Abstract
Zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) learn to produce songs in a manner reminiscent of spoken language development in humans. One candidate gene implicated in influencing learning is the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) subtype 2B glutamate receptor (NR2B). Consistent with this idea, NR2B levels are high in the song learning nucleus LMAN (lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium) during juvenile vocal learning, and decreases to low levels in adults after learning is complete and the song becomes more stereotyped. To test for the role of NR2B in generating song plasticity, we manipulated NR2B expression in LMAN of adult male zebra finches by increasing its protein levels to those found in juvenile birds, using a lentivirus containing the full-length coding sequence of the human NR2B subunit. We found that increased NR2B expression in adult LMAN induced increases in song sequence diversity and slower song tempo more similar to juvenile songs, but also increased syllable repetitions similar to stuttering. We did not observe these effects in control birds with overexpression of NR2B outside of LMAN or with the green fluorescent protein (GFP) in LMAN. Our results suggest that low NR2B subunit expression in adult LMAN is important in conserving features of stereotyped adult courtship song.
Introduction
Similar to speech in humans, song learning in vocal learning birds occurs most predominantly within a critical period during development1–3. Among avian species, zebra finches are “closed-ended” vocal learners and display little ability to further modify their vocalizations beyond the critical period as adults, when their songs become highly stereotyped1, 4–7. However, studies show that even in a closed-ended vocal learner, there are dynamic changes in sensorimotor processes where song stereotypy and the resistance to modify songs increases with age8, 9.Therefore, production of stereotyped songs in adult male zebra finches represents an attractive model system to test hypotheses on neural and genetic mechanisms underlying song stereotypy10.
Vocal learning requires a complex and specialized neural circuitry11–13. All vocal learning birds have seven forebrain song nuclei necessary for learning and producing learned vocalizations (Fig. 1)14–17. These forebrain song nuclei are distributed in two pathways (Fig. 1): (1) an anterior song pathway that includes a cortical-striatal-thalamic loop connecting MAN (magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium) in the cortical analog, to Area X in the striatum, to DLM (dorsal lateral nucleus of the medial thalamus) in the thalamus, and back to MAN, which is necessary for vocal learning and modifying vocalizations in different social contexts18–21; and (2) a posterior vocal pathway that projects from the pallial-cortical song nucleus HVC (a vocal nucleus, no abbreviation) to RA (robust nucleus of the arcopallium), to the midbrain vocal center DM (dorsal medial nucleus of the midbrain) and brainstem (nXIIts) vocal motor neurons that control the muscles of the syrinx, necessary for producing learned vocalizations. The song nuclei are adjacent to non-vocal movement activated regions22, and are proposed to have arisen by duplication of these preexisting motor areas23, suggesting that discoveries in the song learning systems could be relevant to understanding sensorimotor learning broadly.
The lateral part of MAN (LMAN) is a critical output node of the vocal cortical-basal ganglia-thalamic circuit onto RA. Although LMAN is not necessary for singing, neurons in LMAN show strong singing-related immediate early gene expression20 and neural activity that is correlated with song structure24, 25. Artificial manipulation of LMAN by microstimulation while the bird sings induces real time changes in spectral features (e.g. fundamental frequency and amplitude) of individual song syllables4. The dependence on LMAN for song modification is stronger during the juvenile critical period of song learning26–28. However, LMAN lesions in adult male zebra finches also prevent song degradation caused by deafening27, 29. Furthermore, inactivation of LMAN reduces some song acoustic variability seen in adult songs, and also in juvenile songs that are engaged in sensorimotor song learning from tutors, indicating that behavioral variability actively requires LMAN and cortical-striatal-thalamic integration5, 21, 30, 31. A similar role has been proposed for Broca’s area in the anterior cortex and the connected striatal regions in human speech, based on lesions, physiology/imaging, connectivity, and gene expression11, 32 (but see Long et al.33 for an alternative interpretation of HVC playing a similar role as Broca’s area based on cooling experiments, instead of HVC being similar to the laryngeal motor cortex as proposed in refs 11 and 32).
These developmental changes in LMAN are correlated with developmental changes in the NMDAR (N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor) subunits involved in neural plasticity, NR2A (N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor 2A) and NR2B (N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor 2B)34, 35. During the critical period for song development, NR2B mRNA levels in LMAN are higher than NR2A 34, 35. Then as the song becomes crystallized, NR2B expression and currents decreases, and NR2A increases in LMAN34–36, causing NMDAR-mediated synaptic currents to shorten (presumably with NR2B or NR2A dimerizing with the ubiquitous NR1 subunit)37. By the time the animals become adults, NR2B mRNA is lower, whereas NR2A mRNA is higher in LMAN compared to the surrounding nidopallium region (adult patterns also seen in the two other vocal learning lineages, parrots and hummingbirds)38. Importantly, NDMARs are necessary for normal song development35, 39, and LMAN input to RA is regulated primarily by NMDAR 40–42. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the NR2B gene confers increased plasticity to neural circuits43–45, and that its downregulation in adult LMAN may contribute to the molecular mechanisms of increased song stereotypy in adults.
Here, we tested this hypothesis by performing lentivirus-mediated overexpression of the human NR2B in LMAN. We found that this manipulation induced a number of changes to song, including stuttering, slower song tempo, reduced song matches to their original song, higher acoustic pitch, and altered song sequencing, approaching some characteristics found in juvenile song.
Results
To test the hypothesis that NR2B levels in adult LMAN influence song stereotypy, we developed a lentiviral construct eGFPhNR2B (Fig. 2a) containing the human NR2B tagged with enhanced GFP (green fluorescent protein), overexpressed it in LMAN of male zebra finches (NR2B group), and examined singing behavior. Our lentiviral vector was designed off of a previous construct that was shown to have all the properties of NR2B in vivo and in vitro in mammalian neurons46. We decided to test the human gene, because the zebra finch homolog was not completely assembled in the zebra finch genome47, and the NR2B subunit gene is highly conserved, sharing more than 90% amino acid sequence identity between the human and zebra finch38. We generated two control groups: (1) Birds injected with the eGFPhNR2B lentivirus in the motor nidopallium area laterally adjacent to LMAN; and (2) a lentiviral construct expressing eGFP without human NR2B (Fig. 2b) injected into LMAN. When we found no statistical differences between these two control groups, we grouped the data together as one large control group. We first describe the results of the validation of NR2B protein expression from the lentiviral construct, followed by the behavioral results.
Rous Sarcoma Virus (RSV) promoter drives expression of human NR2B in HEK293FT cells
To determine if the custom designed eGFPhNR2B lentiviral plasmid expressed the eGFP-tagged human NR2B protein, we transfected the plasmid in human embryonic kidney 293FT cells (HEK293FT), which normally do not express endogenous NR2B protein48, 49. In such constructs, the amino signal transport sequence upstream of the GFP molecule has been shown to be cleaved off in vivo to successfully transport a fused GFP-NR2B functional protein into the cell membrane46. Consistent with these past constructs, compared to control cells without transfection (Fig. 2c, Lane 1), cells transfected with our eGFPhNR2B lentiviral plasmid expressed high levels of the human NR2B protein of the expected ~180 kD size, as measured by Western blots using a NR2B antibody (Fig. 2c, Lane 2). As measured by immunocytochemistry, cells transfected with the eGFP only and eGFPhNR2B plasmids showed GFP labeling (Fig. 2d,g), and as expected only the eGFPhNR2B transfected cells showed NR2B labeling (Fig. 2e,h). The GFP labeling in both the eGFP only and eGFPnNR2B transfected cells was localized to the cell membrane (Fig. 2h), and for the latter construct, the eGFP label was distinctly 100% co-localized with NR2B labeling in the membrane (Fig. 2g–i). These findings indicate that our eGFP-tagged NR2B human protein is successfully expressed in cells in vitro by the RSV promoter, is in the correct frame in order to be recognized by the NR2B antibody, and is transported to the membrane as expected.
