Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2017 Apr 27;7:1218. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-01242-0

Effectiveness of Immersive Videos in Inducing Awe: An Experimental Study

Alice Chirico 1,, Pietro Cipresso 2, David B Yaden 3, Federica Biassoni 1, Giuseppe Riva 1,2, Andrea Gaggioli 1,2
PMCID: PMC5430774  PMID: 28450730

Abstract

Awe, a complex emotion composed by the appraisal components of vastness and need for accommodation, is a profound and often meaningful experience. Despite its importance, psychologists have only recently begun empirical study of awe. At the experimental level, a main issue concerns how to elicit high intensity awe experiences in the lab. To address this issue, Virtual Reality (VR) has been proposed as a potential solution. Here, we considered the highest realistic form of VR: immersive videos. 42 participants watched at immersive and normal 2D videos displaying an awe or a neutral content. After the experience, they rated their level of awe and sense of presence. Participants’ psychophysiological responses (BVP, SC, sEMG) were recorded during the whole video exposure. We hypothesized that the immersive video condition would increase the intensity of awe experienced compared to 2D screen videos. Results indicated that immersive videos significantly enhanced the self-reported intensity of awe as well as the sense of presence. Immersive videos displaying an awe content also led to higher parasympathetic activation. These findings indicate the advantages of using VR in the experimental study of awe, with methodological implications for the study of other emotions.

Introduction

The sight of stars, the image of earth from orbit, or witnessing childbirth are all experiences often elicit the emotion of awe. Awe is associated with deep feelings of wonder, astonishment, and sometimes fear – it can be both pleasurable and uncomfortable or overwhelming13. Awe is often involved when individuals face something that forces them to adjust their mental schema and to look for new patterns1, 4. According to Keltner and Haidt1 awe is induced by stimuli that are both vast and difficult to accommodate. Awe stimuli include vast and grand panoramas and sweeping vistas, as well as “grand theories” or big ideas.

Psychological study of awe has begun relatively recently. One of the main challenges concerning the experimental study of awe is the question of how to induce an intense feeling of this emotion in controlled settings5, 6. Previous research has suggested that the intensity of the induced emotion changes according to the specific induction procedure7. Recently, virtual reality (VR) has been proposed as a new technique to induce awe5. VR offers several theoretical advantages for eliciting and investigating high-intensity awe experiences. First, previous research has shown that VR-based perceptual stimulations – such as visual, auditorial, tactile and, when feasible, olfactive - can trigger strong emotional reactions810. Furthermore, VR is known to convey a sense of presence - the feeling of “being there” within an environment11, 12 – which has been found positively associated with the intensity of emotions experienced by participants8, 9, 13. From a methodological viewpoint, VR offers high degree of ecological validity: it allows researchers to simulate real-life contexts and situations, which can be used to study participants’ behaviours in highly-controlled laboratory conditions.

Here we tested the potential of immersive videos - the highest realistic form of Virtual Reality that excludes the dimensions of navigation14 - in inducing awe. We compared psychological and physiological responses to immersive displays of both awe inducing and neutral stimuli with the same content displayed on normal 2D screens. We hypothesized that immersive videos would be more effective than 2D screens at inducing awe, as measured through self-report and physiological measures.

Awe

Awe includes at least three subcomponents that differentiate it from other emotions. First, awe is induced from stimuli characterized by vastness and a need for accommodation – stimuli features unique to awe1, 4, 15. Second, awe is composed of several sub-components such as wonder, joy, fear, and reverence1, 15. Consequently, awe can encompass a positive and/or a negative valence: positive awe4 and/or negative awe15. Third, awe can diminish the sense of self, that is, participants report a sense of smallness in the face of something perceived as and larger than one’s self15, 16. This change to one’s sense of self mediates awe’s ability to increase prosocial behaviours towards strangers15, 17.

Awe affects a number of cognitive processes. For example, awe broadens attention18, increasing awareness of others17. Awe can also alter time perception19 and can lead to a decreased estimation of one’s body size20. Further, experimentally-induced awe leads to greater feelings of uncertainty21. Finally, awe increases feelings of connectedness with other people and can lead to greater satisfaction towards life3. In particular, the disposition to live awe frequently prevents from several health risks such as depression or stress-related disorders22.

Inducing awe in the lab

Inducing sufficiently intense awe experiences in the lab is a key challenge for researchers5, 6. Several emotion induction procedures have been used in previous research. Personal narratives consist of asking participants to recount first-hand experiences of awe vocally or in writing. This methodology has been effective for exploring several nuances awe, e.g., refs 4, 15, 23. A similar technique consists in asking participants to read short stories about prototypical awe elicitors, such as a sunset or a beautiful panorama15. Other techniques include the use of awe-induction images, such as grand views or natural phenomena, e.g., refs 6, 24. A more effective version of this technique relies on the use of videos, e.g., refs 15, 17, 25. Finally, awe has also been induced in experimental settings by exposing participants to natural environments (for example, a groove of eucalyptus; ref. 15 or buildings, e.g., refs 16, 26.

While these techniques have been generally effective in inducing awe, they have important limitations. Personal narratives generate recalled emotions that can be drastically different from those actually felt during the original event2729. This limitation is even more crucial in recalling awe experience, since awe is a complex emotional state characterized by a mix of sub-components. Moreover, awe can comprise both positive and negative feelings1, 4. Image and video-based awe induction techniques have the advantage of using standardized stimuli and offer high degree of control on the experimental setting. On the other hand, these techniques typically generate low-intensity emotions that may have limited ecological validity6. This limitation can be overcome by exposing participants to real-life awe-induction scenarios. However, this technique may be unpractical and requires careful control of potential intervenient factors.

In summary, several experimental procedures can induce awe. However, the challenge remains of how to increase the intensity of awe in controlled conditions in order to reproduce the complex processes that characterize the emotion. As Ellard, Farchione, and Barlow30 suggested, emotion induction techniques should be able to set “complex associated processes in motion” (p. 233) not only inducing that emotion, but also the “quality of the associated experience” is crucial (p. 233, italics in the original). In particular, in the case of awe, the authors stress the importance of inducing the genuine and high-intensity emotion outlined by qualitative reports and depicted in literature or art5, 6.

Assessing awe in experimental settings is another methodological challenge. Researchers have generally used psychometric self-report measures of awe. Problematically, however, many studies use single item measures of awe. There are exceptions, such as Schurtz et al.23 who assessed awe through a more complex self-reported measure that addressed the “vastness” and the “need for accommodation” component. However, a validated retrospective measure of awe addressing all its subcomponents does not yet exist. On the other hand, some researchers have used physiological measures. Oveis et al.31 and Shiota et al.24 used the valence-arousal model of emotions32, 33 which differentiates affective states according to two dimensions of physiological arousal and hedonic valence. They found evidence for a sympathetic withdrawal during awe. In general, combining both conventional retrospective self-reports with physiological assessment would be better than either on its own.

Virtual reality as awe-induction technique: the role of presence

VR is a technology that creates the perception of entering computer-generated interactive environments. Often, users can navigate these virtual spaces as if they were real physical spaces. This is achieved by combining different types of displays and stimuli (i.e. visual, auditory, tactile/haptic and sometimes gustative and olfactive) with sensors (i.e. head-tracking, hand tracking etc.) and controllers (hand held, treadmills for walking, etc.). Recently, VR has become of great interest for psychological researchers because of its ability to simulate real-life experiences in a controlled and safe laboratory setting. A further advantage of VR as an experimental tool is its ability to track participants’ behavior while exercising a high level of control on the stimuli delivered to the user. Finally, many forms of VR allow navigating inside the virtual environment by interacting with it. Because of these features, researchers can manipulate several parameters and variables of the participants’ simulated environment, according to the specific needs of the experimental design.

VR has already been used in some emotion-induction studies, for both clinical applications and basic research. In clinical applications, VR has been effective in inducing negative emotional states, such as fear and anxiety. For example, in VR-based treatment of phobias, simulated versions of threatening stimuli have been used to elicit the phobic responses and gradually extinguish them9. VR has also been used in stress-inoculation protocols to simulate and manage stressful situations9, 10, 34. Finally, VR has been used to explore affective and perceptual layers of specific pathologies, e.g., refs 35, 36. In non-clinical settings, VR has been successfully used as mood induction procedure (MIP) to elicit various discrete emotions8, 3739. For example, Felnhofer et al.38 were able to elicit specific affective states (joy, sadness, boredom, anger and anxiety) by exposing participants to different virtual park scenarios which differed in terms of weather, facial expressions on other people, music, and other parameters. The sense of presence, or the illusion of “being there” created by a virtual (or physical) world, is an important element in eliciting strong emotional responses11, 12. Although different theoretical models of presence have been proposed (for a review, see refs 40, 41), most authors agree that presence is a multidimensional phenomenon encompassing several sub-components40, 42. The first component is the feeling of physical space, i.e. the perception of being transported in another physical space. The second component is perceptual realism, which is how much the virtual stimuli resemble the real ones on which they are modelled10, 40. A further component is the extent to which users can feel surrounded by the environment, or the feeling of immersion 9, 40. This dimension is brought about by the capacity to experience a virtual environment from a immersive perspective and sensorial isolation from the real world9. Finally, a crucial component of presence is the degree of interest in a given virtual environment, i.e. their level of engagement 43.

These components of presence are generally associated to the intensity of emotions reported by participants. For example, Baños and colleagues8 found that more emotional content displayed in a virtual immersive environment increases engagement 43. Baños et al.13 found that both engagement and physical space components positively correlated with emotional intensity. Further, the component of immersion within a virtual setting increases the intensity of the emotional reaction9, 40. Generally, it emerged that the more immersive the VR experience is, the higher the levels of presence reported by participants40.

The current study

Previous attempts to elicit awe using VR environments have led to promising qualitative results44, 45 and while the capacity for VR to enhance quantitative measures of awe has been theorized5, it had not yet been directly tested experimentally. In the current study, we tested the potential of one of the highest realistic form of VR that excludes the dimensions of navigation with the virtual environment14. We induced strong feelings of awe by immersing participants in immersive video displays of vast and panoramic scenes of natural beauty from a 360° perspective.

The immersive video is a new video format typically recorded through an apparatus of multiple cameras, or using a specific VR camera composed of several camera lenses embedded into the device. It is used a specific software to integrate the raw footage into a coherent surrounding scene displayed on a Head Mounted Display14. This format is characterized by high degree of pictorial realism and the capacity to move around exploring the environment from an immersive perspective provides a strong illusion of depth, which allows for more immersive experiences. All these elements can support a strong feeling of presence, which in turn, can contribute to elicit a more intense emotion of awe. The main difference between immersive videos and the so-called 2D videos is related to the interaction between vestibular and visual systems. In 2D videos this interaction does not take place, whereas in the immersive videos the visual stimulus changes according to the vestibular one.

Shiota and colleagues4 postulate a specific psychophysiological pattern of awe, even if it has not been empirically tested yet24. Shiota et al.24 exposed participants to 10 positive emotions including awe, and a neutral control condition. They assessed six psychophysiological variables: Cardiac Interbeat Interval (IBI); Cardiac Pre-Ejection Period (PEP); Skin Conductance Responses (SCRs); Respiration Rate; Respiration Synus Arrhythmia (RSA); Mean Arterial Pressure (MAP). They found that awe led to a lengthening of PEP, compared to all the other conditions, as well as significantly lower levels of SCRs, in contrast with “amusement” and “anticipatory enthusiasm” conditions. Authors argued that PEP patterns of awe were consistent with the “sympathetic withdrawal”, that they had theorized. Importantly, cardiac measures, as well as the SCRs, emerged as one of the most relevant psychophysiological components for differentiating awe from other emotions. Nevertheless, although Shiota et al.24 and Oveis et al.31 theorized the role of parasympathetic system in differentiating awe from other emotions, they focused mainly on the sympathetic system.

Here we tested the potential of immersive videos - the highest realistic form of Virtual Reality that excludes the dimensions of navigation24 - in inducing awe. In different conditions, we used awe-inducing content and neutral content. The neutral content condition controlled for the effect of awe-inducing content, e.g., ref. 17. We hypothesized that the immersive presentation would increase awe more than a 2-D screen presentation. We also hypothesized that immersive experiences would enhance the feeling of awe more than non-immersive ones. We measured awe using an integrated methodology featuring both self-reported retrospective measures and physiological assessment of awe.

Results

Analyses were done using IBM SPSS Statistics software (Version 21, release 21.0.0.0 64 bit edition).

Awe, vastness and need for accommodation

H1: Immersive videos induce more intense awe than 2D screen videos.

We carried out a repeated measures ANOVA: 2 (media: 2D screen vs. immersive screen) × 2 (content: neutral vs. awe), with self-reported awe as a dependent measure. Results showed a significant main effect of “content” [(F (1,41) = 125.7; p < 0.001, η2 = 0.754)]: awe-inducing contents on immersive or on a 2D screen were significantly more awe-inducing than neutral ones displayed on immersive or on 2D screen. More, results indicated a significant main effect of “media” [F(1,40) = 34.793, p < 0.01; η2 = 0.153]. Immersive VR displaying awe content or neutral content, compared to the 2D screen videos depicting awe or neutral content elicited a significantly higher sense of self-reported awe. Results showed also a significant interaction effect: there was a more intense sense of awe in awe from immersive video [F(1,41) = 14.133; p < 0.01; η2 = 0.256]. In other words, it was the combination of immersion and awe-inducing content that resulted in the highest level of self-reported awe.

H2: Immersive videos induce a significantly higher sense of “perceived vastness” than 2D screen videos.

We carried out a repeated measures ANOVA: 2 (media: 2D screen vs. immersive screen) × 2 (content: neutral vs. awe) with “perceived vastness” as a measure. Results evidenced significant main effect of “content” [F(1,41) = 91.820; p < 0.01; η2 = 0.691]: Awe-inducing stimuli on immersive or on a 2D screen induced a more intense sense of vastness than neutral immersive video and neutral 2D screen video. Finally, there was also a significant main effect of “media” [(F(1,41) = 7.987; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.459)]. Results did not show a significant interaction effect [F(1,41) = 1.018; p = 0.339; η2 = 0.024]. In other words, immersive videos and awe-inducing video resulted in higher sense of vastness.

H3: Immersive videos induce a significantly higher “need for accommodation” than 2D screen videos.

We carried out a repeated measures ANOVA: 2 (media: 2D screen vs. immersive screen) × 2 (content: neutral vs. awe), with “perceived need for accommodation” as a measure. Results indicated a significant main effect of “content” [F(1,41) = 12.396; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.232]: awe-inducing stimuli displayed both in immersive or on a 2D screen elicited more intense need for accommodation than neutral immersive and 2D video. Moreover, results indicated a significant main effect of “media” [F(1,41) = 18.828 p < 0.001; η2 = 0.315]. Finally, we found a significant interaction effect [F(1,41) = 3.85; p = 0.057; η2 = 0.086)], that is that immersive VR combined with awe-inspiring content resulted in the most intense awe experience.

Sense of presence

H4: Immersive videos induce a significantly higher “physical space” and “engagement” than 2D screen videos.

We carried out a repeated measures ANOVA: 2 (media: 2D screen vs. immersive screen) × 2 (content: neutral vs. awe), with each of the dimensions of presence as measures (i.e., physical space and engagement). There was a main effect of media on physical space [F (1,41) = 150.581; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.79]: Awe-inducing immersive and 2D screen videos were able to significantly enhance the perceived sense of being physically present within the virtual environment more than neutral immersive and 2D screen videos.

There was a main effect of “content” on “engagement” [F (1,41) = 53.975; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.568]: awe-inducing stimuli displayed both on an immersive or 2D screen elicited more intense “engagement” than neutral immersive and neutral 2D video. Finally, there was also a significant main effect of “media” for “engagement” [F (1,41) = 102.801; η2 = 0.715], nonetheless, no significant interaction effect emerged [F(1,41) = 1.907; p = 0.175; η2 = 0.44]. These effects were in line with previous findings in literature which demonstrated the ability of VR to manipulate and enhance the general sense of space46, 47.

Descriptive statistics on awe, vastness, need for accommodation, sense of presence across conditions are shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics of Awe, Vastness, Need for Accommodation, Engagement, and Physical Presence.

Conditions Awe Vastness Need for accommodation Engagement Physical Presence
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Neutral 2D screen 1.500 0.890 1.815 0.876 1.500 0.800 1.620 0.512 1.853 0.783
Awe 2D screen 3.404 1.530 3.190 1.295 1.793 0.988 2.190 0.718 2.008 0.812
Neutral immersive screen 2.071 1.330 2.107 0.787 2.744 1.570 2.415 0.657 3.018 0.771
Awe immersive screen 5.119 1.533 3.756 1.250 2.738 1.575 3.181 0.636 3.326 0.729

Note. n = 42.

Corroborative measures of awe: psychophysiological measures

We reported psychophysiological results according to the two target dimensions of arousal and valence. Regarding arousal, we reported data concerning sympathetic and parasympathetic activation during video exposure. Results concerning valence referred specifically to the activity of Zygomatic Major Muscle and Corrugator Supercilii Muscle.

Repeated measure ANOVA: 2 (media: 2D screen vs. immersive screen) × 2 (content: neutral vs. awe) was carried out with respect to three main indexes of sympathetic and parasympathetic activation, and two indexes of valence. Two indexes referred to sympathetic activation (i.e., Very Low Frequency measures – VLF; Skin Conductance Responses), and one to the parasympathetic activation (High Frequency measure – HF). One index referred to awe negative valence (EMGa) and one to awe positive valence (EMGb). (Two participants’ physiological recordings were not available due to problems with sensors placement).

Sympathetic Autonomous System and awe

We carried out two separated repeated measure ANOVA: 2 (media: 2D screen vs. immersive screen) × 2 (content: neutral vs. awe) for each of the indexes of sympathetic activation. As regard cardiac activity, a significant main effect emerged for sympathetic activation with Very Low Frequency Total Power [F(1,37) = 7.019; p < 0.05; η2 = 0.159]: Very Low Frequency Total Power was significantly higher in the awe and neutral immersive condition compared to awe and neutral 2D conditions.

In terms of Skin Conductance Response, there was a significant main effect of “media” [F(1,37) = 4.590; p < 0.05; η2 = 0.108]: immersive VR displaying awe content or neutral content, compared to 2D screen videos depicting awe or neutral content induced a significantly greater Skin Conductance.

In other words, immersive VR were able to increase sympathetic activation significantly more than 2D screen videos. Indeed, these findings are in line with researches demonstrating the amplifying role of VR on Skin Conductance48, 49. Again, awe content alone did not result as being characterized by a sympathetic activation, thus we chose to deepen this aspect. Therefore, we analyzed the parasympathetic component of awe in order to understand whether it is characterized not only by a sympathetic withdrawal as Shiota et al.4 found, but also by a cholinergic activation.

Parasympathetic Autonomous System and Awe

We carried out a repeated measure ANOVA: 2 (media: 2D screen vs. immersive screen) × 2 (content: neutral vs. awe) with HF (High Frequency) Total Power as a measure. Results indicated a significant effect in the interaction between “media” and “content” [F (1,37) = 5.665; p < 0.05; η2 = 0.133]: immersive awe-inducing videos led to a significant increase in Total Power than or neutral immersive video, awe-inducing 2D screen videos or neutral 2D video. Despite this effect, no main effect of medium and content emerged. This confirmed the hypotheses of previous works on the psychophysiology of awe. In short, a sympathetic withdrawal occurred as well as a parasympathetic activation. This activation was more intense when awe was induced by immersive videos compared with 2D screen videos.

Descriptive statistics on awe, vastness, need for accommodation, sense of presence across conditions are shown in Table 2.

Table 2.

Descriptive Statistics of Very Low Frequency Total Power and Skin Conductance Response (Sympathetic indexes) and Total Power (Parasympathetic index).

Conditions Very Low Frequency Total Power Skin Conductance Response Total Power
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Neutral 2D screen 103.548 236.376 1.745 0.858 366.840 1327.376
Awe 2D screen 90.882 147.919 1.8567 1.066 250.035 775.613
Neutral immersive video 201.194 271.287 2.068 1.146 491.386 1219.688
Awe immersive video 237.309 35.664 2.4428 2.516 905.875 1979.276

Note. n = 40.

Hedonic Valence of awe

Results showed no main effect for “media” [F(1,38) = 0.323; p = 0.573; η2 = 0.08] or “content” [F(1,38) = 3.163; p = 0.083; η2 = 0.077] or interaction significant effect occurred between media and content in the Supercilii activity [F(1,38) = 0.493; p = 0.487; η2 = 0.013]. Results showed that no main effect for “media” [F(1,38) = 0.767; p = 0.387; η2 = 0.020] or “content” [F(1,38) = 1.782; p = 0.179; η2 = 0.047] or interaction significant effect occurred between media and content in the Zygomaticus activity [F(1,38) = 0.105; p = 0.748; η2 = 0.003]. Taken together, these results could suggest a more complex intrinsic pattern of valence characterizing awe as a unique positive emotion in that awe did not appear to produce facial muscle changes commonly associated with presentation of pleasant stimuli.

Discussion

“Literature demonstrated that the more immersive the scenario is, the more intense is the subsequent emotional state elicited9. Specifically, immersive scenarios can increase the sense of presence, or the illusion of “being there” created by a virtual (or physical) world, thus eliciting strong emotional responses11, 12. In other words, presence emerged as an amplifier of emotional responses. Therefore, the primary aim of this study was to test whether a higher realistic form of VR can elicit more intense experiences of awe in the lab. Towards this end, we utilized an integrated methodology combining both retrospective and physiological measures. This combination allowed us to advance the investigation of awe’s physiological correlates. We found that immersive effectively increases the intensity of awe experiences compared to normal 2D videos. Moreover, VR increased the sense of engagement, the sense of physical space, and the perception of vastness, each of which increased self-reported awe.

Furthermore, several physiological measurements were recorded, which resulted in various findings. For Skin Conductance Responses, it was the medium, not the content, that was primarily responsible for alterations on this measure. We also tested Shiota et al.’s4 hypothesis about a parasympathetic activation of awe and found that awe-inducing content displayed on immersive video elicited a stronger parasympathetic activation compared to other immersive contents. However, this result was in line with Shiota et al.4 who found a momentary β-adrenergic activation using 2D screen videos. We adopted a methodology able to strengthen awe intensity, and this was evident also from psychophysiological measures. Awe parasympathetic activation emerged more clearly than in Shiota et al.4. Given the activating potential of immersive VR environments9, 40, it is more surprising that the combination of VR and awe-inspiring contents led to a more intense parasympathetic activation, instead of sympathetic one. These findings highlighted the VR was able to elicit a more intense awe even in the lab.

Finally, we did not find any significant difference between Corrugator Supercilii and Zygomatic Major muscles activity. Awe emerged as an “ambivalent” emotion – as far as facial muscle activity goes - in which positive and negative muscle tones were blended. This could be interpreted as the first experimental evidence of the complex nature of valence in awe, as compared with classical models of emotions50.

More, VR offers several pathways of exploration for awe researchers. For example, one could alter the dimension of vastness by manipulating the level of presence. Other manipulations are possible, such as altering the level of interactivity in environments. Awe has resulted more as a “stimulus-oriented” emotion (i.e., an emotion induced by non-human elicitors) than a “other-oriented” emotion, (i.e., an emotion elicited by social-interactional stimuli)17. However, VR could offer the possibility to investigate the interactional side in a controlled setting. For example, it could be examined how progressively engaging levels of interaction with the environment or with other virtual characters, could enhance the intensity of awe experience or could affect hedonic tune of this experience.

Moreover, VR offers the possibility to investigate the two cognitive appraisals of awe more deeply. For example, as Huron51, Silvia52 and Chirico et al.5 indicated, awe can be conceived as a particular form of surprise, specifically regarding the need for accommodation component5. The basic mechanism is the violation of expectations, which includes several forms of violations51, 53. VR allows creating different versions of expectancy violations. For example, paradoxical scenarios could be reproduced in VR with a high experimental control, such as the illusion of time travelling54, or a strong sensorial discrepancy35. Further, given the relevance of mental-schema violations, this experimental paradigm could be easily replicated for studying the impact of intense feelings of awe on processes based on this mechanism such as creativity39, 55 both at the individual and at the group level56.

