Abstract
We assessed the calming effect of doppel, a wearable device that delivers heartbeat-like tactile stimulation on the wrist. We tested whether the use of doppel would have a calming effect on physiological arousal and subjective reports of state anxiety during the anticipation of public speech, a validated experimental task that is known to induce anxiety. Two groups of participants were tested in a single-blind design. Both groups wore the device on their wrist during the anticipation of public speech, and were given the cover story that the device was measuring blood pressure. For only one group, the device was turned on and delivered a slow heartbeat-like vibration. Participants in the doppel active condition displayed lower increases in skin conductance responses relative to baseline and reported lower anxiety levels compared to the control group. Therefore, the presence, as opposed to its absence, of a slow rhythm, which in the present study was instantiated as an auxiliary slow heartbeat delivered through doppel, had a significant calming effect on physiological arousal and subjective experience during a socially stressful situation. This finding is discussed in relation to past research on responses and entrainment to rhythms, and their effects on arousal and mood.
Introduction
Wearable devices are becoming ubiquitous in everyday life, serving a range of functions, from measuring physical activity and a range of physiological variables to providing feedback on emotional states. Across most such instantiations of wearable technologies, the overarching aim seems to be the quantification of the self1. However, as recent studies suggest, the effects of such tracking devices are debatable2, seem to be short-lived, and consumers often stop using them after a short period of time3. As a result, questions about what would be a more intuitive wearable device that can assist people in their everyday life have motivated the development and design of a new wearable device by Team Turquoise Ltd (http://www.doppel.london/). The doppel device, rather than measuring physiological variables, aims at providing a sensory experience to help people manage the pressures of time and stress in their daily lives by modulating the users’ physiological and potentially emotional and cognitive states during a wide range of everyday life tasks. The doppel delivers an on-demand, discrete, user-controlled, heartbeat-like vibration applied through a wristband. In the final product, the wristband is connected via Bluetooth to a smart phone app where the user measures their resting heart rate and chooses their preferred speed and intensity of tactile stimulation in relation to their own heartrate.
The design of doppel was inspired by several research strands that show how humans respond to and possibly entrain to different rhythms. Entrainment is a broadly used term that, in the context of human behavior and physiology, reflects the voluntary or involuntary synchronization of our brains and bodies to the environment4. For example, the tempo of a song can naturally alter our breathing rate and heart rate5. As a real-life example, choir singers not only harmonise their voices, they can also synchronise their heartbeats6. Vickhoff and colleagues6 monitored the heart rates of singers and found that as the members sang in unison, their pulses began to speed up and slow down at the same rate. Beyond music, several studies highlight entrainment effects in responses to biological rhythms, and the heartbeat is perhaps the most ubiquitous biological rhythm in nature. The mother’s own heartbeat is a particular salient multisensory signal that accompanies the development of the foetus and, not surprisingly, has been shown to have beneficial effects for the infant’s development. For example, the perception of the maternal heartbeat in the womb provides the foetus with an important rhythmic experience7 that is thought to facilitate the formation of the neural basis for entrainment and synchrony skills necessary for a wide range of cognitive processes that underpin vocal, gestural, and gaze communication during mother–infant interactions8. Such entrainment effects to physiological rhythms are evidenced in a wide range of contexts in later life. For example, the heartbeats of a mother and baby will synchronise with one another when they interact closely9, and similar effects have been observed in couples10.
Beyond specific entrainment effects, more general, our brains and bodies also respond naturally to rhythm. In music, tempo has been consistently shown to play an important role in mood induction and physiological changes11–14. For example, listening to slower tempo results in lower arousal and subjective states of positive or calm emotional states12, 15–17, see ref. 18 for review. Conversely, fast rhythms are associated with high arousal and positive and/or arousing emotional states such as joy, excitement, surprise, fear or anger12, 18. Beyond the perception of tempo in music, the physiological importance of faster and slower heart rate as well as of high and low heart rate variability for physical and mental health is well-documented, as well as their role for experienced arousal and cognitive processing in general19. Not only high arousal is physiologically correlated with increased heart rate, and calmness with lower heart rate, as we also intuitively associate higher and lower heart rate with anxiety or high arousal and calmness, respectively. Based on the aforementioned lines of research about the importance of heartbeats as pervasive physiological signals involved in emotional regulation and social interactions, and the effects of slow rhythms on mood, doppel was designed to embed on the users’ body, the ubiquitous biological rhythm of a beating heart as a means of using the felt faster or slower rhythm to modulate levels of arousal and calmness. To that end, the present study was designed to test whether the use of doppel, as opposed to its absence, could have a calming effect on physiological arousal and subjective reports of experienced anxiety during a stressful situation.
Stressful situations are typically associated with increases in physiological arousal and negative affect. However, stressful contexts differ from one another in character. For example, a mental arithmetic challenge differs from a respiratory challenge, which both differ from a social stress challenge. Socially stressful situations may involve the threat of negative social evaluation (e.g., public speaking challenge) or may be accompanied by negative social evaluation resulting in social rejection (e.g., social exclusion manipulation). Given the significance of interoception in cognitive-affective processing20–22, potential changes in interoceptive signals (i.e. signals originating from visceral organs such as the heart) in stressful, negative and affective situations may influence emotional experience, emotion regulation and decision making in these contexts. In such situations, the ability to regulate one’s physiological arousal and emotions may be important for lowering anxiety and for successful performance.
A classic task that has been used in controlled psychological experiments to elicit an ecologically valid and socially stressful situation, is the anticipation of public speech task. Across a large number of studies, it has been shown that anticipation of public speaking is particularly effective at inducing social anxiety with concomitant increases in physiological arousal23–26. Therefore, the current study utilized the speech anticipation manipulation to examine how the use of doppel could enable participants to cope better with their anxiety while they were preparing to deliver a speech to a group of people. Moreover, given the intended use of the product, we were particularly interested in testing the efficacy of its use against a condition where participants were not using it. Importantly, for the purposes of this study, participants were unaware of the true function of doppel as we wanted to minimize expectations and potential confounds. To that end, participants were told that doppel was a device designed to measure their blood pressure. Two groups of participants were tested. Both groups wore the device on their wrist, but only for one group doppel was turned on and vibrated during the anticipation period. Doppel was set to vibrate with a frequency ~20% slower than the participant’s heartbeats, as measured at rest, because previous research has shown that slower rhythms tend to be associated with low arousal and subjective states of calmness/serenity, sadness or boredom18. Before instructions were given and throughout the anticipatory period, levels of physiological arousal were measured through continuous recordings of skin conductance and heart rate. In addition, we collected psychometric data to quantify state anxiety levels before and after the anticipation of public speech task. It was hypothesized, that the doppel device active group would display lower increases in physiological arousal and report lower levels of state anxiety. It should be noted that the method adopted here with doppel differs from biofeedback or false feedback techniques27–29 in which exteroceptive stimulation reflects ongoing cardiac activity and participants are encouraged to pay attention and/or regulate it. Instead, we tested whether periodic vibrotactile stimulation unrelated to participant’s ongoing heartbeats, as opposed to the absence of such stimulation, could have a positive calming effect.
Methods
Participants
A total of 52 (20 Male; mean age = 26.4, s.d. = 5.7) volunteers took part in the study. Twenty five participants (9 Male; mean age = 25.9, s.d. = 5.2) were assigned to the experimental group (i.e. doppel active condition) and 27 (11 Male; mean age = 26.8, s.d. = 6.1) to the control group (i.e. doppel turned off condition). Participants were randomly assigned to each group prior to study start. All participants provided written informed consent to take part in this study, which was approved by the College Ethics Committee of Royal Holloway University of London research ethics committee, and all methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations of the College Ethics Committee.
Procedure
A graphical illustration of the study’s procedure and timeline can be seen in Fig. 1. Participants were comfortably sat in an armchair, given written information about the study and asked to sign a consent form. Skin conductance Ag/AgCl electrodes (MLT117F) with 0.5%-NaCl electrode gel were attached to the phalanges of the middle and ring fingers of the participant’s non-dominant hand. The signal was recorded using a Powerlab 8/35 (https://www.adinstruments.com/), a GSR Amp unit (22 mV constant voltage at 75HZ) and LabChart (v 8.1) software with a recording range of 40 µS and a sampling rate of 1 kHz. A pulse transducer (TN1012/ST; https://www.adinstruments.com/) was also attached to the thumb of non-dominant hand to monitor cardiac activity.
The doppel device (see Fig. 1A) was fitted to the participant’s non-dominant wrist, in a firm but not tight position, and remained off until the period preceding task instructions. The device sits on the inside of the wrist where users expect to feel their pulse. The unit is 38 mm in diameter & 9 mm thick, held in place with a silicon strap. The device provides a double heartbeat-like rhythm tactile sensation. The main casing has a steep metal base and a plastic cover. The casing houses the electronic components: a coin vibration motor that delivers the stimulus, a low energy Bluetooth chip that communicates with an app or computer to upload new rhythms to the device, and a lithium ion battery that powers the device.
For the purposes of this study, participants were told that it was a new generation device to monitor blood pressure, and none of the participants expressed any suspicion about the purpose of the device at any point throughout the entire duration of the study. A single-blind protocol was adopted as necessary to provide the experimenter with appropriate control over doppel (i.e. switch off to those assigned to the control group) and to prevent participants from using cognitive strategies during the task related to the study’s goals.
At the beginning of the experiment, participants were instructed to rest for 5 minutes while sitting still, with the purpose of measuring baseline levels of physiological activity. Average heart rate and skin conductance levels during this period were taken as indices of pre-task physiological activity. After this rest period, the experimenter told participants “I am going to change the settings of the device you have on the wrist to start measuring blood pressure. You might feel some vibration. If you do, don’t worry it’s normal”. The device was then turned on in all cases but switched off after 10 seconds if the participant had been assigned to the control group. The device’s vibration frequency was individually tailored to a frequency ~20% slower (mean = 58.2, s.d. = 6.3) than the participant’s baseline HR (mean = 75.8, s.d. = 12.3), as measured during the rest period. However, upper (65 bpm) and lower (40 bpm) frequency limits were imposed to prevent vibrations to be perceived as fast or unnaturally slow, respectively. In the experimental group, the lower limit was applied to one participant (rest HR = 44.7) and the upper limit to 9 participants (mean rest HR = 88.1, s.d. = 5.5). The average absolute adjustment to lower and upper limits was of 4.4 bpm (s.d. = 3.9). Next, task instructions were provided verbally by the experimenter: “You should now prepare a speech about the use of animals for research. You may present both the pros and cons of using animals in research or give your personal point of view on this issue. You can use this pen and paper to take notes if you wish to. You have 5 minutes to prepare it. After that we will move to the room next door where you should give the speech to 3 or 4 colleagues of mine. The speech should last for 5 minutes”.
At the end of the study, to assess the success of our manipulation, participants were invited, in a semi-structured way, to share their subjective experiences during task. First they were asked “how were you feeling about giving a speech to unfamiliar people”? Then the experimenter directly enquired if participants believed they would actually give the speech. All participants, but one, were convinced they would give the speech.
Subjective Measures
Participants were asked to complete the STAI-Y-130 twice to measure self-reported anxiety levels before the delivery of task instructions and after the task completion. The STAI-Y-1 questionnaire is composed of 20 items (e.g. “I am tense”; “I feel secure”) assessing state anxiety. Each item is rated on a 4-point scale ranging from “not at all” to “very much so”. High scores reflect high anxiety levels. To control for possible differences between groups in trait apprehension about other’s evaluations, participants were asked to complete the Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation questionnaire (bFNE)31. The bFNE is composed of 12 items (e.g. “I am afraid others will not approve of me”) scored on a 5-point scale ranging from “not at all characteristic of me” to “extremely characteristic of me”. High scores reflect high concern about others’ evaluation. To control for possible between-group differences in participants’ general disposition towards public speaking and self-reported trait social anxiety, participants were asked to state their agreement, on 8-point likert scales, on the following sentences: “I like to speak in public” and “I can easily become anxious in social situations” (1- strongly disagree; 8 – strongly agree). These questions and the bFNE were only asked after task completion to avoid inducing task anticipation anxiety or evaluation concerns during baseline physiological recordings. Finally, they rated on 7-point likert scales, their experience during the task according to the dimensions: from relaxing (1) to stressful (7); and from unpleasant (1) to pleasant (7). The questionnaire assessing self-reported social anxiety, attitudes towards public speaking and subjective ratings of participants’ experience during the task was not administered to the first 6 participants (Experimental group: n = 2; Control group: n = 4) due to an experimental error.
Analyses of psychophysiological data
Non-specific-SCR or spontaneous fluctuations in skin conductance (NS-SCRs) analyses were carried out using the SCRalyze b2.18 software (http://pspm.sourceforge.net/ 32) according to a convolution model to estimate the area and the curve (AUC), or skin-conductance level-corrected time integral. The AUC is closely related to the conventional methods of estimation of the number and amplitude of NS-SCRs (for further details see ref. 32). This method has been shown to be a better predictor of autonomic arousal than conventional analysis, and has the advantages of not requiring subjective evaluations as well as being computationally inexpensive32. HR was estimated with the HRV function implemented in Labchart (v 8.1). Visual inspection and manual correction was carried out to correct unidentified or misidentified peaks. Data from three participants (control group) were discarded from HR analyses due to technical failure during pulse recording. The entire five-minute periods during rest and speech preparation were analysed to estimate baseline and task NS-SCRs and HR levels, respectively. Skin conductance levels and HR were entered into separate repeated measures ANOVAs with Time (Baseline; Task) and Group (Experimental; Control) as within- and between-subjects factors, respectively.
Results
Psychophysiological Data
The analysis of NS-SCRs levels revealed a significant main effect of Time (F(1, 50) = 36.69, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.42) confirming, as expected, that speech anticipation resulted in an overall increase in physiological arousal (see Fig. 2A), providing a proof that the task was successful in eliciting arousal. The main effect of Group was not significant (F(1, 50) = 3.21, p = 0.079, ƞ2 = 0.060). Crucially, the critical Time x Group device (F(1, 50) = 5.23, p = 0.027, ƞ2 = 0.095) interaction was significant. Follow-up independent t-tests confirmed lower skin conductance levels in the Experimental group than in Control group during the task (t(1, 50) = 2.24, p = 0.029, Hedges’ g = 0.54) but no difference at baseline (t(1, 50) = 0.271, p = 0.79, Hedges’ g = 0.074). We then investigated changes in average Heart Rate. While the main effect of Time (F(1, 47) = 30.02, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.39) on HR was significant because HR increased during anticipation relative to baseline, the Time x Group interaction (F(1, 47) = 0.47, p = 0.50, ƞ2 = 0.010) was not significantly different. Furthermore, the main effect of Group was not significant (F(1, 47) = 0.67, p = 0.42, ƞ2 = 0.014).
Subjective measures
To assess differences in self-reported anxiety at baseline and after speech preparation, pre and post STAI scores (see Table 1) were entered into a repeated measures ANOVA with Time (baseline and post-task) and Group (Experimental; Control) as within- and between-subjects factors, respectively. Results confirmed that speech preparation successfully induced anxiety in the participants, as revealed by the main effect of Time (F(1, 50) = 53.91, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.52). The interaction Time x Group (F(1, 50) = 4.14, p = 0.047, ƞ2 = 0.077, see Fig. 2B) and the main effect of Group (F(1, 50) = 6.75, p = 0.012, ƞ2 = 0.12) were also significant. Independent t-tests revealed no group differences in anxiety at baseline (t(1, 50) = 1.34, p = 0.19, Hedges’ g = 0.36) but lower levels of anxiety in the Experimental group after speech preparation (t(1, 50) = 2.79, p = 0.007, Hedges’ g = 0.77). We also carried out independent t-tests on the participants’ ratings on the task experience as relaxing/stressful and unpleasant/pleasant. Results showed that participants in the Experimental group rated the experience as less stressful than those in the control group (t(1, 44) = 2.35, p = 0.023, Hedges’ g = 0.68). Ratings on the pleasantness question did not reach statistical significance (t(1, 44) = 1.94, p = 0.059, Hedges’ g = 0.56). Control analyses revealed no differences between groups on the obtained trait measures: (i) fear of negative evaluation (t(1, 50) = 0.30, p = 0.76, Hedges’ g = 0.082); (ii) social anxiety (t(1, 44) = 0.15, p = 0.88, Hedges’ g = 0.044); (iii) and attitude towards public speaking (t(1, 44) = 0.43, p = 0.67, Hedges’ g = 0.12).
Table 1.
STAI pre | STAI Post * | bFNE | Social anxiety | Public speaking | Pleasantness | Stressfulness* | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Experimental Group | 30.2 (8.0) | 38.1 (12.6) | 36.1 (9.8) | 5.00 (2.2) | 3.90 (2.2) | 5.36 (1.2) | 2.96 (1.5) |
Control Group | 33.1 (7.5) | 47.0 (10.3) | 37.0 (10.2) | 5.09 (1.8) | 3.65 (1.9) | 4.39 (1.8) | 4.09 (1.7) |
Discussion
We tested the efficacy of a new wearable device on calmness using a task that typically induces high anxiety, namely, the preparation and anticipation of giving a short public speech to an unfamiliar audience. doppel gives an on-demand, discrete, user-controlled, heartbeat-like vibration applied through a wristband and it aims at modulating the alertness or calmness of the user by adjusting the frequency of the “tactile heartbeat”. In the present experiment, we hypothesized that a slow heart-rate, as opposed to the absence of any vibro-tactile stimulation, would enable participants to remain calmer during the anticipation of public speech. To that end, we told participants that doppel was a blood pressure monitoring device, and only for one of the two groups we turned on doppel to deliver its tactile vibrations during the anticipation of public speech. Additionally, subsequent oral debriefing confirmed that participants believed that they would actually give the speech and thought that, indeed, doppel monitored blood pressure.
Even though, at the beginning of the experiment, the two groups displayed comparable levels of arousal (as measured by Skin Conductance) and of state anxiety, participants in the doppel active condition showed a significantly smaller increase in arousal compared to the control group. Similarly, at the end of their speech preparation, and before they were debriefed, participants in the doppel active condition reported a significantly smaller increase in their state anxiety compared to the control group. No significant differences in average heart rate were found as a result of doppel use. Finally, participants in the doppel active condition found the task significantly less stressful than participants in the control condition. Taken together, the results highlight that the use of doppel had a clear and significant calming effect in both physiological measures of arousal and subjective reports of anxiety during a task that is effective in inducing social stress, suggesting that doppel enabled participants to stay calmer and less anxious, as compared with the condition where the device was worn but was not performing its intended function.
Previous studies have used pharmacological means to study anxiolytic-like effects during anticipation of public speech or during simulated public speaking33. For example, de Oliveira and colleagues33 used intranasal administration of oxytocin, a neuropeptide that is known to be involved in anxiety, as well as cardiovascular and hormonal regulation, and showed that the oxytocin, but not the placebo, group had lower skin-conductance levels during anticipation and actual speech. Biofeedback techniques have also been used to down-regulate anxiety and physiological reactions during emotional situations27, 28. In particular, autonomic regulation training through biofeedback has been shown to reduce subjective and physiological arousal during public speaking27. Here, we add to this literature by showing that the use of heartbeat-like vibrotactile stimulation, as opposed to its absence, can also have an anxiolytic effect. These two techniques (i.e. biofeedback and doppel use), however, differ considerably in terms of the expectations and attention given to the exteroceptive stimuli. Unlike in biofeedback or false feedback approaches, in our active doppel condition, participants were not instructed to regulate their heart rate as a function of these vibrations. Rather than feedback, doppel stimulation is thought to be related to physiological entrainment mechanisms that are not dependent on direct attention or appraisals of interoceptive-exteroceptive matching.
Numerous past studies have also shown how our bodies, including physiological cycles such as the respiratory and cardiac cycles, entrain to different rhythms and how such responses may influence mood11–14, 34. Reviewing a large body of relevant research, Phillips-Silver and colleagues35 show that the ability to perceive and synchronize to a beat is possible across different sensory modalities. The authors suggest a general model of entrainment that builds upon pre-existing adaptations that allow us to perceive and produce rhythmic stimuli, as well as integrate their production and perception with sensory feedback. Extending the effects of rhythms on mood, beyond entrainment per se, Tajadura-Jiménez, Väljamäe and Västfjäll36 reported that heartbeat sounds and their rhythm significantly affected participants’ physiological reactions as well as the emotional judgments of pictures, and their recall. In that study, listening to heartbeat sounds resulted in a small but significant increase in participants’ heart rate with respect to the silence condition, and the sounds significantly influenced emotional responses to affective visual stimuli. In particular, a fast-rate heartbeat (e.g. 110 bpm) sound resulted in higher arousal ratings and enhanced pictures’ memorability during a free-recall task. It is noteworthy that in the present study we did not find any evidence of significant modulations of heart rate, other than the overall increase of heart rate during the anticipation period. This is not surprising given that typically entrainment effects on heart rate are relatively small11, 12, 36, and in the present study we employed a task (i.e., anticipation of public speaking) that is meant to increase heart rate. Entrainment effects on physiological activity have been mostly studied in response to musical stimuli11–14. However, tempo-related entrainment effects on heart rate are less consistent than in other physiological indices, such as skin conductance and respiration frequency, especially when entraining to slow tempo stimuli11, 12, 37, 38. It is possible that this might be related to the non-linear dynamics of cardiac responses to stressors or emotional stimuli. The continuous fluctuations of heart rate due to excitatory and inhibitory regulation processes may explain why average heart rate, like the one reported here, is a less sensitive index to entrainment effects.
Unlike past studies that often use auditory feedback of heartbeats29, 36, 39, 40, instead of sounds, we used heartbeat-like tactile stimulation to examine the effects of a calming “heartbeat” rate on anxiety. While no significant differences were observed in participants’ heartrate, the consistent patterns of changes in skin conductance levels and subjective anxiety reports support the hypothesis that the presence of a slow rhythm, which in the present study was instantiated as an auxiliary slow heartbeat delivered through doppel, can influence physiological arousal and mood. We used slower frequencies from the participants’ own heart rate because, as previous research has suggested, slower rhythms tend to be associated with low arousal and subjective states of positive or calm emotional states12, 18. However, it remains to be tested whether faster or equivalent frequencies to the participant’s heart rate can have similar effects. It is possible that anxiolytic effects can be observed to a range of heart-like vibrotactile stimulation frequencies and even that the “ideal” frequency varies from person to person. Importantly, while we here show the calming effects for slower frequencies, doppel users can select the vibrating frequency that corresponds better with their state and possible the one that seems to be more efficient for their own needs and lifestyles.
While the precise underlying neurophysiological mechanism by which doppel elicits such effects was not directly investigated in the present study, we suggest that the calming effect may be mediated by neural responses to the rhythmic tactile input that the device delivers, especially neural responses in areas that are involved in somatosensation and interoception such as the somatosensory cortex and the insula41,42 and in emotion and rhythm processing such as the basal ganglia43. Future studies must specifically investigate such neural responses in parallel with the attenuated changes in skin conductance to complement the significant and converging physiological and subjective measures of reduced arousal and anxiety obtained through the use of doppel, as reported here.
Acknowledgements
The study was commissioned and funded by Team Turquoise Ltd. Team Turquoise Ltd provided the direct research expenses related to the reimbursement of the participating volunteers and the salary costs for RA.
Author Contributions
M.T. and R.A. designed the study. R.A. collected the data, M.T. and R.A. analyzed the data and co-authored the manuscript. F.T., A.B., N.B. & J.H. contributed to the authorship of the manuscript.
Competing Interests
N.B., A.B., J.H. and F.T. co-own Team Turquoise Ltd. M.T. is an independent consultant for Team Turquoise Ltd.
Footnotes
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Lupton D, The Quantified Self, Polity Press (2016).
- 2.Jakicic JM, et al. Effect of Wearable Technology Combined With a Lifestyle Intervention on Long-term Weight Loss. JAMA. 2016;316:1161–1171. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.12858. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ledger, D. & McCaffrey, D. How the Science of Human Behavior Change Offers the Secret to Long-Term Engagement. Retrieved 4 March 2017 from https://endeavourpartners.net/assets/Endeavour-Partners-Wearables-White-Paper-20141.pdf (2014).
- 4.Ross JM, Balasubramaniam R. Physical and neural entrainment to rhythm: human sensorimotor coordination across tasks and effector systems. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8:576. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Larsen PD, Galletly DC. The sound of silence is music to the heart. Heart. 2006;92:433–4. doi: 10.1136/hrt.2005.071902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Vickhoff B, et al. Music structure determines heart rate variability of singers. Front. Psychol. 2013;4:334. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Webb AR, Heller HT, Benson CB, Lahav A. Mother’s voice and heartbeat sounds elicit auditory plasticity in the human brain before full gestation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:3152–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414924112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Feldman R. Infant Biological Foundations Synchrony and Developmental Outcomes. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007;16:340–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00532.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Feldman R, Magori-Cohen R, Galili G, Singer M, Louzoun Y. Mother and infant coordinate heart rhythms through episodes of interaction synchrony. Infant Behav. Dev. 2011;34:569–577. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ferrer E, Helm JL. Dynamical systems modeling of physiological coregulation in dyadic interactions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2013;88:296–308. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Etzel JA, Johnsen EL, Dickerson J, Tranel D, Adolphs R. Cardiovascular and respiratory responses during musical mood induction. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2006;61:57–69. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.10.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gomez P, Danuser B. Relationships between musical structure and psychophysiological measures of emotion. Emotion. 2007;7:377–387. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Trost W, Labbé C, Grandjean D. Rhythmic entrainment as musical emotion induction mechanism. Neuropsychologia. 2017;96:96–110. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Krabs RU, Enk R, Teich N, Koelsch S. Autonomic effects of music in health and Crohn’s disease: The impact of isochronicity, emotional valence, and tempo. PLoS One. 2015;10:1–30. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ziegler G, Dahnke R, Yeragani VK, Bär K-J. The relation of ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity and heart rate fluctuations at rest. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2009;30:2205–2210. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.07008.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Van Der Zwaag, M. D., Westerink, J. H. D. M. & Van Den Broek, E. L. Emotional and psychophysiological responses to tempo, mode, and percussiveness. Music. Sci. 15, 250–269 (2011).
- 17.Bresin R, Friberg A. Emotion rendering in music: Range and characteristic values of seven musical variables. Cortex. 2011;47:1068–1081. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Gabrielson, A. In Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology (ed. Cross, I., Hallam, S., Thaut, M.) 141–150 (Oxford University Press, 2009).
- 19.Kreibig SD. Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biol. Psychol. 2010;84:394–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Dunn BD, et al. Listening to your heart. How interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychol. Sci. 2010;21:1835–1844. doi: 10.1177/0956797610389191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Garfinkel SN, Seth AK, Barrett AB, Suzuki K, Critchley HD. Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 2015;104:65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Herbert BM, Pollatos O. The Body in the Mind: On the Relationship Between Interoception and Embodiment. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2012;4:692–704. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01189.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Durlik C, Brown G, Tsakiris M. Enhanced interoceptive awareness during anticipation of public speaking is associated with fear of negative evaluation. Cogn. Emot. 2014;28:530–540. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.832654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Egloff B, Schmukle SC, Burns LR, Schwerdtfeger A. Spontaneous emotion regulation during evaluated speaking tasks: Associations with negative affect, anxiety expression, memory, and physiological responding. Emotion. 2006;6:356–366. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Moscovitch DA, Suvak MK, Hofmann SG. Emotional response patterns during social threat in individuals with generalized social anxiety disorder and non-anxious controls. J. Anxiety Disord. 2010;24:785–791. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Stevens S, et al. Heartbeat perception in social anxiety before and during speech anticipation. Behav. Res. Ther. 2011;49:138–143. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.McKinney ME, Gatchel RJ. The comparative effectiveness of heart rate biofeedback, speech skills training, and a combination of both in treating public-speaking anxiety. Biofeedback Self. Regul. 1982;7:71–87. doi: 10.1007/BF00999056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Peira N, Fredrikson M, Pourtois G. Controlling the emotional heart: Heart rate biofeedback improves cardiac control during emotional reactions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2014;91:225–231. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Gray MA, Harrison NA, Wiens S, Critchley HD. Modulation of Emotional Appraisal by False Physiological Feedback during fMRI. PLoS One. 2007;2:e546. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Title. (Consulting Psychologists Press) (1983).
- 31.Leary MR. A Brief Version of the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1983;9:371–375. doi: 10.1177/0146167283093007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bach DR, Friston KJ, Dolan RJ. Analytic measures for quantification of arousal from spontaneous skin conductance fluctuations. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2010;76:52–5. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.de Oliveira DC, Zuardi AW, Graeff FG, Queiroz RH, Crippa JA. Anxiolytic-like effect of oxytocin in the simulated public speaking test. J. Psychopharmacol. 2012;26:497–504. doi: 10.1177/0269881111400642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Anishchenko VS, Balanov AG, Janson NB, Igosheva NB, Bordyugov GV. Entrainment Between Heart Rate and Weak Noninvasive Forcing. Int. J. Bifurc. Chaos. 2000;10:2339–2348. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Phillips-Silver J, Aktipis CA, Bryant GA. The ecology of entrainment: Foundations of coordinated rhythmic movement. Music Percept. 2010;28:3–14. doi: 10.1525/mp.2010.28.1.3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Tajadura-Jiménez A, Väljamäe A, Västfjäll D. Self-Representation in Mediated Environments: The Experience of Emotions Modulated by Auditory-Vibrotactile Heartbeat. CyberPsychology Behav. 2008;11:33–38. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2007.0002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Khalfa S, Roy M, Rainville P, Dalla Bella S, Peretz I. Role of tempo entrainment in psychophysiological differentiation of happy and sad music? Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2008;68:17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bernardi L, Porta C, Sleight P. Cardiovascular, cerebrovascular, and respiratory changes induced by different types of music in musicians and non-musicians: the importance of silence. Heart. 2006;92:445–52. doi: 10.1136/hrt.2005.064600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Lenggenhager B, Azevedo RT, Mancini A, Aglioti SM. Listening to your heart and feeling yourself: effects of exposure to interoceptive signals during the ultimatum game. Exp. Brain Res. 2013;230:233–241. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3647-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Azevedo RT, et al. Participants’ above-chance recognition of own-heart sound combined with poor metacognitive awareness suggests implicit knowledge of own heart cardiodynamics. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:26545. doi: 10.1038/srep26545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Couto B, et al. The man who feels two hearts: the different pathways of interoception. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2014;9:1253–60. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Khalsa SS, Rudrauf D, Feinstein JS, Tranel D. The pathways of interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12:1494–1496. doi: 10.1038/nn.2411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Trost W, et al. Getting the beat: Entrainment of brain activity by musical rhythm and pleasantness. Neuroimage. 2014;103:55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]