Abstract
The colour of our skin and clothing affects how others perceive us and how we behave. Human skin colour varies conspicuously with genetic ancestry, but even subtle changes in skin colour due to diet, blood oxygenation and hormone levels influence social perceptions. In this review, we describe the theoretical and empirical frameworks in which human colour is researched. We explore how subtle skin colour differences relate to judgements of health and attractiveness. Also, because humans are one of the few organisms able to manipulate their apparent colour, we review how cosmetics and clothing are implicated in courtship and competition, both inside the laboratory and in the real world. Research on human colour is in its infancy compared with human psychophysics and colour research in non-human animals, and hence we present best-practice guidelines for methods and reporting, which we hope will improve the validity and reproducibility of studies on human coloration.
This article is part of the themed issue ‘Animal coloration: production, perception, function and application’.
Keywords: attractiveness, clothing, colour, competition, mate choice, skin
1. Introduction
Unlike most other animals, humans have the ability to use and interpret colour where it does not naturally occur [1] and we imbue colours with complex meanings [2]. Colour can indicate group support or membership [3], represent abstract emotional states (e.g. the ‘green-eyed monster’ of Shakespeare's Othello that symbolizes jealousy), and is involved in cultural rituals and symbolism (e.g. mourning is associated with black in the UK, but with white in India and China, and gold and silver in Taiwan [3]). We know how the eye and brain process colour stimuli [4,5], how colour terms are represented in language [6], and how colour influences human behaviour in the context of diverse applied fields such as advertising, workplace productivity and food [7]. By contrast, human colour and its effects on mate choice and competition is an emergent field of research [8].
Individuals vary in colour across their skin, and these differences affect how individuals are perceived [9]. For example, facial skin colour affects perceptions of a person's health and attractiveness [10]. Colour may also be involved in perceptions of emotional state, as suggested by Darwin [11], who described how emotional states might be conveyed by face colour in The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, noting that human faces redden or turn deathly pale with rage. Unlike most other animals, humans modify our appearance through various forms of ornamentation, such as cosmetics and clothing. These voluntary signals may have replaced the physiological signals seen in other non-human animals.
In this review, we will evaluate the theoretical and empirical research on human coloration, focusing on the physiological correlates and perceptual influences on attraction, health and dominance. The first aim of our review is to describe the theoretical and methodological frameworks that underpin this research. The second aim is to review the evidence that facial skin colour is used in judgements of health, that skin and ornamentation colour influence attractiveness and other aspects of mating psychology, and that colour is relevant to dominance and competitive performance. The third aim of our review is to provide guidelines for improving the methods, reporting and reproducibility of studies on human colour, and to highlight topics for future research.
We note that human skin colour varies with genetic ancestry [12,13], and there are well-documented links between such skin colour variation and how individuals are perceived and treated by others [14]. A review of this literature is outside the scope of the current paper, in which we focus instead on variation in skin colour due to sex, diet, hormones and other variables, and on coloured ornamentation. Our methodological guidelines are applicable to the study of all forms of human colour differences.
2. Theoretical and methodological frameworks that underpin research on human colour
(a). Theoretical framework
Many of the research questions that interest scientists who study human colour overlap with those investigated in the non-human animal literature [15], and are therefore grounded within the same theoretical framework. For example, in both humans and non-humans, some colour signals are proposed to arise from the evolutionary pressures on females to select healthy, disease-free mates [16], and on males to display their healthy, disease-free state. Both human and non-human signals are studied from a receiver psychology perspective [17]. This theory posits that signals have two distinct components. The first is the ‘strategic design’ that arises from the selective pressure acting on the content of, or information encoded in, the signal [17]. For example, in humans, facial skin colour is proposed to have evolved to signal health or physical condition [10]. The second aspect of receiver psychology comes from the understanding that many species' signals have arisen for the same function, but the particular signal one species uses is often completely different from the signal another species uses [17]. Therefore, researchers of human and non-human signals also investigate the ‘tactical design’ or ‘efficacy’ of signals—this is what makes the signal detectable, easy to discriminate and/or memorable [17]. For example humans who have redder skin are perceived as more healthy and are judged more attractive by other humans, which are proposed to lead to enhanced signaller fitness. However, unlike many studies in non-human animals, most studies of human colour have not measured, or are unable to measure, true Darwinian fitness.
In addition to receiver psychology, social psychologists Elliot and Maier have developed a model for how colour affects psychological functioning in humans—colour-in-context (CIC) theory [8]. CIC theory has six core premises. The first is that colour is not only about aesthetics, but also carries meaning and functional value. Second, colours become associated with positive or negative meaning and affect behaviour. For example, colours that carry positive associations evoke approach-oriented psychological processes. Third, responses to colour are automatic (responses occur without conscious awareness). Fourth, responses to colour derive from both innate biological preferences but also from learning. Fifth, colour perception varies as a function of the psychological state of the perceiver. Sixth, and finally, that colour carries different meanings in different contexts.
We do not see the two frameworks as mutually exclusive. Both approaches propose that colour carries meaning, and therefore has a function; both recognize that preferences do not have to be unlearned, but can be acquired through experience and learning; and both frameworks emphasize the advantages of studying the cognitive mechanisms of receivers. However, where the two frameworks differ is that the receiver psychology approach can be directly translated into clear, testable hypotheses, whereas it is more difficult to make precise hypotheses with CIC theory (this has also been recognized by Elliot [18]). We see that using principles from both approaches could be useful in research on human colour, because this would not only allow us to understand the strategic component (the content) of signals, but CIC theory could also provide novel ideas about how the signals have their effects. Together, both receiver psychology and CIC theory could provide both ultimate and proximate explanations for human colour. We think that Guilford and Dawkins [17] represent this in the last statement of their paper (p. 10).
However much we understand the strategic component of signal design, we will never explain why signals are the way they are and why they differ so greatly from species to species until we have a clearer idea of how they have their effects.
(b). Methods used in research on human colour
To assess the different methods used, and parameters reported, in research on human colour, we conducted a Web of Science search for papers published between 2014 and 2016 (October) using the terms color (in no searches did entering ‘colour’ provide results beyond those provided by ‘color’) and attractiveness (n = 173 results), color and attraction (n = 271), color/coloration and health (n = 6228 and n = 72), color and health and attractiveness (n = 25), color and dominance (n = 272), color/coloration and competition (n = 535 and n = 63), color and contests (n = 588), and coloration and mate choice (n = 161). We excluded review papers, methodological papers, research on clinical or pathological colour measurement, papers that focused on non-human animals, papers that assessed the colour of products and foods, papers that quantified spatial (i.e. skin topography) rather than chromatic properties of human colour, and papers that focused on appearance without measuring colour, or that measured colour preferences without quantifying colour. The final set of 16 papers included only those that used either a spectrophotometer (n = 2), camera (n = 12) or colour chart (n = 2) to measure colour (see supplementary material).
In comparison to the 60 papers on non-human animals reviewed by White et al. [19], researchers focusing on human colour predominantly use photography (75%), whereas researchers of non-human animal colours are most likely to use a spectrophotometer to measure colour (85%). Photography is fast, allows for distance between the researcher and the participant and permits analysis of a larger area rather than a limited number of point samples [20]. However, perhaps because photography is more accessible and perceived as a simpler process than spectrophotometry, the application and reporting of this method is often less rigorous (see also [21]). Our literature review revealed that detailed descriptions of photograph acquisition and standardization are often missing, and this may impede analysis of the discrepancies between papers and accurate replication of findings [19,21]. Most studies reported the camera model (75%). However, camera lens (25%), camera-to-sitter distance (33%), focal length (17%) and camera settings (25%) were reported less often. Lighting conditions were described in 83% of the papers, although the level of detail varies. A set of guidelines for reporting of parameters would improve the reproducibility of colour measurement research in humans (see table 1).
Table 1.
information to be reported | reason | further discussion |
---|---|---|
Camera model | Camera models differ in spectral sensitivity and responses to changes in light intensity and/or radiance. | Stevens et al. [22,28] |
Camera lens | Optics can introduce spherical and chromatic aberration. | Remondino & Fraser [145] |
Camera-to-sitter distance | The distance of the sitter's face to the film/sensor plane of the camera can affect how faces are perceived. A distance of 2 m was most frequently preferred in one study. | Bryan et al. [146]; Verhoff et al. [147]. |
Focal length | Often confused with the camera-to-sitter distance, focal length is the distance between the lens and the image sensor. Wider angle lenses (short focal length, e.g. 10–28 mm) make a face appear to bulge towards the camera, enlarging the nose relative to the ears, while telephoto lenses (long focal length, e.g. 100 mm +) make a face appear ‘flatter’. A constant focal length of 40–60 mm is advised. | Banks et al. [148]; Třebický et al. [21] |
Camera settings (F-stop, shutter speed, ISO, white balance) | Exposure, aperture and white balancing vary across cameras and will often produce data in which outputs (e.g. RGB values) are incorrectly weighted. | White et al. [19] |
Lighting conditions | Colour perception is not only a function of lightness, chroma and hue, but also the amount and type of ambient light. Cross-polarized filters may be used to capture skin colour information that is not influenced by shadows or highlights. | Fink et al. [149] |
Background colour/sitter's clothing | Clothing colours, especially near to the area being photographed, may cast a notable tint on skin tones (colour spill), or affect the colour saturation and brightness of the photo. Given the evidence that women's choice of clothing is confounded with variables of interest, standard clothing should be used to limit the effects of clothing on appearance. | Lindner & Winkler [150]; Burriss et al. [20] |
Colour standards | Colour standards are necessary to correct for both ambient light and camera biases towards specific wavelengths. Each pixel value should be the same in each colour channel with respect to a grey standard. | Stevens et al. [28] |
Sex of the photographer | The sex of the photographer can influence the sitter's facial temperature. If increases in temperature are associated with blushing, the sex of the photographer may also affect the sitter's skin colour. | Hahn et al. [151] |
Time of year | Differences in sun exposure can affect skin colour measurements. | Jablonski & Chaplin [152] |
Image format | Compressed file formats (e.g. JPEG) may introduce chromatic and spatial artefacts to images, whereas uncompressed formats (i.e. TIFF and RAW) typically do not. If compressed files are used, or if files are compressed during processing, the level of compression should be reported. | Troscianko & Stevens [23] |
Software for image processing | A range of available software programs can be used to analyse and to calibrate the content of digital images, including MATLAB (The Mathworks Inc. Natick, MA, USA), Image J and Adobe Photoshop (Adobe Systems Inc., San Jose, CA, USA). | Stevens et al. [22] |
Linearization of sensor outputs | Cameras respond nonlinearly to changes in light intensity and/or radiance, and are biased towards certain wavebands, particularly the long (red), meaning that data from nonlinear images will almost always under- or overestimate true object values. A linear response to radiance is essential if one is to convert images to visual-system spaces. Given the possibility of eye–camera metamerism, 40–60 colour samples are recommended for using colour charts. | Stevens et al. 2007 [22,28] |
Research on animal colour is increasingly employing methods that model signals in the receiver's colour spaces by incorporating the spectral sensitivity and number of retinal photoreceptors in calculations [22,23]. Research on human colour also models how signals are perceived by the human visual system by calculating the distance between two colours in the International Commission on Illumination (CIE) LAB space (67% of studies in our sample). In this system, a difference between colours is described in ΔE units, where ΔE 1 is the smallest difference visible by the human perceptual system (but see [24], which states a ΔE of 2.2). However, recent psychophysical experiments suggest that the human colour perception system is optimized for detecting relatively subtle changes in facial redness, compared with detecting changes in the redness of other types of stimuli or detecting changes in facial coloration on other colour axes [25]. Discrimination thresholds for within-subject changes in facial redness are very low (e.g. ΔE 0.67 [26]), and when asked to discriminate between the healthiness and attractiveness of faces differing in carotenoid colour (see §3), participants are sensitive to differences of ΔE 1.37 and ΔE 1.55, respectively. There is a clear discrepancy in what value of ΔE constitutes a discriminable difference in the context of human skin colour. ΔE may be as low as 0.67 [26] or as high as 2.2 [24]. This topic warrants further study.
Where colour has not been modelled in CIELAB, researchers have analysed RGB values alone [27]. This is a more convenient method because most consumer cameras output images as JPGs, with each pixel assigned a red, green and blue value. However, the use of uncalibrated RGB values does not represent colour as it is processed by the human visual system, because cameras respond nonlinearly to light intensity and are biased towards certain wavebands, particularly the long (red) [28]. Researchers who neither report correcting for these problems, nor state how the changes in colour they describe would be perceived by humans, could have identified effects that are inaccurate or, if genuine, so small as to be biologically irrelevant [29]. Only 50% of the studies we reviewed reported the colour standards used to standardize colour, and only 33% controlled for the camera's nonlinear responses to changes in light intensity and/or radiance [28]. These details are essential for assessing the validity of colour metrics.
Most (but not all) of the studies on human colour to date have been conducted in Western countries on White participants (mainly university students). Therefore, although statements of universal colour preference pervade the literature [30], readers of this review should keep in mind that there is not solid support for certain colour traits as cross-cultural indicators of good health, fertility or dominance [31].
3. Empirical findings on human colour
(a). Research on colour in health and attractiveness
Perceptions of a person's health and attractiveness are based partly on characteristics of face shape, such as symmetry and masculinity/femininity [32]. Although face shape may reflect a person's physical and mental health [33–35], better estimates of current or recent health may be based on more labile cues, including facial skin colour [10].
(i). Skin colour: blood oxygenation and carotenoids
When researchers give participants the ability to manipulate the overall colour of facial images to optimize apparent healthiness, they increase redness (the a* axis of the CIELAB human colour space; Commission Internationale d'Eclairage), yellowness (b*) and lightness (L*) [36]. Further studies have indicated that yellower and redder skin is also more attractive in male faces [37,38]. Skin yellowness and redness are probably linked to health and attractiveness because these colour properties vary with current health [39].
Regarding long-term physical condition, blood perfusion and oxygenation, which are reduced when a person's health or cardiovascular fitness is poor, are linked to reduced skin redness [40,41]. When asked to adjust the colour of male and female facial images to optimize their healthy and attractive appearance, participants increase skin blood colour [26,42]. When judging for health and attractiveness, participants discriminate between faces differing in oxygenated blood colour at similar ΔE thresholds [26]. This suggests that perceptions of attractiveness and health that are based on skin redness might be closely linked. There is also evidence that facial healthiness mediates the effect of red skin colour on female sexual attractiveness [43].
Focusing on current health, human participants who are injected with a bacterial endotoxin to induce acute sickness exhibit noticeable changes in their skin after only 1 h—facial skin becomes lighter and less red, while arm skin becomes darker, less red and less yellow. Colour changes peak when participants' subjective ratings of illness are at their highest [39].
Skin yellowness and darkness are driven by melanin and carotenoid pigments [44,45]. These pigments are related to health: among other benefits, carotenoid supplements increase T-lymphocyte numbers [46], and melanin protects against ultraviolet radiation damage to DNA [47]. When participants are tasked with optimizing the healthy appearance of faces, they adjust skin colour to increase carotenoid and melanin coloration, but favour differences based on carotenoids [48]. Skin tanning, in which exposure to the sun stimulates melanization of the skin, is attractive in Western countries like the UK [49]. However, carotenoid coloration increases attractiveness more than melanin coloration, and this effect is stronger for female than male faces [50]. Consuming approximately three extra portions of carotenoid-rich fruits and/or vegetables per day is enough to induce a change in skin colour that increases perceptions of health and attractiveness [51].
These facial skin colour preferences are unlikely to be explained by sensory biases or preferences for particular colours. Detection thresholds for changes in carotenoid and blood oxygenation coloration are lower than thresholds for healthiness and attractiveness [26,51], whereas we would expect identical thresholds if social perceptions were based solely on detectable colour differences. Furthermore, the attractiveness of non-face stimuli is not influenced by carotenoid coloration [52]. From a receiver psychology perspective, increases in skin redness and yellowness are detectable and discriminable, and make an effective signal of current as well as long-term health (this is the strategic design of a health signal within a receiver psychology framework).
(ii). Skin colour distribution
In most studies of face colour, photographic stimuli are manipulated such that all areas of facial skin are altered uniformly. These manipulations may not represent how colour varies naturally. Differences in colour homogeneity, due to melanin and haemoglobin distribution patterns, vary with age [53]. The relative luminance or hue of features such as the eyes and the lips and the surrounding skin also influences how faces are perceived: the faces of middle-aged women rated as healthy in appearance tend to have more luminous periorbital regions and sclerae (the whites of the eyes) and redder cheeks and lips [54]. The luminance of the eyebrows, eyes and mouth is lower than that of the surrounding skin in younger women [55], and decreasing the luminance of the features and increasing that of overall facial skin makes female faces more attractive and male faces less attractive [56]. Female faces exhibit greater facial contrast at the eyes and mouth than do male faces, to the extent that varying the contrast of an androgynous face while keeping the shape of the face constant can induce the viewer to perceive the face as male or female [57]. However, most studies on contrast have used greyscale stimuli, which suggest the possibility that viewers are attributing changes in luminance to changes in other colour channels.
(b). Colour and human ornamentation
Unlike other animals, humans are able to modify their appearance through various forms of ornamentation, such as cosmetics and clothing. Most work on this topic has centred on women's use of the colour red to enhance their attractiveness.
(i). Use of cosmetics
Empirical research on make-up use and face colour is in its infancy: authors have generally been interested in natural differences in face colour, and request participants to remove any make-up before being photographed. Russell [58] photographed women both with and without self-applied make-up and reported that the contemporary ‘received style’ of make-up among women in industrialized nations is typified by lips darkened and reddened with lipstick, cheeks reddened with blusher, periorbital regions darkened with eye shadow and mascara, and the overall colour of facial skin rendered more homogeneous with foundation. This style may be popular because it emphasizes the colour patterns that differentiate women from men and young from old [59]. Make-up applied in the received style is attractive to men and influences real-world male behaviour [60–62], as well as enhancing perceptions of women's health, confidence and earning potential [63]. We note that cosmetic use is highly subject to fashion and that wearers may adopt non-received styles of make-up for various reasons, such as group identification.
(ii). Colour of clothing
The effects of clothing colour on how a person is perceived, and particularly the effects of red clothing on female attractiveness, has received a lot of research interest. Men find women pictured in red (rather than blue) clothing to be more attractive (though see [64]), and express a greater willingness to date a woman in red [65]. The red effect persists outside of the laboratory, with female hitchhikers who wear red more likely to receive an offer of a ride from male, but not female, drivers [66]. Women appear to be aware that red clothing enhances their attractiveness to men. Women interested in casual sex choose to wear red clothing in photographs posted to Internet dating websites [67] (men preferentially initiate contact with women who wear red in profile pictures [68]), and prefer red clothing and other forms of ornamentation when expecting to interact with attractive men, while avoiding red ornamentation if an anticipated interaction partner is unattractive [69]. Women perceive other women who wear red (rather than white) as more sexually receptive, and as less faithful to their partners [70]. Women express a greater desire to guard their partner against the advances of a rival wearing red (rather than green) [70], and men are more willing to guard a female partner wearing red (rather than black) [71]. It will be noted that most authors contrast perceptions of red ornamentation with those of a single other colour, and the variety of colours can make it difficult to compare results across studies (but see, e.g. [66,72]).
Although the effects of red ornamentation on the attractiveness of men has received less study, men are perceived to be more attractive when wearing red clothes, an effect that is mediated by perceptions of status [73]. A man's necktie colour may also influence perceived ability in job interviews [74], although it does not affect the perceived competence of politicians [75].
(iii). Extended colour stimuli
Colour does not only have an influence on attraction when displayed directly on the skin, but also when seen in close proximity to a person. Women whose photographs are displayed with a border of red, compared to other colours, are rated as more attractive and as higher in sexual desire and sexual intentions [65,76]. Red does not lead men to view women more positively in general, as it has no effect on ratings of likeability, perceived kindness or perceived intelligence. A red background has more pronounced effects on attractiveness when the woman pictured is rated at baseline as highly attractive [77], or if she is in her twenties rather than her fifties [78]. There is tentative evidence that this effect may generalize to other cultures [76]. Burkinabé men (a group in Burkina Faso for which red generally carries a negative meaning, such as death, sickness or bad luck) rate photographs of woman with red borders more attractive than women bordered by blue. The men also report more interest in meeting and courting the woman in a red-bordered photograph.
(c). Colour and the ovulatory cycle
The females of several primate species advertise their ovulatory status through anogenital swelling [79–81], and facial or perineal skin colour may also vary cyclically [29,82–84]. Although human ovulation is not advertised, as it is in several primate species, there have been reports of women's skin darkening during the luteal (low-fertility) phase of the menstrual cycle and during pregnancy ([85] and references within). However, much of this early evidence came from women's self-reports of facial skin change, and participants were often aware of the studies' aims and hypotheses. Furthermore, these studies often lacked objective quantitative colour metrics [86].
There is increasing evidence that women's faces are rated more attractive during the most fertile phase of their menstrual cycle [87,88]. Cycle effects on facial attractiveness may be underpinned by changes in facial shape [27,89,90]. However, because oestrogen and progesterone levels vary over the menstrual cycle [91], and since oestrogen is associated with many aspects of skin physiology, including ageing, healing, hydration, hair growth, sebum production and pigmentation [92–94], recent research has also focused on hormone-related variation in skin colour.
Early research on hormonal correlates of face colour aimed at revealing differences between women (references within van den Berghe & Frost [85]). We first review that literature before describing more recent research on within-individual differences.
(i). Inter-individual variation in hormones and coloration in women
Oestrogen and progesterone levels vary between women, and those with especially high oestrogen levels during the late follicular (fertile) phase of their cycle are rated as more attractive, feminine and healthy [95,96]. These increased ratings of attractiveness emerge only when women are instructed to remove make-up before being photographed [87,88,95,97], which suggests that natural changes in the skin explain these differences.
Higher oestrogen is associated with greater vascularization and blood vessel dilation [98,99]. Human participants who have had their ovaries removed show changes in the vascularity and luminance of their skin (measured as percentage reflectance with a spectrophotometer) compared to the skin of regularly cycling women [100].
(ii). Intra-individual variation in hormones and coloration in women
The evidence for skin colour change across an individual ovulatory cycle is equivocal. Edwards & Duntley [100] were among the first authors to conduct a within-participants longitudinal study on skin colour changes across the cycle. They reported reduced vascularization during menses, and increased haemoglobin and vascularity after mid-cycle, which could affect perceptions of skin colour (redness) and luminance, but did not model this in human colour space. Samson et al. [97] compared the skin in late follicular (high-fertility) and mid-luteal phases of women's cycles in CIELAB and found no effect of fertility status on any of the three colour dimensions, leading the authors to conclude that ‘differences in men's perceptions of attractiveness and healthiness [are] not driven by these [colour] measures’. In contrast, Oberzaucher et al. [27] took photographs of women near ovulation and during the luteal phase, and extracted mean RGB colour values from cheek patches. While they found that skin was redder at peri-ovulation than during the luteal phase, these results should be interpreted with some caution (see §2b).
Recently, in a longitudinal study, Jones et al. [101] showed that women's facial skin was significantly redder, but not yellower or lighter, when salivary oestradiol was increased during the cycle. Jones et al. [101] suggest that these colour changes may be detectable by the human visual system, given that discrimination thresholds for within-participant changes in facial redness are lower than for non-face stimuli [26]. However, in a similar longitudinal study, Burriss et al. [20] found that redness was slightly heightened between ovulation and menses, with an average amplitude change of about 0.6 ΔE. This difference is unlikely to be perceivable by the human visual system based on the ΔE range where differences become perceivable [24]. However, a blood perfusion change of 0.67 ΔE is detectable [26]. It therefore remains possible that cyclical changes in skin redness are detectable under perfect laboratory conditions.
Elliot & Niesta [65] speculated that women might augment any cyclical changes in skin colour by adopting red ornamentation near ovulation. Women do appear to wear more red and pink clothing when most fertile [102], especially when the weather is inclement and wearing revealing clothing (another style that women may adopt to attract partners [103]) is a less appealing option [104].
(d). Colour in dominance and competitive performance
(i). Colour and its effects on aggression and dominance
Non-human animals often signal dominance to potential opponents through colourful displays, especially during competitive encounters [105–107]. Many animals also plastically change their colour, for example, becoming redder during agonistic encounters (e.g. male turkeys swell their caruncles and become redder) or with changes in dominance (e.g. male mandrills exhibit an increase in testosterone and become redder if a dominance status challenge is successful [108]). However, changes in facial temperature with anger are not always observed in humans [109], nor is it customary to directly measure facial temperature (e.g. forehead pulse amplitude is used as a proxy of facial flushing [110,111]). To the best of our knowledge, no laboratory studies have attempted to investigate the effects of agonistic interactions on measured human facial colour.
It has also been suggested that human male facial skin redness is also androgen-dependent [37,112], but this link is based on research that does not quantitatively measure skin colour as it would be perceived by the human visual system [113]. Increasing the redness of male human facial images enhances women's perceptions of dominance and aggression [37], but whether actual dominance/aggression is associated with testosterone and skin colour remains untested.
(ii). Colour and competitive sporting performance
The influence of colour on sporting performance has received the most attention from researchers of human colour, both conceptually and empirically. The most colourful animal displays often elicit avoidance or withdrawal behaviour in conspecifics [105,114] and heterospecifics [115]. These ‘badges of dominance’ can therefore help individuals to assess the competitive ability of their opponent and avoid the costs of escalated aggressive encounters [116]. It is not only naturally conspicuous stimuli that have this effect; animals whose colour signals are augmented by experimenters are more likely to win contests [117,118]. It is plausible that humans, who are able to manipulate their apparent colour, have augmented physiological dominance signals with voluntary signalling. In support of this idea, it has been demonstrated that testosterone levels are associated with an active choice of red clothing for competitive events [119] (but see [120]), and that individuals who choose red tend to rate their colour as more dominant and aggressive than do those who choose blue [119]. Heart rate is elevated in fighters assigned to wear red compared to those assigned to wear blue, before, during and after physical combat [121].
Hill & Barton [122] examined the outcomes of the 2004 Olympic combat sports of western boxing, taekwondo, Graeco-Roman wrestling and freestyle wrestling. In these sports, the participants are randomly assigned either red or blue apparel; those fighting in red won more fights and more rounds across all weight classes (though see [123,124]). Hill & Barton [122] found no significant effects of clothing colour in female combat sports (see also [125]), which may reflect inherent differences in intrasexual competition between the sexes [126].
In non-combat sports such as association football (soccer), red still seems to have an effect (though see [127]). Hill & Barton [122] found that teams wearing red in the 2004 Union of European Football Associations (UEFA) European Championships scored more goals than when they played in their alternative colours. In the English Football League between 1945 and 2006, the median league position and the mean percentage of maximum possible points were greatest for teams that wore red [128]. Viewing red (rather than blue or green) on a goalkeeper's uniform undermines penalty kick performance [129], and goalkeepers perceive a higher likelihood of saving penalties kicked by players wearing white uniforms rather than red [130].
Viewing red rather than green or blue stimuli has a greater distracting effect for men [131], and red has a negative effect on motor performance of opponents [121,132]. Red arouses more than blue, and the emotional valence of red may differ for men and women [133]. In non-physical contexts, participants who are presented with the colour red in an achievement context (e.g. an IQ test) preferentially choose easier tasks [134], decrease their walking speed [135] and even physically move away from red stimuli [136].
It could be that the colour red has less to do with intrasexual competition than with how competitors are perceived by others, a possibility supported by research showing that, independent of the increases in the wearer's own dominance and aggressiveness, wearing red affects the reactions of sports referees [137]. Hagemann et al. [138], using video compositing software, reversed the colour of the head guards and torso protectors worn by taekwondo fighters, and found that judges who viewed the videos awarded significantly more points to a competitor in red than blue, even though the content of the fight was identical across conditions. Krenn [139] digitally manipulated the colour of soccer kits and found that professional referees judged tackles committed by a player wearing red rather than blue as more harsh. Likewise, in the US National Hockey League (NHL) and National Football League (NFL), teams that wore black uniforms were penalized significantly more than teams in other coloured uniforms. This apparent bias in referees' judgements persisted when teams switched from non-black to black uniforms: those that switched to black were penalized more than those that switched to non-black [140]. Therefore, in a competitive context, red is an effective signal because is has effects on both the wearer (signaller) and receivers (opponent and referees)—this is the tactical design in receiver psychology.
4. Broad conclusion
(a). Suggested improvements for methods and reporting of methods
(i). Methods: creating stimuli
It is common practice to present participants with images of a person whose facial skin colour or clothing colour is manipulated using computer graphics software [65,68,74,75,135,141,142]. This method allows the researcher to limit the influence of confounding variables, but a disadvantage is that the manipulation is often unrealistic. For example, clothing colour saturation or luminance levels are sometimes outside the range one might expect to see under normal conditions of lighting and with standard textiles. Furthermore, the colours of facial stimuli are sometimes unrealistic, embodying colours that are not biologically possible. Although this allows researchers to define what makes an effective signal (the tactical design in receiver psychology), it does not allow researchers to conclude that humans are actually using these signals for the strategic purpose of increasing attractiveness, and that these signals are detectable and discriminable. We encourage researchers to create stimuli based on colour manipulations that vary in ecologically relevant and specific units of discriminability (e.g. [48,51,143]). Researchers should also control for potential wearer effects, whereby the wearer adopts subtly different facial expressions depending on their clothing colour [72], perhaps because they are aware that some types of clothing make them appear more attractive [144]. It is also plausible that any wearer effects are augmented by light reflecting off clothes and casting a noticeable tint on skin tones (colour spill), such that the skin of a person wearing red clothes would appear to be redder and therefore more attractive.
(ii). Reporting of methodological parameters
From our survey of papers published on human colour (§2), we have compiled a list of standardization procedures that are widely used by animal-colour and basic-colour scientists. We list these procedures in table 1 in order that future research provides detailed descriptions of photograph acquisition and standardization. Our objective is to assist researchers to investigate discrepancies between research results and allow accurate replication of research [19,21]. Following these guidelines for reporting of parameters will improve the reproducibility of colour measurement research in humans. We refer readers to White et al. [19] for comprehensive information on spectrophotometry.
5. Concluding remarks
Clearly, overall changes in facial colour and contrast are sufficient to alter face perceptions but do not tell the whole story. For example, while we know that red lips are more attractive, we do not know if this is because they are red or because they are redder than the surrounding skin, or whether dark periorbital regions appear less healthy because they are dark or because they are darker than the surrounding skin. Studies in which the colour of facial skin outside of specific features or regions is manipulated independently, and to the same degree, as the colour of facial skin within these features and regions will allow researchers to test whether any effects of contrast are independent of those attributable to overall colour.
Given the connections between red and aggression and dominance, as well as the suggestion by Changizi et al. [153] that colour vision in primates was selected for discriminating emotional states, socio-sexual signals and threat displays (the strategic design in receiver psychology), future research should focus on the tactical design of these potential signals and investigate the effect of agonistic interactions on human facial colour—whether actual dominance/aggression in men is associated with testosterone and skin colour, and whether these colour differences are detectable, discriminable and memorable to receivers. To the best of our knowledge, no one has yet examined how female colour relates to testosterone.
Future research that replicates in non-White participants the effects seen in studies with White participants may reveal whether skin colour judgements of health and attractiveness are universal, as are preferences for facial symmetry [154]. For example, oestradiol levels are higher at all points of the cycle in African American compared to White American women [155]; as oestrogen is implicated in cyclic variation in phenotype [88,101,156–158], facial attractiveness may vary differently in women of different ethnicities. Cross-cultural research on judgements of health would be worthwhile: one study has shown that Black African observers tend to rely more on skin colour when judging the attractiveness of in-group faces, while White Europeans tend to rely more on face shape [159]. This research would lend support to the strategic design of facial colour—that it signals qualities that are attractive to potential mates or enhance intrasexual competitiveness, and may have evolved through sexual selection to enhance reproductive success (the strategic design). This may also provide important evidence for the evolutionary origins of these potential signals in humans.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank Tim Caro, James Higham and three anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments.
Authors' contributions
H.M.R. collected the data. Both authors interpreted the data and conceived the recommendations and equally contributed to writing the article and the conception of the review.
Competing interests
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
H.M.R. is supported by an Institute Research Fellowship at the Institute of Zoology. R.P.B. is supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation under project no. 162697.
References
- 1.Humphrey N. 1976. The colour currency of nature. In Colour for architecture (eds Porter T, Mikellides B), pp. 95–98 London, UK: Studio-Vista. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hurlbert A, Ling Y. 2012. Understanding colour perception and preference. In Colour design: theories and applications (ed. Best J.), pp. 129–157. Philadelphia, PA: Woodhead. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hurlbert AC, Owen KA. 2014. Biological, cultural, and developmental influences on color preference. In Handbook of color psychology (eds Elliott AJ, Fairchild MD), pp. 435–453. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Mollon JD, Sharpe LT. 1983. Colour vision: physiology and psychophysics. London, UK: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Mollon JD, Pokorny J, Knoblauch K. 2003. Normal and defective colour vision. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Haynie HJ, Bowern C. 2016. Phylogenetic approach to the evolution of color term systems. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 13 666–13 671. ( 10.1073/pnas.1613666113) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Spence C. 2015. On the psychological impact of food colour. Flavour 4, 21 ( 10.1186/s13411-015-0031-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Elliot AJ, Maier MA. 2014. Color psychology: effects of perceiving color on psychological functioning in humans. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65, 95–120. ( 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115035) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Fink B, Grammer K, Matts PJ. 2006. Visible skin color distribution plays a role in the perception of age, attractiveness, and health in female faces. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 433–442. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.08.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jones BC, Little AC, Burt DM, Perrett DI. 2004. When facial attractiveness is only skin deep. Perception 33, 569–576. ( 10.1068/p3463) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Darwin C. 1872. The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London, UK: John Murray. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Jablonski NG, Chaplin G. 2000. The evolution of human skin coloration. J. Hum. Evol. 39, 57–106. ( 10.1006/jhev.2000.0403) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Jablonski NG, Chaplin G.2017. The colours of humanity: the evolution of pigmentation in the human lineage. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 372 , 20160349. ( ) [DOI]
- 14.Hunter M. 2007. The persistent problem of colorism: skin tone, status, and hierarchy. Soc. Compass. 1, 237–254. ( 10.1111/j.1751-9020.2007.00006.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Endler JA, Mappes J. 2017. The current and future state of animal coloration research. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 372, 20160352 ( 10.1098/rstb.2016.0352) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hamilton WD, Zuk M. 1982. Heritable true fitness and bright birds: a role for parasites? Science 218, 384–387. ( 10.1126/science.7123238) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Guilford T, Dawkins MS. 1991. Receiver psychology and the evolution of animal signals. Anim. Behav. 42, 1–14. ( 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80600-1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Elliot AJ. 2015. Color and psychological functioning: a review of theoretical and empirical work. Front. Psychol. 6, 368 ( 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00368) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.White TE, Dalrymple RL, Noble DW, O'Hanlon JC, Zurek DB, Umbers KD. 2015. Reproducible research in the study of biological coloration. Anim. Behav. 106, 51–57. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.05.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Burriss RP, Troscianko J, Lovell PG, Fulford AJC, Stevens M, Quigley R, Payne J, Saxton TK, Rowland HM. 2015. Changes in women's facial skin color over the ovulatory cycle are not detectable by the human visual system. PLoS ONE 10, e0130093 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0130093) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Třebický V, Fialová J, Kleisner K, Havlíček J. 2016. Focal length affects depicted shape and perception of facial images. PLoS ONE 11, e0149313 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0149313) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Stevens M, Stoddard MC, Higham JP. 2009. Studying primate color: towards visual system-dependent methods. Int. J. Primatol. 30, 893–917. ( 10.1007/s10764-009-9356-z) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Troscianko J, Stevens M. 2015. Image calibration and analysis toolbox – a free software suite for objectively measuring reflectance, colour and pattern. Method Ecol. Evol. 6, 1320–1331. ( 10.1111/2041-210X.12439) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Brainard DH. 2003. Color appearance and color difference specification. In The science of color (ed. Shevell SK.), pp. 191–216. Boston, MA: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Tan KW, Stephen ID. 2013. Colour detection thresholds in faces and colour patches. Perception 42, 733–741. ( 10.1068/p7499) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Re DE, Whitehead RD, Xiao D, Perrett DI. 2011. Oxygenated-blood colour change thresholds for perceived facial redness, health, and attractiveness. PLoS ONE 6, e17859 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0017859) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Oberzaucher E, Katina S, Schmehl SF, Holzleitner IJ, Mehu-Blantar I, Grammer K. 2012. The myth of hidden ovulation: shape and texture changes in the face during the menstrual cycle. J. Evol. Psychol. 10, 163–175. ( 10.1556/JEP.10.2012.4.1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Stevens M, Párraga CA, Cuthill IC, Partridge JC, Troscianko TS. 2007. Using digital photography to measure animal colouration. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. Lond. 90, 211–237. ( 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2007.00725.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Higham JP, Brent LJN, Dubuc C, Accamando AK, Engelhardt A, Gerald MS, Heistermann M, Stevens M. 2010. Color signal information content and the eye of the beholder: a case study in the rhesus macaque. Behav. Ecol. 21, 739–746. ( 10.1093/beheco/arq047) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Taylor C, Clifford A, Franklin A. 2013. Color preferences are not universal. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142, 1015–1027. ( 10.1037/a0030273) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Langlois JH, Kalakanis L, Rubenstein AJ, Larson A, Hallamm M, Smoot M. 2000. Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychol. Bull. 126, 390–423. ( 10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.390) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Little AC, Jones BC, DeBruine LM. 2011. Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research. Phil. Trans. R Soc. B 366, 1638–1659. ( 10.1098/rstb.2010.0404) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Shackelford TK, Larsen RJ. 1997. Facial asymmetry as an indicator of psychological, emotional and physiological distress. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 456–466. ( 10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.456) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Thornhill R, Gangestad SW. 2006. Facial sexual dimorphism, developmental stability, and susceptibility to disease in men and women. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 131–144. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.06.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Milne BJ, Belsky J, Poulton R, Thomson WM, Caspi A, Kieser J. 2003. Fluctuating asymmetry and physical health among young adults. Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 53–63. ( 10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00120-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Stephen ID, Law Smith MJ, Stirrat MR, Perrett DI. 2009. Facial skin coloration affects perceived health of human faces. Int. J. Primatol. 30, 845–857. ( 10.1007/s10764-009-9380-z) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Stephen ID, Oldham FH, Perrett DI, Barton RA. 2012. Redness enhances perceived aggression, dominance and attractiveness in men's faces. Evol. Psychol. 10, 562–572. ( 10.1177/147470491201000312) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Stephen ID, Scott IML, Coetzee V, Pound N, Perrett DI, Penton-Voak IS. 2012. Cross-cultural effects of color, but not morphological masculinity, on perceived attractiveness of men's faces. Evol. Hum. Behav. 33, 260–267. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.10.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Henderson AJ, Lasselin J, Lekander M, Olsson MJ, Powis SJ, Axelsson J, Perrett DJ. 2017. Skin colour changes during experimentally-induced sickness. Brain Behav. Immun. 60, 312–318. ( 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.11.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Muhe L, Oljira B, Degefu H, Jaffar S, Weber MW. 2000. Evaluation of clinical pallor in the identification and treatment of children with moderate and severe anaemia. Trop. Med. Int. Health. 5, 805–810. ( 10.1046/j.1365-3156.2000.00637.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Armstrong N, Welsman J. 2001. Peak oxygen uptake in relation to growth and maturation in 11- to 17–year-old humans. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 85, 546–551. ( 10.1007/s004210100485) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Stephen ID, Coetzee V, Law Smith M, Perrett DI. 2009. Skin blood perfusion and oxygenation colour affect perceived human health. PLoS ONE 4, e5083 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0005083) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Pazda AD, Thorstenson CA, Elliot AJ, Perrett DI. 2016. Women's facial redness increases their perceived attractiveness: mediation through perceived healthiness. Perception 45, 739–754. ( 10.1177/0301006616633386) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Stamatas GN, Zmudka BZ, Kollas N, Beer JZ. 2004. Non-invasive measurements of skin pigmentation in situ. Pigment Cell Res. 17, 618–626. ( 10.1111/j.1600-0749.2004.00204.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Alaluf A, Heinrich U, Stahl W, Tronnier H, Wiseman S. 2002. Dietary carotenoids contribute to normal human skin color and UV photosensitivity. J. Nutr. 132, 399–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Alexander M, Newmark H, Miller RG. 1985. Oral β-carotene can increase the number of OKT4+ cells in human blood. Immunol. Lett. 9, 221–224. ( 10.1016/0165-2478(85)90036-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Miyamura Y, et al. 2007. Regulation of human skin pigmentation and responses to ultraviolet radiation. Pigment Cell Melanoma Res. 20, 2–13. ( 10.1111/j.1600-0749.2006.00358.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Stephen ID, Coetzee V, Perrett DI. 2011. Carotenoid and melanin pigment coloration affect perceived human health. Evol. Hum. Behav. 32, 216–217. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.09.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Smith KL, Cornelissen PL, Tovée MJ. 2007. Color 3D bodies and judgements of human female attractiveness. Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 48–54. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.05.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Lefevre CE, Perrett DI. 2015. Fruit over sunbed: carotenoid skin coloration is found more attractive than melanin coloration. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 68, 284–293. ( 10.1080/17470218.2014.944194) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Whitehead RD, Re D, Xiao D, Ozakinci G, Perrett DI. 2012. You are what you eat: within-subject increases in fruit and vegetable consumption confer beneficial skin-color changes. PLoS ONE 7, e32988 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0032988) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Lefevre CE, Ewbank MP, Calder AJ, von dem Hagen E, Perrett DI. 2013. It is all in the face: carotenoid skin coloration loses attractiveness outside the face. Biol. Lett. 9, 20130633 ( 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0633) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Matts PJ, Fink B, Grammer K, Burquest M. 2007. Color homogeneity and visual perception of age, health, and attractiveness of female facial skin. J. Am. Acad. Dermatol. 57, 977–984. ( 10.1016/j.jaad.2007.07.040) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Jones AL, Porcheron A, Sweda JR, Morizot F, Russell R. 2016. Coloration in different areas of facial skin is a cue to health: the role of cheek redness and periorbital luminance in health perception. Body Image 17, 57–66. ( 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.02.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Porcheron A, Mauger E, Russell R. 2013. Aspects of facial contrast decrease with age and are cues for age perception. PLoS ONE 8, e57985 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0057985) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Russell R. 2003. Sex, beauty, and the relative luminance of facial features. Perception 32, 1093–1107. ( 10.1068/p5101) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Russell R. 2009. A sex difference in facial contrast and its exaggeration by cosmetics. Perception 38, 1211–1219. ( 10.1068/p6331) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Russell R. 2010. Why cosmetics work. In The science of social vision (eds Adams RB, Ambady N, Nakayama K, Shimojo S), pp. 186–204 Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Jones AL. 2015. Cosmetics alter biologically-based factors of beauty: evidence from facial contrast. Evol. Psychol. 13, 210–229. ( 10.1177/147470491501300113) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Guéguen N. 2012. Does red lipstick really attract men? An evaluation in a bar. Int. J. Psychol. Stud. 4, 206–209. ( 10.5539/ijps.v4n2p206) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Mulhern R, Fieldman G, Hussey T, Lévêque JL, Pineau P. 2003. Do cosmetics enhance female Caucasian facial attractiveness? Int. J. Cosmet. Sci. 25, 199–205. ( 10.1046/j.1467-2494.2003.00188.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Jacob C, Gueguen N, Boulbry G, Ardiccioni R. 2010. Waitresses’ facial cosmetics and tipping: a field experiment. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 29, 188–190. ( 10.1016/j.ijhm.2009.04.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Nash R, Fieldman G, Hussey T, Lévêque J-L, Pineau P. 2006. Cosmetics: they influence more than Caucasian female facial attractiveness. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 36, 493–504. ( 10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00016.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Peperkoorn LS, Roberts SC, Pollet TV. 2016. Revisiting the red effect on attractiveness and sexual receptivity: No effect of the color red on human mate preferences. Evol. Psychol. 14, 1–13 ( 10.1177/1474704916673841) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Elliot AJ, Niesta D. 2008. Romantic red: red enhances men's attraction to women. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95, 1150–1164. ( 10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1150) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Guéguen N. 2012. Color and women hitchhikers’ attractiveness: Gentlemen drivers prefer red. Color Res. Appl. 37, 76–78. ( 10.1002/col.20651) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Elliot AJ, Pazda AD. 2012. Dressed for sex: red as a female sexual signal in humans. PLoS ONE 7, e34607 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0034607) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Guéguen N, Jacob C. 2013. Color and cyber-attractiveness: red enhances men's attraction to women's internet personal ads. Color Res. Appl. 38, 309–312. ( 10.1002/col.21718) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Niesta Kayser D, Agthe M, Maner JK. 2016. Strategic sexual signals: Women's display versus avoidance of the color red depends on the attractiveness of an anticipated interaction partner. PLoS ONE 11, e0148501 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0148501) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Pazda AD, Prokop P, Elliot AJ. 2014. Red and romantic rivalry: viewing another woman in red increases perceptions of sexual receptivity, derogration, and intentions to mate-guard. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 40, 1260–1269. ( 10.1177/0146167214539709) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Prokop P, Pazda AD. 2016. Women's red clothing can increase mate-guarding from their male partner. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 98, 114–117. ( 10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.021) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Roberts SC, Owen RC, Havlíček J. 2010. Distinguishing between perceiver and wearer effects in clothing color-associated attributions. Evol. Psychol. 8, 147470491000800304 ( 10.1177/147470491000800304) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Elliot AJ, Niesta Kayser D, Greitemeyer T, Lichtenfeld S, Gramzow RH, Maier MA, Liu H. 2010. Red, rank, and romance in women viewing men. J. Exp. Psychol. 139, 399–417. ( 10.1037/a0019689) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Maier MA, Elliot AJ, Lee B, Lichtenfeld S, Barchfeld P, Pekrun R. 2013. The influence of red on impression formation in a job application context. Motiv. Emotion 37, 389–401. ( 10.1007/s11031-012-9326-1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Kramer RSS. 2016. The red power(less) tie: perceptions of political leaders wearing red. Evol. Psychol. 14, 147470491665163 ( 10.1177/1474704916651634) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Elliot AJ, Tracy JL, Pazda AD, Beall AT. 2013. Red enhances women's attractiveness to men: first evidence suggesting universality. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 165–168. ( 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.07.017) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Young SG. 2015. The effect of red on male perceptions of female attractiveness: moderation by baseline attractiveness of female faces. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 45, 146–151. ( 10.1002/ejsp.2098) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Schwarz S, Singer M. 2013. Romantic red revisited: red enhances men's attraction to young, but not menopausal women. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 161–164. ( 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.08.004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Deschner T, Heistermann M, Hodges K, Boesch C. 2003. Timing and probability of ovulation in relation to sex skin swelling in wild West African chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus. Anim. Behav. 66, 551–560. ( 10.1006/anbe.2003.2210) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Higham JP, MacLarnon AM, Ross C, Heistermann M, Semple S. 2008. Baboon sexual swellings: information content of size and color. Horm. Behav. 53, 452–462. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.11.019) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Barelli C, Heistermann M, Boesch C, Reichard UH. 2007. Sexual swellings in wild white-handed gibbon females (Hylobates lar) indicate probability of ovulation. Horm. Behav. 51, 221–230. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.10.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Dubuc C, Brent LJN, Accamando AK, Gerald MS, MacLarnon A, Semple S, Heistermann M, Engelhardt A. 2009. Sexual skin color contains information about the timing of the fertile phase in free-ranging Macaca mulatta. Int. J. Primatol. 30, 777–789. ( 10.1007/s10764-009-9369-7) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Setchell J, Wickings EJ, Knapp LA. 2006. Signal content of red facial coloration in female mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 2395–2400. ( 10.1098/rspb.2006.3573) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Fujita S, Sugiura H, Mitsunaga F, Shimizu K. 2004. Hormone profiles and reproductive characteristics in wilde female Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Am. J. Primatol. 64, 367–375. ( 10.1002/ajp.20086) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.van den Berghe PL, Frost P. 1986. Skin color preference, sexual dimorphism and sexual selection: a case of gene-culture co-evolution? Ethnol. Racial Stud. 9, 87–113. ( 10.1080/01419870.1986.9993516) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 86.McGuinness BW. 1961. Skin pigmentation and the menstrual cycle. Br. Med. J. 2, 563–565. ( 10.1136/bmj.2.5251.563) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Roberts SC, Havlíček J, Flegr J, Hruskova M, Little AC, Jones BC, Perrett DI, Petrie M. 2004. Female facial attractiveness increases during the fertile phase of the cycle. Biol. Lett. 271, S270–S272. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2004.0174) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Puts DA, Bailey DH, Cárdenas RA, Burriss RP, Welling LLM, Wheatley JR, Dawood K. 2013. Women's attractiveness changes with estradiol and progesterone across the ovulatory cycle. Horm. Behav. 63, 13–19. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Bobst C, Lobmaier JS. 2012. Men's preference for the ovulating female is triggered by subtle face shape differences. Horm. Behav. 62, 413–417. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.07.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Bobst C, Lobmaier JS. 2014. Is preference for ovulatory female's faces associated with men's testosterone levels? Horm. Behav. 66, 487–492. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.06.015) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Mihm M, Gangooly S, Muttukrishna S. 2011. The normal menstrual cycle in women. Anim. Reprod. Sci. 124, 229–236. ( 10.1016/j.anireprosci.2010.08.030) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Thornton MJ. 2002. The biological action of estrogens on the skin. Exp. Dermatol. 11, 487–502. ( 10.1034/j.1600-0625.2002.110601.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Shah MG, Maibach HI. 2001. Estrogen and skin. Am. J. Clin. Dermatol. 2, 143–150. ( 10.2165/00128071-200102030-00003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Thornton MJ. 2005. Oestrogen functions in skin and skin appendages. Expert Opin. Ther. Targets. 9, 617–629. ( 10.1517/14728222.9.3.617) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Law Smith MJ, et al. 2006. Facial appearance is a cue to oestrogen levels in women. Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 135–140. ( 10.1098/rspb.2005.3296) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Probst F, Bobst C, Lobmaier JS. 2016. Testosterone-to-estradiol ratio is associated with female facial attractiveness. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 69, 89–99. ( 10.1080/17470218.2015.1024696) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Samson N, Fink B, Matts P. 2011. Does a woman's skin color indicate her fertility level? Preliminary findings. Swiss. J. Psychol. 70, 199–202. ( 10.1024/1421-0185/a000057) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Brincat MP. 2000. Hormone replacement therapy and the skin. Maturitas 35, 107–117. ( 10.1016/S0378-5122(00)00097-9) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Charkoudian N, Stephens DP, Pirkle KC, Kosiba WA, Johnson JM. 1999. Influence of female reproductive hormones on local thermal control of skin blood flow. J. Appl. Physiol. 87, 1719–1723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Edwards EA, Duntley SQ. 1949. Cutaneous vascular changes in women in reference to the menstrual cycle and ovariectomy. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 57, 501–509. ( 10.1016/0002-9378(49)90235-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Jones BC, Hahn AC, Fisher CI, Wincenciak J, Kandrik M, Roberts SC, Little AC, DeBruine LM. 2015. Facial coloration tracks changes in women's estradiol. Psychoneuroendocrino 56, 29–34. ( 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.02.021) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Beall AT, Tracy JL. 2013. Women more likely to wear red or pink at peak fertility. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1837–1841. ( 10.1177/0956797613476045) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Goetz CD, Easton JA, Buss DM. 2014. Women's perceptions of sexual exploitability cues and their link to sexual attractiveness. Arch. Sex. Behav. 43, 999–1008. ( 10.1007/s10508-013-0188-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Tracy JL, Beall AT. 2014. The impact of weather on women's tendency to wear red or pink when at high risk for conception. PLoS ONE 9, e88852 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0088852) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Pryke SR, Andersson S, Lawes MJ, Piper SE. 2002. Carotenoid status signaling in captive and wild red-collared widowbirds: independent effects of badge size and color. Behav. Ecol. 13, 622–631. ( 10.1093/beheco/13.5.622) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Bakker TCM, Milinksi M. 1991. Sequential female choice and the previous male effect in sticklebacks. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 29, 205–210. ( 10.1007/BF00166402) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Setchell JM, Wickings EJ. 2005. Dominance, status signals, and coloration in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Ethology 111, 25–30. ( 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.01054.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Bergman TJ, Ho L, Beehner JC. 2009. Chest color and social status in male geladas (Theropithecus gelada). Int. J. Primatol. 30, 791–806. ( 10.1007/s10764-009-9374-x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Ax AF. 1953. The physiological differentiation between fear and anger in humans. Psychosom. Med. 15, 433–442. ( 10.1097/00006842-195309000-00007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Drummond PD. 1999. Facial flushing during provocation in women. Psychophysiology 36, 325–332. ( 10.1017/S0048577299980344) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Drummond PD, Quah SH. 2001. The effect of expressing anger on cardiovascular reactivity and facial blood flow in Chinese and Caucasians. Psychophysiology 38, 190–196. ( 10.1111/1469-8986.3820190) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Stephen ID, Perrett DI. 2015. Color and face perception. In Handbook of color psychology (eds Elliot AJ, Fairchild MD, Franklin A), pp. 568–584. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 113.Edwards EA, Duntley SQ. 1939. The pigments and color of living human skin. Am. J. Anat. 65, 1–33. ( 10.1002/aja.1000650102) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Pryke SR. 2009. Is red an innate or learned signal of aggression and intimidation? Anim. Behav. 78, 393–398. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.05.013) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Khan SA, Levine WJ, Dobson SD, Kralik JD. 2011. Red signals dominance in male rhesus macaques. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1001–1003. ( 10.1177/0956797611415543) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Maynard Smith J, Harper DGC. 1988. The evolution of aggression: can selection generate variability? Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 319, 557–570. ( 10.1098/rstb.1988.0065) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Lack DL. 1965. The life of the Robin. London, UK: Collins. [Google Scholar]
- 118.Cuthill IC, Hunt S, Cleary C, Clark C. 1997. Colour bands, dominance, and body mass regulation in male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 264, 1093–1099. ( 10.1098/rspb.1997.0151) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Farrelly D, Slater R, Elliott HR, Walden HR, Wetherell MA. 2013. Competitors who choose to be red have higher testosterone levels. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2122–2124. ( 10.1177/0956797613482945) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Hackney AC. 2006. Testosterone and human performance: influence of the color red. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 96, 330–333. ( 10.1007/s00421-005-0059-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Dreiskaemper D, Strauss B, Hagemann N, Büsch D. 2013. Influence of red jersey color on physical parameters in combat sports. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 35, 44–49. ( 10.1123/jsep.35.1.44) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Hill RA, Barton RA. 2005. Red enhances human performance in contests. Nature 435, 293 ( 10.1038/435293a) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Falcó C, Conchado A, Estevan I. 2016. The effect of color on the use of electronic body protectors in taekwondo matches. Percept. Motor Skill. 122, 812–824. ( 10.1177/0031512516649958) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Fortunato L, Clauset A. 2016. Revisiting the effect of red on competition in humans. bioRxiv. ( 10.1101/086710) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Curby DG. 2016. Effect of uniform color on outcome of match at Senior World Wrestling Championships 2015. Int. J. Wrest Sci. 6, 62–64. ( 10.1080/21615667.2016.1210266) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Buss DM. 1988. The evolution of human intrasexual competition: tactics of mate attraction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 616–628. ( 10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.616) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.García-Rubio MA, Picazo-Tadeo AJ, González-Gómez F. 2011. Does a red shirt improve sporting performance? Evidence from Spanish football. Appl. Econ. Lett. 18, 1001–1004. ( 10.1080/13504851.2010.520666) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Attrill MJ, Gresty KA, Hill RA, Barton RA. 2008. Red shirt colour is associated with long-term team success in English football. J. Sport Sci. 26, 557–582. ( 10.1080/02640410701736244) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Greenlees IA, Eynon M, Thelwell RC. 2013. Color of soccer goalkeepers’ uniforms influences the outcome of their penalty kicks. Percept. Motor Skill. 117, 1–10. ( 10.2466/30.24.PMS.117x14z6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Greenlees I, Leyland A, Thelwell R, Filby W. 2008. Soccer penalty takers’ uniform colour and pre-penalty kick gaze affect the impressions formed of them by opposing goalkeepers. J. Sport Sci. 26, 569–576. ( 10.1080/02640410701744446) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Ioan S, Sandulache M, Avramescu S, Ilie A, Neacsu A, Zagrean L, Moldovan M. 2007. Red is a distractor for men in competition. Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 285–293. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.03.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Payen V, Elliot AJ, Coombes SA, Chalabaev A, Brisswalter J, Cury F. 2011. Viewing red prior to a strength test inhibits motor output. Neurosci. Lett. 495, 44–48. ( 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.03.032) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Shibasaki M, Masataka N. 2014. The color red distorts time perception for men, but not for women. Sci. Rep. 4, 5899 ( 10.1038/srep05899) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Elliot AJ, Maier MA, Moller AC, Friedman R, Meinhardt J. 2007. Color and psychological functioning: the effect of red on performance attainment. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 136, 154–168. ( 10.1037/0096-3445.136.1.154) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Meier BP, D'Agostino PR, Elliot AJ, Maier MA, Wilkowski BM. 2012. Color in context: psychological context moderates the influence of red on approach- and avoidance-motivated behavior. PLoS ONE 7, e40333 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0040333) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Elliot AJ, Maier MA, Binser MJ, Friedman R, Pekrun R. 2009. The effect of red on avoidance behavior in achievement contexts. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 35, 365–375. ( 10.1177/0146167208328330) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Feltman R, Elliot AJ. 2011. The influence of red on perceptions of relative dominance and threat in a competitive context. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 33, 308–314. ( 10.1123/jsep.33.2.308) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Hagemann N, Strauss B, Leißing J. 2008. When the referee sees red. Psychol. Sci. 19, 769–771. ( 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02155.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Krenn B. 2014. The impact of uniform color on judging tackles in association football. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 15, 222–225. ( 10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.11.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Frank MG, Gilovich T. 1988. The dark side of self- and social perception: black uniforms and aggression in professional sports. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 74–85. ( 10.1037/0022-3514.54.1.74) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Buechner VL, Maier MA, Lichtenfeld S, Schwarz S. 2014. Red - take a closer look. PLoS ONE 9, e108111 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0108111) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Wen F, Zuo B, Wu Y, Sun S, Liu K. 2014. Red is romantic, but only for feminine females: sexual dimorphism moderates red effect on sexual attraction. Evol. Psychol. 12, 719–735. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Higham JP, Hughes KD, Brent LJN, Dubuc C, Engelhardt A, Heistermann M, Maestriperi D, Santos LR, Stevens M. 2011. Familiarity affects the assessment of female facial signals of fertility by free-ranging male rhesus macaques. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 3452–3458. ( 10.1098/rspb.2011.0052) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Lõhmus M, Sundström LF, Björklund M. 2009. Dress for success: human facial expressions are important signals of emotions. Ann. Zool. Fenn. 46, 75–80. ( 10.5735/086.046.0108) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Remondino F, Fraser C.2006. Digital camera calibration methods: considerations and comparisons. In ISPRS Commission V Symposium ‘Image Engineering and Vision Metrology’, Dresden, Germany, 25–27 September. Dresden, Germany: Institute of Photogrammetry and Remote Sensing of TU Dresden.
- 146.Bryan R, Perona P, Adolphs R. 2012. Perspective distortion from interpersonal distance is an implicit visual cue for social judgments of faces. PLoS ONE 7, e45301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Verhoff MA, Witzel C, Kreutz K, Ramsthaler F. 2008. The ideal subject distance for passport pictures. Forensic Sci. Int. 178, 153–156. ( 10.1016/j.forsciint.2008.03.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Banks MS, Cooper EA, Piazza EA. 2014. Camera focal length and the perception of pictures. Ecol. Psychol. 26, 30–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Fink B, Bunse L, Matts PJ, D'Emiliano D. 2012. Visible skin colouration predicts perception of male facial age, health and attractiveness. Int. J. Cosmet. Sci. 34, 307–310. ( 10.1111/j.1468-2494.2012.00724.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Lindner A, Winkler S. 2014 What impacts skin color in digital photos? In IS&T/SPIE Electronic Imaging (p. 901505). Bellingham, Washington, USA: International Society for Optics and Photonics. [Google Scholar]
- 151.Hahn AC, Whitehead RD, Albrecht M, Lefevre CE, Perrett DI. 2012. Hot or not? Thermal reactions to social contact. Biol. Lett. 8, 864–867. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2012.0338) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Jablonski N, Chaplin G. 2000. The evolution of human skin coloration. J. Hum. Evol. 39, 57–106. ( 10.1006/jhev.2000.0403) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Changizi MA, Zhang Q, Shimojo S. 2006. Bare skin, blood and the evolution of primate colour vision. Biol. Lett. 2, 217–221. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2006.0440) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Little AC, Apicella CL, Marlowe FW. 2007. Preferences for symmetry in human faces in two cultures: data from the UK and the Hadza, an isolated group of hunter-gatherers. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 3113–3117. ( 10.1098/rspb.2007.0895) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Marsh EE, Shaw ND, Kingman KM, Tiamfook-Morgan TO, Yiamalas MA, Sluss PM, Hall JE. 2011. Estrogen levels are higher across the menstrual cycle in African-American women compared with Caucasian women. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 96, 3199–3206. ( 10.1210/jc.2011-1314) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Roney JR, Simmons LW, Gray PB. 2011. Changes in estradiol predict within-women shifts in attraction to facial cues of men's testosterone. Psychoneuroendocrino 36, 742–749. ( 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.10.010) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Roney JR, Simmons ZL. 2008. Women's estradiol predicts preference for facial cues of men's testosterone. Horm. Behav. 53, 14–19. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.09.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Pisanski K, Hahn AC, Fisher CI, DeBruine LM, Feinberg DR, Jones BC. 2014. Changes in salivary estradiol predict changes in women's preferences for vocal masculinity. Horm. Behav. 66, 493–497. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.07.006) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Coetzee V, Greeff JM, Stephen ID, Perrett DI. 2014. Cross-cultural agreement in facial attractiveness preferences: the role of ethnicity and gender. PLoS ONE 9, e99629 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0099629) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.