Furthermore, here we focused mainly on a visual stimulation of awe. However, it could be useful to analyze the impact of other sensorial channels on awe emergence. For example, music has been demonstrated as an effective inductor of awe51, 53 and other complex states, e.g., refs 57, 58. However, specific musical features responsible for awe elicitation have not been investigated yet. With this regard, music can be used for two purposes. First, it could be tested how auditory stimuli, such as selected musical pieces, combined with VR could improve awe induction. Second, it could be possible to investigate the best combination between specific musical violations and immersive virtual experiences. Finally, a more visionary perspective could be to visually translate musical features into concurrent visual stimuli creating extremely engaging experiences of awe.

In terms of other measures, VR offers the opportunity for future investigations of the dynamics of awe using neuroimaging. These include Electroencephalography (EEG), Near Infrared Spectroscopy (NIRS), and Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). In this last case other forms of VR could be used, for example a CAVE in which the participants are physically immersed and surrounded by screens on which the images are back-projected59.

Finally, awe has been conceived as one of the key components of a sudden and enduring personal change60 which can be supported by the use of VR61, 62. Therefore, a future step could be analyzing the long-term effects of VR induced awe. With this regard, it could be insightful to consider a long-term measure of awe such as the variations related to the endocrine system. Specifically, it could be useful detecting enduring changes after awe exposure and not only in terms of awe proneness, as it has been successfully done22.

Limitations

Despite the potential of VR in inducing a more intense version of awe, some limitations exist. This study could be improved mainly regarding a measurement aspect. This research was based on a single-item self-reported measure of awe, in line with literature on this emotion, e.g., refs 4, 15, 21. However, we addressed this issue by integrating the self-reported assessment with a psychophysiological measurement of awe. Nevertheless, we considered only the peripheral system, and did not investigate how this interacts with the central nervous system. More, we focused on two naturalistic awe-inducing contents, but it would be possible to include also social stimuli, such as a crowded space or the presence of a relevant person. Finally, it could be useful also to consider if different naturals scenarios, such as negative natural phenomena, or interactional experiences could impact similarly on awe induction.

Conclusions

This study has important implications for emotion research methodology. First, VR does enhance the intensity of awe experiences in laboratory setting. Second, VR allows researchers to modulate various dimensions of awe induction stimuli, thus teasing apart different subcomponents of awe.

VR offers to opportunity to observe human responses in simulated as well as completely novel environments that feel quite real to participants. We now have the capability to understand how human beings respond to mundane, unusual, dangerous, and awe-inspiring circumstances – all from the safety of the laboratory.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The study included 42 participants, who all voluntarily took part in the study (22 females − mean age = 22.82; S.D. = 2.343; 20 males − mean age = 22.3; S.D. = 2.7). Participants were undergraduate students recruited through campus announcements at an Italian University. Participants who (at the time of the experiment) reported vestibular and/or balance disorders were excluded. Only two participants had tried immersive videos using HMDs. Thus, we considered this variable irrelevant for the analysis. The experimental protocol was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore prior to data collection. Each participant provided written informed consent for study participation. Written consent and all methods were carried out in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration.

Stimuli

Stimuli Selection

Awe-inducing and neutral content was used. This content was selected after a preliminary study that tested the effectiveness of various content for awe elicitation in a separate sample of 36 participants (Chirico et al., in press)63. In this study, participants watched at 4 video contents: (i) amusing; (ii) awe-inspiring 1 (showing a grand vista on the mountains); (iii) awe-inspiring 2 (depicting a scene of tall trees in a forest); (iv) neutral (hens wandering on grass). Each participant watched at each video once in a counterbalanced order. Participants then rated the extent to which they experienced several different emotional states such as Anger, Awe, Amusement, Disgust, Fear, Pride, Sadness, and Joy. Each of these videos was created using ShotCut video-editing free online tool. Results indicated that the video depicting a scene of tall trees in a forest was the most effective for eliciting awe, and that video of hens wandering on grass did not induce awe (i.e., this video elicited low levels of each assessed emotion) - thus we used these two videos for the current study.

Selected stimuli and contents

The two videos chosen – an awe-inducing video and a neutral video - were manipulated to the two different mediums of display. Each video was displayed as immersive VR or on a 2D screen. This resulted in 4 conditions:

  • Neutral video on a 2D screen;

  • Awe-inducing video on a 2D screen;

  • Neutral video on immersive screen;

  • Awe-inducing video on immersive screen.

Specifically, each of the four videos was composed of the following subsections: (i) a black screen lasting 6000 milliseconds; (ii) a sound (lasting 500 milliseconds) that served as a signal to the experimenter to start physiological recordings; (iii) a black screen lasting 8000 milliseconds after the sound; (iv) the beginning of the video.

Videos were displayed using Samsung Gear VR, a head mounted virtual reality display. Each video lasted 2 minutes (excluding the i, ii, iii subsections). KolorEyes App was used to manipulate the dimension of immersion, by using the “immersive” option (to activate immersive display) or “2D” to display video on a 2D screen. Kolor Eyes 1.5 App is a free immersive video-player for Windows, Mac, HTML5, iOS and Android. This app allows tracking participants’ head orientation both in the 2D screen condition and in the immersive video condition.

Measures

Self-reported measures

After video exposure, participants were required to report the extent to which they experienced awe, presence, the sense of vastness and the need for accommodation, as follows:

  • (i)

    Awe was assessed with a single item likert self-report measure among other items measuring eight distinct emotions (from 1 = not at all; to 7 = extremely): Anger; Awe; Disgust; Fear; Pride; Sadness, Amusement and Joy. This questionnaire was used to obtain a measure of “global perceived awe”.

  • (ii)

    Presence was assessed using two sub-scales (“Engagement” and “Physical Space”) of the ITC-Sense of Presence Inventory (ITC-SOPI)43. The ITC-SOPI is a 42-items on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree; 5 = Strongly agree) questionnaire. This questionnaire is composed of four subscales which demonstrated good internal consistency, showing a Cronbach Alpha ranging between 0.76 and 0.94: Sense of Physical Space (0.94); Engagement (0.89); Ecological Validity (0.76); Negative Effects (0.77). We focused on the two subscales of Physical Space and Engagement since they have already resulted relevant regarding emotional intensity.

  • (iii)

    Perceived vastness was assessed using four items: 1. What I watched provided me with a deep sense of vastness; 2. I felt small in front of what I watched; 3. I felt meaningless in front of what I saw; 4. I felt my sense of self diminish in front of what I saw). Cronbach Alpha = 0.77.

  • (iv)

    Perceived need for accommodation was assessed using four items: 1. It was hard to grasp what was going on in the video; 2. I felt confused and bewildered in front of what saw; 3. I was struck by the video). Cronbach Alpha = 0.81.

This questionnaire, which included perceived vastness and perceived need for accommodation dimensions, was created according to the guidelines provided by Schurtz et al.23 and Piff et al.15.

Psychophysiological measures

The ongoing experience of awe was assessed through physiological measures that have been used in previous studies on awe24, 31. Since our approach is new, we sought to corroborate the assessment of awe with self-report and physiological measures. Our aim was two-fold. First, we chose to draw from previous findings for detecting awe using physiological measures. Moreover, we decided to advance the psychophysiological knowledge on awe by paying more attention to the role of the parasympathetic system. To these ends, we measured peripheral nervous system (PNS) activation by using various wearable noninvasive biosensors:

  • A biosensor to record Skin Conductance Response (SCR). SCR depends on the activity of the sweat gland which is controlled by the sympathetic nervous system. It is an index of psychophysiological arousal64. We recorded SCRs with two electrodes placed on the palmar surfaces of the distal phalanges of the index and ring fingers of the dominant hand. Skin Conductance (SC) is expressed in microsiemens (µS) representing the average of the cleaned signal during a given experimental epoch.

  • Blood Volume Pulse (BVP) is a signal obtained through a photoplethysmograph biosensor, which measures fluctuations in blood volume in a specific tissue with a light-emitting diode. The amount of infrared light transmitted to the photoplethysmograph is a function of the amount of blood saturating specific tissue regions. BVP was recorded to measure complex cardiovascular activity to gather information on sympathetic and parasympathetic activations during experimental epochs.

  • Two surface electromyography (sEMG) biosensors recorded muscular automatic micro-contractions of both the Corrugator Supercilii Muscle (following corrugator) and the Zygomatic Major Muscle (following zygomatic). Corrugator activity is sensitive to unpleasant stimuli65, and does not depend on the awareness of the eliciting stimulus66. We used this measure as an index of positive and negative emotional valence as Zygomatic activity has been shown to respond to pleasant stimuli65, 67.

A ProComp Infinity 8-channel (Thought Technology Ltd, Montreal, Canada) was used to record all physiological measures during each video session (the experimental epochs), with a sampling rate at 256 Hz for BVP and SCR and at a 2048 Hz for the two EMG. Heart rate variability (HRV) measures were calculated through a custom script in Matlab 7.10.0 (R2010a).

Inter-Beat Interval (IBI) was extracted from the Blood Volume Pulse sensor. It consisted in a measure comparable with the R-R peaks interval extracted from the electrocardiogram. According to the guidelines of Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology68, typical temporal and spectral HRV measures (by the means of Fourier spectral methods) were extracted to evaluate the response of the autonomic nervous system. The rhythms were considered as very low frequency (VLF < 0.04 Hz), low-frequency (LF, 0.04 to 0.15 Hz), and high frequency (HF, 0.15 to 0.4 Hz) oscillations.

About the sEMG, since the raw electromyography is a collection of positive and negative electrical signals, their frequency and amplitude provide information on the contraction or rest state of the muscle. Amplitude is measured in microvolts (μV). As the subject contracts a muscle, the number and amplitude of the lines increase, and, as the muscle relaxes, the number and amplitude of the lines decrease. We considered the Root Mean Square (RMS) to rectify the raw signal and converted it to an amplitude envelope. According to Blumenthal and colleagues69, facial EMG corrugator and zygomatic can be considered the best measure for negative and positive emotion valence, respectively.

Procedure

First, participants provided informed consent document. Then, participants were provided both by a written and an oral description of the study, and sensors for physiological measurement were applied. The protocol included 4 video-viewing trials. Each participant watched each video once in a counterbalanced order. During video exposure, cardiovascular activity (with BVP), electrodermal response (with SCR), and facial muscular activity (corrugator and zygomatic) were recorded. Specifically, a baseline measure was obtained (3 min length) while they were sitting comfortably. Participants then put on a virtual reality head-mounted display (i.e., Samsung Gear VR for Samsung Galaxy Note 4) and they received standardized instructions about how to use VR. When participants indicated that they were ready to begin Figs 1, 2 and 3 the experimenter touched the lateral pad to start the video. After each video exposure, participants completed the self-report ratings described above. This procedure was repeated four times, one time for each condition, with each participant. Participants were instructed to explore the video freely and to have their arms lie in the same position for all the experimental session. The entire experiment lasted about 55 minutes.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Interaction effect of media and content with awe as a measure. Error bars indicate standard errors of the means.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Interaction effect of medium and condition with HF Total power as a measure. Error bars indicate standard errors of the means.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

The sequence of video subsections. Image of Tall trees was taken from Pixabay (credits: Pixabay, https://pixabay.com/it/sequoia-foresta-redwood-274158/).

Author Contributions

Authors contributed according to their competences and interests. A.C. and A.G. conceived the main idea of the article. A.C. collected all data. P.C. carried out statistical analyses and signal processing. A.C. wrote the first draft of the manuscript, while P.C., G.R., F.B., and D.B.Y. contributed to the final writing of the manuscript by giving suggestions regarding the issues related to the rhetoric and to the literature. A.G. supervised the entire work. All authors contributed to the manuscript, read, and approved the final version.

Competing Interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Keltner D, Haidt J. Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cogn. Emot. 2003;17:297–314. doi: 10.1080/02699930302297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Yaden DB, et al. The overview effect: Awe and self-transcendent experience in space flight. Psychol Conscious. 2016;3:1. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Krause N, Hayward RD. Assessing whether practical wisdom and awe of God are associated with life satisfaction. Psycholog. Relig. Spiritual. 2015;7:51–59. doi: 10.1037/a0037694. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Shiota MN, Keltner D, Mossman A. The nature of awe: Elicitors, appraisals, and effects on self-concept. Cogn. Emot. 2007;21:944–963. doi: 10.1080/02699930600923668. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Chirico, A., Yaden, D., Riva, G. & Gaggioli, A. The Potential of Virtual Reality for the Investigation of Awe. Frontiers in Psychology7, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01766 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 6.Silvia PJ, Fayn K, Nusbaum EC, Beaty RE. Openness to experience and awe in response to nature and music: Personality and profound aesthetic experiences. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts. 2015;9:376–384. doi: 10.1037/aca0000028. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ray, R. D. & Gross, J. J. Emotion elicitation using filmsInHandbook of emotion elicitation and assessment (eds Coan, A. & Allen, J. J. B.) 9–28 (Oxford University Press, 2007).
  • 8.Baños RM, et al. Immersion and emotion: their impact on the sense of presence. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2004;7:734–741. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2004.7.734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Diemer, J., Alpers, G. W., Peperkorn, H. M., Shiban, Y. & Mühlberger, A. The impact of perception and presence on emotional reactions: a review of research in virtual reality. Front. Psychol. 6, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00026 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 10.Parsons, T. D. Virtual Reality for Enhanced Ecological Validity and Experimental Control in the Clinical, Affective and Social Neurosciences. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00660 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 11.Riva, G. & Waterworth, J. A. Presence and the Self: A cognitive neuroscience approach. Presence connect3 (2003).
  • 12.Riva G, Waterworth JA, Waterworth EL. The layers of presence: a bio-cultural approach to understanding presence in natural and mediated environments. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2004;7:402–416. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2004.7.402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Baños RM, et al. Presence and emotions in virtual environments: The influence of stereoscopy. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2008;11:1–8. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2007.9936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Slater M, Sanchez-Vives MV. Enhancing Our Lives with Immersive Virtual Reality. Frontiers in Robotics and AI. 2016;3:74. doi: 10.3389/frobt.2016.00074. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Piff PK, Dietze P, Feinberg M, Stancato DM, Keltner D. Awe, the small self, and prosocial behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2015;108:883–99. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Joye Y, Dewitte S. Up speeds you down. Awe-evoking monumental buildings trigger behavioral and perceived freezing. J. Environ. Psychol. 2016;47:112–125. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.05.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Prade, C. & Saroglou, V. Awe’s effects on generosity and helping. J Posit Psychol, 1–9 (2016).
  • 18.Sung, B. & Yih, J. Does interest broaden or narrow attentional scope? Cogn. Emot. 1–10, doi:10.1080/02699931.2015.1071241 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 19.Rudd M, Vohs KD, Aaker J. Awe expands people’s perception of time, alters decision making, and enhances well-being. Psychol. Sci. 2012;23:1130–1136. doi: 10.1177/0956797612438731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.van Elk, M., Karinen, A., Specker, E., Stamkou, E. & Baas, M. ‘Standing in Awe’: The Effects of Awe on Body Perception and the Relation with Absorption. Collabra2, doi:10.1525/collabra.36 (2016).
  • 21.Valdesolo, P. & Graham, J. Awe, uncertainty, and agency detection (vol. 25, pg 170, 2014) Psychol. Sci25, 844–844 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 22.Stellar JE, et al. Positive affect and markers of inflammation: Discrete positive emotions predict lower levels of inflammatory cytokines. Emotion. 2015;15:129–33. doi: 10.1037/emo0000033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Schurtz DR, et al. Exploring the social aspects of goose bumps and their role in awe and envy. Motiv. Emot. 2012;36:205–217. doi: 10.1007/s11031-011-9243-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Shiota MN, Neufeld SL, Yeung WH, Moser SE, Perea EF. Feeling good: autonomic nervous system responding in five positive emotions. Emotion. 2011;11:1368–78. doi: 10.1037/a0024278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Van Cappellen P, Saroglou V. Awe activates religious and spiritual feelings and behavioral intentions. Psycholog. Relig. Spiritual. 2012;4:223–236. doi: 10.1037/a0025986. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Negami, H. Awe-Inducing Interior Space: Architectural Causes and Cognitive Effects (2016).
  • 27.Aaker J, Drolet A, Griffin D. Recalling mixed emotions. J. Consum. Res. 2008;35:268–278. doi: 10.1086/588570. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Robinson MD, Clore GL. Belief and feeling: evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychol. Bull. 2002;128:934–960. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Levine LJ, Safer MA. Sources of bias in memory for emotions. Curr. Di.r Psychol. Sci. 2002;11:169–173. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00193. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ellard KK, Farchione TJ, Barlow DH. Relative effectiveness of emotion induction procedures and the role of personal relevance in a clinical sample: a comparison of film, images, and music. J. Psychopatho.l Behav. 2012;34:232–243. doi: 10.1007/s10862-011-9271-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Oveis C, et al. Resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia is associated with tonic positive emotionality. Emotion. 2009;9:265–70. doi: 10.1037/a0015383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Russell JA, Barrett LF. Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: dissecting the elephant. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1999;76:805–819. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Lang PJ. The emotion probe: studies of motivation and attention. Am. Psychol. 1995;50:372–385. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.50.5.372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Serino S, et al. Toward a validation of cyber-interventions for stress disorders based on stress inoculation training: a systematic review. Virtual Real. 2014;18:73–87. doi: 10.1007/s10055-013-0237-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Serino S, et al. Virtual reality body swapping: A tool for modifying the allocentric memory of the body. Cyberpsych. Beh. Soc. N. 2016;19:127–133. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2015.0229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Pallavicini F, et al. Testing augmented reality for cue exposure in obese patients: an exploratory study. Cyberpsych. Beh. Soc. N. 2016;19:107–114. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2015.0235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Baños RM, et al. Positive mood induction procedures for virtual environments designed for elderly people. Interact Comput. 2012;24:131–138. doi: 10.1016/j.intcom.2012.04.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Felnhofer A, et al. Is virtual reality emotionally arousing? Investigating five emotion inducing virtual park scenarios. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 2015;82:48–56. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2015.05.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Serrano B, Botella C, Baños RM, Alcañiz M. Using virtual reality and mood-induction procedures to test products with consumers of ceramic tiles. Comput. Human Behav. 2013;29:648–653. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.10.024. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Coelho C, Tichon J, Hine TJ, Wallis G, Riva G. Media presence and inner presence: the sense of presence in virtual reality technologies. From communication to presence: Cognition, emotions and culture towards the ultimate communicative experience. 2006;11:25–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Riva, G. & Waterworth, J. A. Being present in a virtual world in The oxford handbook of virtuality (ed. Grimshaw, M.) 205–221 (Oxford University Press, 2014).
  • 42.Lessiter, J., Freeman, J., Keogh, E. & Davidoff, J. In III international workshop on PresenceTechnical University of Delft, Delft (2000).
  • 43.Lessiter J, Freeman J, Keogh E, Davidoff J. A cross-media presence questionnaire: The ITC-Sense of Presence Inventory. Presence. 2001;10:282–297. doi: 10.1162/105474601300343612. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Gallagher, S., Janz, B., Reinerman, L., Bockelman, P. & Trempler, J. A Neurophenomenology of Awe and Wonder: Towards a Non-Reductionist Cognitive Science (Palgrave Macmillan, 2015).
  • 45.Gallagher S, Reinerman-Jones L, Sollins B, Janz B. Using a simulated environment to investigate experiences reported during space travel. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science. 2014;15:376–394. doi: 10.1080/1463922X.2013.869370. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Riva, G., Waterworth, J. & Murray, D. Interacting with Presence: HCI and the Sense of Presence in Computer-mediated Environments (de Gruyter, 2014).
  • 47.Riva G. Is presence a technology issue? Some insights from cognitive sciences. Virtual Reality. 2009;13:159–169. doi: 10.1007/s10055-009-0121-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Schuemie MJ, Van Der Straaten P, Krijn M, Van Der Mast CA. Research on presence in virtual reality: A survey. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 2001;4:183–201. doi: 10.1089/109493101300117884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Wiederhold, B. K., Davis, R., & Wiederhold, M. D. The effect of immersiveness on physiology. Virtual Environments in Clinical Psychology and Neuroscience. In Virtual Environments in Clinical Psychology and Neuroscience (eds Riva, G., Wiederhold, B. K., Molinari, E.) 52–60 (IOS Press, 1998).
  • 50.Trnka R, Lačev A, Balcar K, Kuška M, Tavel P. Modelling semantic emotion space using a 3D hypercube-projection: an innovative analytical approach for the psychology of emotions. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:522. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Huron, D. B. Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation (MIT press, 2006).
  • 52.Silvia PJ. Confusion and interest: The role of knowledge emotions in aesthetic experience. Psychol. Aesthet. Crea.t Arts. 2010;4:75–80. doi: 10.1037/a0017081. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lorini E, Castelfranchi C. The cognitive structure of surprise: looking for basic principles. Topoi. 2007;26:133–149. doi: 10.1007/s11245-006-9000-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Friedman D, et al. A method for generating an illusion of backwards time travel using immersive virtual reality-an exploratory study. Front. Psychol. 2014;5:943. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Damian, R. I. & Simonton, D. K. Four Psychological Perspectives on Creativity. Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences: An Interdisciplinary, Searchable, and Linkable Resource, doi:10.1002/9781118900772.etrds0134 (2015).
  • 56.Gaggioli A, Mazzoni E, Milani L, Riva G. The creative link: Investigating the relationship between social network indices, creative performance and flow in blended teams. Comput. Human Behav. 2015;42:157–166. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.12.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Chirico, A., Serino, S., Cipresso, P., Gaggioli, A. & Riva, G. When music “flows”. State and trait in musical performance, composition and listening: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 6, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00906 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 58.Gaggioli A, Chirico A, Mazzoni E, Milani L, Riva G. Networked Flow in musical bands. Psychol. Music. 2017;45:283–297. doi: 10.1177/0305735616665003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Cruz-Neira, C., Sandin, D. J. & DeFanti, T. A. In Proceedings of the 20th annual conference on Computer graphics and interactive techniques. 135–142 (ACM).
  • 60.Schneider, K. J. Awakening to Awe: Personal Stories of Profound Transformation (Jason Aronson, 2009).
  • 61.Riva, G. et al. Positive and transformative technologies for active ageing. In Studies in Health Technology and Informatics Vol. 220, 308–315 (IOS Press, 2016). [PubMed]
  • 62.Riva, G., Baños, R. M., Botella, C., Mantovani, F. & Gaggioli, A. Transforming Experience: The Potential of Augmented Reality and Virtual Reality for Enhancing Personal and Clinical Change. Front. Psychiatry7, doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00164 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 63.Chirico, A., Cipresso, P., Gaggioli, A. A Process for Selecting and Validating Awe-Inducing Audio-Visual Stimuli. In 6th EAI International Symposium on Pervasive Computing Paradigms for Mental Health (Barcellona, in press).
  • 64.Boucsein, W. Electrodermal activity (Springer Science & Business Media, 2012).
  • 65.Larsen JT, Norris CJ, Cacioppo JT. Effects of positive and negative affect on electromyographic activity over zygomaticus major and corrugator supercilii. Psychophysiology. 2003;40:776–785. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Dimberg U, Thunberg M, Elmehed K. Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychol. Sci. 2000;11:86–89. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Larsen, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Poehlmann, K. M., Ito, T. A. & Cacioppo, J. T. The psychophysiology of emotion. Handbook of emotions (eds Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M. & Feldman Barrett, L.) Ch. 11, 180–195 (2008).
  • 68.Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation93, 1043–1065 (1996). [PubMed]
  • 69.Blumenthal TD, Cuthbert BN, Filion DL, Hackley S, Lipp OV, Van Boxtel A. Committee report: Guidelines for human startle eyeblink electromyographic studies. Psychophysiology. 2005;42:1–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00271.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES