Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2017 May 1;114(20):5300–5305. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1700712114

Variation in the β-endorphin, oxytocin, and dopamine receptor genes is associated with different dimensions of human sociality

Eiluned Pearce a, Rafael Wlodarski a, Anna Machin a, Robin I M Dunbar a,b,1
PMCID: PMC5441808  PMID: 28461468

Significance

Social behavior in mammals is underpinned by a number of social neuropeptides. Most studies, however, focus on a single neuropeptide (often oxytocin), and invariably only in the context of reproductive relationships. Here, we examine the associations between the six main social neuropeptides (endorphins, oxytocin, vasopressin, dopamine, serotonin, and testosterone) and social indices in three separate social domains (disposition, dyadic/romantic relationships, and social network). We show that each neuropeptide is quite specific in its domain of influence, although endorphins and dopamine influence all three social domains. The results suggest that the social importance of endorphins may have been underestimated.

Keywords: genetics, empathy, romantic relationships, social networks, social neuropeptides

Abstract

There is growing evidence that the number and quality of social relationships have substantial impacts on health, well-being, and longevity, and, at least in animals, on reproductive fitness. Although it is widely recognized that these outcomes are mediated by a number of neuropeptides, the roles these play remain debated. We suggest that an overemphasis on one neuropeptide (oxytocin), combined with a failure to distinguish between different social domains, has obscured the complexity involved. We use variation in 33 SNPs for the receptor genes for six well-known social neuropeptides in relation to three separate domains of sociality (social disposition, dyadic relationships, and social networks) to show that three neuropeptides (β-endorphin, oxytocin, and dopamine) play particularly important roles, with each being associated predominantly with a different social domain. However, endorphins and dopamine have a much wider compass than oxytocin (whose effects are confined to romantic/reproductive relationships and often do not survive control for other neuropeptides). In contrast, vasopressin, serotonin, and testosterone play only limited roles.


It has become apparent, over the last decade, that individual differences in the number and quality of people’s social relationships have direct implications for their health and well-being, including effects on mortality greater than well-established risk factors such as smoking, alcohol abuse, and lack of exercise (14). Those with functioning social relationships generally experience greater happiness and longevity, quicker recovery from ill health and surgery, and decreased risk of illness, while also reducing their risk of developing addictions and psychopathology or exhibiting antisocial behavior (57). Likewise, in both primates (8, 9) and equids (10), social network size influences longevity, reproductive fitness, and the ability to cope with social stress.

The impact of genetic variation on individual differences in social behavior and cognition occurs mainly through altering the function of a key set of neuropeptides. Although these neuropeptides may act as a cascade, most studies have focused on individual neuropeptides. Much has been made, for example, of the role of oxytocin as a correlate of pair bonding in small mammals (11, 12), and arguably humans (13), but recent studies have indicated that pair bonding in voles also involves the β-endorphin system (14, 15). Such cases raise doubts about claims that particular neuropeptides are responsible for broad categories of behavior.

The μ-opioid receptor (16) has been of particular interest because it is the target of β-endorphin, which seems to play an important role in maintaining social bonds in primates (including humans) (1719). β-Endorphin is a likely candidate in the maintenance of long-term relationships in humans because, unlike oxytocin, it can underpin relationships outside of the context of intercourse or parturition and operates effectively at both the dyadic and group levels (2022). It has specifically been argued that, whereas oxytocin may be a universal feature of mammalian sociality, primates’ unusual lifelong bonded relationships requires a more robust neurochemical underpinning, and the endorphin system was exapted for this purpose (23). It may be especially important in this respect that, whereas oxytocin acts endogenously (modulating an individual’s social style), β-endorphin acts exogenously (its release can be triggered in others by, for example, social touch) (18, 20).

Most social neuroscience suffers from a second important limitation: It invariably concentrates on a very narrow spectrum of social behavior (mainly romantic and parent–child bonding, empathy, and dyadic interactions in economic games) or has used meaninglessly broad definitions of sociality (as in a recent metaanalysis that failed to find a significant association between the two most-studied OXTR SNPs, rs53576 and rs2254298, and “social behavior”) (24). Primates, in particular, are characterized by multilayered social groups in which individuals are embedded in an inner core of intense relationships, which in turn are embedded in larger, more loosely structured social networks (25, 26). Intense, lifelong social bonds and structured social complexity of this kind are rare outside the primates (27, 28). Limiting interest to dyadic relationships, as most social neuroscience does, massively underestimates the complexity involved in primate, and especially human, sociality. An important distinction needs to be drawn between social dispositions, intimate dyadic relationships, and the wider social network within which the individual is embedded. Details of the measures used to index these variables are given in Materials and Methods and SI Appendix.

Variation in OXTR SNPs has been linked to the ability to identify emotions and empathic concern (29), and variation in OXTR and AVPR1a alleles has been associated with individual capacities for affective and cognitive empathy, respectively (30). Positron emission tomography (PET) and genetic studies have linked β-endorphin (μ-opioid) receptor variation with attachment style (31, 32), with OPRM1 variation explicitly related to fearful attachment styles (33). In addition, Gillath (34) found that, whereas variants of DRD2 are related to anxious attachment, HTR2A variation may be related to avoidant attachment.

Romantic relationship status, relationship quality, and sexual behaviors have been linked to OXTR variation in women and AVPR1a variation in men, although this sexual division is not always straighforward (12, 3537). In addition, it has become clear that other neuropeptides also play a significant role in the formation and maintenance of sexual relationships. For instance, both the dopamine and opioid systems have been shown to interact to maintain monogamous pair bonds in prairie voles (15, 38). In humans, DRD2 and HTR2A variation has been linked to different “loving styles” (39) and DRD1 and DRD2 genotypes have been linked to age at first sexual intercourse (40), whereas HTR1A variation is associated with romantic relationship formation (41). In addition, OPRM1 and HTR2A have been associated with variation in mate-choice success differentially in the sexes in “speed-dating” situations (42). Furthermore, AR variation has been associated with relationship status and testosterone response in opposite-sex interactions (43, 44). Here, we examine variation in these genes in relation to both romantic relationship quality [Relationship Assessment Scale (RAS)] and sexual attitudes and behaviors [Sociosexual Orientation Inventory Revised (SOI-R)] more generally.

By comparison, with work on dyadic relationships, the role of neuropeptides in the wider social context beyond the dyad has been almost totally ignored. What little work has been done suggests that variation in OXTR can predict the size and diversity of social networks via its influence on social temperament (45), as well as being related to feelings of social connectedness (46) and “general sociality” (47). Other studies have, however, provided indirect evidence for a relationship between endorphins and social network size (48). Aside from these, little has been done to examine the influence of other neuropeptides on wider social network. The present study provides a significant attempt to evaluate the role these neuropeptides play at this larger social scale.

We genotyped 33 candidate SNPs from nine genes (SI Appendix, Table S1 and SI Materials and Methods) for 757 white British individuals (423 female) who did not report any previous psychological conditions for which they had received treatment. For analysis, we used PLINK, a software package for genetic analysis. PLINK allows us to test simultaneously for three alternative allelic conformations [additivity (add), dominance (domdev), and heterozygotic (geno 2df) effects]. We also checked for interactive effects with sex and age; whereas simple associations between sociality and sex or age are not themselves of direct interest in this study, we nonetheless report these results.

Results

There were significant associations (P < 0.05) between SNPs and all of the sociality measures across the three domains (SI Appendix, Table S2). Fig. 1 shows graphs for one representative SNP from each gene in each of the three domains of sociality, and Fig. 2 provides an overall summary of the significant results in the form of a heat map.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Results for one representative SNP from each neuropeptide in each of the three domains of sociality (disposition, relationships, and wider network) for males (light shading) and females (dark shading). Significant gene effects on sociality are represented by plots with gray shaded plot areas and include the model parameter t value. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Heat map showing percentage of SNPs for each neuropeptide that is significantly associated with behavioral indices of the three domains of sociality (disposition, relationships, and wider network). Number of SNPs in each case are as follows: testosterone, 1; endorphin, 5; vasopressin, 2; oxytocin, 10; dopamine, 4; and serotonin, 2. Number of scales are as follows: disposition, 4; relationship quality, 2; and network, 2.

Disposition.

One OPRM1 SNP, rs3778151, showed a significant effect of genotype on anxious attachment (geno_2df, P = 0.025; Fig. 1), whereas another, rs648893, showed an interaction effect between the additive model and sex (add x sex, P = 0.049). One OXTR SNP (rs237897) also showed a significant positive effect on anxious attachment (geno_2df, P = 0.017). No SNPs showed significant associations with avoidant attachment. The AR SNP showed a significant negative additive effect on anxious attachment (add, P = 0.009, Fig. 1). Although the OPRM1 SNPs remained significantly associated with anxious attachment in models controlling for AR rs6152 or OXTR rs237897, neither the AR or OXTR SNPs were significantly associated with anxious attachment when controlling for the other SNP.

In terms of the empathy measures, OPRM1 rs3778151 was found to have a significant nonlinear relationship with Empathy Quotient (EQ) (domdev, P = 0.025), with the negative effect suggesting that the minor allele has lower EQ scores than expected from an additive model. In addition, the ANNK1 variant rs1800497 showed a significant effect on EQ (geno_2df, P = 0.46). OPRM1 rs3778151 also had a significant additive effect with sex on Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET) scores (add x sex, P < 0.001), as did AVPR1a rs11174811 (Fig. 1: add x sex, P = 0.021), whereas OXTR rs2228485 had a nonlinear effect on RMET (domdev, P = 0.049). The AVPR1a and OPRM1 SNPs remained significantly associated with RMET when controlling for OXTR rs2228485, but only OPRM1 rs37778151 showed a significant partial relationship when AVPR1a was taken into account. AVPR1a remained significantly associated with RMET when OPRM1 rs1799971 was included in the model. However, OPRM1 rs3778151 SNP was not significantly linked to EQ once ANKK1 rs1800497 was included in the model, but the latter did not show a significant partial effect either.

Dyadic Relationships.

Two OPRM1 SNPs showed significant effects on RAS scores of relationship quality (rs2075572: domdev x sex, P = 0.027; rs648893: geno_2df, P = 0.036), as did two OXTR SNPS: rs2268490 (domdev, P = 0.032) and rs4686302 (geno_2df, P = 0.025), whereas a third OXTR SNP, rs2254298, showed a trend toward significance (geno_2df, P = 0.052). The OPRM1 SNPs remained significantly associated with RAS independently of the OXTR SNPs, but the opposite was not true.

Seven of the 10 OXTR SNPs were found to have significant associations with SOI-R scores: rs237887 (additive x sex, P = 0.014; Fig. 1), rs2268490 (geno_2df, P = 0.046), rs2254298 (geno_2df, P = 0.046), rs13316193 (geno_2df, P = 0.046), rs53576 (domdev, P = 0.035), rs237897 (domdev x sex, P = 0.034), and rs4686302 (add x sex, P = 0.047). DRD1 rs265981 was also found to interact with sex and be linked to SOI-R scores (domdev x sex, P = 0.039). OXTR rs237887, rs13316193, rs53576, rs237897, and rs4686302 remained significantly associated with SOI-R scores independently of the DRD1 SNP rs265981, and the latter remained significantly associated with SOI-R independently of the OXTR SNPs that were significantly linked to this measure.

Wider Network.

In terms of personal network size, there were significant associations with OXTR rs237887 (geno_2df, P = 0.025), DRD2 SNP rs4648317 (add x sex, P = 0.006), and the HTR1a SNP rs6295 (domdev x sex, P = 0.012, Fig. 1). Both DRD2 and HTR1a remained significantly associated with network size when models also controlled for OXTR variation, but OXTR SNPs did not show significant partial effects in these models.

For feelings of connection to their local community [measured using Aron’s Inclusion of Other in Self (IOS) scale] (49), significant associations were found for OPRM1 rs1799971 (domdev, P = 0.027), OXTR rs53576 (domdev, P = 0.025), and DRD2 rs1076560 (add x sexp = 0.029, Fig. 1) and ANNK1 rs1800497 (downstream from DRD2; add x sex, P = 0.029). It should be noted that due to smaller numbers of minor allele homozygotes, the variance for this group is greater than for carriers of the major allele for both rs1076560 (see Fig. 1) and rs1800497. Although OPRM1 rs1799971 did not remain significantly associated with IOS scores when controlling for the variance in OXTR and DRD2/ANKK1, both the OXTR and DRD2/ANKK1 SNPs remained significantly associated with IOS in models that controlled for rs1799971 or each other.

Overall Distribution.

Running a large number of statistical tests risks inflating significance levels; however, we used an optimized permutation test that is generally considered to be conservative (SI Appendix). Moreover, less than 18% of possible relationships between genes and behaviors are in fact significant (many of which do not survive controlling for the influence of other neuropeptides).

Of greater interest, however, is whether the various neuropeptides are equally effective in all domains. To assess this question, we can treat the data in Fig. 2 as a straightforward contingency table, with a P value of 0.05 as a convenient criterion for inclusion. These data are not subject to multiple comparisons effects because the P value is simply a criterion for a binary partition of the dataset (SI Appendix). The distribution of significant effects shown in Fig. 2 is highly nonrandom (χ2 = 51.7, df = 10, P < 0.0001), with disproportionately higher frequencies than expected for endorphins, oxytocin, and dopamine. Partitioning χ2 for these three neuropeptides yields nonsignificant (i.e., random) distributions across the three social domains for endorphins (χ2 = 4.7, df = 2, P = 0.097) and dopamine (χ2 = 0.7, df = 2, P = 0.716), but a significantly nonrandom distribution for oxytocin (χ2 = 7.5, df = 2, P = 0.023). In sum, oxytocin is very specific in its functional domain (romantic relationships), whereas endorphins and dopamine both have broad spectrum effects.

Discussion

Hitherto, the majority of studies looking at genetic variation and sociality have focused on one neuropeptide (usually oxytocin), thereby ignoring confounds due to the potentially parallel roles of other neuropeptides. In addition, researchers have invariably focused on a narrow facet of sociality. As a result, it remains unclear whether particular neuropeptides have a general role in creating or sustaining social relationships or are more closely allied to specific domains of social engagement. By simultaneously comparing many of the candidate receptor genes previously independently linked to human social behavior, and in the context of three different social domains, this study provides a more nuanced picture.

Our results suggest that anxious, but not avoidant, attachment is associated with OPRM1, AR, and OXTR variation. Both measures of empathy were related to OPRM1 genotypes: there were significant associations with empathy (RMET) for three of five OPRM1 SNPs, one of which was also significantly associated with the EQ. However, only OPRM1 remained significant when controlling for other significant genes. Romantic relationship quality and sexual attitudes/behavior were significantly and strongly associated with variation in OXTR (70% of SNPs showing a significant effect on sociosexuality, SOI-R), as well as two OPRM1 SNPs and the DRD1 SNP. The OXTR SNPs rs2268490 and rs4686302 were linked to both RAS and SOI-R scores, and OXTR rs2254298 was significantly linked to SOI-R (and approached significance in relation to RAS, an index of relationship quality), though these relationships did not survive controlling for OPRM1. Vasopressin had a significant association only with RAS, confirming an earlier finding that it is predictive of high levels of promiscuity (39), but in a way suggestive of impulsivity or lack of inhibition rather than promiscuity per se (act first, and think about the consequences afterward). The two measures of engagement in a wider social network were related to variation in HTR1a (network size) and the dopamine genes DRD2 (network size: rs4648317; IOS: rs1076560) and ANKK1 (IOS), as well as one OPRM1 SNP (IOS) and two OXTR SNPs (one for IOS, one for social network size).

As has previously been found in both voles and humans, we found strong associations between oxytocin receptor variation and romantic relationships. Although a smaller number of OXTR SNPs were found to be linked to measures of wider social engagement (corroborating some previous findings) (50), it may be that this relationship is mediated through the partner’s network. An individual’s ability to empathize, and their typical way of relating in close relationships (their attachment style), is also likely to feed into their romantic relationships, and a small number of OXTR SNPs were found to be tied to attachment style in close friendships and measures of empathy in this study, echoing past work (30, 31). Whereas previous studies focused on empathy or attachment independently of other social domains, such that the unique effects of OXTR variation on different facets of sociality could not be assessed, the current study provides convincing evidence that the primary impact of OXTR variation in the human social sphere is specific to reproductive relationships (romantic/sexual and parent–child), as it is in voles and other small mammals. Note, however, that, despite its prominence in the literature, the significance of the role played by oxytocin often disappears when we control for endorphins, even in the context of romantic relationships, suggesting that endorphins may be more important than oxytocin even here. This finding may support the suggestion that the neurochemical mechanism underpinning long-term relationships is the enodgenous opioid system rather than oxytocin (a neurochemical, which is incapable of maintaining a long-term impact on behavior and may exert most of its influence at the commencement of romantic relationships) (35, 51).

Dopamine receptor variants were most closely linked with social engagement beyond the dyad. Social network size was also the only index of sociality that showed a significant association with variation in serotonin receptor 1a. To our knowledge, dopamine function has never before been studied in relation to social interaction beyond the dyad, with past research showing links only to romantic relationships or parenting (15, 39, 40). By comparing different social domains, we provide the insight that variation in genes associated with dopamine receptor 2 may be predominantly linked to engagement in wider social groups. This relationship may reflect the fact that many of the behavioral mechanisms we use for simultaneously bonding multiple individuals involve activities (laughter, singing, dancing) (21, 22, 52) that are pleasurable. In contrast, variation in the DRD1 variant rs265981 was linked only to individual differences in sociosexual orientation, and this relationship remained robust even when controlling for variance in OXTR. Whereas the second finding makes sense in terms of the well-known reward function of dopamine, the mechanism facilitating the first finding is not at all clear and begs investigation.

In contrast, OPRM1 variations were most strongly linked to individual differences in attachment style and the ability to identify emotional facial expressions (RMET), as well as a self-report measure of empathy (EQ). The density of μ-opioid receptors has previously been found to be related to attachment style, though mainly with the avoidance dimension (32). Importantly, OPRM1 variation was also associated with both dyadic relationship quality and community integration, which may reflect the way social dispositions affect these higher-level outcomes. Perhaps because of its role in the pain system, much previous work on β-endorphin has focused on “social pain” resulting from social rejection (53, 54). However, it is becoming increasingly clear that β-endorphin is also involved in the creation and maintenance of social relationships (19, 51), and this may be reflected in the finding that OPRM1 variation is linked to the extent to which individuals experience social rewards (31) rather than to pain per se. Recent PET work in humans has confirmed that the μ-opioid receptors are activated by touch by a romantic partner (20), while, in synchronous group activities, endorphin release is thought to play a key part in bonding group members (21, 22). As with the effects of allogrooming in nonhuman primates (17, 18), it may be the reward effects of opiates that is important in this context or something to do with a sense of trust engendered by the opioid-induced feelings of relaxation and calmness in the presence of another individual.

The finding that OPRM1 variation is most strongly linked to the ability to empathize hints at one mechanism by which β-endorphin creates social connections, namely through increasing the capacity to read others’ emotions and put oneself in their place. Variation in the ability to empathize likely impacts on romantic relationship quality as well, going some way to explaining our finding that OPRM1 variation was also associated with relationship quality (RAS) scores. Similarly, the fact that one of the OPRM1 SNPs and DRD2 variation correlates with degree of community integration may reflect known interactions between the dopamine and endorphin systems in both humans and rodents (40, 55, 56), whereby endorphin activation may itself trigger a dopamine response. It may be that variation in individuals’ capacities to empathize (linked with endorphin function) feeds into their dopamine-mediated ability to maintain support networks within their community.

In sum, two important conclusions emerge: (i) most neuropeptides are associated with one particular social domain, and (ii) only endorphin and dopamine are widely active across all three domains. These findings reinforce recent research suggesting the wide-ranging influence of both dopamine and endorphin in the formation of bonds within a range of social contexts. In part, this may reflect an interaction with oxytocin and testosterone and the close structural relationship between the oxytocin- and endorphin-producing neurons in the hypothalamus (19) and the limbic dopaminergic reward pathway (57).

Given the evidence that poor relationships and network disintegration lead to morbidity and mortality risks even greater than those of alcohol abuse, lack of exercise. and smoking (1), our findings have important practical implications. Whereas the deleterious effects of these other risk factors are well established in the public sphere, have been closely studied by researchers, and are relatively well understood, our understanding of human and animal sociality, its neurobiological underpinnings, and functional implications remain patchy at best. In addition, our results may have wider implications for understanding the evolution of sociality, in that network size and relationship quality are known to have significant impacts on reproduction and fitness in primates and other mammals (810). It seems that the endorphin and dopamine systems may play especially important roles in mediating this relationship. Ultimately, what is important about endorphins may be that it is possible to trigger their activation in other individuals via behavioral mechanisms, something that seems not to be possible for oxytocin.

Materials and Methods

For full details, see SI Appendix. For the data, see Dataset S1.

Participants.

The final sample consisted of 757 individuals (423 female) aged 18–65 (mean 40.6) years who identified themselves as Caucasian, were physically healthy, did not use recreational drugs, were not on drug replacement therapy, or taking medication for a psychological condition. Ethics approval for the study was granted by the University of Oxford Central Ethics Committee (Ref: MS-IDREC-C2-2015-005). All participants provided written informed consent.

Questionnaires.

Participants completed self-report questionnaires presented digitally on mobile devices. These included: the RMET, the EQ scale, and the Experiences of Close Relationships scale as indices of social disposition; the SOI-R and the RAS as indices of dyadic relationship quality; and the number of close friends and family index and the IOS scale as indices of wider network engagement. For details, see SI Appendix.

Genotyping.

A single sample of saliva was taken from each participant using an OrageneDNA collection kit (2 mL of saliva), which renders the samples acellular. The DNA was extracted using Kleargene technology. We genotyped 33 candidate SNPs from nine genes (SI Appendix, Table S1). These included 11 candidate SNPs for OXTR (oxytocin), 3 SNPs for AVPR1a (vasopression), 6 SNPs for OPRM1 (β-endorphins), 3 SNPs for DRD1, 3 for DRD2, 1 for ANKK1 (located downstream from DRD2) (dopamine), 1 SNP for HTR1A, 2 for HTR2A (serotonin), and 1 SNP for AR (testosterone).

Analysis.

Analysis was conducted using PLINK version 1.9 (SI Appendix shows further details). Participants with less than 90% coverage were removed (11 in total). All of the SNPs had at least 95% coverage. Only 4 of the SNPs differed significantly from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium in the full sample (OXTR rs237897 P = 0.048, OXTR rs2228485 P = 0.045, DRD1 rs265981 P = 0.007, and OPRM1 rs648893 P = 0.01). Clustering according to linkage disequilibrium (LD) pruned 7 SNPs from the main sample and 2 were excluded due to very low minor allele frequencies, leaving 10 OXTR SNPs, 2 AVPR1a SNPs, 5 OPRM1 SNPs, 1 AR SNP, 1 DRD1 SNP, 2 DRD2 SNPs, 1 ANKK1 SNP, and the HTR1a and HTR2a SNPs in the final analysis. We used an optimized permutation test to control for multiple testing. Genotypic models were applied to all of the diploid SNPs, including add, domdev, and geno_2df, as well as age, sex, and the sex x genotype interactions. To test the independence of associations between particular SNPs and specific social variables, we ran post hoc models that controlled for the effect of additional SNPs that also showed significant associations with the same social variable.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary File
pnas.1700712114.sapp.pdf (489.9KB, pdf)
Supplementary File
pnas.1700712114.sd01.xlsx (522.1KB, xlsx)

Acknowledgments

We thank Dianne Newbury for genetic and statistical advice; Cole Robertson, Jacques Launay, Bronwyn Tarr, Pádraig MacCarron, Jackie Thompson, and Anna Szala for help with data collection; the Edinburgh, Cheltenham, and British Science Festivals; and the University of Oxford Natural History Museum (especially Janet Stott), for allowing us to collect data at their events and venues. The work was funded by European Research Council Advanced Investigator Grant 295663 (to R.I.M.D.).

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1700712114/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  • 1.Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Layton JB. Social relationships and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. PLoS Med. 2010;7:e1000316. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Cacioppo JT, Cacioppo S. Social relationships and health: The toxic effects of perceived social isolation. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2014;8:58–72. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Umberson D, Montez JK. Social relationships and health: A flashpoint for health policy. J Health Soc Behav. 2010;51:S54–S66. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Eisenberger NI. Social ties and health: A social neuroscience perspective. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23:407–413. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Schindler A, Sack P-M. Exploring attachment patterns in patients with comorbid borderline personality and substance use disorders. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2015;203:820–826. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Delvecchio E, Di Riso D, Lis A, Salcuni S. Adult attachment, social adjustment, and well-being in drug-addicted inpatients. Psychol Rep. 2016;118:587–607. doi: 10.1177/0033294116639181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Eaves LJ, Prom EC, Silberg JL. The mediating effect of parental neglect on adolescent and young adult anti-sociality: A longitudinal study of twins and their parents. Behav Genet. 2010;40:425–437. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9336-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J. Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science. 2003;302:1231–1235. doi: 10.1126/science.1088580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Silk JB, et al. Strong and consistent social bonds enhance the longevity of female baboons. Curr Biol. 2010;20:1359–1361. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.05.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Cameron EZ, Setsaas TH, Linklater WL. Social bonds between unrelated females increase reproductive success in feral horses. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:13850–13853. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900639106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Carter CS, DeVries AC, Getz LL. Physiological substrates of mammalian monogamy: The prairie vole model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1995;19:303–314. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(94)00070-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Donaldson ZR, Young LJ. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science. 2008;322:900–904. doi: 10.1126/science.1158668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Carter CS, Grippo AJ, Pournajafi-Nazarloo H, Ruscio MG, Porges SW. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and sociality. In: Inga DN, Rainer L, editors. Progress in Brain Research. Elsevier; The Netherlands: 2008. pp. 331–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Resendez SL, Kuhnmuench M, Krzywosinski T, Aragona BJ. κ-Opioid receptors within the nucleus accumbens shell mediate pair bond maintenance. J Neurosci. 2012;32:6771–6784. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5779-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Resendez SL, et al. μ-Opioid receptors within subregions of the striatum mediate pair bond formation through parallel yet distinct reward mechanisms. J Neurosci. 2013;33:9140–9149. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4123-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Inoue K, Burkett JP, Young LJ. Neuroanatomical distribution of μ-opioid receptor mRNA and binding in monogamous prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) and non-monogamous meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus) Neuroscience. 2013;244:122–133. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.03.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Fabre-Nys C, Meller RE, Keverne EB. Opiate antagonists stimulate affiliative behaviour in monkeys. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1982;16:653–659. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(82)90432-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Keverne EB, Martensz ND, Tuite B. Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1989;14:155–161. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(89)90065-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Machin AJ, Dunbar RIM. The brain opioid theory of social attachment: A review of the evidence. Behaviour. 2011;148:985–1025. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Nummenmaa L, et al. Social touch modulates endogenous μ-opioid system activity in humans. Neuroimage. 2016;138:242–247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Weinstein D, Launay J, Pearce E, Dunbar RI, Stewart L. Group music performance causes elevated pain thresholds and social bonding in small and large groups of singers. Evol Hum Behav. 2016;37:152–158. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Dunbar RIM, et al. Social laughter is correlated with an elevated pain threshold. Proc Biol Sci. 2012;279:1161–1167. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.1373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Broad KD, Curley JP, Keverne EB. Mother-infant bonding and the evolution of mammalian social relationships. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2006;361:2199–2214. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH. A sociability gene? Meta-analysis of oxytocin receptor genotype effects in humans. Psychiatr Genet. 2014;24:45–51. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e3283643684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kudo H, Dunbar RIM. Neocortex size and social network size in primates. Anim Behav. 2001;62:711–722. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hill RA, Bentley RA, Dunbar RIM. Network scaling reveals consistent fractal pattern in hierarchical mammalian societies. Biol Lett. 2008;4:748–751. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Shultz S, Dunbar RIM. The evolution of the social brain: Anthropoid primates contrast with other vertebrates. Proc Biol Sci. 2007;274:2429–2436. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.0693. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Shultz S, Dunbar R. Encephalization is not a universal macroevolutionary phenomenon in mammals but is associated with sociality. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:21582–21586. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1005246107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Rodrigues SM, Saslow LR, Garcia N, John OP, Keltner D. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation relates to empathy and stress reactivity in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:21437–21441. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909579106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Uzefovsky F, et al. Oxytocin receptor and vasopressin receptor 1a genes are respectively associated with emotional and cognitive empathy. Horm Behav. 2015;67:60–65. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Troisi A, et al. Social hedonic capacity is associated with the A118G polymorphism of the mu-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) in adult healthy volunteers and psychiatric patients. Soc Neurosci. 2011;6:88–97. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2010.482786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Nummenmaa L, et al. Adult attachment style is associated with cerebral μ-opioid receptor availability in humans. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36:3621–3628. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22866. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Troisi A, et al. Variation in the μ-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) moderates the influence of early maternal care on fearful attachment. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2012;7:542–547. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Gillath O, Shaver PR, Baek J-M, Chun DS. Genetic correlates of adult attachment style. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2008;34:1396–1405. doi: 10.1177/0146167208321484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Schneiderman I, Kanat-Maymon Y, Ebstein RP, Feldman R. Cumulative risk on the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) underpins empathic communication difficulties at the first stages of romantic love. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014;9:1524–1529. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Walum H, et al. Variation in the oxytocin receptor gene is associated with pair-bonding and social behavior. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71:419–426. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Walum H, et al. Genetic variation in the vasopressin receptor 1a gene (AVPR1A) associates with pair-bonding behavior in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:14153–14156. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803081105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Resendez SL, et al. Dopamine and opioid systems interact within the nucleus accumbens to maintain monogamous pair bonds. eLife. 2016;5:15325. doi: 10.7554/eLife.15325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Emanuele E, Brondino N, Pesenti S, Re S, Geroldi D. Genetic loading on human loving styles. Neuroendocrinol Lett. 2007;28:815–821. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Miller WB, et al. Dopamine receptor genes are associated with age at first sexual intercourse. J Biosoc Sci. 1999;31:43–54. doi: 10.1017/s0021932099000437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Liu J, Gong P, Zhou X. The association between romantic relationship status and 5-HT1A gene in young adults. Sci Rep. 2014;4:7049. doi: 10.1038/srep07049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Wu K, Chen C, Moyzis RK, Greenberger E, Yu Z. Gender interacts with opioid receptor polymorphism A118G and serotonin receptor polymorphism −1438 A/G on speed-dating success. Hum Nat. 2016;27:244–260. doi: 10.1007/s12110-016-9257-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Zitzmann M, Depenbusch M, Gromoll J, Nieschlag E. X-chromosome inactivation patterns and androgen receptor functionality influence phenotype and social characteristics as well as pharmacogenetics of testosterone therapy in Klinefelter patients. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2004;89:6208–6217. doi: 10.1210/jc.2004-1424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Roney JR, Simmons ZL, Lukaszewski AW. Androgen receptor gene sequence and basal cortisol concentrations predict men’s hormonal responses to potential mates. Proc Biol Sci. 2010;277:57–63. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1538. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Creswell KG, et al. OXTR polymorphism predicts social relationships through its effects on social temperament. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015;10:869–876. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Chang SC, et al. Are genetic variations in OXTR, AVPR1A, and CD38 genes important to social integration? Results from two large U.S. cohorts. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;39:257–268. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.09.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Li J, et al. Association of oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) rs53576 polymorphism with sociality: A meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0131820. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Johnson KVA, Dunbar RIM. Pain tolerance predicts human social network size. Sci Rep. 2016;6:25267. doi: 10.1038/srep25267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Aron A, Aron EN. Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1992;63:596–612. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Ebstein RP, Israel S, Chew SH, Zhong S, Knafo A. Genetics of human social behavior. Neuron. 2010;65:831–844. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Dunbar RIM. The social role of touch in humans and primates: Behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34:260–268. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Tarr B, Launay J, Cohen E, Dunbar R. Synchrony and exertion during dance independently raise pain threshold and encourage social bonding. Biol Lett. 2015;11:20150767. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Way BM, Taylor SE, Eisenberger NI. Variation in the mu-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) is associated with dispositional and neural sensitivity to social rejection. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:15079–15084. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812612106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Bonenberger M, Plener PL, Groschwitz RC, Grön G, Abler B. Polymorphism in the µ-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) modulates neural processing of physical pain, social rejection and error processing. Exp Brain Res. 2015;233:2517–2526. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4322-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Peciña M, Love T, Stohler CS, Goldman D, Zubieta J-K. Effects of the Mu opioid receptor polymorphism (OPRM1 A118G) on pain regulation, placebo effects and associated personality trait measures. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:957–965. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Devine DP, Leone P, Pocock D, Wise RA. Differential involvement of ventral tegmental mu, delta and kappa opioid receptors in modulation of basal mesolimbic dopamine release: In vivo microdialysis studies. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1993;266:1236–1246. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Ulmer-Yaniv A, et al. Affiliation, reward, and immune biomarkers coalesce to support social synchrony during periods of bond formation in humans. Brain Behav Immun. 2016;56:130–139. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary File
pnas.1700712114.sapp.pdf (489.9KB, pdf)
Supplementary File
pnas.1700712114.sd01.xlsx (522.1KB, xlsx)

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES