.

Methods

We used data from French GAZEL cohort members who had undergone post-retirement cognitive testing (n=2,149). Psychosocial job characteristics were measured on average four years before retirement using Karasek’s Job Content Questionnaire (job demands, job control, demand-control combinations). We tested associations between these exposures and post-retirement performance on tests of executive function, visual-motor speed, psycho-motor speed, verbal memory, and verbal fluency using OLS regression.

Results

Low job control during working life was negatively associated with executive function, psychomotor speed, phonemic fluency, and semantic fluency after retirement (p’s<.05) even after adjustment for demographics, socioeconomic status, health and social behaviours, and vascular risk factors. Both passive (low-demand, low-control) and high-strain (high-demand, low-control) jobs were associated with lower scores on phonemic and semantic fluency when compared to low-strain (low-demand, high-control) jobs.

Conclusions

Low job control, in combination with both high and low job demands, is associated with post-retirement deficits in some, but not all, cognitive domains. In addition to work stress, associations between passive work and subsequent cognitive function may implicate lack of cognitive engagement at work as a risk factor for future cognitive difficulties.

Introduction

Given lack of effective treatments for cognitive impairment at older ages, much research has explored modifiable determinants of later-life cognitive function. Psychosocial workplace conditions represent one such risk factor because of their ubiquity and their association with cardiovascular disease (CVD), which shares vascular risk factors with cognitive impairment.1

One prominent measure of these conditions is the Karasek demand-control model, in which combinations of psychological demands and control exert influences on health.2 Although CVD is the most-studied outcome of job demands and control,3 the construct is associated with several long-term health conditions, including depression, fatigue, and functional health.4,5 The demand-control combination may also be a risk factor for long-term cognitive impairment. High-demand, low-control (known as high-strain) work has been associated with cognitive deficits in some, but not all, studies; overall, low control shows more consistent association with cognitive impairment than does high demands.6 In addition to job strain, other psychosocial work conditions associated with later-life cognitive outcomes include occupational complexity (with people, data, and things) that rewards cognitive effort and decision-making,7 work organizational factors such as working hours,8 and environmental enrichment.9

Most studies of demand-control combinations and health have focused on high-strain jobs. But from a neuropsychological perspective, passive (low-demand, low-control) jobs may be risk factors for future cognitive impairment as well; the latter portion of the demand-control matrix is not well-studied in relation to cognitive function in later life. The plausibility of this association is suggested by the occupational complexity literature, which points to evidence that cognitive stimulation at work promotes intellectual flexibility and stability.10 Cognitive reserve theories, which suggest that aspects of life experience such as education protect individuals from experiencing symptoms of early cognitive decline, also implicate lack of cognitive engagement in cognitive impairment.11,12

While much research has focused on predictors of clinical dementia or global cognitive function13, few studies have tested associations between such exposures and more than one domain of cognition. A more nuanced understanding of domains that are (and are not) associated with prior exposure to certain working conditions would help explain mechanisms linking occupational conditions and cognitive health in later life. Such findings would point to elements of work environments that could be enhanced or modified to preserve cognitive function. Although a few studies of the demand-control model have examined multiple domains of cognitive function,14,15 association with key domains such as processing speed have not been tested.

The present study aims to test the separate and combined associations between job demands and job control with multiple domains of cognitive function after retirement. We hypothesized that exposure to high-demand, low-control (high-strain) jobs, indicating high work stress, as well as low-demand, low-control (passive) jobs, indicating lack of engagement at work, would be associated with worse performance in domains of verbal fluency, visual-motor speed, and executive function at older ages. These hypotheses are based on evidence to date of the long-term impact of both cognitive disengagement and stress on cognitive function in occupational and non-occupational settings and on prior studies of domains of cognitive function associated with psychosocial occupational conditions.6,14,15

Methods

Sample

The study was conducted in GAZEL, a cohort of 20,625 French workers now retired from the national gas and electricity company. The cohort was assembled in 1989 and has been followed since, drawing data from annual self-report questionnaires, company administrative records, and occasional in-person health examinations. Detailed information regarding this cohort is available elsewhere.16

We use data from GAZEL participants undergoing cognitive examinations in 2010. Of the 7,890 who lived in the testing centers’ catchment areas and thus were eligible for this study, 3,828 initially responded to the invitation to participate but only 2,962 of those older than 55 years (the age cutoff for inclusion) were ultimately tested. We eliminated 43 people not yet retired at testing. Of the 2,919 who were eligible and were tested, 2,149 (74%) had exposure data and were included in analyses. Please see Supplemental Figure A for a flowchart of inclusion.

Outcome: Cognitive function

Our outcome of interest was multiple dimensions of post-retirement cognitive function. Neuropsychologists administered a battery of tests to participants. The Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) subscale of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale measures general cognitive function and the score was the number of items completed correctly.17 The Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) measures general cognitive function and orientation and the score is the number of questions answered correctly.18 The Free and Cued Selective Reminding Tests (FCSRT)-Immediate and – Delayed measure verbal memory and the score is the number of items on a list that were correctly recalled.19. Phonemic fluency was assessed as the number of words starting with “F” generated in one minute, and semantic fluency was assessed as the number of animal types generated in one minute.20 Trailmaking Tests A and B (TMT-A/B assessed time in seconds to complete visual-motor speed and executive function tasks respectively).21,22 All tests are scored so that higher scores indicate better cognitive function. During analysis, all tests are converted to standardized regression coefficients so that the magnitude of effects is comparable across tests.

