Skip to main content
Nature Communications logoLink to Nature Communications
. 2017 May 15;8:15230. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15230

Neural circuits for long-term water-reward memory processing in thirsty Drosophila

Wei-Huan Shyu 1, Tai-Hsiang Chiu 1, Meng-Hsuan Chiang 2, Yu-Chin Cheng 1, Ya-Lun Tsai 1, Tsai-Feng Fu 3, Tony Wu 4, Chia-Lin Wu 1,2,4,a
PMCID: PMC5440665  PMID: 28504254

Abstract

The intake of water is important for the survival of all animals and drinking water can be used as a reward in thirsty animals. Here we found that thirsty Drosophila melanogaster can associate drinking water with an odour to form a protein-synthesis-dependent water-reward long-term memory (LTM). Furthermore, we found that the reinforcement of LTM requires water-responsive dopaminergic neurons projecting to the restricted region of mushroom body (MB) β′ lobe, which are different from the neurons required for the reinforcement of learning and short-term memory (STM). Synaptic output from α′β′ neurons is required for consolidation, whereas the output from γ and αβ neurons is required for the retrieval of LTM. Finally, two types of MB efferent neurons retrieve LTM from γ and αβ neurons by releasing glutamate and acetylcholine, respectively. Our results therefore cast light on the cellular and molecular mechanisms responsible for processing water-reward LTM in Drosophila.


Distinct subsets of dopaminergic PAM neurons have been shown to be involved in short-term and long-term memory for sugar reward. Here the authors report the neural circuits and the cellular and molecular mechanisms for short-term and long-term memory for water reward in thirsty Drosophila.


In the past 40 years, Pavlovian conditioning has been widely used to study memory formation at the molecular and circuit level in the fruit fly, Drosophila. The formation of olfactory aversive long-term memory (LTM) in Drosophila requires 5–10 multiple training sessions (with inter-trial intervals) pairing odour with electric shock1,2. However, hungry Drosophila can be trained with a single 2-min training session pairing odour with sucrose to form a robust appetitive sugar-reward LTM3,4. In addition, a recent study has indicated that thirsty Drosophila can also be trained with a single 2-min training session pairing odour with water to form a water-reward memory5.

Physiological and behavioural studies have demonstrated that dopaminergic neurons signal rewarding events in the brains of both mammals and Drosophila6,7,8. There are about 120–130 dopaminergic protocerebral anterior medial (PAM) neurons, and the output of these PAM neurons is mainly restricted to the horizontal lobes of the mushroom bodies (MBs)7,8,9. These dopaminergic PAM neurons convey sugar reinforcements for olfactory memory in the fly brain7,8, and two recent studies have indicated that short-term and long-term sugar reinforcements are delivered via distinct populations of dopaminergic PAM neurons in the fly brain10,11.

Here we found that flies form a water-reward long-lasting memory that requires protein synthesis and LTM-related gene expression. Consistent with findings relating to water-reward short-term memory, LTM also requires the normal function of the osmosensitive ion channel Pickpocket 28 (PPK28) for water tasting12. Adult-stage-specific expression of a dCREB2-b repressor in MBs, the centre of olfactory learning and memory in the insect brain, abolished LTM while leaving short-term memory (STM) intact. A recent study identified that dopaminergic PAM-γ4 neurons process water reinforcement signal into MBs in learning5. Interestingly, we found that LTM utilized a distinct population of dopaminergic PAM neurons innervating the MB β′1 region that differ from those involved in the reinforcement of learning and STM. Moreover, LTM required normal expression of the D1-like dopamine DopR1 receptor in α′β′ neurons of MBs. The MBs are composed of thousands of neurons with axonal projections and these can be divided into three different cell types, αβ, α′β′ and γ neurons13,14. Here, we have determined that the output from α′β′ neurons is required for consolidation, whereas the output from γ and αβ neurons is required for the retrieval of LTM. Finally, the outputs from MB-M6 (MBON-γ5β′2a) and MB-V3 (MBON-α3) neurons are required for the retrieval of LTM with the release of glutamate and acetylcholine, respectively.

Results

A single training session pairing water with odour forms LTM

To test whether flies form robust water-reward memories, we conditioned flies that were water deprived for 16 h at 25 °C by simultaneously delivering one of two training odours along with water in a T-maze. We observed a robust memory score after a single 2-min training session, and found no significant decline in memory performance within 32 h (Fig. 1a; Supplementary Figs 1–3). It has been shown that water-reward learning (3-min memory) requires water tasting via the osmosensitive ion channel PPK28 (refs 5, 12; Fig. 1b), so we further asked whether the formation of later parts of memory also need normal ppk28 expression. We found that flies with homozygous ppk28 mutations had defective 3-h and 24-h memories (Fig. 1b). This memory lasted to 24 h after a single water conditioning session, which led us to ask whether this robust 24-h memory constitutes LTM. LTM formation in all animals requires de novo protein synthesis15,16,17,18. Previous studies have demonstrated that 24-h memories after shock-punishment or sugar-reward conditioning require de novo protein synthesis in Drosophila1,4, which prompted us to examine whether 24-h water-reward memory requires de novo protein synthesis. We administered the protein synthesis inhibitor cycloheximide (CXM) 16 h before and after water conditioning, and found a marked decline in memory performance at 24 h but not 3 min or 3 h after conditioning, suggesting that the 24-h memory does constitute LTM (Fig. 1c).

Figure 1. Flies form water-reward LTM.

Figure 1

(a) Olfactory memory curve after water conditioning in wild-type flies. After a single training session, different fly populations were tested once for water memory 0 (3 min), 1, 3, 6, 12, 24 and 32 h after training. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=9, 8, 8, 8, 8, 8 and 7 from left to right points). P>0.05; analysis of variance (ANOVA). (b) PPK28 is required for 3-min, 3-h and 24-h water-reward memories. Genotypes are wild-type (WT) and ppk28 mutant flies, respectively. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8, 8, 10, 10, 8 and 8 from left to right bars). *P<0.05; t-test. (c) Flies were either fed 35 mM CXM in dry sucrose (+CXM) or dry sucrose alone (−CXM) during the water-deprivation period before and after training (see Methods). Different fly populations were tested once for 3-min, 3-h or 24-h memory. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8 for each bar). *P<0.05; t-test. (d) The 3-min and 3-h memories were unaffected by crammer (cer), tequila (teq), or radish (rsh) mutations. 24-h memory performances of crammer, tequila and radish mutant flies were significantly different from those of wild-type (WT) flies. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=9, 8, 8, 8, 12, 8, 8, 8, 11, 8, 9 and 8 from left to right bars). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. (e) Adult-stage-specific expression of the dCREB2-b repressor in MBs abolished 24-h memory, leaving 3-min and 3-h memory intact. Left panel, the R13F02-GAL4 expression pattern (green). The brain was immunostained using DLG antibody (magenta). Scale bar, 50 μm. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8 for each bar). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test.

The Drosophila crammer gene encodes an inhibitor of the cathepsin subfamily of cysteine proteases and the tequila gene encodes a Drosophila neurotrypsin ortholog, both of which are involved in shock-punishment LTM19,20. The Drosophila radish gene encodes 23 predicted cAMP-dependent protein kinase (PKA) phosphorylation sequences and the protein may function as a small GTPase that plays a role in shock-punishment memory formation21. It has also been shown that the crammer, tequila and radish genes are involved in sugar-reward LTM formation in Drosophila4. We therefore asked whether radish, crammer and tequila genes are involved in water-reward LTM. We found that LTM was severely disrupted in radish, crammer and tequila mutants while the learning and STM (3-h memory) remained intact (Fig. 1d). Together with the CXM experiments, we concluded that a single-session pairing water with odour presentation induces a protein-synthesis-dependent LTM with some genetic similarities to shock-punishment and sugar-reward LTMs1,4,19,20.