RSV promoter drives expression of human NR2B gene in LMAN in the avian brain
To determine if these constructs could infect LMAN cells and overexpress the human NR2B protein in vivo in the avian brain, we performed bilateral injections of the eGFPhNR2B, or eGFP lentiviruses in LMAN of adult male zebra finches (Fig. 2j; n = 4 each). Three weeks after injections, we analyzed serial brain sections by immunocytochemistry and found strong GFP-labeled cells in most of the LMAN song nucleus with both constructs (see Fig. 2k for example). As found in vitro, in vivo expression was localized to the cell membrane.
To quantify the amount of NR2B overexpression and determine if the known developmental changes in mRNA expression35 are translated to protein expression, we measured NR2B protein levels by Western blots on microdissected LMAN song nuclei and a comparable sized region of the adjacent lateral nidopallium of juvenile and adult birds (Fig. 2l) following a protocol used previously in our laboratory50. We calculated the normalized NR2B protein levels to the actin levels of each sample. In all animals without viral injections, we detected the endogenous ~180 kD NR2B protein (Fig. 2m). Relative to 30-day old juveniles, NR2B showed decreased protein expression in both LMAN and the adjacent nidopallium in adults (Fig. 2m,o). Also in both juveniles and adults, expression in LMAN was lower than the adjacent nidopallium, but the developmental decrease in LMAN was larger (Fig. 2m,o), consistent with what is seen anatomically at the mRNA level using in situ hybridizations34, 35. Importantly, injections of the eGFPhNR2B lentivirus in LMAN in adults brought back NR2B protein levels to the same as those seen in juveniles (Fig. 2n,o). Because the predicted size of the GFP-tagged NR2B protein is only 6 kD larger than the endogenous NR2B protein, the ~180 kD band that we detected likely reflects both endogenous and overexpressed human NR2B. In the transfected brain regions (Fig. 2n), as well as in the transfected cultured cells (Fig. 2c), there is an additional lower molecular weight band, which we interpret to be possibly a processed alternative isoform of NR2B generated from our vector, or a degraded or unfinished protein product of NR2B. There was no detectable change in actin expression levels in the control and injected LMAN samples, indicating that the increased NR2B levels detected is not due to a general increase in protein expression or overloading of the Western blots for the experimental condition.
We performed immunohistochemistry using the NR2B antibody on frozen serial brain sections of animals that were injected and recorded for song behavior for up to 2.5 months. Animals injected with the eGFPhNR2B lentivirus in LMAN had significantly more cells expressing high levels of NR2B protein compared to the control animals (Fig. 3a,b; expression level determined by unbiased threshold function and cell counts made blind to group; see methods). These results also show that the overexpression of NR2B protein in the brain was long lasting.
Overexpression of human NR2B in LMAN results in stuttering, and increases in song tempo and song sequence variability
Stuttering
To assess the potential impact of NR2B overexpression in LMAN on singing behavior, we first focused on global structure of the song motifs. The most auditorily and visually apparent result on sonograms, not often seen in adult or juvenile birds, was stuttering (Fig. 4a–c). Assessed quantitatively, before surgery, the mean number of syllables produced across motifs did not differ between birds selected for control (7.58 ± 0.98) and NR2B overexpression (9.62 ± 0.83) injections prior to surgery (Fig. 5a; p = 0.14; two sample Wilcoxon test). The mean number of syllables in the motifs of the control birds also did not change post-surgery (7.56 ± 0.99; Fig. 5a). In contrast, birds with NR2B overexpression in LMAN showed increased number of syllables per motif (13.58 ± 1.19; Fig. 5a). This increase was in part due to increased end syllable repetition, characteristic of stuttering (Fig. 4c–e). All the birds that repeated syllables, particularly at the end of the motifs, started to do so by 4 weeks after surgery (Fig. 4f), and continued to stutter at the time we sacrificed them 3 months after surgery to collect their brains. This timing indicates that it was the NR2B overexpression that caused the stuttering, and not possible mechanical injury to LMAN from the surgery, which would have been immediate. The total number of introductory notes at the beginning of the first motif of a song bout, which can be several per motif and more with directed singing, also increased in some of the NR2B overexpression birds (Fig. 5b). The percentage of motifs showing stuttering was high, which went from an average of ~10% and ~72% for end syllable and introductory notes respectively, to ~87% and ~85% after NR2B overexpression (Fig. 4g). No such changes were seen in control birds (Fig. 4g, controls combined). Since stuttering is not normally found in juvenile birds, these findings indicate that NR2B overexpression in adults result in aberrant stuttering.
Song tempo
Because there is a dramatic increase in song tempo (produced faster) from juvenile development to adulthood51, and because song tempo is also known to change throughout the day52, we measured the songs consistently at the same time of day for all animals throughout the course of the recordings. Whereas control birds (both groups combined) showed no change before and after surgery, the NR2B LMAN overexpression group showed an average 1.7-fold increase in song motif duration (Fig. 5c). This increase was due more to the increased number of syllables (i.e. stuttering), as we found that the mean intersyllable gaps between all syllables increased in both control and NR2B LMAN overexpression birds (Fig. 5d). However, in controls, this was associated with a concomitant decrease in the total average syllable duration, which presumably compensated for the silent gap increases, resulting in no overall change in total motif duration of the control group (Fig. 5c). The NR2B group did not show this concomitant decrease in average syllable duration (Fig. 5e). This suggests a natural continued age-dependent decrease in syllable duration (independent of silent gap increases) past the critical period, which was blocked with NR2B overexpression. Taken together, we infer that NR2B overexpression in LMAN partially reversed and further prevented the age-dependent tempo increases in adult birds, where they become more like juvenile birds.
Song sequence
To quantify syllable sequencing, we used commonly studied measures of sequence linearity, sequence consistency, and sequence stereotypy of zebra finch songs29, 53, 54. Juvenile birds are known to have higher variability in these measures compared to adults5, 55. In control adult birds (both groups combined), we found considerable individual variability pre and post surgery, but overall group means for all three sequence measures remained unchanged (Fig. 6a–c). In contrast, consistent with our hypothesis, the NR2B overexpression LMAN group exhibited dramatically lower group mean scores of consistency and stereotopy, and a strong trend for linearity (significant with the outlier Purple364 removed, p = 0.02, paired Wilcoxon signed rank test; Fig. 6a–c). There was however, no significant group change in percentage sequential match (Fig. 6d), a measure of how temporally similar a sequence is from one song to another. Thus, overexpression of NR2B reduced the characteristic features of stereotyped sequencing of adult courtship songs by changing the songs to be more variable from one rendition to another.
Overexpression of human NR2B in LMAN cause increased overall song syllable acoustic similarity, changes in pitch, but not in mean acoustic features
Syllable acoustic similarity and accuracy
Because LMAN is known to control the level of variability in acoustic structure of song syllables4, 30, 56, and these are more variable in juveniles than adults, we measured the overall degree of song syllable acoustic similarity pre versus post-surgery (a measure that summarizes multiple acoustic features into one value; see methods). There was a very small (mean change from pre to post = 1.10 ± 0.36), but significant decrease in the control group in the degree of percentage similarity from one song motif to another song motif (Fig. 7a), indicating a slight increase in variability of acoustic features with age or due to the surgery. However, in the NR2B LMAN overexpression group, for most birds there were bigger decreases (mean change from pre to post = 5.6 ± 1.92) in overall similarity (Fig. 7a). Consistent with these findings, the NR2B overexpression group, but not controls, showed a robust and significant decrease in percentage accuracy (Fig. 7b).
Syllable structure
We next measured which specific syllable acoustic features changed, including pitch, fundamental frequency, entropy, goodness of pitch, and amplitude modulation, all of which are known to be sensitive to LMAN perturbations27, 29, 30, 54 and some of which are more variable in juvenile birds. We found increased mean pitch in the NR2B group (Supplementary Fig. S1a), but no significant group mean differences of the other four acoustic parameters (Supplementary Fig. S1b–e). To examine variability more directly, we calculated the standard deviation (SD) of each of the acoustic features, but found no significant differences in overall SD variability in any of the syllable acoustic features we analyzed (Supplementary Figs S2 and S3). For a second measure, we also did not detect any significant changes in group means for the coefficient of variation (% c.v) of the syllable acoustic features in the control or NR2B groups (Supplementary Fig. S4).