Exposure: Psychosocial working conditions

Workplace psychosocial exposures were assessed using the modified French version of the Karasek Job Content Questionnaire (JCQ).23 The JCQ measures perceptions of job control (assessing control over work time and tasks; 9 items; α=0.77) and job demands (assessing constraints imposed by expectations for fast, intense, or excessive work; 9 items; α=0.80). For each item, workers are asked whether they strongly agree, agree, disagree, or strongly disagree with a statement; an example of a statement is “My work requires me to be creative.” Items are reverse-scored when applicable. Scores for items in each subscale are summed.

To quantify the psychosocial work environment, we first analysed each dimension (demands and control) continuously. Both were normally distributed in the sample. Second, we dichotomized those continuous scores at the sample median for each subscale, classifying each person as low or high on that dimension. We then categorized participants into four quadrants based on their demand and control categories: low-demand/low-control (“passive”), low-demand/high-control (“low strain”), high-demand/high-control (“active”), and high-demand/low-control (“high strain”).2 We opted to use quadrants of demand and control, rather than testing for interaction between continuous scores on these dimensions, based on evidence of specific health risk factors and outcomes associated with each of the four quadrants.24 Low-strain work was chosen as the reference group based on evidence that this quadrant is expected to yield the most favorable health outcomes of the four quadrants.24

The JCQ was administered in 1997 and 1999 self-report questionnaires. Individuals reported on the job held at the time of the questionnaire. Because not all individuals respond to each questionnaire, we maximized the proportion of individuals with exposure information by either taking 1997 or 1999 Karasek scores completed prior to retirement or, if both 1997 and 1999 scores were available and completed prior to retirement, taking the average score. This follows the convention of other GAZEL studies.4,25 We observed normal distribution of scores for demands (mean 23, standard deviation [SD] 4, range 9–36) and control (mean 72, SD 10, range 24–96).

Covariates

We adjusted for covariates associated with cognitive function and potentially related to working conditions. Occupational grade during the year of demand-control assessment was extracted from company records and classified across three standard categories: executive, manager/foreman, and low-wage worker 26; retirement year, birth year, and gender were also extracted from this database. Education was collected at study baseline and dichotomized into less than secondary school versus secondary school completion and above.

We included several social, behavioural, and health factors collected by self-report in 2010: social factors (current marital status: married/cohabiting versus all other; years since retirement at testing); health factors (depressive and anxious symptoms, dichotomized into depressed/not depressed;27 alcohol consumption, categorized as non-drinker, light drinker, moderate drinker, heavy drinker; smoking status, categorized as current smoker/current non-smoker); and self-reported vascular risk factors28 (hypertension, high cholesterol, history of myocardial infarction, diabetes mellitus, and body mass index in kg/m2; BMI). All vascular risk factors were dichotomous (yes/no) except BMI, which was categorized (normal weight/underweight, overweight, obese).

Statistical analyses

We used OLS regression to model predictors of each cognitive test score. First, we modelled associations between continuous demands and each cognitive outcome; second, we modelled associations between continuous control and each outcome; third, we modelled associations between quadrants of demands and control and each outcome. For each set of analyses, we adjusted for covariates sequentially: Model 1 is adjusted for age and gender. Model 2 is adjusted for Model 1 covariates plus education and occupational grade. Model 3 is adjusted for Model 2 covariates plus alcohol, smoking, marital status, depression, and vascular risk factors.

We then tested for statistical interaction between gender and job strain. For models in which the gender-job strain interaction was statistically significant at the 0.05 level, we stratified by gender.

All analyses were performed using SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).

Results

Approximately 32% of participants had passive jobs and 21% had high-strain jobs; 22% were low-strain and 25% were active. We first examined distribution of socioeconomic, demographic, and health factors (Table 1). Approximately half had less than a high school education, but as testing occurred close to retirement and nearly all people were promoted during their tenure, only about 7% had low-wage jobs at exposure assessment. About three-quarters were men and nearly half were either overweight or obese. At testing, the average age was 64.50 (SD 2.86) years and participants had been retired for 8.41 (SD 3.17) years. The mean age at retirement was 56.09 (SD 2.51); in the cohort, retirement age is statutory and is calculated based on physical difficulty of work across one’s career.29

Table 1.