The cAMP-responsive element binding protein (CREB) is specifically required for LTM formation, probably through de novo gene expression4,22. We used UAS/GAL4 system to restrict the expression of the dominant-negative dCREB2-b transgene in MBs and combined with temperature-sensitive tub-GAL80ts to suppress GAL4 transcriptional activity throughout the development by keeping flies at 18 °C permissive temperature. We found that adult-stage-expression of dCREB2-b in MBs specifically abolished water-reward LTM but not learning or STM (Fig. 1e; Supplementary Fig. 4a).

Distinct dopaminergic neurons are required for STM and LTM

To identify dopaminergic PAM neurons that reinforce water reward, we first used a dopa decarboxylase (DDC)-GAL4 line that labels 118 dopaminergic PAM neurons innervating almost all of the MB horizontal lobes7,23(Supplementary Figs 4b and 5a). Using a UAS for the temperature-sensitive dynamin mutant shibire (UAS-shits), which inhibits vesicle recycling and synaptic transmission at 32 °C restrictive temperature, we blocked neuronal output in the majority of PAM neurons labelled by DDC-GAL4 and this disrupted 3-min memory. In addition, blocking the output of DDC-GAL4 neurons using shits during acquisition impaired 3-h and 24-h memories, suggesting that DDC-GAL4 neurons reinforce water reward in both the STM and LTM (Fig. 2a). A recent study using R48B04-GAL4 flies has indicated that PAM-γ4 neurons reinforce 3-min water memory5. Consistent with this previous study, blocking the output of R48B04-GAL4 neurons using shits disrupted 3-min memory (Fig. 2b). Surprisingly, we found that blocking the output of R48B04-GAL4 neurons during water conditioning impaired 3-h but not 24-h water memory suggesting that R48B04-GAL4 neurons only reinforce water reward in the STM (Fig. 2b). R48B04-GAL4 labels PAM neurons projecting to the β′2, γ5, γ4, γ2 and γ1 regions of the MB lobes (Supplementary Figs 4c and 5b). This prompted us to ask whether distinct PAM neurons reinforce water reward in the LTM5,11. More interestingly, we independently blocked the output of VT19841-GAL4 neurons using shits during water conditioning that specifically impaired LTM while leaving STM intact (Fig. 2c). VT19841-GAL4 labels PAM neurons projecting to the β′2, β′1, β1 and α1 regions of the MB lobes (Supplementary Figs 4d and 5c). Furthermore, blocking the output of VT6554-GAL4 neurons using shits during water conditioning impaired neither STM nor LTM (Fig. 2d). VT6554-GAL4 labels PAM neurons projecting to β2, β1, α1, β′2 and γ5 regions of the MB lobes (Supplementary Figs 4e and 5d). The PAM neurons that exclusively innervate the MB lobe in VT19841-GAL4 but not in VT6554-GAL4 or R48B04-GAL4 are PAM-β′1 neurons, which suggests these neurons are likely candidates for conveying water reward to the LTM. We therefore independently identified neurons labelled by VT8167-GAL4 from the Vienna Tile collection, which drives UAS-mCD8::GFP;UAS-mCD8::GFP expression in ∼13 tyrosine hydroxylase (TH)-positive dopaminergic PAM neurons that specifically innervate the β′1 region of the MB lobe24,25 (Fig. 2e, Supplementary Figs 4f and 6). Strikingly, blocking the output of VT8167-GAL4 neurons using shits during water conditioning disrupted LTM but not STM (Fig. 2f). More importantly, the LTM performance was restored to the same level as that of the wild-type after removing the dopaminergic PAM-β′1 neurons from the VT8167-GAL4 expression pattern using the overlapping R58E02-GAL80 transgene5,7 (Supplementary Fig. 7). These data suggest that dopaminergic PAM-β′1 neurons convey water reward to the LTM (Fig. 2f; Supplementary Fig. 7). Moreover, blocking the output of VT6554-GAL4 neurons by using shits during sugar/odour association disrupted the sugar-reward LTM in hungry flies (Fig. 3). This result suggests independent acquisition of water and sugar reward LTMs through distinct dopaminergic inputs10,11,26. Last, blocking the output of VT8167-GAL4 neurons using shits during sugar conditioning in hungry flies did not affect the sugar-reward LTM, also suggesting the presence of distinct association sites for water- and sugar-reward LTMs (Fig. 3)10,11,26.

Figure 2. LTM requires reinforcing dopamine from PAM-β′1 cluster.

Figure 2

(a) Blocking DDC-GAL4 neuronal output using shits impaired learning, and blocking DDC-GAL4 neuronal output during memory acquisition significantly impaired STM and LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8 for each bar). *P<0.05; analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Tukey's test. (b) Blocking R48B04-GAL4 neuronal output using shits impaired learning, and blocking R48B04-GAL4 neuronal output during memory acquisition significantly impaired STM but not LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8 for each bar). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. (c) Blocking VT19841-GAL4 neuronal output using shits did not affect learning, and blocking VT19841-GAL4 neuronal output during memory acquisition significantly impaired LTM but not STM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8, 8, 8, 9, 14, 9, 8, 12 and 8 from left to right bars). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. (d) Blocking VT6554-GAL4 neuronal output using shits did not affect learning, and blocking VT6554-GAL4 neuronal output during memory acquisition did not affect STM or LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8, 8, 8, 7, 7, 7, 9, 8 and 7 from left to right bars). P>0.05; ANOVA. (e) The expression pattern for VT8167-GAL4 (green). The brain was immunostained using DLG antibody (magenta). Scale bar, 20 μm. (f) Blocking VT8167-GAL4 neuronal output using shits did not affect learning, and blocking VT8167-GAL4 neuronal output during memory acquisition significantly impaired LTM but not STM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8, 8, 8, 8, 8, 8, 10, 10 and 10 from left to right bars). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test.

Figure 3. Sugar-reward LTM requires reinforcing from dopaminergic neurons other than the PAM-β′1 neurons.

Figure 3

Blocking the output of PAM-β′1 neurons during sugar conditioning did not impair the sugar-reward LTM; whereas, blocking the outputs of PAM-α1, -β2, -β1, β′2 and -γ5 neurons during sugar conditioning disrupted the sugar-reward LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8 for each bar). *P<0.05; analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Tukey's test.

We then tried to identify distinct PAM neuron clusters that were sufficient to reinforce STM or LTM by activating a UAS for the temperature-sensitive Ca2+-permeable cation channel, TrpA1 (UAS-TrpA1) that depolarizes neurons when flies are exposed to a temperature of 31 °C. Pairing the activation of TrpA1 with odour presentation in DDC-GAL4 flies induced robust learning, STM and LTM (Fig. 4a). However, pairing the activation of a subset of PAM neurons with odour presentation in R48B04-GAL4 flies induced learning and STM, but not LTM (Fig. 4b). By contrast, pairing the activation of the PAM-β′1 neuronal subset with odour presentation in VT8167-GAL4 flies only induced LTM but not learning or STM (Fig. 4c). We also observed significant aversive memory performance in the control flies as the increased temperature apparently functions as an aversive reinforcement27.

Figure 4. Distinct dopaminergic neurons induce STM and LTM.