These findings indicate that except for pitch, the decreased similarity and accuracy maybe best explained by an interacting combination of acoustic features showing greater variability with NR2B overexpression in LMAN.
Manipulated protein levels in LMAN correlate best with song motif duration and sequence match
Here, we explicitly addressed the issue of causality to determine whether animals with manipulated overexpression of NR2B protein in LMAN have differences in song sequencing and acoustic features with the proportion of cells expressing NR2B. This would also allow us to determine more subtle changes that are sensitive to NR2B expression levels. We found a strong correlation between mean number of NR2B-labeled cells (high protein levels) with variability in two sequence measures: variability in mean song motif duration (Fig. 8a) and variability in percentage sequential match (Fig. 8b). Although, we did not include the control groups in our regression analyses, the control group means (red boxes in Fig. 8) were at the lower end of the range for the NR2B group (blue boxes in Fig. 8), consistent with the levels or number of cells expressing NR2B influencing these song features. Interestingly, we did not detect any significant correlations with mean number of syllable repetitions and the number of NR2B-expressing cells (Fig. 8c). We also did not detect significant correlations with other song sequencing or acoustic features (Supplementary Figs S5 and S6). Overall, our results indicate that NR2B has an important role in modulating variability in the temporal, sequencing, and some acoustic features of songs.
Discussion
Our study provides important advances on understanding the role of the NR2B subunit gene in production and maintenance of stereotyped courtship song in adult zebra finches. Here we tested the hypothesis that the developmentally down-regulated levels of NR2B may be important in conferring song stereotypy in adult males. We discovered that overexpression of NR2B bilaterally in LMAN causes aberrant repetition of beginning (introductory notes) and end-syllables similar to stuttering, and reversion back to more of a juvenile state to the lengthening of the song motif duration and increased variability of song sequencing and global acoustic structure. Considering that in adult zebra finches lesions of LMAN do not result in robust changes in the overall structure of the song6, 21, 57, or in variability in syllable sequencing6, 21, 30, these findings indicate that overexpression of NR2B to a more juvenile state reactivates a more dominant role of LMAN in influencing song plasticity. Below we discuss the implications of our findings within the context of utilizing zebra finches as a model to explore cortical-basal-ganglia circuit function and dysfunction.
The syllable repetition findings have important parallels to human dysfluencies, namely stuttering and verbal apraxia. The increased syllable repetitions we observed shares many features of developmental and neurogenic stuttering observed in humans who have suffered damage to the cortical-basal ganglia circuit58 or increased activation in this circuit; the more a person stutters, the more neurons of this network are activated59. Developmental stuttering refers to speech repetitions or prolongations that affect mostly children at a young age, whereas the rare neurogenic stuttering or acquired stuttering is due to adult brain lesions and have been described to be a core aspect of some aphasias60–62. In humans, individuals with neurogenic stuttering are more predisposed to stutter at the beginning of a sentence63, 64, although other dysfluencies have been reported where stuttering occurs at the end of the final sounds65, 66. The repetitions at the end of the motifs we observed usually occurred after the first motif of a bout, suggesting that the birds become stuck in sequencing from one motif to the next. In humans, motor-speech disorders such as verbal apraxia are often associated with repetitions of phonemes similar to stuttering67, 68. Theories posit that an important mechanism causal to stuttering is impairment of the basal ganglia circuits that produce timing cues for the initiation of motor movement in speech58. Our findings in zebra finches support this view.
We do not believe that this induced stuttering from NR2B overexpression in LMAN is a reversion to the juvenile state. Only a small percentage of a population (up to ~7%) of laboratory-raised male zebra finches are naturally known to stutter69. Although stuttering can be learned from adult tutors in juvenile zebra finches69, it is not known to be part of the normal song learning process. Rather, we believe that this is aberrant behavior due to NR2B overexpression in adult LMAN. Previous studies have reported that adult LMAN lesions do not affect introductory notes30 nor cause stuttering for other syllables54. Kubikova et al.54 found a high incidence of stuttering at the end of the song motifs with Area X neurotoxic lesions, but only in birds that already had a predisposition for a small number of repeats before the lesions54. We suggest that while lesion inactivation of LMAN does not induce stuttering, activation or enhancement of its function through NR2B overexpression causes the birds to have difficulty sequencing between motifs, by getting stuck at the beginning and end syllables when switching from one motif to another. Syllables produced in between or at the end of motifs are more variable and less stereotyped than those within the motif28, possibly making them more sensitive to perturbations.
Consistent with the stuttering findings, our findings of increased song motif tempo in the NR2B overexpression (i.e. LMAN activation) group is the opposite to that found with lesion inactivation of LMAN in zebra finches27, 54, indicating the change we observed is a gain in LMAN function with NR2B overexpression. The normal tempo changes of zebra finch song over juvenile to adult development, could be due to stretching and compressing proportionally syllables and silent gaps52. Consistent with our findings in control birds, gaps, but not syllables are known to shorten over the first year of life of an adult bird51, and recently it has been shown to be more genetically controlled than learning syllable imitation in zebra finches70. Other studies have reported no decreases in intersyllable gaps, but a decline in syllable durations over 1.5 years of the adult life of a male71, whereas studies in Bengalese finches report that songs slow down later as the bird ages, primarily due to increase in intersyllable gaps72. It is unclear why the findings from these studies are equivocal, but they may be attributed to methodological differences. We speculate that NR2B in LMAN is likely important in modulating the variability in syllable duration, and not intersyllable gaps, since gaps appeared to increase in both control and NR2B groups. We also speculate that the higher levels of NR2B in manipulated adults induced a partial juvenile-like state, by preventing syllable durations to shorten over time, while simultaneously allowing the intersyllable gaps to increase.
We were surprised to find that NR2B overexpression in LMAN had a larger impact on song sequencing relative to syllable acoustic features, since variability in such syllable acoustic features is reduced after LMAN lesions before they impact sequencing, consistent with the idea that its target nucleus RA is involved more in controlling syllable acoustic structure than sequencing73, 74. Instead, our results are consistent with those reported for Area X lesions54, where the authors noted more apparent sequencing changes in adult animals. This is perhaps due to our manipulation enhancing LMAN function with an intact Area X. Nevertheless, we still found some syllable acoustic changes, such as decreases in stereotypy and increases in pitch after NR2B overexpression, indicating that NR2B levels in LMAN impact acoustic features to some extent. In adult zebra finches, microstimulation of LMAN induces variability in pitch4, whereas lesions or ablation of LMAN results in prolonged reduction in the variability of syllable pitch29, 30, 75, 76. We speculate that NR2B function in LMAN may have a more dominant role in controlling sequencing behavior either through intrinsic actions or its action on the downstream nucleus AreaX, and have a less dominant role in acoustic behavior through its action on RA.
To explain our findings at a molecular level, we propose the following hypothesis: Since NR2B relative to NR2A causes the NMDA receptor channel complex with NR1 to stay open longer44, 77, increased NR2B expression could slow down songs by increasing channel opening time, preventing the syllables from compressing in length as the bird ages, among other effects. The increased channel opening time with NR2B has been proposed to allow LMAN to integrate multiple synaptic inputs during juvenile development for vocal learning78; however, song learning does not require slow NMDA-EPSCs79, even though blocking NMDA receptor activity during tutoring decreases the number of syllables that are learned by the juveniles80. In light of our findings, we hypothesize that the naturally decreasing NR2B during development may be also critical in ensuring the characteristic faster song tempo of courtship songs that females prefer over the slower and more variable undirected songs18, 81, 82.
An alternative hypothesis is that introducing the exogenous human NR2B in LMAN may disrupt or alter the function of the endogenous NR2B receptor or the overexpressed NR2B may not be expressed in the appropriate cell type and cause a change as a result. Since we are unaware of any other studies in songbirds that have manipulated NR2B or its interacting receptor levels in LMAN, we can only speculate on such a possibility from similar studies of genetic enhancement in mammals. Previous studies in mammals that have overexpressed NR2B in the forebrain, using a similar construct as ours, have reported enhancement of prefrontal cortical long-term potentiation (LTP) and working memory, which was abolished by blocking with an NR2B subunit antagonist83. NR2B subunit overexpression also enhances NMDA receptor activation and channel opening, and learning and memory in transgenic mice84. NR2B endogenous expression in our experiments also appeared in the larger cells (i.e. projection neurons) of LMAN that normally express NR2B. If NR2B were overexpressed in other cells that do not normally express NR2B, the protein would not be expected to function or compete with endogenous function since there would not be an NR1 subunit to bind with and no endogenous NR2B to compete with. Further studies will be required to adequately address the effects of NR2B overexpression in songbirds and other models to better understand how it affects endogenous NR2B function. Nevertheless, the combined findings and studies indicate that overexpression of NR2B in songbird LMAN and other brain regions in mammals is able to at least partially recapitulate some plastic states.
In conclusion, our findings suggest that changing the expression of one gene in adult LMAN to mimic juvenile levels is sufficient to recapitulate a partial juvenile song behavior state. This includes decreased song tempo, and increased sequence and overall acoustic variability. These findings support the hypothesis that low NR2B levels are important in allowing stereotypy of adult courtship songs in male zebra finches. These changes were also accompanied by an unintended side effect of stuttering, which could be also potentially explained as a sequencing change. This neurogenic stuttering in zebra finches shares some common features to human stuttering, and therefore the zebra finch model may represent a potential tractable and translational model to test hypotheses on cortical-basal ganglia circuitry dysfunctions that could lead to new therapeutic insights.
Methods
Lentiviral vector constructs and production
Since the zebra finch NR2B gene sequence was not completely assembled from the zebra finch genome47, we designed a custom lentiviral construct with the full-length sequence of the human NR2B cDNA (Open Biosystem clone ID 8322672; GenBank Accession #BC113620.1). We followed cloning techniques described in Luo et al.46 to optimize overexpression of the human NR2B gene, in this case in zebra finches, with an enhanced GFP protein tagged as a reporter gene driven by the RSV promoter (eGFPhNR2B; Fig. 2a). This construct design was made based off of the pEGFP-NR2B plasmid containing the rat NR2B sequence that is commercially available from Addgene (Addgene plasmid #17925). The signal peptide of the human NR2B differs from the rat (or mouse) NR2B by only two amino acids, where the arginine (R) in human is replaced with a serine (S) in rat where serine is the 5th amino acid located past the signal sequence (Fig. 2a). We constructed the expression vector for eGFPhNR2B by inserting the eGFP cDNA sequence in frame with the NR2B subunit between the 5th and 6th codons after the predicted sequence for signal peptide where the eGFP is in place of the serine. This signal peptide is cleaved off leaving a fused amino terminal GFP and carboxy terminal NR2B protein46. Using a variety of experiments including electrophysiology, Luo et al.46 had reported that a similarly GFP tagged NR2B protein does not interfere with the NR2B protein function. Similar designs of other NMDA receptors tagged with eGFP have been published before where such constructs have been shown to be functional without any defects of channel function due to the insertion of the eGFP after the predicted signal peptide recognition sequence85, 86. Following previous studies87, 88, we also generated a self-inactivating eGFP lentivirus89 that contained the enhanced GFP protein alone as the reporter gene driven by the same RSV promoter (Fig. 2b).
We prepared the lentiviral vectors by transfecting 6 × 106 HEK293FT cells with 5 μg of the vesicular stomatitis virus glycoprotein (VSVg) envelope encoding plasmid, 15 μg of the delta-8.9 packaging plasmid, and 20 μg of the promoter-reporter or promoter-gene of interest plasmids using Lipofectamine 2000 (Life Technologies, Grand Island, NY). After 72 hours, we harvested the supernatant from one 10 cm culture plate, filtered them with a 0.45 μm filter, and centrifuged them at 26,000 rpm for 2 hours at 4 °C to obtain the pellet. We resuspended the pellets and serially diluted the lentivirus to transduce HEK293FT cells. After 72 hours, we counted the number of labeled HEK293FT cells to calculate the viral titers (eGFPhNR2B used for brain injections was 9.9 × 106 ffu/ml and eGFP was 5.2 × 106 ffu/ml).
Transfection of lentiviral constructs into HEK293FT cells
To test if the eGFPhNR2B lentiviral plasmid expressed the eGFP-tagged NR2B protein, we initially performed cell culture experiments on HEK293FT cells. HEK293FT cells were cultured in 10 cm plates in serum plus DMEM (Life Technologies) medium (10% FBS+ L-Glutamine, Pen-Strep). Thereafter, the cultures were split into 7.5 × 105 cell samples plated on 6 well plates. Cells were allowed to reach 80–90% confluency and 1 μg of the eGFPhNR2B plasmid DNA was added along with 2 μl of Lipofectamine 2000 in Opti-MEM Reduced Serum medium (Life Technologies). Control wells were not transfected with any plasmid DNA. HEK293FT cells were incubated for 2 days at 37 °C after which we detected protein expression either directly on slides by immunocytochemistry and by Western blots, as described below.
Immunocytochemistry on HEK293FT cells transfected with the eGFPhNR2B plasmid to detect NR2B protein
We performed immunocytochemistry using HEK293FT cells on 24 well plates on PDL coated coverslips. For all experimental conditions, 50 μl HEK293FT cells from a confluent 10 cm dish per well were transfected with 1 μg of eGFPhNR2B plasmid DNA and 1 μl of Lipofectamine 2000. For detection of GFP expression by fluorescence, we rinsed the cells with 0.1 M PBS (phosphate buffered saline) and fixed them with 1 ml of 4% paraformaldehyde for 15 min at room temperature and then washed the cells with 1 ml of cold 0.1 M PBS (3 × 5 min). The control wells were mounted with Vectashield mounting medium with DAPI (H-1200, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, California). For detection of GFP with an antibody, in other wells we permealized the cells with 1 ml PBST (0.1 M PBS + 0.1% Triton X) for 10 min at room temperature. We blocked background for the cells with 1 ml of PBST + 1% BSA (Bovine serum albumin) for 30 min at room temperature. The primary GFP antibody (GFP Clone 3E6 Mouse IgG2A monoclonal antibody, Cat. #A-11120, Molecular Probes, Eugene, OR, USA) was added at a dilution of 1:1000, and incubated with the cells at 4 °C overnight. We washed the cells (3 × 10 min) with 1 ml of 0.1 M PBS and incubated the cells with the secondary antibody (Alexa Fluor 484 Goat anti-mouse IgG, Cat. #A-11001, Molecular Probes) at a dilution of 1:1000 for 30 min at room temperature in the dark. Finally, we washed the cells in 1 ml of 0.1 M PBS (3 × 10 min) and mounted them with the Vectashield mounting medium with DAPI.
For detection of NR2B expression, we rinsed the cells from a separate set of wells with 1 ml of 0.1 M PBS and fixed them in 1 ml of 4% paraformaldehyde for 15 min at room temperature. We washed the cells in 1 ml of cold 0.1 M PBS three times for 5 min each and stored the cells overnight at 4 °C in PBS. On the next day we blocked background signal from the cells with 500 μl of 0.1 M PBS + 1% BSA for 30 min at room temperature. We added the mouse NR2B monoclonal primary antibody (Cat. #MA1-2014, Thermo Scientific, Rockford, IL, USA) at a dilution of 1:1000 in 200 μl of 0.1 M PBS + 1% BSA and incubated the cells at room temperature for 2 hr. We washed the cells in 1 ml of 0.1 M PBS three times for 5 min and incubated the cells with the secondary antibody (Alexa Fluor 555 Goat anti-mouse IgG1, Molecular Probes) at a dilution of 1:1000 in 200 μl of 0.1 M PBS + 1% BSA for 1 hr at room temperature in the dark. Finally, we washed the cells in 1 ml of 0.1 M PBS three times for 5 min and mounted them with the Vectashield mounting medium with DAPI. For all methods, expression was then detected under fluorescence microscope settings using a Leica DMI 4000b microscope (Leica Microsystems, IL, USA).
Western Blot
When conducting Western blots to detect protein levels from both microdissected brain tissue and HEK293FT cells, the samples that were transfected with the eGFPhNR2B plasmid, or without the plasmid (control cells), were collected and stored at −20 °C until further processing. We lysed the cells from HEK293FT cells or brain tissue with SDS-PAGE 2X sample buffer (a solution containing stacking buffer (10% SDS, 80% glycerol, and 1 ml of β - mercaptoethanol, ≥99%) to isolate the protein. We homogenized the protein lysates and ran 10 μl of each in Western blots, and incubated the blots with the mouse monoclonal NR2B primary antibody (1:500, Thermo Scientific, Cat. #MA1-2014) overnight at 4 °C or with Actin controls in separate blots (Anti-actin monoclonal antibody, Millipore), followed by a secondary antibody CFTM680 labeled Goat Anti Mouse IgG1 (1:5000, Biotium 20065) for 2 hr at room temperature. All western blots were performed under the same experimental condition and the blots quantified using ImageJ.
Subjects and housing
We collected 41 adult male zebra finches (~120–180 days old) and 4 30-day old juvenile birds (3 males and 1 female) from our breeding colony at Duke University to conduct pilot studies to test the lentivirus, and for final experimental manipulations. We housed the birds in cages in groups of four inside sound-attenuation recording chambers, provided them with food and water ad libitum, and maintained a constant photoperiod of 12:12 h light/dark cycle. We assigned 26 birds for pilot studies and housed the remaining birds for further manipulations later on. All animal procedures were approved by the Duke University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC), and all methods carried out in accordance with the guidelines and regulations of the IACUC-approved protocol number A095-14-04.
Lentivirus injections
We anaesthetized male zebra finches with isoflurane anesthesia (1–2%; flow 1 L/min) and placed them in a stereotaxic apparatus (Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA). We used stereotaxic coordinates and multiunit electrophysiological recordings of the higher spontaneous activity in LMAN compared to surrounding nidopallium to locate LMAN bilaterally: 4.2–4.8 mm rostrally, 1.6–1.8 mm laterally, 2.2–2.4 mm ventrally. Once found, we injected the eGFPhNR2B or the control eGFP lentivirus (32.2 nl per injection totaling a final volume of ~1.2 μl) into LMAN using a glass micropipette attached to a pressure injection unit (Drummond Nanoinject II, Drummond Scientific Company, Broomall, PA). For a control brain region, we injected the lentiviral constructs in another set of animals into the nidopallium region adjacent to LMAN, using the following coordinates: 2.25 mm rostrally, 1.7 mm laterally, 2.2–2.3 mm ventrally. After surgery, we allowed the birds to awake and isolated them in a cage under a heat lamp with food and water for 48 hr before returning them to their original housing cage inside the sound attenuation boxes. To check for GFP expression in LMAN, we sacrificed a subset of birds injected with the eGFPhNR2B lentivirus (n = 3), or eGFP lentivirus (n = 3), 2.5 weeks after injections and found strong labeling of GFP in LMAN neurons indicating that both lentiviruses were able to infect LMAN.
Microdissection and Western blot analyses of song nuclei to verify overexpression of NR2B protein
To validate overexpression of the NR2B protein within LMAN and to compare the levels with 30-day old juvenile birds, we performed Western blot analyses on brain tissue collected from a pilot set of adult birds (n = 7) that were injected with the eGFPhNR2B lentivirus, juvenile birds as mentioned above (n = 4), and from adult control birds with no injections (n = 11). At ~35–40 days post-lentiviral injections, we removed birds housed in recording chambers and immediately decapitated them. We quickly dissected and rinsed the brains in chilled PBS, and then mounted them in a tissue slicer (Stoelting Tissue Slicer, Cat. #51415, Wood Dale, IL, USA). Following the protocol described by Whitney et al.50, we first used a razor to cut off part of the brainstem to create a flat surface with the forebrain on a tissue block cutting surface. We positioned the cerebellum region against a thin plexiglass wall with Vetbond 3 M tissue adhesive on the surface of the tissue slicer. We cut 500 μm slices of the brain tissue in the coronal plane, starting from the front of the brain. The tissue slices were placed in a Petri dish (kept on ice) containing cold 0.1 M PBS that contained a protease inhibitor cocktail (cOmplete ULTRA Tablets, Cat. No. 06538304001; Roche, Indianapolis, IN, USA). We placed the Petri dish under a dissecting microscope and using fine surgical scissors (Cat. No. 15000-00; Fine Science Tools, Foster City, USA) and forceps (Cat. No. 11252-00; Fine Science Tools), we identified sections with song nuclei and quickly dissected out LMAN, Area X, and an adjacent nidopallium area of comparable size of LMAN from both hemispheres using a darkfield setting on an Olympus MVX10 microscope (Olympus, Japan). We were easily able to localize the song nuclei due to their denser neural fibers (white) observed in the darkfield setting. The dissected tissue was immediately frozen in microcentrifuge tubes placed in a dry ice-ethanol bath and then stored at −20 °C until further processing. Protein was isolated following SDS-PAGE 2X sample buffer as described before, and NR2B protein levels were detected using Western blot analysis using methods described previously.
Song recordings and analyses
To assess the effect of manipulations on singing behavior, we recorded courtship (directed) songs from a set of birds (n = 17) that had been acclimatized in sound boxes for 2–4 days, at which time most birds sang at a normal singing rate of ~30–40 bouts of songs within the first one hour of singing. We focused on directed songs which are more stereotyped (less variable) and which males sing more intensely as opposed to undirected songs90. We inadvertently did not record sufficient amounts of undirected songs before surgery (due to too few minutes of recordings). However, we don’t expect results to be dramatically different with undirected songs, in that they tend to be more variable than directed songs, and our hypothesis was to specifically test for inducing song variability with NR2B overexpression. We recorded songs from each male for three weeks before surgical manipulations to capture the within-individual variation in their songs since males can vary their songs from one rendition to another rapidly. We define songs as a sequence of syllables that start with one or more introductory notes, continued with at least one or more motifs, and are surrounded by more than 2 seconds of silence. We stimulated directed songs by frequent switching every 5–10 minutes of presenting two female birds in the male cages in the morning for up to 1 hour, in which it is shown that the males almost exclusively produce directed songs20, 91. We used animals that sang more than 30 bouts of song per hour and produced short bursts of intense singing behavior that indicate courtship singing behavior. We recorded the songs with microphones (model SR0, Earthworks, Milford, NH, USA) that had a flat frequency response up to 20 kHz, connected to a computer with Avisoft RECORDER (www.avisoft.com) software at 16 bits and 44-kHz sampling rate. After we had collected enough presurgery directed song recordings, to characterize their song syllables within song motifs we assigned birds randomly to each of three surgery groups for lentiviral gene manipulations: (1) Birds that received injections of the eGFP lentivirus in LMAN (control group); (2) Birds that received injections of the eGFPhNR2B lentivirus in nidopallium (control group); and (3) Birds that received injections of the eGFPhNR2B lentivirus in LMAN (NR2B group). After surgery, we allowed the birds to recover for 2–3 days before recording their songs again for up to 2.5 months postsurgery. Birds were housed in groups of the same four individuals they were housed with before surgery, and their songs recorded one day every other week. We also ensured that the songs were selected and processed blind to the treatment group.
We analyzed the song syllable sequencing and acoustic features using Luscinia92, Sound Analysis Pro (SA+)93, 94, or a custom designed MATLAB software (MathWorks) developed by Dr. Masashi Tanaka53 based on the SVM algorithm95 generated by Dr. Kosuke Hamaguchi. For all acoustic analyses, we used 60 song bouts randomly chosen from each of both the presurgery, and postsurgery recordings of all individual males for a total of 120 songs for each bird. For each bird, we manually calculated the total syllable numbers within each motif, and number of introductory notes per motif, both before and after manipulations using Luscinia, and then averaged the scores for presurgery and postsurgery conditions. Using Sound Analysis Pro we computed the means of syllable or sequence similarity scores, and their associated song syllable acoustic features, including entropy, mean change in fundamental frequency, pitch, goodness of pitch, amplitude modulation, and their variances. These scores were averaged across all 60 song motifs for each of the presurgery and postsurgery songs. The three song similarity measurements included: (1) percentage similarity, that computes the threshold level of similarity between songs; (2) percentage of song syllable accuracy, that indicates the average local similarity score; and (3) sequential match, which computes the extent to which similar syllables across songs correspond in temporal order. Using the MathWorks program, we computed the means of intersyllable gaps of all syllables, syllable duration of all syllables, and song bout duration of song motifs. To compare the sequencing features before and after lentiviral injections in the control and NR2B groups, we calculated mean syllable sequence linearity, consistency, and stereotypy scores from the 120 song motifs using MathWorks as described in Tanaka et al.53. We calculated linearity as internal linearity55 (number of syllable types −1/number of transition types) with syllables including the introductory notes, but where repetition of introductory notes (such as iiiii) was excluded so that the syllable sequence “iiiiiabcd” produces the linearity score of 1. We calculated the consistency score as (number of typical transitions/number of transitions), where typical transitions were determined for each preceding syllable as the most frequently observed syllable transitions in the song. The stereotypy score was the average of linearity and consistency scores.
Tissue collection, sectioning, and immunocytochemical detection of NR2B protein levels in LMAN
After their last recording session around 2.5 months after surgery, we removed the birds from the recording boxes, decapitated them, dissected and rinsed the brains in 0.1 M PBS within minutes, embedded the brain tissue in Tissue-Tek OCT Compound (Sakura, Finetek, Torrance, CA) in a plastic block mold and quickly froze the Tissue Tek around the brain on a dry ice-ethanol bath. The brain was stored at −80 °C until further processing. Brains were sectioned in sagittal planes at 16 μm in 6–20 series on a cryostat and the sections mounted on plus charged slides (Fisher Scientific, Pittsburgh, PA, USA), and again stored at −80 °C until further processing for immunohistochemistry.
To identify overexpression of NR2B protein, sections mounted on slides were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 10 min, washed 5 X in 0.1 M PBS for 5 min each, and then incubated in 1% H2O2 in 0.1 M PBS for 30 min. After three 5 min washes in 0.1 M PBS, sections were incubated in 2% normal goat serum (NGS) in 0.1 M PBS with the addition of 0.2% Triton X-100 for 1 hr to prevent non-specific binding. The sections were then incubated for 72 hr with a primary antibody rabbit polyclonal anti-NR2B (Abcam, Cambridge, MA, USA), diluted 1:500 in PBS-T-N (0.2% Triton X-100 + 2% NGS) at 4 °C. The specificity of this antibody has been tested before in other avian species (Dr. Melvin Rouse, University of California, San Diego, personal communication) but we modified the protocol for use in zebra finches. After three 5 min washes in 0.2% PBS-T, the sections were incubated for 2 hr with a biotinylated goat anti-rabbit IgG antibody (Vector Laboratories Inc., Burlingame, CA, USA) at a dilution of 1:250 in PBS-T. After three 5 min washes in PBS-T, the sections were incubated in avidin-biotin horseradish-peroxidase complex (Vectastain Elite ABC kit Standard, PK-6100, Vector Laboratories) at a dilution of 1:200 in PBS-T for 1 hr. The sections were then washed twice for 5 min in 0.2% PBS-T, once in 0.1 M PBS, and then incubated in DAB buffer solution (SigmaFast DAB tablets, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) for 25 min. After five to six 5-min washes in 0.1 M PBS and one 5-min wash in deionized water, the slides were coverslipped with Vectashield Mounting medium with DAPI and allowed to dry for several days before microscopy.
We used brightfield microscopy on an Olympus MVX10 microscope to capture images of serial brain sections that spanned rostro-caudally (8–16 sections) containing the LMAN song nucleus from all subjects. To quantify the density of positive immuno-labeled cells, we developed a custom semi-automated Macro written in the Fiji (ImageJ) language that computes the number of stained cells in an image based on a darkness threshold selected by the user. We created a binary (black or white) mask of cells, determined a threshold for each image and counted labeled cells with the built in Particle Analyzer function in Fiji. The person quantifying the labeled cells using the macro was blind to the treatment groups of all animals.
Statistical Analyses
All statistical analyses were performed in R (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria) and in SPSS (IBM SPPS Statistics for Macintosh, Version 23.0). When examining group means of both control and NR2B groups for pre- and post-surgery differences in song acoustic features we performed separate nonparametric paired Wilcoxon signed-rank tests for each comparison using the “wilcox.test (paired = TRUE)” function in R. To examine variability of song acoustic features, we computed the standard deviation (SD), and the coefficient of variation (c.v.) expressed as a percentage and used paired Wilcoxon signed-rank tests in R to test for statistical significance. To analyze if NR2B protein levels were correlated with song acoustic features we used linear regression models in SPSS. To analyze the stuttering effect and end syllable repetitions in the NR2B group from presurgery levels to different weeks after injections we used paired t tests in R. To analyze the NR2B protein levels between groups we used two sample Wilcoxon tests in R.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
This study was funded by grants provided by the Howard Hughes Medical Institute and the NIH Pioneer award to EDJ. We thank Dr. Robert Lachlan for help with the song analyses using Luscinia, and Dr. Kosuke Hamaguchi and Dr. Masashi Tanaka also for help with song analyses using the custom developed MATLAB MathWorks song analyses software. We also thank Dr. Kosuke Hamaguchi for advice on the multiunit electrophysiological recordings in LMAN during surgery, Dr. Anne E. West for providing reagents and equipment to perform Western blots, Dr. Melvin Rouse for providing guidance on the immunohistochemistry on zebra finch brain tissue, and Joshua Jones Macopson for help with the immunohistochemistry.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed experiments: M.C., E.E.F., and E.D.J. Performed the experiments: M.C., E.E.F., L.F.C., M.E.K., and R.A.S. Analyzed the data: M.C., E.E.F., L.F.C., R.A.S., M.E.K., A.S., E.D.J. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: E.D.J., M.C., A.S., L.F.C., M.E.K. Wrote the paper: M.C., E.D.J.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Liang-Fu Chen and Emma E. Fridel contributed equally to this work.
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at doi:10.1038/s41598-017-00519-8
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Mukta Chakraborty, Email: chakraborty.mukta@gmail.com.
Erich D. Jarvis, Email: ejarvis@rockefeller.edu
References
- 1.Marler P, Peters S. Selective vocal learning in a sparrow. Science. 1977;198:519–521. doi: 10.1126/science.198.4316.519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Thorpe WH, Pilcher PM. The nature and characteristics of subsong. British Birds. 1958;51:509–514. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Nottebohm F. Auditory experience and song development in the Chaffinch, Fringilla coelebs. IBIS. 1968;110:549–568. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1968.tb00063.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kao MH, Doupe AJ, Brainard MS. Contributions of an avian basal ganglia-forebrain circuit to real-time modulation of song. Nature. 2005;433:638–643. doi: 10.1038/nature03127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Olveczky, B. P., Andalman, A. S. & Fee, M. S. Vocal experimentation in the juvenile songbird requires a basal ganglia circuit. PLoS. Biol. 3, e153, doi:05-PLBI-RA-0094R1/pbio.0030153 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 6.Scharff C, Nottebohm F. A comparative study of the behavioral deficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finch song system: implications for vocal learning. J. Neurosci. 1991;11:2896–2913. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-09-02896.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Funabiki Y, Funabiki K. Factors limiting song acquisition in adult zebra finches. Dev. Neurobiol. 2009;69:752–759. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Williams H. Birdsong and singing behavior. Ann. NY. Acad. Sci. 2004;1016:1–30. doi: 10.1196/annals.1298.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.McDonald KS, Kirn JR. Anatomical plasticity in the adult zebra finch song system. J. Comp. Neurol. 2012;520:3673–3686. doi: 10.1002/cne.23120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Brainard M, Doupe AJ. Postlearning consolidation of birdsong: stabilizing effects of age and anterior forebrain lesions. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:2501–2517. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-07-02501.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Jarvis ED. Learned birdsong and the neurobiology of human language. Ann. NY. Acad. Sci. 2004;1016:749–777. doi: 10.1196/annals.1298.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Nowicki S, Searcy WA. The evolution of vocal learning. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2014;28C:48–53. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bolhuis JJ, Okanoya K, Scharff C. Twitter evolution: converging mechanisms in birdsong and human speech. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;11:747–759. doi: 10.1038/nrn2931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Konishi M. Birdsong: from behavior to neuron. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1985;8:125–170. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.08.030185.001013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Jarvis ED, Mello CV. Molecular mapping of brain areas involved in parrot vocal communication. J. Comp. Neurol. 2000;419:1–31. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(20000327)419:1<1::AID-CNE1>3.0.CO;2-M. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Jarvis ED, et al. Behaviourally driven gene expression reveals song nuclei in hummingbird brain. Nature. 2000;406:628–632. doi: 10.1038/35020570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Gahr M. Neural song control system of hummingbirds: comparison to swifts, vocal learning (songbirds) and nonlearning (suboscines) passerines, and vocal learning (budgerigars) and nonlearning (dove, owl, gull, quail, chicken) nonpasserines. J. Comp. Neurol. 2000;426:182–196. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20001016)426:2<182::AID-CNE2>3.0.CO;2-M. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Woolley SC, Doupe AJ. Social context-induced song variation affects female behavior and gene expression. PLoS. Biol. 2008;6:e62. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Stepanek L, Doupe AJ. Activity in a cortical-basal ganglia circuit for song is required for social context-dependent vocal variability. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;104:2474–2486. doi: 10.1152/jn.00977.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Jarvis ED, Scharff C, Grossman MR, Ramos JA, Nottebohm F. For whom the bird sings: context-dependent gene expression. Neuron. 1998;21:775–788. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80594-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bottjer SW, Meisner EA, Arnold AP. Forebrain lesions disrupt development but not maintenance of song in passerine birds. Science. 1984;224:901–903. doi: 10.1126/science.6719123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Feenders G, et al. Molecular mapping of movement-associated areas in the avian brain: a motor theory for vocal learning origin. PLoS. ONE. 2008;3:e1768. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Chakraborty, M. & Jarvis, E. D. Brain evolution by brain pathway duplication. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 370, doi:10.1098/rstb.2015.0056 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 24.Hessler NA, Doupe AJ. Singing-related neural activity in a dorsal forebrain-basal ganglia circuit of adult zebra finches. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:10461–10481. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-23-10461.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hessler NA, Doupe AJ. Social context modulates singing-related neural activity in the songbird forebrain. Nat. Neurosci. 1999;2:209–211. doi: 10.1038/6306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Williams H, Mehta N. Changes in adult zebra finch song require a forebrain nucleus that is not necessary for song production. J. Neurobiol. 1999;39:14–28. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4695(199904)39:1<14::AID-NEU2>3.0.CO;2-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Brainard MS, Doupe AJ. Interruption of a basal ganglia-forebrain circuit prevents plasticity of learned vocalizations. Nature. 2000;404:762–766. doi: 10.1038/35008083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Horita H, Wada K, Jarvis ED. Early onset of deafening-induced song deterioration and differential requirements of the pallial-basal ganglia vocal pathway. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:2519–2532. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06535.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Nordeen KW, Nordeen EJ. Deafening-induced vocal deterioration in adult songbirds is reversed by disrupting a basal ganglia-forebrain circuit. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:7392–7400. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6181-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kao MH, Brainard MS. Lesions of an avian basal ganglia circuit prevent context-dependent changes to song variability. J. Neurophysiol. 2006;96:1441–1455. doi: 10.1152/jn.01138.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Goldberg JH, Fee MS. Vocal babbling in songbirds requires the basal ganglia-recipient motor thalamus but not the basal ganglia. J. Neurophysiol. 2011;105:2729–2739. doi: 10.1152/jn.00823.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Pfenning, A. R. et al. Convergent transcriptional specializations in the brains of humans and song-learning birds. Science346, doi:10.1126/science.1256846 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 33.Long MA, et al. Functional Segregation of Cortical Regions Underlying Speech Timing and Articulation. Neuron. 2016;89:1187–1193. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Singh TD, Basham ME, Nordeen EJ, Nordeen KW. Early sensory and hormonal experience modulate age-related changes in NR2B mRNA within a forebrain region controlling avian vocal learning. J. Neurobiol. 2000;44:82–94. doi: 10.1002/1097-4695(200007)44:1<82::AID-NEU8>3.0.CO;2-W. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Basham ME, Sohrabji F, Singh TD, Nordeen EJ, Nordeen KW. Developmental regulation of NMDA receptor 2B subunit mRNA and ifenprodil binding in the zebra finch anterior forebrain. J. Neurobiol. 1999;39:155–167. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4695(199905)39:2<155::AID-NEU1>3.0.CO;2-S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.White SA, Livingston FS, Mooney R. Androgens modulate NMDA receptor-mediated EPSCs in the zebra finch song system. J. Neurophysiol. 1999;82:2221–2234. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.5.2221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Livingston FS, Mooney R. Development of intrinsic and synaptic properties in a forebrain nucleus essential to avian song learning. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:8997–9009. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-23-08997.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wada K, Sakaguchi H, Jarvis ED, Hagiwara M. Differential expression of glutamate receptors in avian neural pathways for learned vocalization. J. Comp. Neurol. 2004;476:44–64. doi: 10.1002/cne.20201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Aamodt SM, Nordeen EJ, Nordeen KW. Blockade of NMDA receptors during song model exposure impairs song development in juvenile zebra finches. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 1996;65:91–98. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1996.0010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Mooney R, Konishi M. Two distinct inputs to an avian song nucleus activate different glutamate receptor subtypes on individual neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1991;88:4075–4079. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.10.4075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Stark LL, Perkel DJ. Two-stage, input-specific synaptic maturation in a nucleus essential for vocal production in the zebra finch. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:9107–9116. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-20-09107.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Kubota M, Saito N. NMDA receptors participate differentially in two different synaptic inputs in neurons of the zebra finch robust nucleus of the archistriatum in vitro. Neurosci. Lett. 1991;125:107–109. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(91)90002-B. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Papouin T, Oliet SH. Organization, control and function of extrasynaptic NMDA receptors. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 2014;369:20130601. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Perez-Otano I, Ehlers MD. Learning from NMDA receptor trafficking: clues to the development and maturation of glutamatergic synapses. Neuro-Signals. 2004;13:175–189. doi: 10.1159/000077524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.MacDonald JF, Jackson MF, Beazely MA. Hippocampal long-term synaptic plasticity and signal amplification of NMDA receptors. Crit. Rev. Neurobiol. 2006;18:71–84. doi: 10.1615/CritRevNeurobiol.v18.i1-2.80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Luo JH, et al. Functional expression of distinct NMDA channel subunits tagged with green fluorescent protein in hippocampal neurons in culture. Neuropharmacology. 2002;42:306–318. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(01)00188-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Warren WC, et al. The genome of a songbird. Nature. 2010;464:757–762. doi: 10.1038/nature08819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Collett VJ, Collingridge GL. Interactions between NMDA receptors and mGlu5 receptors expressed in HEK293 cells. British Journal of Pharmacology. 2004;142:991–1001. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjp.0705861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Domingues A, et al. Expression of NR1/NR2B N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors enhances heroin toxicity in HEK293 cells. Ann. NY. Acad. Sci. 2006;1074:458–465. doi: 10.1196/annals.1369.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Whitney O, et al. Core and region-enriched networks of behaviorally regulated genes and the singing genome. Science. 2014;346:1256780. doi: 10.1126/science.1256780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Glaze CM, Troyer TW. Development of temporal structure in zebra finch song. J. Neurophysiol. 2013;109:1025–1035. doi: 10.1152/jn.00578.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Glaze CM, Troyer TW. Temporal structure in zebra finch song: implications for motor coding. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:991–1005. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3387-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Tanaka, M., Singh Alvarado, J., Murugan, M. & Mooney, R. Focal expression of mutant huntingtin in the songbird basal ganglia disrupts cortico-basal ganglia networks and vocal sequences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA113, E:1720–1727, doi:10.1073/pnas.1523754113. Epub Mar 7 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 54.Kubikova L, et al. Basal ganglia function, stuttering, sequencing, and repair in adult songbirds. Sci. Rep. 2014;4:6590. doi: 10.1038/srep06590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Foster EF, Bottjer SW. Lesions of a telencephalic nucleus in male zebra finches: Influences on vocal behavior in juveniles and adults. J. Neurobiol. 2001;46:142–165. doi: 10.1002/1097-4695(20010205)46:2<142::AID-NEU60>3.0.CO;2-R. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Woolley SC, Kao MH. Variability in action: Contributions of a songbird cortical-basal ganglia circuit to vocal motor learning and control. Neuroscience. 2015;296:39–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Nordeen KW, Nordeen EJ. Long-term maintenance of song in adult zebra finches is not affected by lesions of a forebrain region involved in song learning. Behav. Neural. Biol. 1993;59:79–82. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(93)91215-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Alm PA. Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. J. Commun. Disord. 2004;37:325–369. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Vanhoutte S, et al. Increased motor preparation activity during fluent single word production in DS: A correlate for stuttering frequency and severity. Neuropsychologia. 2015;75:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Ringo CC, Dietrich S. Neurogenic stuttering: An analyseis and critique. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology. 1995;3:111–122. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Lundgren K, Helm-Estabrooks N, Klein R. Stuttering Following Acquired Brain Damage: A Review of the Literature. J. Neurolinguistics. 2010;23:447–454. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Luchsinger, R. & Arnold, G. E. Voice, Speech and Language (1965).
- 63.Tani T, Sakai Y. Analysis of five cases with neurogenic stuttering following brain injury in the basal ganglia. J. Fluency. Disord. 2011;36:1–16. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2010.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Scott KS. Dysfluency in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences. 2015;193:239–245. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.03.266. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Lebrun Y, Vanborsel J. Final Sound Repetitions. J. Fluency. Disord. 1990;15:107–113. doi: 10.1016/0094-730X(90)90037-S. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Stansfield J. Word-Final Disfluencies in Adults with Learning-Difficulties. J. Fluency. Disord. 1995;20:1–10. doi: 10.1016/0094-730X(93)00001-V. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Johns DF, Darley FL. Phonemic variability in apraxia of speech. J. Speech. Hear. Res. 1970;13:556–583. doi: 10.1044/jshr.1303.556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Trost, J. Apraxia dysfluency in patient’s with Broca’s aphasia. Paper presented at the American Speech Hearing Association annual convention; Chicago, IL (1971).
- 69.Helekar SA, Espino GG, Botas A, Rosenfield DB. Development and adult phase plasticity of syllable repetitions in the birdsong of captive zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) Behav. Neurosci. 2003;117:939–951. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.5.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Araki M, Bandi MM, Yazaki-Sugiyama Y. Mind the gap: Neural coding of species identity in birdsong prosody. Science. 2016;354:1282–1287. doi: 10.1126/science.aah6799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Pytte CL, Gerson M, Miller J, Kirn JR. Increasing stereotypy in adult zebra finch song correlates with a declining rate of adult neurogenesis. Dev. Neurobiol. 2007;67:1699–1720. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Cooper BG, et al. Age-related changes in the Bengalese finch song motor program. Neurobiology of Aging. 2012;33:564–568. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.04.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Sober SJ, Wohlgemuth MJ, Brainard MS. Central contributions to acoustic variation in birdsong. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:10370–10379. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2448-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Fee MS, Kozhevnikov AA, Hahnloser RH. Neural mechanisms of vocal sequence generation in the songbird. Ann. NY. Acad. Sci. 2004;1016:153–170. doi: 10.1196/annals.1298.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Hampton CM, Sakata JT, Brainard MS. An avian basal ganglia-forebrain circuit contributes differentially to syllable versus sequence variability of adult Bengalese finch song. J. Neurophysiol. 2009;101:3235–3245. doi: 10.1152/jn.91089.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Thompson JA, Basista MJ, Wu W, Bertram R, Johnson F. Dual pre-motor contribution to songbird syllable variation. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:322–330. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5967-09.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Paoletti P, Bellone C, Zhou Q. NMDA receptor subunit diversity: impact on receptor properties, synaptic plasticity and disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2013;14:383–400. doi: 10.1038/nrn3504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Mooney R. Neurobiology of song learning. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2009;19:654–660. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Livingston FS, White SA, Mooney R. Slow NMDA-EPSCs at synapses critical for song development are not required for song learning in zebra finches. Nat. Neurosci. 2000;3:482–488. doi: 10.1038/74857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Basham ME, Nordeen EJ, Nordeen KW. Blockade of NMDA receptors in the anterior forebrain impairs sensory acquisition in the zebra finch (Poephila guttata) Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 1996;66:295–304. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1996.0071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Sossinka R, Bohner J. Song Types in the Zebra Finch Poephila-Guttata-Castanotis. Z Tierpsychol. 1980;53:123–132. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1980.tb01044.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Riebel K. Song and Female Mate Choice in Zebra Finches: A Review. Adv. Study. Behav. 2009;40:197–238. doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(09)40006-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Cui Y, et al. Forebrain NR2B overexpression facilitating the prefrontal cortex long-term potentiation and enhancing working memory function in mice. PLoS One. 2011;6:e20312. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Tang YP, et al. Genetic enhancement of learning and memory in mice. Nature. 1999;401:63–69. doi: 10.1038/43432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Barria A, Malinow R. Subunit-specific NMDA receptor trafficking to synapses. Neuron. 2002;35:345–353. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00776-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Perez-Otano I, et al. Assembly with the NR1 subunit is required for surface expression of NR3A-containing NMDA receptors. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:1228–1237. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-04-01228.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Roberts TF, Klein ME, Kubke MF, Wild JM, Mooney R. Telencephalic neurons monosynaptically link brainstem and forebrain premotor networks necessary for song. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:3479–3489. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0177-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Roberts TF, Tschida KA, Klein ME, Mooney R. Rapid spine stabilization and synaptic enhancement at the onset of behavioural learning. Nature. 2010;463:948–952. doi: 10.1038/nature08759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Lois C, Hong EJ, Pease S, Brown EJ, Baltimore D. Germline transmission and tissue-specific expression of transgenes delivered by lentiviral vectors. Science. 2002;295:868–872. doi: 10.1126/science.1067081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Zann, R. A. The Zebra Finch: A Synthesis of Field and Laboratory Studies. (Oxford University Press, 1996).
- 91.Teramitsu I, White SA. FoxP2 regulation during undirected singing in adult songbirds. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:7390–7394. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1662-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Lachlan, R. F. Luscinia: a bioacoustics analysis computer program. Version 1.0 [Computer program]. www.lusciniasound.org (2007).
- 93.Tchernichovski O, Nottebohm F, Ho CE, Pesaran B, Mitra PP. A procedure for an automated measurement of song similarity. Anim. Behav. 2000;59:1167–1176. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Tchernichovski O, Mitra PP, Lints T, Nottebohm F. Dynamics of the vocal imitation process: how a zebra finch learns its song. Science. 2001;291:2564–2569. doi: 10.1126/science.1058522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Hamaguchi K, Mooney R. Recurrent interactions between the input and output of a songbird cortico-basal ganglia pathway are implicated in vocal sequence variability. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:11671–11687. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1666-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.