Occupational, demographic, and health characteristics of the study sample (n=2149). Years refer to the year that the variable was collected; for reference, GAZEL baseline was in 1989, exposure data was collected 1997–1999, and cognitive outcomes were assessed in 2010.

n/mean %/SD
Educational attainment, 1989
 Less than HS 1140 53.05
 HS or above 1009 46.95
Job category, 1997–1999
 Executive 982 45.7
 Manager/professional 1003 46.67
 Lower-wage 161 7.49
Gender, 1989
 Man 1642 76.41
 Woman 507 23.59
Depressive/anxious symptoms, 2009
 Not depressed or anxious 1870 87.02
 Depressed or anxious 279 12.98
Smoking status, 2010
 Nonsmoker 2006 93.35
 Current smoker 143 6.65
Alcohol consumption, 2010
 Non-drinker 246 11.45
 Light drinker 1195 55.61
 Moderate drinker 527 24.52
 Heavy drinker 181 8.42
BMI, 2010
 Normal/underweight 1030 47.93
 Overweight 916 42.62
 Obese 203 9.45
High cholesterol, 2010
 No 1596 74.27
 Yes 553 25.73
Diabetes, 2010
 No 2047 95.25
 Yes 102 4.75
History of MI, 2010
 No 2116 98.46
 Yes 33 1.54
Marital status, 2010
 Not married/cohabiting 320 15.43
 Married/cohabiting 1754 84.57
Mean age at testing 64.50 2.86
Mean years since retirement 8.41 3.17

All outcomes were collected and analysed as continuous variables. Except for FCSRT tests and MMSE, scores were normally distributed in the sample (Table 2). Due to left skewness of the latter variables, we initially cubed them to normalize them for analysis. However, as associations in models using cubed outcomes were not substantively different from models using the untransformed variables and were difficult to interpret, we present untransformed results here for comparability with other tests.

Table 2.

Distribution of demand-control scores and cognitive test scores in the sample

Cognitive Test Mean SD Min 25% Median 75% Max
Job content questionnaire dimensions
Demands 22.93 3.66 9 21 23 25 36
Control 72.28 9.68 24 66 72 78 96
Cognitive tests
 General cognitive status (MMSE) 28.36 1.37 21 28 29 29 30
 Psychomotor speed (DSST) 48.73 9.67 0 42 48 55 88
 Verbal memory (short-term recall) (FCSRT Immediate) 45.59 3.34 11 45 47 48 48
 Verbal memory (delayed recall) (FCSRT Delayed) 15.53 0.99 2 15 16 16 16
 Phonemic fluency 14.29 4.23 2 11 14 17 30
 Semantic fluency 22.79 5.33 0 19 23 26 42
 Visual-motor speed (TMT-A) 0.67 0.23 0.08 0.53 0.67 0.83 2.18
 Executive function (TMT-B) 0.33 0.14 0.00 0.25 0.34 0.43 1.00

First, we tested associations between job demands and each outcome, adjusting successively for demographic, socioeconomic, and behavioural/vascular covariates (Table 3). In age- and gender-adjusted models, we found that increasing job demands were significantly associated with better performance on Trailmaking Test B (B 0.002; SE 0.001; p=0.005) (Table 2). However, the association was attenuated to non-statistical significance after adjusting for SES (p=0.356). In contrast, increasing job demands were associated with worse DSST performance in fully adjusted models (B −0.122; SE 0.057; p=0.033).

Table 3.

Standardized associations (β, SE) between job demands, job control, and eight different continuous cognitive outcomes.

Job demands (continuous) Job control (continuous)

β (SE) P β (SE) P
Mini-Mental State Exam
 M1: age, gendera 0.037 (0.021) 0.092 0.064 (0.022) 0.004
 M2: M1+education, occupationb 0.001 (0.022) 0.958 0.001 (0.024) 0.965
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc 0.001 (0.023) 0.677 −0.004 (0.024) 0.854
Trailmaking Test A
 M1: age, gendera 0.019 (0.021) 0.387 0.095 (0.022) <.0001
 M2: M1+education, occupationb −0.007 (0.022) 0.749 0.054 (0.024) 0.021
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc −0.005 (0.023) 0.817 0.048 (0.024) 0.051
Trailmaking Test B
 M1: age, gendera 0.060 (0.021) 0.005 0.126 (0.022) <.0001
 M2: M1+education, occupationb 0.020 (0.022) 0.356 0.059 (0.024) 0.012
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc 0.027 (0.022) 0.225 0.055 (0.024) 0.023
FCSRTd-Immediate
 M1: age, gendera 0.026 (0.022) 0.231 0.000 (0.022) 0.984
 M2: M1+education, occupationb 0.010 (0.022) 0.660 −0.032 (0.024) 0.180
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc 0.008 (0.023) 0.696 −0.042 (0.024) 0.086
FCSRTd-Delayed
 M1: age, gendera 0.038 (0.022) 0.079 0.018 (0.022) 0.428
 M2: M1+education, occupationb 0.024 (0.022) 0.287 −0.010 (0.024) 0.697
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc 0.027 (0.023) 0.239 −0.015 (0.025) 0.547
Digit Symbol Substitution Test
 M1: age, gendera −0.004 (0.021) 0.852 0.094 (0.022) <.0001
 M2: M1+education, occupationb −0.051 (0.021) 0.016 0.021 (0.023) 0.343
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc −0.046 (0.022) 0.034 0.02 (0.023) 0.479
Phonemic fluency
 M1: age, gendera 0.025 (0.022) 0.259 0.114 (0.022) <.0001
 M2: M1+education, occupationb −0.011 (0.022) 0.617 0.060 (0.023) 0.011
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc −0.009 (0.023) 0.676 0.068 (0.024) 0.006
Semantic fluency
 M1: age, gendera 0.016 (0.021) 0.458 0.131 (0.022) <.0001
 M2: M1+education, occupationb −0.025 (0.022) 0.250 0.067 (0.023) 0.004
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc −0.026 (0.022) 0.247 0.075 (0.022) 0.002
a

