Skip to main content
Upsala Journal of Medical Sciences logoLink to Upsala Journal of Medical Sciences
. 2017 Apr 24;122(2):119–126. doi: 10.1080/03009734.2017.1294636

Gambling frequency and symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in relation to problem gambling among Swedish adolescents: a population-based study

Charlotta Hellström a,b,, Philippe Wagner a, Kent W Nilsson a, Jerzy Leppert a, Cecilia Åslund a
PMCID: PMC5441372  PMID: 28436719

Abstract

Aim

To investigate the associations between gambling frequency, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms, and problem gambling among adolescent boys and girls. One hypothesis was that adolescents with increased ADHD symptoms have a higher frequency of gambling compared to adolescents with fewer ADHD symptoms.

Method

A population-based sample of adolescents (aged 15–18 years) completed a questionnaire on demographics, gambling habits, ADHD symptoms, and problematic gambling; 1412 adolescents (from 4440 sampled) with gambling experience were included in the final sample.

Results

A zero-inflated negative binomial regression analysis revealed that increased ADHD symptoms, higher gambling frequency, and higher age were associated with lower odds for being non-susceptible to gambling problems. Moreover, gambling frequency interacted with ADHD symptoms in predicting probability of being non-susceptible to gambling problems. However, when analysing those already susceptible to problem gambling, ADHD symptoms did not modify the effect of gambling frequency on the expected magnitude of gambling problems. In susceptible individuals, problem gambling increased with both increased ADHD symptoms and increased gambling frequency, but the level of problems due to gambling frequency did not change depending on the ADHD symptom level. There was an interaction effect between sex and gambling frequency in relation to gambling problems.

Conclusions

Adolescents with ADHD symptoms seem to be more sensitive to gambling, in terms of being susceptible to developing gambling problems. However, once susceptible, adolescents with ADHD symptoms are affected by gambling frequency similarly to other susceptible participants.

Keywords: Adolescent, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms, gambling, problem gambling, Problem Gambling Severity Index

Introduction

Approximately 0.2%–0.3% of the general population is diagnosed with gambling disorders (1) leading to individual suffering and major societal costs. Problematic gambling has been conceptualized, defined, and measured in different ways (2). In the present study, the terms problematic gambling, problem gambling, and gambling problems have been used interchangeably to describe the negative effects of gambling. Individual demographics such as sex and age are risk factors for problematic gambling (3). Among young adults, problematic gambling has been associated with negative emotions and personality issues (4). Moreover, problematic gambling behaviour has been associated with poor general health (5), and individuals diagnosed with a gambling disorder have high rates of comorbidity with other mental health disorders such as depression, anxiety, and personality disorders (1).

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has been associated with adolescent gambling behaviours, although gambling problems seem to be more related to hyperactivity/impulsivity than to inattention (6). Faregh and Derevensky found no interactions between ADHD symptoms and gambling in relation to gambling pathology; however, ADHD subtypes and gambling severity were unequally associated with depressive affect and emotional problems (7).

Young people are more neurobiologically vulnerable compared with adults and are at higher risk of developing problematic gambling, partly because of their underdeveloped ability to predict consequences due to actions (i.e. gambling) (8). Hyperactivity, low impulse control, and inattention are important issues in research about gambling behaviour among adolescents (7). ADHD at younger ages often persists into adulthood (9). The prevalence of ADHD ranges from 2.2% to 17.8% among children and adolescents aged 10–20 years (9), whereas the prevalence of ADHD among adults is estimated at 2.5% (1). The most common reason for child and adolescent psychiatry referral is symptoms of either depression or ADHD (10). Impulse control problems, as well as a lower capability to predict consequences due to one’s actions, are common factors among adolescents with ADHD (8,11). This might further increase the risk for other problems and disorders, such as negative social relationships and mental health disorders (8,11).

Frequencies of different ADHD symptoms differ between boys and girls. For example, externalizing disorders such as hyperactivity and impulsive behaviours are more common among boys with ADHD; girls with ADHD more often show internalizing disorders including inattention (12). Internalizing disorders may be more difficult to identify than hyperactivity. Furthermore, inattention has been suggested as the most common ADHD subtype (7). Since girls with ADHD have more inattention problems, there is a significant possibility that ADHD among girls remains underdiagnosed (13). Young adults who reported earlier childhood ADHD symptoms experience more gambling-related problems than are found in the general population (14). However, other studies report contradictory results (15). Nevertheless, impulsivity is inherent in both problematic gambling and ADHD (16), in which the hyperactivity and impulsivity subtypes are more closely related to gambling than is the inattention subtype (7). Individuals with hyperactive behaviour prefer quicker, smaller rewards instead of larger rewards that take longer to achieve (17). These impulsive ‘quick win’ preferences are also present among problem gamblers (18). With the growing interest in adolescents’ gambling habits, there is a need for greater knowledge about ADHD symptoms among adolescent gamblers (6).

The study aim was to investigate the associations between gambling frequency, ADHD symptoms, and problematic gambling among adolescent boys and girls. Our hypothesis was that the additive effect of increased gambling frequency and increased ADHD symptoms would be associated with increased gambling problems.

Materials and methods

Participants and procedures

This study was part of SALVe 2012, a survey distributed biennially by the Swedish County Council of Västmanland to monitor the health and life situation of the county’s adolescent population. The adolescent study population is from a county considered fairly representative of Swedish society because of its distribution of educational, income, and employment levels, as well as its urban and rural areas (19). The survey includes demographics, ADHD symptoms, and gambling habits and problematic gambling items. All students in the 9th grade (15–16-year-olds) of compulsory school and the second year of upper secondary school (17–18-year-olds) in Västmanland were the target population. The students answered the questionnaire during class (administered by their teacher) and were informed that participation was voluntary, anonymous, and that they could end their participation at any time. Non-attending students were given a second chance to complete the questionnaire and were defined as late-respondents. The final sample was 4440 adolescents (2186, 49.2% boys; 126 late-respondents). In the final statistical models, only adolescents with gambling experience were included (n = 1412). A flow chart of the study population is presented in Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Flow chart of the study population. Administrative drop-out refers to students from classes or schools that did not participate in SALVe 2012. External non-respondents are students who were absent on the day of data collection and did not return their questionnaire by mail or who declined to participate. Those who did not sufficiently complete the questions for the present study were referred to as ‘internal non-respondents’.

Ethical considerations

The study followed the Swedish guidelines for studies of social science and humanities according to the Declaration of Helsinki. According to Swedish law (Ethical Review Act 2003:460), this type of anonymous study is not subject to ethical approval.

Measures

Sex. Participants were coded as boy (code 1) or girl (code 2).

Parents’ country of birth. Participants with both parents born in Sweden or Scandinavia were classified as Scandinavian ethnicity (code 1); those with at least one parent born outside Scandinavia were classified as non-Scandinavian ethnicity (code 2).

Family constellation. Parents were coded based on living together (code 1) or not living together (code 2).

Parental employment status. Parents were coded as ‘both employed’ (code 1) or ‘one or both unemployed’ (code 2).

Subjective socio-economic status (SES). SES was reported on a 7-point Likert scale (20), where participants were asked to rank the SES of their family; steps 1–3 indicated the lowest status, steps 4–5 was categorized as medium status, and step 7 indicated the highest status. Participants were asked to imagine that society could be understood as a ladder where people at the lowest steps have the least money and those at the highest top are the ones with a lot of money. By referring to how wealthy they thought their own family was when compared to the rest of the society, they were asked to place their family on the scale.

ADHD symptoms. The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self Report Scale (ASRS) (21) was used. Of the 18 ASRS questions, the first six are valid for use alone as a short screening (ASRS-S) (22,23), which was used here. Participants were asked about their frequency of ADHD symptoms within the last 6 months, with response options: never (0 points), rarely (1 point), sometimes (2 points), often (3 points), and very often (4 points); giving a range of 0–24 points. The ASRS-S was validated in an adult non-clinical sample with sensitivity 68.7% and specificity 99.5% (21). To create a dichotomized ADHD symptom variable, the cut-off score of ≥4 was used (21). Scores of 0–3 were categorized as no ADHD (code 0); 4 or more were categorized as ADHD (code 1).

Attention deficit and hyperactivity subgroups. The ASRS-S has two latent factors, attention deficit and hyperactivity (24), that were investigated as separate summation indices in subgroup analyses. Internal consistency values were 0.79–0.87 for the inattention subscale and 0.68–0.89 for the hyperactivity subscale (23). A dichotomous cut-off point was set for each item (21).

Gambling frequency. Items included: Online gambling frequency (‘How often do you gamble on poker, casino or similar on the Internet for real money?’); Offline poker gambling frequency (‘How often do you play poker for money (not online)?’); Gambling frequency on the lottery or other games (‘How often do you gamble for money on the lottery or other games (horse-racing, scratch cards, sports, etc., not online)?’); and Gambling frequency on slot machines (‘How often do you gamble on slot machines (the sort you can win money on, not online)?’).

Response options for each gambling frequency question were: never (0 points), a few times a year (1 point), once a month (2 points), 2–4 times a month (3 points), 2–3 days a week (4 points), 4–5 days a week (5 points), and 6–7 days a week (6 points) (Table 1). We examined the associations between the four gambling frequency variables to identify components. Factor analysis (varimax with Kaiser normalization) revealed one component with an eigenvalue over 1.0 (2.497), factor loading = 0.763–0.853. Cumulative variance was 62.414%. Finally, the gambling frequency index was created by summing the factor scores for each item.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics for the sample of adolescents aged 15–18 years.

Total Boys Girls
n (%) n (%) n (%) Chi-square p
Age
 15–16 years 2011 (45.3%) 997 (45.6%) 1014 (45.0%)
 17–18 years 2429 (57.7%) 1189 (54.4%) 1240 (55.0%) 0.173 0.677
Ethnicity
 Scandinavian 3434 (78.6%) 1654 (77.3%) 1780 (79.8%)
 Non-Scandinavian 937 (21.4%) 487 (22.7%) 450 (20.2%) 4.274 0.039
Parental living condition
 Parents living together 2802 (63.3%) 1413 (65.0%) 1389 (61.7%)
 Parents not living together 1622 (36.7%) 761 (35.0%) 861 (38.3%) 5.067 0.024
Parental working status
 Both parents employed 3582 (81.4%) 1774 (82.1%) 1808 (80.6%)
 At least one parent unemployed 821 (18.6%) 387 (17.9%) 434 (19.4%) 1.524 0.217
Symptoms of ADHD
 No symptoms of ADHD 3605 (81.2%) 1819 (83.2%) 1786 (79.2%)
 Symptoms of ADHD 835 (18.8%) 367 (16.8%) 468 (20.8%) 11.480 <0.001
Online gambling frequency
 Never 3834 (90.2%) 1714 (82.6%) 2120 (97.5%)
 A few times a year 164 (3.9%) 138 (6.7%) 26 (1.2%)
 Once a month 88 (2.1%) 77 (3.7%) 11 (0.5%)
 2–4 times a month 76 (1.8%) 65 (3.1%) 11 (0.5%)
 2–3 days a week 31 (0.7%) 28 (1.4%) 3 (0.1%)
 4–5 days a week 21 (0.5%) 20 (1.0%) 1 (0.0%)
 6–7 days a week 35 (0.8%) 32 (1.5%) 3 (0.5%) 266.480 <0.001
Offline poker gambling frequency
 Never 3887 (89.2%) 1734 (81.4%) 2153 (96.6%)
 A few times a year 305 (7.0%) 248 (11.6%) 57 (2.6%)
 Once a month 84 (1.9%) 77 (3.6%) 7 (0.3%)
 2–4 times a month 44 (1.0%) 36 (1.7%) 8 (0.4%)
 2–3 days a week 20 (0.5%) 17 (0.8%) 3 (0.1%)
 4–5 days a week 7 (0.2%) 7 (0.3%) 0 (0.0%)
 6–7 days a week 12 (0.3%) 12 (0.6%) 0 (0.0%) 267.702 <0.001
Offline gambling frequency on the lottery or other games
 Never 3159 (72.4%) 1459 (68.4%) 1700 (76.1%)
 A few times a year 789 (18.1%) 348 (16.3%) 441 (19.7%)
 Once a month 207 (4.7%) 141 (6.6%) 66 (3.0%)
 2–4 times a month 105 (2.4%) 89 (4.2%) 16 (0.7%)
 2–3 days a week 70 (1.6%) 62 (2.9%) 8 (0.4%)
 4–5 days a week 19 (0.4%) 17 (0.8%) 2 (0.1%)
 6–7 days a week 17 (0.4%) 17 (0.8%) 0 (0.0%) 175.575 <0.001
Offline gambling frequency on slot machines
 Never 3664 (84.1%) 1677 (78.8%) 1987 (89.1%)
 A few times a year 554 (12.7%) 331 (15.6%) 223 (10.0%)
 Once a month 67 (1.5%) 56 (2.6%) 11 (0.5%)
 2–4 times a month 31 (0.7%) 28 (90.3%) 3 (0.1%)
 2–3 days a week 17 (0.4%) 13 (1.3%) 4 (0.2%)
 4–5 days a week 9 (0.2%) 8 (0.4%) 1 (0.0%)
 6–7 days a week 14 (0.3%) 14 (0.7%) 0 (0.0%) 119.554 <0.001
Problem Gambling Severity Index
 No risk/low risk of problem gambling (0–2 points) 1222 (86.5%) 763 (81.6%) 459 (96.2%)
 Moderate risk of problem gambling (3–7 points) 117 (8.3%) 106 (11.3%) 11 (2.3%)
 High risk of problem gambling (8 points) 73 (5.2%) 66 (7.1%) 7 (1.5%) 57.992 <0.001