Figure 4

(a) Pairing odour with TrpA1 activation of DDC dopaminergic neurons during memory acquisition resulted in significant learning, STM and LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=6, 6, 6, 8, 8, 8, 6, 6, and 6 from left to right bars). *P<0.05; analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Tukey's test. (b) Pairing odour with TrpA1 activation of R48B04 dopaminergic neurons during memory acquisition resulted in significant learning and STM, but not LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=6, 6, 6, 8, 8, 8, 6, 8 and 8 from left to right bars). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. (c) Pairing odour with TrpA1 activation of VT8167 dopaminergic neurons during memory acquisition resulted in significant LTM, but not learning or STM. For the water-sated group control, water-deprived flies were allowed to drink water for 30 min before heat and odour conditioning. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8 for each bar). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. (d) Retention of induced memory in thirsty DDC-GAL4/UAS-TrpA1, VT8167-GAL4/UAS-TrpA1, and R48B04-GAL4/UAS-TrpA1 flies. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8, 8, 9 and 8 from left to right points in DDC/TrpA1 group; N=8, 7, 10, and 8 from left to right points in VT8167/TrpA1 group; N=8 for each point in R48B04/TrpA1 group). (e) Flies with mutations in the DopR1 (dumb2) show impaired LTM. Genetically restoring DopR1 expression in α′β′ neurons rescued LTM in dumb2 flies to the level of wild-type (WT) flies. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=12, 8, 7, 7, and 6 from left to right bars). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test.

Flies ordinarily need to be thirsty to form water-reward memories5; therefore, we tested whether implanted memories can also be formed by pairing the activation of subsets of dopaminergic PAM neurons with odour presentation in water-sated flies. This artificial implanted LTM was suppressed when flies were allowed to drink water before training, indicating that memory implanted by PAM-β′1 neurons is thirst-dependent (Fig. 4c, right panel). In contrast, the implanted memory scores remained significant in water-sated DDC-GAL4/UAS-TrpA1 and R48B04-GAL4/UAS-TrpA1 flies (Supplementary Fig. 8). It is possible that some dopaminergic neurons among the DDC-GAL4- and R48B04-GAL4-expressing neurons represent reward, other than water5,11. Consistent with this notion, Huetteroth and colleagues reported that the activation of R48B04-GAL4- and 0273-GAL4-expressing dopaminergic neurons paired with odour presentation induces artificial implantation of STM and LTM in food- and water-sated flies, respectively11.

To compare the artificially implanted STMs and LTMs in thirsty flies, DDC-GAL4/UAS-TrpA1 flies were tested in parallel with R48B04-GAL4/UAS-TrpA1 and VT8167-GAL4/UAS-TrpA1 flies. We found that retention of the induced memory in R48B04-GAL4/UAS-TrpA1 flies lasted to 3 h but totally disappeared after 9 h. By contrast, the induced memory in VT8167-GAL4/UAS-TrpA1 flies gradually developed 9 h after conditioning and lasted to 24 h (Fig. 4d). This result suggests that implanted STM is different from implanted LTM in thirsty flies, since STM only lasts for few hours while LTM is gradually formed after a few hours and can last for 1 day (Fig. 4d).

LTM utilizes DopR1 in α′β′ neurons

It has been shown that the D1-like dopamine receptor DopR1 in γ neurons is required for water learning5. Our data indicated that dopaminergic PAM-β′1 neurons reinforce water reward to the LTM. We therefore asked whether DopR1 also plays a role in LTM in MBs. We used DopR1 mutant flies containing the dumb2 mutation that was generated using a piggyBac insertion, PBac{WH}DopRf02676, with a terminal UAS site for the GAL4-driven misexpression of the adjacent gene28. A previous study revealed that the dumb2 mutant flies are defective in their water-reward learning5. Here we showed that dumb2 flies are also defective in LTM (Fig. 4e). More importantly, we were able to restore the LTM of dumb2 mutants to the wild-type level by expressing DopR1 in α′β′ neurons (VT57244-GAL4)9,29 but not in αβ (C739-GAL4) or γ (VT44966-GAL4)30 neurons (Fig. 4e; Supplementary Figs 4g–i and 9). Taken together, we conclude that dopaminergic PAM-β′1 neurons signal water reward to the LTM in α′β′ neurons via DopR1 receptors. The dopaminergic neurons responsible for the reinforcing effects of water on LTM are different from those that are critical for the reinforcing effects of water on learning and STM or the reinforcing effects of sugar memories.

PAM-β′1 neurons are responsive to water drinking

Since the PAM-β′1 neurons specifically provide instructive water reinforcement to the LTM, we further tested whether the water intake evokes a response in these neurons by expressing a calcium reporter UAS-GCaMP6 in DDC-GAL4 flies31. In thirsty flies, drinking water caused a strong increase in GCaMP6 fluorescence in PAM-β′1, -β′2 and -γ4 neurons, compared with a lesser response in PAM-γ5 neurons in MB horizontal lobes (Fig. 5a–c). To specifically identify calcium responses in PAM-β′1 neurons, we used UAS-GCaMP6 driven by VT8167-GAL4, which only labels PAM-β′1 neurons. Drinking water indeed evoked a robust increase in GCaMP6 fluorescence in the PAM neurons that innervate to the β′1 region of MB lobe (Fig. 5d–f). Combining these results with those of a previous study, we can conclude that water reinforcement of LTM is conveyed by PAM-β′1 neurons, whereas water reinforcement of STM is conveyed by PAM-γ4 neurons5 (Figs 2 and 4).

Figure 5. Drinking water evokes an increased calcium response in PAM-β′1 neurons in thirsty flies.

Figure 5

(a) GCaMP6 response to water intake in distinct groups of PAM neurons driven by DDC-GAL4 in thirsty (left panel) or water-sated (right panel) flies, respectively. Scale bar, 20 μm. (b,c) Time-lapse recordings of GCaMP6 responses (b) in thirsty (left panel) or water-sated (right panel) flies; the average responses (c) to water intake in distinct groups of PAM neurons driven by DDC-GAL4. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8 for thirsty flies; N=11 for water-sated flies). NS, not significant (P>0.05). *P<0.05; t-test. (d) GCaMP6 response to water intake in PAM-β′1 neurons driven by VT8167-GAL4 in thirsty (left panel) or water-sated (right panel) flies, respectively. Scale bar, 20 μm. (e,f) Time-lapse recordings of GCaMP6 responses (e) in thirsty (left panel) or water-sated (right panel) flies; the average responses (f) to water intake in PAM-β′1 neurons driven by VT8167-GAL4. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8 for thirsty flies; N=10 for water-sated flies). *P<0.05; t-test.

Neurotransmission from MBs is required for LTM

Because the dopaminergic signals reinforcing water-reward LTM are delivered to the β′ lobe of the MB, we first investigated the role of α′β′ neurons during LTM processing. We genetically expressed shits in α′β′ neurons using VT30604-GAL4 or VT57244-GAL4 flies (Supplementary Figs 4g,j and 10a,b). Blocking the output from α′β′ neurons during consolidation but not during acquisition or retrieval disrupted LTM (Fig. 6a,b). We therefore further investigated the roles of other subsets of MB neurons during LTM formation. We genetically expressed shits in αβ neurons using C739-GAL4 or VT49246-GAL4 flies (Supplementary Figs 4h,m and 10c,d), or expressed shits in γ neurons using R16A06-GAL4 or 5HT1B-GAL4 flies (Supplementary Figs 4k,l and 10e,f). Blocking the output of αβ or γ neurons during acquisition and consolidation did not impair LTM, whereas blocking the output of αβ or γ neurons during retrieval significantly abolished LTM (Fig. 6c–f). Furthermore, all the manipulated flies showed normal odour acuity and water preference at 32 °C restrictive temperature (Supplementary Fig. 10g–k). Together, our results indicate that the output from α′β′ neurons is only required for consolidation, whereas the output from γ and αβ neuron is only required for the retrieval of LTM.

Figure 6. MB neural subsets play distinct roles during LTM processing.