Adjusted for age and gender

b

Adjusted for age, gender, educational attainment, and occupational grade at testing

c

Adjusted for age, gender, educational attainment, occupational grade at testing, depressive and anxious symptoms, smoking status, alcohol consumption, BMI, high cholesterol, diabetes, history of myocardial infarction, years since retirement at testing, and marital status

d

Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test

Significant statistical interaction between exposure and participant sex

Next, we tested associations between increasing job control and each cognitive outcome (Table 3). In general, we found stronger associations between control and cognitive outcomes, compared with demands. Better performance on three of eight cognitive tests remained significantly associated with higher job control after adjustment for all covariates: Trailmaking Test B (B 0.001; SE 0.000; p=0.020), phonemic fluency (B 0.037; SE 0.013; p=0.006), and semantic fluency (B 0.033; SE 0.011; p=0.002). Both DSST and MMSE were significantly associated with higher job control in age- and gender-adjusted models, but associations were not robust to adjustment for SES markers. Associations between short-term and long-term recall (FCSRT-Immediate and -Delayed) and job control were not statistically significant. Of note, high job control is considered a positive occupational condition 24; thus, the corollary of these findings is that lower job control is associated with worse performance. When we broke job control into its component parts (skill discretion and decision authority), both components had associations with similar cognitive outcomes (Supplemental Table 4), although associations between skill discretion and Trailmaking Tests A and B reached statistical significance (0.052 and 0.009 respectively, while associations between decision authority and Trailmaking Tests A and B did not (p=0.086 and 0.085 respectively).

Last, we tested associations between demand-control combinations and the domains of cognitive function (Table 4), using low-strain participants as the reference group.

Table 4.

Standardized associations (β, SE) between job strain quadrants and eight different continuous cognitive outcomes.

Low strain Passive Active High strain

β SE P β SE P β SE P
Mini-Mental State Exam
 M1: age, gendera (ref) −0.05 (0.02) 0.075 0.01 (0.03) 0.615 −0.03 (0.03) 0.272
 M2: M1+education, occupationb (ref) −0.01 (0.03) 0.853 −0.01 (0.03) 0.868 −0.01 (0.03) 0.697
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc (ref) −0.01 (0.03) 0.794 −0.01 (0.03) 0.848 −0.003 (0.03) 0.913
Trailmaking Test A
 M1: age, gendera (ref) −0.09 (0.03) 0.001 −0.02 (0.03) 0.554 −0.06 (0.03) 0.020
 M2: M1+education, occupationb (ref) −0.06 (0.03) 0.042 −0.03 (0.03) 0.324 −0.05 (0.03) 0.071
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc (ref) −0.05 (0.03) 0.074 −0.02 (0.03) 0.397 −0.04 (0.03) 0.122
Trailmaking Test B
 M2: M1+age, gender (ref) −0.09 (0.02) 0.001 0.02 (0.03) 0.452 −0.01 (0.03) 0.636
 M3: M2+educ, occ (ref) −0.04 (0.03) 0.126 0.00 (0.03) 0.998 0.01 (0.03) 0.739
 M4: M3+social, vascular (ref) −0.04 (0.03) 0.153 0.001 (0.03) 0.942 0.02 (0.03) 0.470
FCSRTd-Immediate
 M1: age, gendera (ref) −0.01 (0.03) 0.655 0.02 (0.03) 0.415 0.001 (0.02) 0.962
 M2: M1+education, occupationb (ref) 0.01 (0.03) 0.789 0.01 (0.03) 0.623 0.01 (0.02) 0.723
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc (ref) 0.01 (0.03) 0.757 0.01 (0.03) 0.722 0.02 (0.03) 0.399
FCSRTd-Delayed
 M1: age, gendera (ref) −0.03 (0.03) 0.327 −0.01 (0.03) 0.687 0.00 (0.03) 0.979
 M2: M1+education, occupationb (ref) −0.01 (0.03) 0.793 −0.01 (0.03) 0.490 0.01 (0.03) 0.789
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc (ref) −0.01 (0.03) 0.649 −0.03 (0.03) 0.367 0.01 (0.03) 0.661
Digit Symbol Substitution Test
 M1: age, gendera (ref) −0.10 (0.03) 0.0002 0.002 (0.03) 0.940 −0.07 (0.03) 0.007
 M2: M1+education, occupationb (ref) −0.05 (0.03) 0.068 −0.02 (0.03) 0.395 −0.04 (0.03) 0.060
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc (ref) −0.05 (0.03) 0.070 −0.02 (0.03) 0.444 −0.04 (0.03) 0.104
Phonemic fluency
 M1: age, gendera (ref) −0.09 (0.03) 0.002 0.01 (0.03) 0.775 −0.09 (0.03) 0.002
 M2: M1+education, occupationb (ref) −0.05 (0.03) 0.069 −0.01 (0.03) 0.671 −0.07 (0.03) 0.011
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc (ref) −0.06 (0.03) 0.058 −0.01 (0.03) 0.639 −0.07 (0.03) 0.016
Semantic fluency
 M1: age, gendera (ref) −0.11 (0.03) <.0001 0.02 (0.03) 0.470 −0.09 (0.03) 0.001
 M2: M1+education, occupationb (ref) −0.07 (0.03) 0.016 0.00 (0.03) 0.988 −0.07 (0.03) 0.011
 M3: M2+social, vascular risksc (ref) −0.08 (0.03) 0.006 −0.01 (0.03) 0.634 −0.07 (0.03) 0.008
a

Adjusted for age and gender

b

Adjusted for age, gender, educational attainment, and occupational grade at testing

c

Adjusted for age, gender, educational attainment, occupational grade at testing, depressive and anxious symptoms, smoking status, alcohol consumption, BMI, high cholesterol, diabetes, history of myocardial infarction, years since retirement at testing, and marital status

d

Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test

Significant statistical interaction between exposure and participant sex

We found no statistically significant associations between active (high-demand, high-control) work and any cognitive outcome. In age- and gender-adjusted models, passive work was significantly associated with lower scores on five tests: TMT-A (B −0.045; SE 0.014; p=0.001), TMT-B (B −0.038; SE 0.009; p=0.001), DSST (B −2.115; SE 0.057; p<0.0001); phonemic fluency (B −1.029; SE 0.323; p=0.002), and semantic fluency (B −1.021; SE 0.254; p <0.0001). However, SES adjustment attenuated associations to non-significance for TMT-B, DSST and phonemic fluency. After adjusting for behavioural and vascular risk factors, only semantic fluency remained associated with passive work (B −0.072; SE 0.261; p=0.006).

High-strain (high-demand, low-control) work was associated with reduced functioning on similar tests as passive work in minimally adjusted models: TMT-A, DSST, phonemic fluency, and semantic fluency. However, high-strain work was somewhat more robust to adjustment for covariates than was passive work; it was significantly associated with lower scores on both phonemic fluency (B −0.897, SE 0.365; p=0.016) and semantic fluency (B −0.770; SE 0.285; p=0.007) in fully adjusted models. Again, we observed no significant associations between demand-control quadrants and either FCSRT test, transformed or untransformed.

Given evidence of statistical interaction between gender and job demands and control for certain cognitive outcomes, we conducted gender-stratified analyses when such interactions were statistically significant (Supplementary Tables 1 and 2). We found that certain relationships were present among men, but not among women: among men, increased job control was associated with better scores on the DSST, and high-strain jobs were associated with worse DSST performance. Conversely, we found that certain relationships were present among women, but not among men: among women, increased control was associated with worse scores on the FCSRT-Immediate, passive work was associated with better scores on FCSRT-Immediate and Delayed, active jobs were associated with better performance on FCSRT-Immediate and Delayed, and high-strain jobs were associated with better FCSRT-Immediate and Delayed scores.

Discussion

We found that low job control during working life was negatively associated with performance in several cognitive domains after retirement: executive function, psychomotor speed, phonemic fluency, and semantic fluency; associations were robust to adjustment for SES, health and social behaviours, and vascular risk factors. Both passive and high-strain jobs were associated with lower scores on verbal fluency. However, associations between demand-control combinations and other domains of cognitive function were largely attenuated to non-statistical significance following adjustment for SES.

Many studies of the demand-control model and chronic disease outcomes have proposed stress response as the mechanism linking greater work-related stress with poorer health. Our findings extend on this research by pointing to the possibility that biological processes underpinning associations between job characteristics with cognitive function are unique because certain stressors—such as having high job demands—may be as mentally stimulating as they are stress response-provoking, particularly in combination with relatively high job control (i.e., active jobs), hence leading to positive or null findings in relation to cognitive and dementia outcomes.30 However, other stressors—such as lack of autonomy—may be true risk factors for dementia.31

A few studies14,15 have examined associations between the demand-control model and specific dimensions of cognitive function. Elovaino et al. (2009) found similar associations between verbal fluency and low control as were seen in the present study, whereas Agbenikey et al (2015) found decline in verbal learning and memory. Although we did not find association with the latter domains, our study measured incident cognitive function while theirs measured decline. But aside from those papers, most studies of job characteristics and cognitive function have either examined a single cognitive domain or have used clinical outcomes, such as Alzheimer’s disease diagnosis. Our extensive battery of cognitive tests permits a nuanced analysis of neurobiological processes underpinning previously observed associations.

We found associations between lower job control and impaired performance in visual-motor speed (TMT-A), executive function (TMT-B), phonemic fluency (f-test), and semantic fluency (animal test). The findings that lower job control relates to speed and executive function may have relevance to subsequent risk of dementia in these participants as poor performance on these two domains has been linked to preclinical Alzheimer’s disease.32 Additionally, a recent consensus paper states that, at the group level, cognitive changes preceding Alzheimer’s disease onset initially consist of subtle decreases in episodic memory.33 The job control subscale asks questions like, “In my work, I learn new things”, “My work requires me to be creative”, “In my work, I have repetitive tasks”, and “My work allows me to make decisions”. Control thus reflects task complexity, on-going learning, and self-direction, which in non-occupational settings are protective against cognitive decline or dementia.34 Those findings help explain our observed associations between job control and impairment in the relatively complex tasks presented by TMT-A (measuring visual-motor speed) and the verbal fluency tests, although the absolute magnitude of associations was small. The intellectual flexibility required for certain occupational tasks may promote continuation of such flexibility after retirement. Job control may also enhance executive function by promoting decision-making and reasoning,6 an explanation supported by observed associations between lower job control and poorer TMT-B (executive function) performance.

Job demands were assessed by questions such as “My work requires me to work very fast”, “My work necessitates periods of concentration”, and “I am not asked to do an excessive amount of work”. We found that higher job demands were associated with somewhat higher TMT-B scores but were associated with somewhat lower DSST scores. These mixed findings contrast to prior findings of high job demands and risk of cardiovascular disease and depression,35,36 in which effects are more consistently negative. It is possible that certain mental demands at work may build cognitive reserve, forestalling development of cognitive difficulties at older ages.37

We explicitly examine outcomes associated with passive work, a quadrant receiving little recent attention in the epidemiologic literature, despite initial findings that it may damage health by inhibiting learning ability and contributing to learned helplessness.24 Here, exposure to passive work was associated with lower verbal fluency, as was high-strain work. The environmental complexity hypothesis suggests that work and leisure environments that both challenge and engage participants predict better cognitive outcomes.10,38 The latter characteristics are consistent with active (high-demand, high-control) jobs, the opposite of passive jobs. Although active jobs are fast-paced and demanding (thus exerting certain stressors), they encourage autonomous decision-making.24 People with active jobs showed no associations with impaired cognitive function in any of our analyses despite being characterized by high job demands. In contrast, passive jobs lack both self-direction and complexity, potentially to the detriment of future brain function.7,12

We found that, for certain tests (the FCSRT-Immediate and Delayed and the DSST), associations were different for men and women, and in some cases the direction of association was reversed compared with the main effects. While the FCSRT tests were not significantly associated with job characteristics overall, associations did emerge when stratified by gender. Additionally, while passive work was associated with worse cognitive performance on certain tests in the overall cohort, it was associated with better performance among women on the FCSRT tests. This is potentially attributable to prior findings that the demand-control measures may perform differently in men and women, and that the measures may not adequately capture the psychosocial stressors to which women are exposed at work.39

A final notable finding was marked attenuation of associations between low-control work (whether combined with high or low demands) and most cognitive tests upon adjustment for SES, associations that had been statistically significant adjusted only for age and gender. Socioeconomic gradients in cognitive function are well-documented; education and occupation explain a substantial proportion of social disparities in older-age cognitive function.40 This has been attributed both to direct effects (cognitive reserve, occupational complexity) and indirect effects of lifelong SES on cognitive function (early-life environments, CVD, behavioural risk factors).41,42 The observed attenuated may have occurred because of co-variation of low job control with low SES markers. In addition, occupation-based SES may partially reflect physical and chemical occupational hazards also associated with cognitive function. However, the robustness of certain associations to adjustment for SES suggests that job conditions and SES do not fully explain observed relationships.

One limitation is that individuals are not randomly assigned to psychosocial work characteristics, but possibly self-select into certain jobs based on cognitive abilities, introducing selection bias. This is a limitation of all observational studies of associations between job characteristics and cognitive function. Our demand-control measure was taken relatively late in the career, and only at one time point, on average at age 52 and four years before retirement. Assessment of psychosocial work environment at multiple points during working career would be preferable. As our sample is relatively young and healthy, lack of association between our exposures and certain cognitive tests could also be explained by metrological properties of neuropsychological tests and also floor and ceiling effects. We also do not have baseline measures of cognitive function. Although controlling for education and occupational grade may alleviate this issue to some extent, we still cannot refute the possibility of reversed causality or cognitive selection into jobs as possible alternate explanations.

However, a strength is our use of a longitudinal cohort study with objectively-assessed cognitive outcome data and exposure data that was collected prospectively with respect to the outcome. Additionally, we included strong controls for social and vascular risk factors, a weakness of prior studies. Another strength is that education and occupation are less tightly intertwined in GAZEL than in other cohorts because many workers hired in their early twenties as less-educated blue-collar workers were promoted to executives by the end of their careers.43 Thus, later-life psychosocial job characteristics are not mere proxies for educational attainment, itself a strong predictor of later-life cognitive function.

Conclusions

Although not conclusive, our study indicates that low-control jobs during working life may be associated with subtle impairments in cognitive function in early old age. As such jobs are primarily occupied by the less-educated—who are already at risk for poor cognitive function, potentially exacerbated by adverse occupational conditions43—interventions to improve psychosocial working conditions may reduce social disparities in cognitive function at older ages.

What this paper adds.

  • Certain psychosocial work environments may be associated with cognitive health after retirement

  • Little research has focused on relationships between dimensions of the psychosocial work environment and post-retirement cognitive outcomes in specific domains

  • Exposure to jobs with low psychological control was associated with prolonged deficits in executive function, psychomotor speed, phonemic fluency, and semantic fluency after retirement

  • These relationships were partially, but not fully, explained by workers’ pre-retirement socioeconomic status

  • Findings suggest that lack of cognitive engagement at work may be associated with long-term cognitive impairment, in addition to the impact of classically stressful working conditions on cognitive outcomes.

Supplementary Material

1

Supplementary Table 1: Gender-stratified models for demands and control, for cognitive tests in which gender-by-exposure interactions were statistically significant at the 0.05 level.

Supplementary Table 2: Quadrants of job demands and control, stratified by gender

Supplementary Table 3: Pairwise differences in cognitive performance by job strain quadrant (adjusted for all covariates)

Supplementary Table 4: Association of skill discretion and decision authority (the two components of job control) with multiple cognitive outcomes, adjusting for age, gender, educational attainment, occupational grade at testing, depressive and anxious symptoms, smoking status, alcohol consumption, BMI, high cholesterol, diabetes, history of myocardial infarction, years since retirement at testing, and marital status

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge the contribution of the participating Health Screening Centers. Findings were initially presented orally at the APA/NIOSH Work, Stress, and Health conference (May 2015, Atlanta, GA). This work is part of a project funded by the Agence Nationale de la Recherche (ANR, French National Research Agency), and Agence Française de Sécurité Sanitaire de l’Environnement et du Travail (AFSSET, French Agency for Sanitary Security of Environment and Work). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Footnotes

Conflicts of interest: None to declare.

Contributorship statement: ES co-generated hypotheses, conducted statistical analyses, and drafted the manuscript. RA provided guidance on theoretical and statistical approaches and edited the manuscript. MG and MZ edited the manuscript. CB co-generated hypotheses and edited the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Launer LJ. Demonstrating the case that AD is a vascular disease: epidemiologic evidence. Ageing Research Reviews. 2002;1:61–77. doi: 10.1016/s0047-6374(01)00364-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Karasek R, Brisson C, Kawakami N, Houtman I, Bongers P, Amick B. The Job Content Questionnaire (JCQ): an instrument for internationally comparative assessments of psychosocial job characteristics. J Occup Health Psychol. 1998;3:322–55. doi: 10.1037//1076-8998.3.4.322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kivimäki M, Nyberg ST, Batty GD, et al. Job strain as a risk factor for coronary heart disease: a collaborative meta-analysis of individual participant data. The Lancet. 2012;380:1491–1497. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60994-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Wahrendorf M, Sembajwe G, Zins M, Berkman L, Goldberg M, Siegrist J. Long-term Effects of Psychosocial Work Stress in Midlife on Health Functioning After Labor Market Exit—Results From the GAZEL Study. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2012 doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bonde JPE. Psychosocial factors at work and risk of depression: a systematic review of the epidemiological evidence. Occupational and Environmental Medicine. 2008;65:438–445. doi: 10.1136/oem.2007.038430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Then FS, Luck T, Luppa M, et al. Systematic review of the effect of the psychosocial working environment on cognition and dementia. Occupational and environmental medicine. 2014;71:358–365. doi: 10.1136/oemed-2013-101760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Andel R, Vigen C, Mack WJ, Clark LJ, Gatz M. The effect of education and occupational complexity on rate of cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s patients. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2006;12:147–152. doi: 10.1017/S1355617706060206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Virtanen M, Singh-Manoux A, Ferrie JE, et al. Long Working Hours and Cognitive Function. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2009;169:596–605. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Then FS, Luppa M, Schroeter ML, König H-H, Angermeyer MC, Riedel-Heller SG. Enriched environment at work and the incidence of dementia: results of the Leipzig longitudinal study of the aged (LEILA 75+) PloS one. 2013;8:e70906. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kohn ML, Schooler C. The reciprocal effects of the substantive complexity of work and intellectual flexibility: A longitudinal assessment. American Journal of sociology. 1978:24–52. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wilson RS, de Leon CFM, Barnes LL, et al. Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. JAMA. 2002;287:742–748. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.6.742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2002;8:448–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Andel R, Infurna FJ, Hahn Rickenbach EA, Crowe M, Marchiondo L, Fisher GG. Job strain and trajectories of change in episodic memory before and after retirement: results from the Health and Retirement Study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2015;69:442–446. doi: 10.1136/jech-2014-204754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Agbenyikey W, Karasek R, Cifuentes M, et al. Job Strain and Cognitive Decline: A Prospective Study of the Framingham Offspring Cohort. The international journal of occupational and environmental medicine. 2015;6:534-79-94. doi: 10.15171/ijoem.2015.534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Elovainio M, Ferrie JE, Singh-Manoux A, et al. Cumulative exposure to high-strain and active jobs as predictors of cognitive function: the Whitehall II study. Occupational and Environmental Medicine. 2009;66:32–37. doi: 10.1136/oem.2008.039305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Goldberg M, Leclerc A, Bonenfant S, et al. Cohort profile: the GAZEL Cohort Study. Int J Epidemiol. 2007;36:32–39. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyl247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Wechsler D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised. The Psychological Corporation; New York: 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state” : A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Grober E, Buschke H. Genuine memory deficits in dementia. Developmental Neuropsychology. 1987;3:13–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Borkowski JG, Benton AL, Spreen O. Word fluency and brain damage. Neuropsychologia. 1967;5:135–140. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Arbuthnott K, Frank J. Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: validation using a set-switching paradigm. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2000;22:518–528. doi: 10.1076/1380-3395(200008)22:4;1-0;FT518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2004;19:203–214. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Niedhammer I. Psychometric properties of the French version of the Karasek Job Content Questionnaire: a study of the scales of decision latitude, psychological demands, social support, and physical demands in the GAZEL cohort. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 2002;75:129–44. doi: 10.1007/s004200100270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Karasek R, Theorell T. Healthy work: stress, productivity, and the reconstruction of working life. Basic books; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Sabbath E, Glymour MM, Descatha A, et al. Biomechanical and psychosocial occupational exposures: Joint predictors of post-retirement functional health in the French GAZEL cohort. Advances in Life Course Research. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.alcr.2013.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.INSEE. Nomenclature des professions et catégories socioprofessionnelles - PCS 2003. Paris, France: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A Self-Report Depression Scale for Research in the General Population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Helzner EP, Luchsinger JA, Scarmeas N, et al. Contribution of vascular risk factors to the progression in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2009;66:343–8. doi: 10.1001/archneur.66.3.343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Descatha A, Duval S, Sabbath E, Vuotto G. Difficult working conditions, retirement, and reform in France: what are the roles of the medical social worker and primary care physician? Health Soc Work. 2012;37:55–7. doi: 10.1093/hsw/hls007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Andel R, Crowe M, Hahn EA, et al. Work-Related Stress May Increase the Risk of Vascular Dementia. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2012;60:60–67. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03777.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Karp A, Andel R, Parker MG, Wang HX, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Mentally Stimulating Activities at Work During Midlife and Dementia Risk After Age 75: Follow-Up Study From the Kungsholmen Project. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2009;17:227–236. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e318190b691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bäckman L, Jones S, Berger A-K, Laukka EJ, Small BJ. Cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology. 2005;19:520. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dubois B, Harald H, Feldman B. Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: Definition, natural history, and diagnostic criteria. Alzheimer’s and Dementia. 2016:12. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Vemuri P, Lesnick TG, Przybelski SA, et al. Association of lifetime intellectual enrichment with cognitive decline in the older population. JAMA neurology. 2014;71:1017–1024. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.DeSanto Iennaco J, Cullen MR, Cantley L, Slade MD, Fiellin M, Kasl SV. Effects of Externally Rated Job Demand and Control on Depression Diagnosis Claims in an Industrial Cohort. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2010;171:303–311. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kuper H, Marmot M. Job strain, job demands, decision latitude, and risk of coronary heart disease within the Whitehall II study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57:147–153. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.2.147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bosma H, van Boxtel MPJ, Ponds RWHM, Houx PJ, Burdorf A, Jolles J. Mental Work Demands Protect Against Cognitive Impairment: MAAS Prospective Cohort Study. Experimental Aging Research. 2003;29:33. doi: 10.1080/03610730303710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Andel R, Crowe M, Kåreholt I, Wastesson J, Parker MG. Indicators of job strain at midlife and cognitive functioning in advanced old age. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2011;66:287–291. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Vermeulen M, Mustard C. Gender differences in job strain, social support at work, and psychological distress. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology. 2000;5:428–440. doi: 10.1037//1076-8998.5.4.428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Turrell G, Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, et al. Socioeconomic position across the lifecourse and cognitive function in late middle age. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2002;57:S43–51. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.1.s43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Whalley LJ, Dick FD, McNeill G. A life-course approach to the aetiology of late-onset dementias. The Lancet Neurology. 2006;5:87–96. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70286-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Sabia S, Marmot M, Dufouil C, Singh-Manoux A. Smoking history and cognitive function in late midlife: evidence from the Whitehall II study. European Journal of Public Health. 2007;17:82–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Sabbath E, Glymour MM, Berr C, et al. Occupational solvent exposure and cognition: does the association vary by level of education? Neurology. 2012;78:1754–60. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182583098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

Supplementary Table 1: Gender-stratified models for demands and control, for cognitive tests in which gender-by-exposure interactions were statistically significant at the 0.05 level.

Supplementary Table 2: Quadrants of job demands and control, stratified by gender

Supplementary Table 3: Pairwise differences in cognitive performance by job strain quadrant (adjusted for all covariates)

Supplementary Table 4: Association of skill discretion and decision authority (the two components of job control) with multiple cognitive outcomes, adjusting for age, gender, educational attainment, occupational grade at testing, depressive and anxious symptoms, smoking status, alcohol consumption, BMI, high cholesterol, diabetes, history of myocardial infarction, years since retirement at testing, and marital status

RESOURCES