Problem gambling. Only participants identified as gamblers completed the Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI) (25), nine questions on gambling behaviour used for measuring gambling problem severity (range 0–27 points). The PGSI was originally categorized into four levels separating non-problem gambling and low risk for problem gambling (26). For all analyses, we used the PGSI summation index as a continuous variable (25). With the exception of the descriptive analyses (Table 1), the PGSI was categorized into three levels with the first two combined: 1) non-problem gambling and low risk for problem gambling (0–2 points); 2) moderate risk for problem gambling (3–7 points); and 3) high risk for problem gambling (8 points). PGSI internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) was 0.92.

Statistical analyses

Fisher’s exact probability test was used to investigate whether the gambling frequency index, ADHD symptoms, and the PGSI differed between late-respondents and other participants. Sex differences were analysed using Pearson’s chi-square and Mann–Whitney U tests. Factor analysis (varimax with Kaiser normalization) was used to examine associations between gambling frequency and to identify components or higher-order groups of gambling frequency within different types of gambling forms. Kendall’s Tau and Cramer’s V were used to investigate correlations between study variables of gambling frequency (online gambling frequency, offline poker gambling frequency, gambling frequency on the lottery or other games, gambling frequency on slot machines), ADHD symptoms, and PGSI.

Because most study participants did not have gambling problems according to the PGSI, and because a few cases were quite severe, PGSI data were significantly skewed. The magnitude of skew was such that the analyses could not be conducted using ordinary analysis methods. Instead, because the skew was partly caused by multiple zero-order measurements, zero-inflated negative binomial regression was used. This method combines and simultaneously estimates two separate regression models: the first is a logistic regression model accounting for the excess zero measurements; the second is a negative binomial regression model used to analyse the expected PGSI measurements. Model I accounts for the excess zeros by modelling the probability of being non-susceptible to gambling problems. It demands a non-event as the outcome because the main interest is in the excess of zeros. The outcome of being non-susceptible to gambling problems is therefore in contrast to the standard logistic regression that would model the risk of having gambling problems. The subsequent results are interpreted as the odds ratio (OR) of being non-susceptible to developing gambling problems. The model included covariates: ADHD symptoms, gambling frequency index, a gambling frequency index by sex interaction, and a gambling frequency index by ADHD symptom interaction (the last-mentioned was of primary interest).

Model II used negative binomial regression to analyse the magnitude of gambling problems in susceptible individuals. Resulting estimates were interpreted as the ratio of expected PGSI points, given that the participant is susceptible, and included the same covariates. The same statistical analyses were performed on the ADHD symptoms subgroups hyperactivity and inattention. All analyses were controlled for potential confounding demographic variables (age, sex, parents’ country of birth, parental working status, parental living condition, and SES). As part of the modelling process, to maximize statistical power, covariates and factors that were not statistically significant and that did not alter the gambling by ADHD interaction estimate, whether included or removed, were removed from the model.

The level for statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. Analyses were performed using IBM SPSS statistics, version 22 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY) and STATA version 12 (StataCorp, College Station, TX).

Results

Roughly 10% of adolescents gambled on online poker/casino and offline poker; gambling on slot machines was slightly more common (Table 1). Nearly one in three adolescents reported gambling on lotteries, horse-racing, scratch cards, and sports. Compared with girls, boys had higher frequencies of gambling in every form. Online poker and/or casino were most common among weekly gamblers.

About one-third (n = 1412) of the sample reported gambling according to the PGSI; 117 participants fulfilled the criterion for moderate risk of problem gambling and 73 for problem gambling. The mean PGSI was markedly higher among boys (M = 1.64, SD = 3.68) than girls (M = 0.40, SD = 1.80, Z = −9.007, p < 0.001) (data not shown). Late-respondents did not differ on the main variables of gambling frequency, ADHD symptoms, and PGSI (data not shown). Most participants were of Scandinavian ethnicity and lived with both parents, who were predominantly employed (Table 1).

Mean SES was marginally higher among boys (M = 4.56, SD = 1.02) than girls (M = 4.37, SD = 0.97, Z = −5.867, p < 0.001). About one in four participants reported ADHD symptoms (Table 1). Mean ADHD symptoms among boys (M = 8.52, SD = 4.73) was lower than among girls (M = 9.42, SD = 4.73, Z = −6.128, p < 0.001).

Table 2 shows the correlation coefficients between gambling frequency, ADHD symptoms, and PGSI. Correlations were found for all gambling forms, indicating that gamblers often use several methods. All gambling forms were moderately correlated with PGSI; ADHD symptoms were weakly correlated with PGSI. Although statistically significant, the ADHD symptoms index was only negligibly correlated with the different gambling forms.

Table 2.

Kendall’s Tau (and Cramer’s V for sex) correlations between study variables.

Total population (n = 4440) Gambling population (n = 1412)
Factors 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Sex 1 0.144*** 0.250*** 0.248*** 0.201*** 0.166*** 0.353*** 0.250***
2. Symptoms of ADHD 1 0.040** 0.043** 0.035** 0.065** 0.050** 0.134**
3. Online gambling frequency 1 0.508** 0.310** 0.302** 0.503** 0.417**
4. Offline poker gambling frequency 1 0.351** 0.365** 0.534** 0.424**
5. Offline gambling frequency on the lottery or other games 1 0.334** 0.721** 0.341**
6. Offline gambling frequency on slot machines 1 0.592** 0.369**
7. Gambling frequency index 1 0.431**
8. Problem gambling 1

** p < 0.01 level (2-tailed); *** p < 0.001 level (2-tailed).

The ORs in Table 3 for Model I show the probability of being non-susceptible to developing gambling problems since the model demanded a non-event as the outcome. The interpretation of the logistic regression in Model I is therefore that an OR above 1.0 indicates an increased probability of being non-susceptible to gambling problems and an OR below 1.0 indicates a decreased probability of being non-susceptible to gambling problems (i.e. an increased probability of being susceptible). As part of the modelling process, covariates that were not statistically significant and that did not alter the gambling by ADHD interaction estimate were removed. An increase in ADHD symptoms was associated with lower probability of being non-susceptible to gambling problems (Table 3, Model I). Higher gambling frequency and higher age were associated with lower probability of being non-susceptible to gambling problems (Table 3, Model I). A statistically significant interaction was observed between ADHD symptoms and the gambling frequency index when studying the probability of being non-susceptible for developing gambling problems (Table 3, Model I).

Table 3.

Zero-inflated negative binomial regression analysis, adjusted for an excess of zero values for associations between investigated variables; dependent variable: PGSI.

Model I. Analysis of the chance of not becoming susceptible to developing gambling problems.a
OR p 95% CI
Age 0.48 0.001 0.30 0.75
ADHD symptoms index 0.95 0.041 0.90 1.00
Gambling frequency index 0.46 0.009 0.26 0.82
ADHD symptoms × Gambling frequency index 0.92 0.023 0.85 0.99
Model II. Analysis of adolescents already susceptible to problem gambling.b
IRR p 95% CI
ADHD symptoms index 1.03 0.031 1.003 1.07
Gambling frequency index 1.34 <0.001 1.20 1.51
Sex (ref: boys) 0.25 <0.001 0.16 0.38
Sex × Gambling frequency index 1.67 <0.001 1.34 2.13
a

Model I: adjusted for covariates sex and parents’ country of birth.

b

Model II: adjusted for covariate age.

In Table 3, Model II, the zero-inflated negative binomial regression revealed that an increase in gambling frequency index and ADHD symptoms was related to higher PGSI scores among individuals susceptible to problem gambling. Susceptible girls were less likely to develop gambling problems than susceptible boys, and a significant interaction effect was revealed between sex and gambling frequency in relation to gambling problems (p < 0.001).

The results in Table 3, Model I, show that the effect of gambling frequency index on becoming susceptible to gambling problems increases with increasing ADHD symptoms. The magnitude of these differences is further illustrated in Figure 2, which shows that increased ADHD symptoms and increased gambling frequency were associated with increased PGSI scores, although causality was not investigated.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Illustration of the association between gambling frequency and the Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI). The expected degree of gambling problems in the study sample, irrespective of susceptibility to gambling problems, plotted against gambling frequency for different degrees of ADHD symptoms according to the ADHD symptom index.

Furthermore, when analysing the probability of being non-susceptible to developing gambling problems within the ADHD subgroups, neither hyperactivity symptoms nor inattention symptoms revealed significant interaction effects between the degree of hyperactivity/inattention symptoms and gambling frequency index (Z = −0.37, p = 0.711 and Z = −0.70, p = 0.482) (data not shown).

However, the zero-inflated negative binomial regression used to analyse ADHD subgroups revealed significant interaction effects between the degree of hyperactivity symptoms and gambling frequency index in relation to PGSI scores among individuals susceptible to problem gambling (Z = −2.23, p = 0.026), while no significant interaction effect between inattention symptoms and gambling frequency was found (Z = −1.86, p = 0.063) (data not shown).

Discussion

This study explored the associations between gambling frequency, ADHD symptoms, and PGSI among Swedish adolescents. Adolescents with ADHD may be more sensitive to gambling frequency, in terms of becoming susceptible to developing a gambling problem. However, once susceptible, adolescents with ADHD symptoms are equally affected compared with other susceptible participants based on gambling frequency. Furthermore, gambling frequency seems to be of greater importance for problem gambling than ADHD symptoms among adolescents.

In previous research, the associations between gambling frequency, gambling problems, and ADHD symptoms have been reported with varying results (6,7,27), causing debate about possible causality. In the present study, gambling frequency and ADHD symptoms interacted in relation to the probability of being non-susceptible to gambling problems. However, in individuals already susceptible to gambling problems, the association between gambling frequency and gambling problems did not change depending on the ADHD symptom level.

Notably, girls already susceptible to gambling problems had lower odds for developing gambling problems than did boys. Furthermore, interaction effects were revealed between sex and gambling frequency regarding problem gambling, indicating that sex may not be a simple determining PGSI factor but also an influencing factor for gambling frequency. This finding contributes to previous research showing sex to be a primary risk factor for developing problem gambling behaviours (3). Since adolescents are more neurologically vulnerable than adults to developing addictions (8), it would be of further interest to investigate whether boys and girls differ within those neurobiological factors associated with how reward system neural circuits contribute to the development of gambling problems. The higher prevalence of ADHD among girls may be partly explained by our use of the ASRS-S, for which respondents self-report ADHD symptoms. In previous research (13), girls’ ADHD symptoms were often undetected, in part because girls with ADHD have more inattention problems than do boys with ADHD, who are more hyperactive. Nevertheless, the ASRS-S has been validated and is considered a reliable screening instrument (23). Since hyperactivity symptoms interacted with the gambling frequency index for those already susceptible to gambling problems, and inattention symptoms did not (although those differences were minor), we suggest that further research should assess interaction effects within ADHD subgroups as well as psychological and social differences between boys and girls (12).

It should be mentioned that data from SALVe 2012 have previously been analysed in Hellström et al. (28). Regardless of different aims and included study variables, there are obviously similarities due to study planning, study population, and methods described in the present study.

Several limitations of the study should be noted. First, because all analyses were based on self-reports, there is a risk of information bias due to false or inaccurate answers or misunderstanding the questions. Second, since the design was cross-sectional, we could only speculate on the possible direction of associations because there was no way to predict causality. Third, although gambling activity is not exclusive to adolescents, our sample was limited to this population; the results should not necessarily be considered representative of other age groups. Fourth, gambling activities are more common among boys (29), whereas girls more often suffer from mental health issues (22). The influences of undetected ADHD symptoms among girls should be noted since inaccurate measures for detecting ADHD symptoms in girls may mean that prevalence among this group is underestimated (22).

Fifth, we investigated only ADHD symptoms, not clinical diagnosis. However, self-reported screening for ADHD symptoms may be an effective complement for identifying adolescents at psychiatric risk (22). ASRS-S is seen as an accurate and reliable method for this purpose (23). Our ability to find interaction effects was limited due to low power, because few adolescents in our sample reported gambling problems. This may be partly because gambling under 18 years of age is illegal in Sweden. The fact that only about one-third of the adolescents gambled may have influenced our results. That said, the statistical method of zero-inflated negative binomial regression was specifically chosen to adjust for the skew in the study population caused by an excess of zero measurements on the PGSI. Further, adolescents with gambling problems may have participated in the study to a lesser extent. It is possible that they prefer to gamble instead of participate in school activities and may have been absent from school on the day of data collection. However, late-respondents did not differ from other participants on any of the dependent or independent factors; non-respondents tend to be similar to late-respondents on survey studies (30).

We were unable to investigate further age differences, and age was instead included as a covariate in all analyses. Future research on the associations between ADHD and gambling problems should consider possible age effects.

How well the PGSI separates low-frequency gamblers from medium-frequency gamblers has been the subject of debate (25). Because the present study only used these categorical variables as descriptive measures and the continuous PGSI summation index for all other analyses, the risk of underestimating effects is more likely than the risk of overestimating them. Although analyses were controlled for potential confounding variables, there is the risk that other confounding factors that were not adjusted for may have interfered.

The present study also has several strengths. To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the associations between the frequency of different gambling forms and ADHD symptoms in relation to problem gambling among adolescents. Another strength is the large community adolescent sample from a county that may be considered to represent Sweden as a whole (19). The rather high participation rate reduces the risk of selection bias. The results may thus generalize to adolescent populations from other countries with similar cultures and living conditions.

The exploratory design of the present study contributes to the growing literature on associations between problematic gambling behaviour and ADHD. Adolescents with ADHD symptoms seem to be more sensitive to gambling in terms of becoming susceptible for developing gambling problems. However, once susceptible, adolescents with ADHD symptoms are affected similarly to other susceptible participants based on gambling frequency. Interaction effects between the frequency of gambling and ADHD subgroups (hyperactivity and inattention) further suggest that different personality traits are associated to problem gambling. Information about factors related to gambling problems may be of particular interest to mental health care, psychiatry, psychology, and social work clinicians, as well as policy-makers, parents, and teachers involved in adolescent health and development.

Consistent with previous research, the co-occurrence of gambling frequency and ADHD symptoms in relation to problematic gambling further emphasizes the importance of co-screening for both gambling and ADHD symptoms when examining problematic gambling among adolescents (31). However, replication of the results and identification of the direction of causality are needed before interventions should be developed. Moreover, further investigation of gambling frequency and ADHD symptoms in relation to problem gambling in clinical settings is needed.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all the participating students and school staff who made this study possible.

C.H., C.Å., and K.W.N. were responsible for the study concept and design. C.H., C.Å., and P.W. performed the statistical analyses and C.H. drafted the manuscript. C.H., C.Å., P.W., K.W.N. and J.L. critically reviewed the content and read and approved the final version of the manuscript for publication.

Funding Statement

Grants from the following funds and organizations are acknowledged: The Svenska Spel Research Council, the Uppsala and Örebro Regional Research Council, the Fredrik and Ingrid Thurings Foundation, the County Council of Västmanland, the König-Söderströmska Foundation, the Swedish Psychiatric Foundation, and the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research (FAS). The study sponsors had no role in the study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report.

Disclosure statement

The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Notes on contributors

Charlotta Hellström, PhD, is a Lecturer and Head of Department for Public Health Sciences at School of Health, Care and Social Welfare/Division of Public Health Sciences. Mälardalen University, Västerås, Sweden.

Philippe Wagner is a Statistician as well as a PhD-student at Centre for Clinical Research, Uppsala University, Västmanland County Hospital, Västerås, Sweden.

Kent W. Nilsson is a Professor of Psychiatry at Centre for Clinical Research, Uppsala University, Västmanland County Hospital, Västerås, Sweden.

Jerzy Leppert is a Professor Emeritus of Medicine, Centre for Clinical Research, Uppsala University, Västmanland County Hospital, Västerås, Sweden.

Cecilia Åslund is an Adjunct Professor (Docent) of Psychiatry at Centre for Clinical Research, Uppsala University, Västmanland County Hospital, Västerås, Sweden.

References

  • 1.American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Blaszczynski A, Nower L.. A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling. Addiction. 2002;97:487–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Johansson A, Grant JE, Kim SW, Odlaug BL, Gotestam KG.. Risk factors for problematic gambling: a critical literature review. J Gambl Stud. 2009;25:67–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Slutske WS, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Poulton R.. Personality and problem gambling: a prospective study of a birth cohort of young adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:769–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Statens folkhälsoinstitut. Spel om pengar och spelproblem i Sverige 2008/2009. Huvudresultat från SWELOGS befolkningsstudie. Östersund: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Clark C, Nower L, Walker DM.. The relationship of ADHD symptoms to gambling behaviour in the USA: results from the national longitudinal study of adolescent health. Int Gambl Stud. 2013;13:37–51. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Faregh N, Derevensky J.. Gambling behavior among adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Gambl Stud. 2011;27:243–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Derevensky JL, Gupta R, Winters K.. Prevalence rates of youth gambling problems: are the current rates inflated? J Gambl Stud. 2003;19:405–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Skounti M, Philalithis A, Galanakis E.. Variations in prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder worldwide. Eur J Pediatr. 2007;166:117–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Staller JA. Diagnostic profiles in outpatient child psychiatry. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 2006;76:98–102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Becker S, Luebbe A, Langberg J.. Co-occurring mental health problems and peer functioning among youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a review and recommendations for future research. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2012;15:279–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Levy F, Hay DA, Bennett KS, McStephen M.. Gender differences in ADHD subtype comorbidity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44:368–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sonnby K. Co-occurring symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and depression. Sex, aetiology, help-seeking and assessment. PhD thesis. Uppsala, Sweden. Uppsala University; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Breyer JL, Botzet AM, Winters KC, Stinchfield RD, August G, Realmuto G.. Young adult gambling behaviors and their relationship with the persistence of ADHD. J Gambl Stud. 2009;25:227–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Barnes GM, Welte JW, Hoffman JH, Dintcheff BA.. Shared predictors of youthful gambling, substance use, and delinquency. Psychol Addict Behav. 2005;19:165–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Luman M, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA.. The impact of reinforcement contingencies on AD/HD: a review and theoretical appraisal. Clin Psychol Rev. 2005;25:183–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Petry NM. Pathological gamblers, with and without substance abuse disorders, discount delayed rewards at high rates. J Abnorm Psychol. 2001;110:482–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.SCB Description of the population in Sweden 2008. Population statistics unit. Örebro: Statistics Sweden; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Åslund C, Starrin B, Leppert J, Nilsson KW.. Social status and shaming experiences related to adolescent overt aggression at school. Aggressive Behav. 2009;35:1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, Demler O, Faraone S, Hiripi E, et al. . The World health Organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med. 2005;35:245–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Sonnby K, Aslund C, Leppert J, Nilsson KW.. Symptoms of ADHD and depression in a large adolescent population: co-occurring symptoms and associations to experiences of sexual abuse. Nord J Psychiatry. 2011;65:315–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sonnby K, Skordas K, Olofsdotter S, Vadlin S, Nilsson KW, Ramklint M.. Validation of the World health organization adult ADHD self-report scale for adolescents. Nord J Psychiatry. 2015;69:216–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hesse M. The ASRS-6 has two latent factors: attention deficit and hyperactivity. J Atten Disord. 2013;17:203–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Orford J, Wardle H, Sproston K, Erens B, Griffiths M.. PGSI and DSM-IV in the 2007 British gambling prevalence survey: reliability, item response, factor structure and inter-scale agreement. Int Gambl Stud. 2010;10:31–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ferris J, Wynne H. The Canadian problem gambling index (final report). Ottawa, Ontario, Canada: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Breyer JL, Botzet A, Winters K, Stinchfield R, August G, Realmuto G.. Young adult gambling behaviors and their relationship with the persistence of ADHD. J Gambl Stud. 2009;25:227–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hellström C, Nilsson KW, Leppert J, Åslund C.. Effects of adolescent online gaming time and motives on depressive, musculoskeletal and psychosomatic symptoms. Ups J Med Sci. 2015;120:263–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jackson A, Dowling N, Thomas S, Bond L, Patton G.. Adolescent gambling behaviour and attitudes: a prevalence study and correlates in an Australian population. Int J Ment Health Addiction. 2008;6:325–52. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Miller LE, Smith KL.. Handling nonresponse issues. J Extension. 1983;21:45–50. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Gupta R, Derevensky J.. A treatment approach for adolescents with gambling problems In: Derevensky J, Gupta R, editors. Gambling problems in youth. New York: Springer; 2005. p. 165–88. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Upsala Journal of Medical Sciences are provided here courtesy of Upsala Medical Society

RESOURCES