Figure 6

(a) Blocking the output of α′β′ neurons (VT30604-GAL4) using shits during consolidation but not during acquisition or retrieval, impaired LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8 for each bar). *P<0.05; analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Tukey's test. (b) Blocking the output of α′β′ neurons (VT57244-GAL4) using shits during consolidation but not during acquisition or retrieval, impaired LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8 for each bar). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. (c) Blocking the output of γ neurons (R16A06-GAL4) using shits during retrieval but not during acquisition and consolidation, impaired LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8 for each bar). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. (d) Blocking the output of γ neurons (5HT1B-GAL4) using shits during retrieval, but not during acquisition and consolidation, impaired LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8 for each bar). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. (e) Blocking the output of αβ neurons (VT49246-GAL4) using shits during retrieval but not during acquisition and consolidation, impaired LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8 for each bar). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. (f) Blocking the output of αβ neurons (C739-GAL4) using shits during retrieval but not during acquisition and consolidation, impaired LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8 for each bar). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test.

MB-M6 and MB-V3 are necessary for the retrieval of LTM

Since the output from γ neurons is required for the retrieval of LTM, we further examined the role of γ efferent neurons in LTM. The dendrites of MB efferent MB-M6 neurons innervate the tip of the MB γ lobe and also the ventral part of the β′ lobe tip, from which axons project to the superior medial protocerebrum32,33,34 (Fig. 7a,b; Supplementary Fig. 4n–p). It has been shown that activity in MB-M6 neurons is required for retrieval of both sugar-reward LTM34 and shock-punishment long-term anesthesia-resistant memory (LT-ARM)33. Therefore, we asked whether activity in MB-M6 is also required for the retrieval of water-reward LTM. We expressed shits in MB-M6 neurons using three independent GAL4 drivers (R27G01-GAL4, E1255-GAL4 and VT57242-GAL4), which all label the same MB-M6 neurons in the fly brain (Supplementary Figs 4n–p and 11a–c). Blocking the output from MB-M6 neurons during retrieval, but not during acquisition and consolidation, impaired LTM (Fig. 7c,d; Supplementary Fig. 11d). Moreover, blocking the MB-M6 output during retrieval did not affect odour acuity, water preference (Supplementary Fig. 11e,f), or STM (Supplementary Fig. 11g). Finally, adult-stage-specific silencing of the vesicular glutamate transporter (VGlut) in MB-M6 neurons disrupted LTM, suggesting that glutamatergic transmission from MB-M6 neurons is functionally required for retrieving LTM from γ neurons of MBs (Fig. 7e; Supplementary Fig. 11h,i).

Figure 7. LTM is read out through MB-M6 and MB-V3.

Figure 7

(a) The expression pattern of R27G01-GAL4 (green). The brain was immunostained with DLG antibody (magenta). White arrowheads indicate the somas of MB-M6. Scale bar, 20 μm. (b) The expression pattern of E1255-GAL4 (green). The brain was immunostained with DLG antibody (magenta). White arrowheads indicate the somas of MB-M6. Scale bar, 20 μm. (c) Blocking MB-M6 neurons (R27G01-GAL4) output using shits during retrieval but not during acquisition and consolidation, impaired LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8, 8, 8, 9, 9, and 9 from left to right bars). *P<0.05; analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Tukey's test. (d) Blocking MB-M6 neurons (E1255-GAL4) output using shits during retrieval but not during acquisition and consolidation, impaired LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=10, 10, 10, 9, 9, and 9 from left to right bars). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. (e) Adult-stage-specific knockdown of VGlut in MB-M6 neurons (R27G01-GAL4) impaired LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. (f) The expression pattern of G0239-GAL4 (green). The brain was immunostained with DLG antibody (magenta). White arrowheads indicate the somas of MB-V3. Scale bar, 20 μm. (g) The expression pattern of MB082C-GAL4 (green). The brain was immunostained with DLG antibody (magenta). White arrowheads indicate the somas of MB-V3. Scale bar, 20 μm. (h) Blocking MB-V3 neurons (G0239-GAL4) output using shits during retrieval but not during acquisition and consolidation, impaired LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8, 7, 7, 6, 6 and 7 from left to right bars). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. (i) Blocking MB-V3 neurons (MB082C-GAL4) output using shits during retrieval but not during acquisition and consolidation, impaired LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=8). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test. (j) Adult-stage-specific knockdown of ChAT in MB-V3 neurons (G0239-GAL4) impaired LTM. Each value represents mean±s.e.m. (N=9). *P<0.05; ANOVA followed by Tukey's test.

MB-V3 comprises two pairs of MB efferent neurons with dendrites that innervate the tip of the MB α lobe and axons that project to the superior dorsofrontal protocerebrum35,36,37 (Fig. 7f,g; Supplementary Fig. 4q,r). Neurotransmission from MB-V3 is required for the retrieval of both sugar-reward37 and shock-punishment36 LTMs. Therefore, we examined whether activity in MB-V3 neurons is also required for the retrieval of water-reward LTM. We expressed shits in MB-V3 neurons using G0239-GAL4 flies and found that blocking the output from MB-V3 neurons during retrieval but not during acquisition and consolidation impaired LTM (Fig. 7h). These behavioural results were further corroborated using an independent GAL4 driver in MB082C-GAL4 flies, which labels MB-V3 and MBON-α′2 neurons32 (Fig. 7i). Blocking the output of MB-V3 neurons did not affect odour acuity, water preference (Supplementary Fig. 12a), or STM (Supplementary Fig. 12b). Cholinergic transmission from MB-V3 is necessary for sugar-reward LTM37, which prompted us to examine the role of acetylcholine in MB-V3 neurons during water-reward LTM processes. Adult-stage-specific silencing of choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) in MB-V3 neurons disrupted LTM, suggesting that cholinergic transmission from MB-V3 neurons is functionally required for retrieving LTM from αβ neurons of MBs (Fig. 7j and Supplementary Fig. 12c,d).

Discussion

The key finding from our current study is that flies form a robust water-reward memory that lasts for one day after a single training session pairing odour with water for 2 min (Fig. 1a). The 24-h water memory is disrupted by CXM-mediated inhibition of protein synthesis or mutations of the crammer, tequila, or radish genes (Fig. 1c,d). Moreover, adult-stage-specific expression of the dominant-negative form of CREB (dCREB2-b) in MBs specifically disrupted 24-h but not the 3-min and 3-h water-reward memories (Fig. 1e), suggesting that 24-h water-reward memory per se belongs to the LTM4,22.

Ample studies on the role of dopaminergic PAM neurons in the reinforcement of STM and LTM have focused on the sugar-reward assays7,8,10,11. In sugar-reward memories, sweet-taste-reinforced STM and nutrient-dependent LTM are mediated by distinct sets of dopaminergic PAM neurons7,8,10,11. Whether water-reward STM and LTM utilize different PAM neuron clusters for their reinforcing signals is still uncertain. Here, we determined that PAM-β′1 neurons convey the water reward to the MB β′ lobe for LTM, and are different to the PAM-γ4 neurons that convey the water reward to the MB γ lobe for learning5 and STM (Figs 2, 4 and 8). In thirsty flies, pairing the activation of PAM-γ4 neurons with odour presentation induced an implanted memory immediately and this memory decayed within 9 h after conditioning, whereas the implanted memory gradually forms at 9 h and lasts to 24 h after conditioning by pairing the activation of PAM-β′1 neurons with odour presentation (Fig. 4d). Interestingly, activating of a large group of PAM neurons with odour presentation in DDC-GAL4/UAS-TrpA1 flies always induces a higher implanted 24-h memory score than pairing the activation of PAM-β′1 neurons with odour presentation in VT8167-GAL4/UAS-TrpA1 flies (Fig. 4d). It is possible that some dopaminergic neurons in the DDC-GAL4-expressing population represent reward event other than water, since the activation of water-sated DDC-GAL4 flies paired with odour presentation still induced significant 3-min, 3-h and 24-h memories (Supplementary Fig. 8a). In addition, artificially activating the majority of PAM neurons in 0273-GAL4 flies and pairing this with odour presentation can induce a significant 7-day implanted memory even in food- and water-sated flies11.

Figure 8. A model of water-reward LTM and STM brain circuits.

Figure 8

LTM requires reinforcement from ∼13 dopaminergic PAM-β′1 neurons that innervate a restricted zone of the MB β′ lobe. This is different from the STM-reinforcing dopaminergic neurons (PAM-γ4) that innervate the MB γ lobe. After association, the LTM consolidation requires the synaptic output in α′β′ neurons, whereas the LTM retrieval requires the output in αβ and γ neurons. Finally, the LTM is read out from the αβ and γ neurons via cholinergic MB-V3 neurons and glutamatergic MB-M6 neurons, respectively.

Our calcium imaging data indicate that water intake simultaneously activated PAM-γ4 and PAM-β′1 neurons in thirsty flies, suggesting that both PAM clusters respond to water consumption (Fig. 5). Consistent with the calcium imaging data, DopR1 expression in γ neurons is sufficient for learning5, whereas DopR1 in α′β′ neurons is specific for LTM formation (Fig. 4e). The results above suggest that water drinking activates multiple reward signals to different MB lobes to form complementary STM and LTM responses, which is a concept similar to the synaptic facilitation of the tail-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia38 or the findings of recent sugar-reward memory studies in fruit flies10,11. It has been shown that dopaminergic PAM-α1, -β1 and -β2 neurons convey the sugar reward to the LTM in a manner dependent on hunger state10,11(Fig. 3). However, blocking PAM-α1, -β1 and -β2 neurons during water conditioning did not affect both short-term and long-term water-reward memories, implying that functionally subdivided neuronal circuits convey sugar- and water-reinforced stimuli into LTMs (Figs 2d and 3).

PAM neurons mediate the neuronal activity of MBs elicited by dopamine signalling that controls innate or learned memory-relevant behaviours in fruit flies5,7,8,9,26,39,40. The MBs play a role in sensory integration and processes olfactory learning and memory in Drosophila14,41. We found that the output from α′β′ neurons is only required for memory consolidation, whereas the output from γ or αβ neurons is only required for memory retrieval, suggesting that long-lasting water memory is first registered in α′β′ neurons and is eventually formed in αβ and γ neurons by stabilizing activity from α′β′ neurons during memory consolidation (Fig. 6). Synaptic outputs from α′β′ neurons during the consolidation of shock-punishment and sugar-reward LTM have not been well examined. However, the output from αβ neurons is required for the retrieval of both shock-punishment and sugar-reward LTMs, whereas the output from γ neurons is dispensable42,43. Our results showed that the retrieval of water-reward LTM requires outputs from both αβ and γ neurons (Fig. 6c–f), which differs from the other olfactory associative LTMs at the circuit level of MBs42,43. These results show that water-reward LTM is consolidated in αβ and γ neurons, and is stabilized there with the activity of α′β′ neurons during consolidation (Fig. 8). Several studies have proposed the system consolidation concept in the context of Drosophila olfactory memories43,44,45,46. In both sugar-reward and shock-punishment LTM, the neurotransmission in α′β′, αβ and γ neurons is required for at least 3 h after conditioning. In contrast, the expression of 24-h memories is independent of the α′β′, and γ neuron activities, requiring neurotransmission in only the αβ neurons43. Furthermore, the expression of DopR1 in the γ neurons is sufficient to fully support the shock-punishment STM and LTM in the dumb2 mutant, suggesting that the dopamine-mediated shock/odour association is registered in γ neurons and finally stabilized in αβ neurons for long-term storage through system consolidation45. The mechanism underlying the communication between different neurons of the MBs during the consolidation of olfactory LTM remains uncertain. However, the neurons that are highly ramified in the MBs and maintain memories during consolidation have been identified. The axons and dendrites of dorsal paired medial (DPM) neurons are evenly distributed in whole MBs29,47. The synaptic output of DPM neurons is only required during the memory consolidation phase and is dispensable during the acquisition and retrieval of both sugar-reward and shock-punishment memories48,49,50. It has been proposed that the DPM neurons are receptive and allow transmission to the α′β′ neurons; this recurrent feedback loop stabilizes the olfactory memory in αβ neurons of MBs51. This recurrent synapse in the α′β′–DPM loop sustains the activity during memory consolidation and is unstable without any inhibitory inputs. Notably, GABAergic anterior paired lateral (APL) neurons are electrically coupled to DPM neurons, which can provide lateral inhibition to maintain the synaptic specificity within the α′β′–DPM recurrent networks50,52,53. We suspect that after water conditioning, persistent neural activity arising in the recurrent α′β′–DPM networks stabilizes the consolidating LTM signals to the αβ and γ neurons. Thus, it will be of interest to determine the physiological properties of DPM and APL neurons during water-reward LTM consolidation in the future.

In addition, it was found in a recent study that the PAM-α1 neurons, αβ neurons, and αβ efferent MBON-α1 neurons form a feedback recurrent circuit to drive the formation of sugar-reward LTM26. The activity in the αβ and MBON-α1 neurons is required during the acquisition, consolidation and retrieval of sugar-reward LTM26,42,43. Therefore, the sugar-reward LTM formed in the αβ neurons is retrieved through the MBON-α1 neurons26. The functions of the MBON-α1 neurons in water-reward LTM remain unclear. It is noteworthy that in contrast to its role in sugar-reward LTM26, the activity in αβ neurons is only required during the retrieval phase, and not during the acquisition and consolidation phases of water-reward LTM (Figs 6e,f and 8). This suggests that water-reward LTM is unlikely to be encoded by a recurrent feedback loop within the MB αβ neuronal circuitry during memory acquisition and consolidation.

Neurotransmission from MB-M6 neurons is required to retrieve both shock-punishment and sugar-reward memories33,34. The MB-M6 neurons have dendrites in the MB γ5 and β′2 lobes and axons outside the MBs, indicating their role as MB efferent neurons (Fig. 7a,b). Blocking MB-M6 activity during memory retrieval impaired water-reward LTM (Fig. 7c,d; Supplementary Fig. 11d). Because the output from γ neurons is required for retrieval of LTM but the output of α′β′ neurons is dispensable, we concluded that MB-M6 neurons are responsible for retrieving water-reward LTM from γ neurons of MBs (Figs 6a–d and 7c,d; Supplementary Fig. 11d). Moreover, acute knockdown of VGlut in MB-M6 neurons impaired water-reward LTM, suggesting that glutamatergic neurotransmission in MB-M6 neurons plays a role in water-reward LTM retrieval (Fig. 7e). On the other hand, the dendrites of MB-V3 specifically connect to the tip of the MB α lobe, and blocking the output from MB-V3 neurons during memory retrieval impairs both shock-punishment and sugar-reward LTMs36,37 (Fig. 7f,g). Here, we have further identified that synaptic output from MB-V3 neurons is also required for the retrieval of water-reward LTM, implying that MB-V3 neurons likely participate in generalized circuits for retrieving all olfactory associative LTMs from αβ neurons of MBs36,37 (Fig. 7h,i). Furthermore, acute knockdown of ChAT in MB-V3 neurons impaired water-reward LTM suggesting that cholinergic transmission from MB-V3 neurons is necessary for the memory retrieval process (Fig. 7j). These data suggest that MB-M6 and MB-V3 neurons retrieve water-reward LTM from γ and αβ neurons via glutamatergic and cholinergic signalling, respectively (Figs 7 and 8). It is unlikely that MB-M6 and MB-V3 neurons directly convey the output of water-reward LTM to the same downstream neuron because the MB-M6 axons project to the superior medial protocerebrum and MB-V3 axons project to the superior dorsofrontal protocerebrum24,34,36,37. Identifying the neuronal circuits receiving inputs from MB-M6 and MB-V3 neurons would be an interesting topic for future study.

Methods

Fly stocks

Fly stocks were raised on standard cornmeal food at 25 °C and 60% relative humidity on a 12-h:12-h light:dark cycle. UAS-shits, UAS-mCD8::GFP;UAS-mCD8::GFP, UAS-TrpA1, C739-GAL4, E1255-GAL4, MB082C-GAL4, G0239-GAL4, 5HT1B-GAL4, UAS-dCREB2-b, UAS-GCaMP6m, hs-flp;;UAS>rCD2, y+>mCD8::GFP and tub-GAL80ts flies were obtained from Ann-Shyn Chiang. R58E02-GAL80 and UAS-shits(JFRC100) flies were obtained from Suewei Lin. PPK28 mutant flies were obtained from Scott Waddell. VT8167-GAL4, VT44966-GAL4, VT6554-GAL4 and VT57242-GAL4 flies were obtained from the Vienna Tile Library, Vienna Drosophila Resource Center (VDRC). R27G01-GAL4, crammer mutant (P{EP}cerG6085), tequila mutant (PBac{WH}teqf01792) and UAS-ChATRNAi (TRiP collection) flies were obtained from the Bloomington stock center. The UAS-VGluTRNAiflies were obtained from the Vienna Drosophila RNAi Center. VT30604-GAL4, VT57244-GAL4, VT49246-GAL4, R16A06-GAL4, DDC-GAL4, VT19841-GAL4, radish mutant (rsh1) and dumb2mutant (PBac{WH}DopRf02676) fly strains have been described previously9,29,30,54. Genotype information for all figures is provided separately in the Supplementary Information (Supplementary Note 1).

Whole-mount immunostaining

Fly brain samples were dissected in PBS and fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 20 min at 25 °C. After fixation, the brain samples were incubated in PBS containing 1% Triton X-100 and 10% normal goat serum (PBS-T) and degassed in a vacuum chamber to expel tracheal air with six cycles (depressurize to 270 mm Hg then hold for 10 min). Next, the brain samples were blocked and permeabilized in PBS-T at 25 °C for 2 h and then incubated in PBS-T containing 1:10 diluted mouse 4F3 anti-discs large (DLG) monoclonal antibody (AB 528203, Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, University of Iowa) or 1:200 diluted mouse anti-tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) monoclonal antibody (22941, ImmunoStar) or 1:1250 diluted rabbit anti-DopR1 antibody (supplied by F.W. Wolf)55 at 25 °C for 1 day. After washing in PBS-T three times, the samples were incubated in 1:200 biotinylated goat anti-mouse IgGs (31800, Molecular Probes) or 1:200 biotinylated goat anti-rabbit IgGs (31820, Molecular Probes) at 25 °C for 1 day. Next, brain samples were washed and incubated in 1:500 Alexa Fluor 635 streptavidin (Molecular Probes) at 25 °C for 1 day. After extensive washing, the brain samples were cleared and mounted in FocusClear (CelExplorer). After mounting, sample brains were then imaged using a Zeiss LSM 700 confocal microscope with a 40 × C-Apochromat water-immersion objective lens or a 63 × LCI Plan-Neofluar objective lens. To overcome the limited field of view, some samples were imaged twice, one for each hemisphere, with overlaps in between. We then combined the two parallel image stacks into a single data set with the on-line stitch function of ZEN software, using the overlapping region to align the two stacks.

Behavioural assays

Flies were deprived of water by keeping them in a glass milk bottle containing a 6 cm × 3 cm piece of dry sucrose-soaked filtre paper (a filtre paper was soaked in saturated sucrose solution and allowed to dry before use) at 18, 23, 25 or 30 °C and 20–30% humidity for 16 h before water conditioning. To avoid bias related to the odour concentration used in our behavioural assays, the constant air speed and odour concentrations were adjusted by varying the diameters of the odour vials until the naive flies distributed themselves equally when given a choice between 3-octanol (OCT) and 4-methylcyclohexanol (MCH). An air flow meter (Gilmont) was used to adjust and maintain the air speed at 750 ml min−1 in all the training and testing tubes throughout the experiment. Groups of ∼50 water-deprived flies were loaded in the training tube of the T-maze (CelExplorer) and provided flies with a stream of relatively odourless ‘fresh' room air for 1 min. The flies were first exposed to one odour for 2 min (unconditioned stimulus, CS−: OCT or MCH) in a tube lined with dry filtre paper, followed by 1 min of fresh room air. The flies were then transferred to another tube that contained a water-soaked filtre paper and exposed to a second odour for another 2 min (conditioned stimulus, CS+: OCT or MCH). Finally, the flies were transferred to a clean training tube and exposed to fresh room air for 1 min. In the testing phase, flies were presented with a choice between CS+ and CS− odours in a T-maze for 2 min. At the end of this 2-min period, flies were trapped in either T-maze arm, anaesthetized and counted. From this distribution, a performance index (PI) was calculated as the number of flies running toward the conditioned odour minus the number of flies running towards the unconditioned odour, divided by the total number of flies and multiplied by 100. For the calculation of individual PI, naive flies were first trained by pairing water with OCT (CS+), and the index (PIO) was tested. Next, another group of naïve flies was trained by pairing water with MCH (CS+), and the index (PIM) was tested. A single PI was calculated as the average from single PIO and PIM values. If all the flies failed to learn, then the index would be 0; if they all approached the water-associated odour (perfect learning), the index would be 100. Learning was measured immediately (3 min) after training. To evaluate the STM and LTM, the trained flies were kept in a plastic vial that contains a 1.5 cm × 3 cm piece of a dried sucrose-soaked filtre paper during the intervals; STM assessment was performed 3 h after training and LTM assessment was performed 24 h after training in a T-maze. In shits experiments for LTM, flies were kept at 23 °C and 60% humidity throughout development. To block acquisition, flies were shifted to 32 °C for 30 min before and during training, and shifted back to 23 °C immediately after training and during testing. To block consolidation, flies were trained at 23 °C, shifted to 32 °C for 8 h after training, shifted back to 23 °C for 16 h, and tested. To block retrieval, flies were trained at 23 °C maintained at 23 °C for 23.5 h, and then shifted to 32 °C for 30 min and tested. For the adult-stage-specific RNAi-mediated knockdown with tubP-GAL80ts, flies were kept at 18 °C until eclosion and then shifted to 30 °C for 3 days before water conditioning. The water-reward memory assays were also performed at 30 °C as the experimental groups. For the control groups, flies were kept at 18 °C after eclosion and throughout the experiment. For the TrpA1-related implanted memory experiments, thirsty flies were presented with one odour at 23 °C for 2 min. After that, flies were then transferred into pre-warmed tube and immediately presented with a second odour at 31 °C for 2 min. The same flies were then transferred to 23 °C and tested for 3 min memory. To test 3 h memory, flies were trained as above and transferred into plastic vials containing a dry sucrose-coated filtre paper until testing. To test 24-h memory, flies were trained as above and these trained flies were first transferred into plastic vials containing a water-soaked filtre paper for 2 min and then stored in plastic vials containing a dry sucrose-coated filtre paper for 24 h before testing.

For the sugar-reward memory, the flies were food-deprived for 16 h before conditioning in glass milk bottles containing a 6 cm × 3 cm piece of filtre paper soaked in water. A group of ∼50 food-deprived flies was loaded in the training tube of the T-maze (CelExplorer) and was provided with a stream of relatively odourless ‘fresh' room air for 1 min. The flies were first exposed to one odour for 2 min (CS−: OCT or MCH) in a tube lined with dry filtre paper, followed by 1 min of fresh room air. The flies were then transferred to another tube that contained a dried sucrose-soaked filtre paper and exposed to a second odour for another 2 min (CS+: OCT or MCH). Finally, the flies were transferred to a clean training tube and exposed to fresh room air for 1 min. To evaluate the sugar-reward LTM, the trained flies were kept in a plastic vial that contains a 1.5 cm × 3 cm piece of water-soaked filtre paper during the intervals; the LTM assessment was performed 24 h after training in a T-maze4.

Drug feeding

A solution of 35 mM cycloheximide (CXM; Sigma-Aldrich) was prepared in saturated sucrose. For the 24-h assay, wild-type flies were fed dry sucrose-coated filtre paper impregnated with CXM for 16 h before water conditioning and then again fed CXM during the 24-h interval. The total duration of CXM feeding was 40 h (16+24=40). For the 3-h memory assay, wild-type flies were fed dry sucrose-coated filtre paper impregnated with CXM for 16 h, then given a brief 2-min water supplement and fed CXM for another 21 h. Subsequently, flies were trained by water conditioning after 37 h (16+21=37) of CXM feeding and again fed CXM during the 3-h interval. The total duration of CXM feeding was 40 h (37+3=40). For the learning assay, wild-type flies were fed dry sucrose-coated filtre paper impregnated with CXM for 16 h, then given a brief 2-min water supplement and again fed CXM for another 24 h. After that, flies were trained by water conditioning and tested for 3-min memory. The total duration of CXM feeding was 40 h (16+24=40).

Calcium imaging

DDC-GAL4- and VT8167-GAL4-expressing UAS-GCaMP6 flies were water-deprived for 10–12 h before experiments. We then immobilized each fly in a 250-μl pipette tip, opened a window on the head capsule using fine tweezers and immediately added a drop of adult haemolymph-like (AHL) saline (108 mM NaCl, 5 mM KCl, 2 mM CaCl2, 8.2 mM MgCl2, 4 mM NaHCO3, 1 mM NaH2PO4, 5 mM trehalose, 10 mM sucrose and 5 mM HEPES (pH 7.5, 265 mOsm)) to prevent dehydration. After removing the small trachea and excessive fat with fine tweezers, the fly and pipette tip were fixed to a coverslip by tape, and a 40 × water immersion objective (W Plan-Apochromat 40 × /1.0 DIC M27) was used for imaging. A drop of water was delivered to the fly using a custom-made water delivery device. Time-lapse recordings of changes in GCaMP intensity before and after water consumption were performed using a Zeiss LSM700 microscope with a 40 × water immersion objective, an excitation laser (488 nm), and a detector for emissions passing through a 555 nm short-pass filtre. An optical slice with a resolution of 512 × 512 pixels was continuously monitored for 45 s at 2 frames per second. Regions of interest were manually assigned to anatomically different regions of the MB lobes. The change in intensity was normalized to the pre-stimulus intensity (ΔF(n)/F=(F(n)−F)/F). The average intensity of the ROIs from 5 frames before water stimuli were used as the F, where F(n) is the average intensity of the ROIs form 5 frames after water stimuli. The intensity maps were generated using ImageJ and Amira 5.2 software for all functional calcium imaging studies.

Single-neuron imaging

For genetic FLP-out labelling, flies carrying the hs-flp/+; +/+; VT8167-GAL4/UAS>rCD2,y+>mCD8::GFP transgene were heat-shocked at 37 °C at the 4-day pupal stage for 10 min. Each sample brain was counterstained with anti-DLG antibody and imaged using a Zeiss LSM700 confocal microscope with a 40 × C-Apochromat water immersion objective lens or a 63 × LCI Plan-Neofluar objective lens. Individual single-neuron images were captured, segmented and warped to the standard MB neuropil structures using Amira 5.2 software, as previously described9,29,35.

Odour avoidance and water preference assay

For odour avoidance, groups of roughly 50 untrained water-deprived flies were subjected to a 2-min test trial in the T-maze. Different groups were given a choice between either OCT or MCH versus ‘fresh' room air. The odour avoidance index was calculated as the number of flies in the fresh-room-air tube minus the number in the odour tube, divided by the total number of flies, and multiplied by 100. For water preference, water-deprived flies were given 2 min to choose between tubes in a T-maze, with one tube containing a water-soaked filtre paper and the other containing a dry filtre paper. The water preference index was calculated as the number of flies in the wet tube minus the number of flies in the dry tube, divided by the total number of flies, and multiplied by 100.

Statistical analysis

All raw data were analysed parametrically using Prism 5.0 software (GraphPad). Because of the nature of their mathematical derivation, performance indices were distributed normally. Hence, the data from more than two groups were evaluated by one-way analysis of variance and Tukey's multiple comparisons tests. Data from only two groups were evaluated by paired t-test. A statistically significant difference was defined as *P<0.05. All the data in bar graphs were presented as mean±s.e.m. No statistical methods were used to predetermine the sample sizes, but the sample sizes in this study are similar to those reported in our previous publications29,30,52,56 and also similar to those reported in many previous publications from other groups1,5,8,10,11,33,36,37,42,44,45,49,50,53. The flies in different treatment conditions (that is, CXM fed [+CXM] or CXM unfed [−CXM] groups) were randomized and no data points were excluded. The data collections and analyses were performed without blinding, but during conditioning all groups were trained and tested in parallel. The variance was tested in each group of the data and the variance was similar among all genotypes. All the data distribution was assumed to be normal, but this was not formally tested. The data were collected and processed randomly and no individual data were excluded in each experimental trial. Each experiment was performed in multiple days and has been successfully reproduced more than once.

Data availability

All the relevant data supporting the findings of this study are available within the article and its Supplementary Information files or from the corresponding author upon request.

Additional information

How to cite this article: Shyu, W.-H et al. Neural circuits for long-term water-reward memory processing in thirsty Drosophila. Nat. Commun. 8, 15230 doi: 10.1038/ncomms15230 (2017).

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Figures and Supplementary Note 1

ncomms15230-s1.pdf (5.1MB, pdf)
Peer Review File
ncomms15230-s2.pdf (6.6MB, pdf)

Acknowledgments

We thank Suewei Lin, Ann-Shyn Chiang, Scott Waddell, Kristin Scott, Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center, Vienna Drosophila RNAi Center, Vienna Tile (VT) Library and Fly Core in Taiwan for providing fly stocks. We also thank Fred W. Wolf for the rabbit anti-DopR1 antibody. This work was supported by grants from the Ministry of Science and Technology 105-2321-B-182-001 and 104-2311-B-182-002 and grants from the Chang Gung Memorial Hospital CMRPD1E0061-3 and BMRPC75.

Footnotes

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Author contributions C.-L.W. and W.-H.S. conceived and designed the experiments. W.-H.S., T.-H.C., M.-H.C., C.-L.W., Y.-C.C. and Y.-L.T. performed the experiments. C.-L.W., W.-H.S., T.-F.F. and T.W. analysed the data. C.-L.W. wrote the paper and supervised the project.

References

  1. Tully T., Preat T., Boynton S. C. & Del Vecchio M. Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila. Cell 79, 35–47 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Isabel G., Pascual A. & Preat T. Exclusive consolidated memory phases in Drosophila. Science 304, 1024–1027 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Tempel B. L., Bonini N., Dawson D. R. & Quinn W. G. Reward learning in normal and mutant Drosophila. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 80, 1482–1486 (1983). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Krashes M. J. & Waddell S. Rapid consolidation to a radish and protein synthesis-dependent long-term memory after single-session appetitive olfactory conditioning in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 28, 3103–3113 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Lin S. et al. Neural correlates of water reward in thirsty Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1536–1542 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Schultz W., Dayan P. & Montague P. R. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275, 1593–1599 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Liu C. et al. A subset of dopamine neurons signals reward for odour memory in Drosophila. Nature 488, 512–516 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Burke C. J. et al. Layered reward signalling through octopamine and dopamine in Drosophila. Nature 492, 433–437 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Shih H. W. et al. Parallel circuits control temperature preference in Drosophila during ageing. Nat. Commun. 6, 7775 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Yamagata N. et al. Distinct dopamine neurons mediate reward signals for short- and long-term memories. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 578–583 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Huetteroth W. et al. Sweet taste and nutrient value subdivide rewarding dopaminergic neurons in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 25, 751–758 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cameron P., Hiroi M., Ngai J. & Scott K. The molecular basis for water taste in Drosophila. Nature 465, 91–95 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Crittenden J. R., Skoulakis E. M., Han K. A., Kalderon D. & Davis R. L. Tripartite mushroom body architecture revealed by antigenic markers. Learn. Mem 5, 38–51 (1998). [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Heisenberg M. Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 266–275 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. McGaugh J. L. Time-dependent processes in memory storage. Science 153, 1351–1358 (1966). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Daniels D. Acquisition, storage, and recall of memory for brightness discrimination by rats following intracerebral infusion of acetoxycycloheximide. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 76, 110–118 (1971). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Jaffe K. Effect of cycloheximide on protein synthesis and memory in praying mantis. Physiol. Behav. 25, 367–371 (1980). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Davis H. P. & Squire L. R. Protein synthesis and memory: a review. Psychol. Bull. 96, 518–559 (1984). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Didelot G. et al. Tequila, a neurotrypsin ortholog, regulates long-term memory formation in Drosophila. Science 313, 851–853 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Comas D., Petit F. & Preat T. Drosophila long-term memory formation involves regulation of cathepsin activity. Nature 430, 460–463 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Folkers E., Waddell S. & Quinn W. G. The Drosophila radish gene encodes a protein required for anesthesia-resistant memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 17496–17500 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Yin J. C. et al. Induction of a dominant negative CREB transgene specifically blocks long-term memory in Drosophila. Cell 79, 49–58 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Aso Y. et al. Three dopamine pathways induce aversive odour memories with different stability. PLoS Genet. 8, e1002768 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Aso Y. et al. The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning. Elife 3, 04577 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Shuai Y. et al. Dissecting neural pathways for forgetting in Drosophila olfactory aversive memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, E6663–E6672 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ichinose T. et al. Reward signal in a recurrent circuit drives appetitive long-term memory formation. Elife 4, 10719 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Galili D. S. et al. Converging circuits mediate temperature and shock aversive olfactory conditioning in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 24, 1712–1722 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Thibault S. T. et al. A complementary transposon tool kit for Drosophila melanogaster using P and piggyBac. Nat. Genet. 36, 283–287 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Wu C. L., Shih M. F., Lee P. T. & Chiang A. S. An octopamine-mushroom body circuit modulates the formation of anesthesia-resistant memory in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 23, 2346–2354 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Yang C. H. et al. Additive expression of consolidated memory through Drosophila mushroom body subsets. PLoS Genet. 12, e1006061 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Chen T. W. et al. Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature 499, 295–300 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Aso Y. et al. Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila. Elife 4, 04580 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Bouzaiane E., Trannoy S., Scheunemann L., Placais P. Y. & Preat T. Two independent mushroom body output circuits retrieve the six discrete components of Drosophila aversive memory. Cell Rep. 11, 1280–1292 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Owald D. et al. Activity of defined mushroom body output neurons underlies learned olfactory behavior in Drosophila. Neuron 86, 417–427 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Chiang A. S. et al. Three-dimensional reconstruction of brain-wide wiring networks in Drosophila at single-cell resolution. Curr. Biol. 21, 1–11 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Pai T. P. et al. Drosophila ORB protein in two mushroom body output neurons is necessary for long-term memory formation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 7898–7903 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Placais P. Y., Trannoy S., Friedrich A. B., Tanimoto H. & Preat T. Two pairs of mushroom body efferent neurons are required for appetitive long-term memory retrieval in Drosophila. Cell Rep. 5, 769–780 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Emptage N. J. & Carew T. J. Long-term synaptic facilitation in the absence of short-term facilitation in Aplysia neurons. Science 262, 253–256 (1993). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lewis L. P. et al. A higher brain circuit for immediate integration of conflicting sensory information in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 25, 2203–2214 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Cohn R., Morantte I. & Ruta V. Coordinated and compartmentalized neuromodulation shapes sensory processing in Drosophila. Cell 163, 1742–1755 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Davis R. L. Olfactory memory formation in Drosophila: from molecular to systems neuroscience. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 275–302 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Trannoy S., Redt-Clouet C., Dura J. M. & Preat T. Parallel processing of appetitive short- and long-term memories in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 21, 1647–1653 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Cervantes-Sandoval I., Martin-Pena A., Berry J. A. & Davis R. L. System-like consolidation of olfactory memories in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 33, 9846–9854 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Krashes M. J., Keene A. C., Leung B., Armstrong J. D. & Waddell S. Sequential use of mushroom body neuron subsets during Drosophila odor memory processing. Neuron 53, 103–115 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Qin H. et al. Gamma neurons mediate dopaminergic input during aversive olfactory memory formation in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 22, 608–614 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Dubnau J. & Chiang A. S. Systems memory consolidation in Drosophila. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 84–91 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Waddell S., Armstrong J. D., Kitamoto T., Kaiser K. & Quinn W. G. The amnesiac gene product is expressed in two neurons in the Drosophila brain that are critical for memory. Cell 103, 805–813 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Keene A. C. et al. Diverse odour-conditioned memories require uniquely timed dorsal paired medial neuron output. Neuron 44, 521–533 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Keene A. C., Krashes M. J., Leung B., Bernard J. A. & Waddell S. Drosophila dorsal paired medial neurons provide a general mechanism for memory consolidation. Curr. Biol. 16, 1524–1530 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Pitman J. L. et al. A pair of inhibitory neurons are required to sustain labile memory in the Drosophila mushroom body. Curr. Biol. 21, 855–861 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Keene A. C. & Waddell S. Drosophila olfactory memory: single genes to complex neural circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 341–354 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Wu C. L. et al. Heterotypic gap junctions between two neurons in the Drosophila brain are critical for memory. Curr. Biol. 21, 848–854 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Liu X. & Davis R. L. The GABAergic anterior paired lateral neuron suppresses and is suppressed by olfactory learning. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 53–59 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Wu C. L., Fu T. F., Chou Y. Y. & Yeh S. R. A single pair of neurons modulates egg-laying decisions in Drosophila. PLoS ONE 10, e0121335 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kong E. C. et al. A pair of dopamine neurons target the D1-like dopamine receptor DopR in the central complex to promote ethanol-stimulated locomotion in Drosophila. PLoS ONE 5, e9954 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Wu C. L. et al. Specific requirement of NMDA receptors for long-term memory consolidation in Drosophila ellipsoid body. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1578–1586 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Figures and Supplementary Note 1

ncomms15230-s1.pdf (5.1MB, pdf)
Peer Review File
ncomms15230-s2.pdf (6.6MB, pdf)

Data Availability Statement

All the relevant data supporting the findings of this study are available within the article and its Supplementary Information files or from the corresponding author upon request.


Articles from Nature Communications are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES