Abstract
Published research demonstrates an association between exposure to media sexual content and a variety of sex-related outcomes for adolescents. What is not known is the mechanism through which sexual content produces this “media effect” on adolescent beliefs, attitudes, and behavior. Using the Integrative Model of Behavioral Prediction, this paper uses data from a longitudinal study of adolescents ages 16–18 (n=460) to determine how exposure to sexual media content influences sexual behavior. Path analysis and structural equation modeling demonstrated that intention to engage in sexual intercourse is determined by a combination of attitudes, normative pressure, and self efficacy but that exposure to sexual media content only affects normative pressure beliefs. By applying the Integrative Model, we are able to identify which beliefs are influenced by exposure to media sex and improve the ability of health educators, researchers, and others to design effective messages for health communication campaigns and messages pertaining to adolescents’ engaging in sexual intercourse.
Keywords: Integrative Model, media effects, adolescent sexual behavior
Introduction
Adolescents (ages 8 to 18 years old) spend on average over 7.5 hours per day with media (Rideout et al., 2010) and throughout this time they are likely to be exposed to sexual content. For example, youths ages 8 to 18 spend an average of 4.5 hours per day watching television content (Rideout, et al, 2010). Although the amount of sex content portrayed in television shows is increasing (Kunkel et al., 2005; Kunkel et al., 2003; Kunkel et al., 2001; Kunkel et al., 1996), messages pertaining to safer sex and sexual risk and responsibilities (Kunkel et al., 2005) are infrequent. Research suggests that exposure to sexual content on television and other media is associated with cognitive factors such as expectations about sex, perceptions about peer sexual behavior, and permissive attitudes about sex (AMERG, 2008; Ashby et al., 2006; Bleakley et al., 2009; Bleakley et al., 2008a; Brown & Newcomer, 1991; Brown et al., 2006; Collins, 2005; Collins et al., 2009; Escobar-Chaves et al., 2005; L’Engle et al., 2006; Pardun et al., 2005; Somers & Tynan, 2006; Ward, 2002; Ward & Friedman, 2006) as well as sexual behaviors as sexual initiation (Collins, 2004) and the timing of sexual intercourse (Aubrey et al., 2003). A study by Chandra et al. (2008) also reported a longitudinal association between watching sex on television as a teen (ages 12–17 years) and pregnancy before the age of 20.
While television accounts for the majority of an adolescent’s time with media, exposure to sexual content in movies and music is also linked to teen sexual activity (Pardun, L’Engle & Brown, 2005; Martino et al., 2006). Among White youth aged 12 to 14, the cumulative effects of sexual content exposure from music, movies, television, and magazines on sexual behavior increased the likelihood of having engaged in sexual intercourse two years later (Brown et al., 2006) but for African-American adolescents there was no significant effect of exposure to sexual content on their precoital or coital sexual behaviors. A growth curve analysis of the trajectories of sexual behavior and exposure to sexual content by Hennessy et al. (2009a) also demonstrated that the correlations between changes in exposure to sexual media content in multiple media and changes in sexual behavior were much higher for White adolescents than for African-American adolescents. For the sample overall, the correlations between sexual activity and exposure to sexual content were modest and declined with the age of the respondent. Thus while evidence exists to support a relationship between exposure to sex in the media (including television, music, and movies), the results seem to vary across racial and ethnic groups.
While longitudinal studies like those discussed above (e.g., Bleakley et al., 2008a; Brown et al., 2006; Collins, 2005; Hennessy et al., 2009a) demonstrate a causal relationship between exposure to sexual content and behavior, the psychosocial mechanisms by which exposure to media sexual content influences sexual behavior is rarely described. One exception is the study by Martino et al. (2005) which uses three socio-cognitive variables – safe-sex efficacy, peer descriptive norms, and negative outcome expectancies - to explain the relationship between television sexual content and sexual initiation in a sample of virgins ages 12–16. The authors found that the relationship between sex and TV and sexual initiation was completely mediated by these three social-cognitive variables. This paper elaborates on the socio-cognitive approach by using the Integrative Model of Behavioral Prediction as a theoretical basis for explaining how exposure to sexual content affects specific social-cognitive beliefs that determine an adolescent’s intentions to engage, as well as their actual engagement, in sexual intercourse.
Understanding the mechanisms through which sexual content exposure influences sexual behavior has implications for intervention design as well as for the identification of those youth at risk of prematurely engaging in sexual intercourse. Specifically, the information to be learned by applying the Integrative Model improves the ability of health educators, researchers, and others to design effective messages for health communication campaigns and/or messages. As stated by Fishbein & Izer (2003, p. 172):
“…proper use of theory should help the researcher identify whether, in any given population, a particular behavior is determined by attitudinal, normative, or efficacy considerations, or some combination thereof. It should further lead to the identification of a number of behavioral, normative, or control beliefs that clearly discriminate between people who do or do not engage in the behavior in question, that, beliefs that are highly correlated with the intention or behavior. The question is which of these beliefs a communication should address.”
In the analyses set forth here, we focus on which beliefs important to the prediction of one’s intention to have sexual intercourse are influenced by exposure to sexual media content.
The Integrative Model of Behavioral Prediction
The “Integrative Model of Behavioral Prediction” (Ajzen & Albarracín, 2007; Fishbein, 2000, 2008, 2010) is a psychosocial model of behavior that is a synthesis of the Theory of Reasoned Action (TRA), Social-Cognitive Theory, the Health Belief Model, and the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB). (See Fishbein (2008), page 838 for a graphical depiction of the IM.) The focus of the model is one’s intention to perform a specific behavior (the “target behavior”) as both a dependent variable and as a predictor of behavior. That is, the model is concerned with the factors influencing intention formation as well as with the relationship between intentions and subsequent performance of the target behavior. The IM assumes that behavior is primarily determined by intentions, although one may not always be able to act on one’s intentions because environmental factors and/or a lack of skills and abilities may make performance difficult if not impossible. Often skills and abilities as well as other contextual environmental factors are not measured and then a measure of perceived control or self-efficacy is used as a proxy for the factors influencing actual control. Therefore, behavior is seen as a function of both intentions and self-efficacy. Intention to perform a specific behavior is a function of one’s favorableness or unfavorableness towards personally performing the behavior (i.e., attitudes), perceptions about what others think and do with regards to performing the behavior (i.e., perceived normative pressure), and beliefs about one’s ability to perform the behavior assuming that one wanted to do so (i.e., self-efficacy or perceived control). Each of these constructs (labeled as “direct measures” in Figure 1) is determined by a corresponding set of salient underlying beliefs.
For example, attitudes are determined by one’s beliefs that performing the behavior will lead to certain positive or negative consequences (i.e., outcome expectancies). Some behavioral beliefs about having sex might be that “having sex will increase intimacy with a partner” or “having sex will result in getting a sexually transmitted disease”. The more one believes that the behavior will lead to positive and not negative outcomes, the more favorable should be the attitude toward the behavior. Perceived normative pressure is determined by beliefs that specific referents think the individual should or should not perform the target behavior (these are “injunctive” normative beliefs) and by beliefs about whether similar others such as friends or sex partners are or are not performing the target behavior (these are “descriptive” normative beliefs, see Cialdini, Reno & Kallgren, 1990). The more one believes that their important others think they should have sex and the more they believe that others like them are having sex, the stronger will be their perceived social pressure to have sex. A third type of belief underlies perceived control or self-efficacy. These beliefs refer to one’s ability to perform the target behavior (e.g., having vaginal sex) under a variety of challenging circumstances, such as if the respondent had no regular sex partner.
Note that in the IM background variables such as personality traits (e.g., sensation seeking), demographic characteristics, media exposure, and past behavior (grouped under the “External Variables” column in Figure 1) influence behavior only indirectly. That is, the influence of background or precursor variables on behavior is assumed to be completely mediated by the more proximal variables in the IM. But whether a given background variable of interest will or will not have an effect on those proximal variables (i.e., the underlying beliefs) is an empirical question and thus, in Figure 1, these paths are dashed.
How Exposure to Sexual Media Content Might Operate to Affect Adolescent Sexual Behavior
Social-cognitive theories like the IM assume that children and adolescents, who may not all have first hand sexual experience, can learn and make inferences about sex and relationships from media exposure. If sexual portrayals in media shape adolescent beliefs, attitudes, perceived norms, and feelings of self-efficacy with respect to sexual behavior, then these sexual portrayals will also influence the adolescents’ intentions to engage in, as well as their actual performance of, various sexual behaviors. As Martino et al. (2005) summarize: “Social-cognitive theory…contends that people observe important role models, make inferences and attributions, and acquire scripts, schemas, and normative beliefs that then guide their subsequent behavior. This theoretical perspective would predict that adolescents learn sexual behaviors and their likely consequences by watching TV” (p. 914). Consistent with this view is the assumption that media provide adolescents with “information” about sex and sexuality in the same way that a friend does. Indeed, some research supports the idea that the media acts as a “super peer” and is a source of information about sex when or if adolescents are unable to learn from their peer group (Brown et al., 2005). Another study shows that using friends and/or media as sources of information about sex is associated with increased self efficacy for having sexual intercourse (Bleakley et al., 2009).
To translate these types of empirical findings into the language of the IM as applied to the sexual media content/adolescent sexual behavior association, we would say: to affect adolescent sexual behavior, the “information” conveyed in sexual media content must change the underlying beliefs about performing the target behavior. Once the underlying beliefs are determined or altered, these beliefs affect the corresponding direct measures and the direct measures alter intentions to perform the behavior. The increased intentions to perform the behavior should lead to performing the behavior although the causal model reflected in the IM is complex and media exposure may affect all three IM pathways to behavior (attitudes, norms, and self-efficacy) or only a subset of them.
In this paper we examine which arms of the IM are affected a specific “background/external variable”: exposure to sexual media content. Based on prior research (Martino et al., 2005; Bleakley et al., 2009), and what we know about the nature of sexual content in some media (Kunkel et al, 2005), we hypothesize that exposure to sexual content in the media will affect all three determinants of intentions: attitudes, perceived normative pressure, and self efficacy. Exposure to sexual content will result in more positive attitudes about sexual intercourse, will increase the extent to which adolescents believe their peers are having sexual intercourse (i.e. the descriptive norms) and will also increase their beliefs that they can overcome barriers to having sex (i.e., will increase their self-efficacy).
Methods and Measures
The Annenberg Sex and Media Study (ASAMS)
ASAMS was a five-year investigation of the relationship between exposure to sex in the media and self-reported sexual behavior in adolescents. It was designed to investigate the extent to which exposure to sexual content in the media shapes adolescents’ sexual development. In ASAMS, the analytic variables used are those of the IM, so ASAMS contains direct measures of intentions, attitudes, perceived normative pressure and self-efficacy with respect to “my having vaginal sex in the next 12 months” as well as of the beliefs underlying these constructs. In addition, it assesses exposure to sexual content in five different media: movies, TV, music, magazines and video games. The self-report sexual data and the measure of exposure to sexual media content are used here in combination with the IM variables to model the relationship between underlying beliefs, the direct measures, intentions to have vaginal sex, and behavior.
ASAMS Study Design
This paper uses data from the ASAMS longitudinal survey of adolescents from the greater Philadelphia area. The first wave of data collection occurred in the spring and summer of 2005; wave 2 occurred 1 year later in the spring and summer of 2006, and wave 3 in the summer of 2007. Only data from waves 2 and 3 are used for this analysis because we have more variability in the behavioral outcome as our adolescent respondents mature. Adolescents were recruited through print and radio advertisements, direct mail, and word of mouth to complete the survey. Eligibility criteria for the initial survey included age at the time of the survey (14, 15, or 16) and race/ethnicity (White, African-American, or Hispanic). A quota samplings design was used to achieve equal stratas of adolescents by age, gender, and race. In practice, Hispanic adolescents were difficult to locate and recruit, and are underrepresented in the sample. Written parental consent and teen assent were collected for all participants and study protocols were approved by the University of Pennsylvania IRB.
The web-based survey was accessible from any computer with internet access. Participants were given the option of taking the survey at the University or an off-site location (e.g. home, school, or community library). For example, in wave 1, the majority of the participants (84.9%) took the survey at home or another off-site location; 15.1% took the survey at the University. Enrolled adolescents were given a password to access the survey, as well as an ID number and personal password to ensure confidentiality and privacy protection. On average adolescents took one hour to complete the survey and were given compensation of $25 for completing each wave of the survey. Respondents’ who completed all three waves received an additional $25.
After submitting participant assent/parental consent forms, 547 adolescents completed the survey at wave 1. Ninety two percent of the sample was retained in wave 2 (n=501), and of those respondents, 95% (n=474) completed the survey in wave 3. Because of their relatively small sample sizes, adolescents of “other” ethnicity are excluded from the present analysis (n=14), resulting in a final sample of 460 youth who participated in both waves 2 and 3. The sample was 63.3% female, 44.6% African American, 44.6% White, and 11.5% Hispanic. The mean participant age at wave 2 was 16 years (SD=0.81).
Measures
IM Measures
The survey collected measures of intention, attitudes, perceived normative pressure, and self efficacy with respect to engaging in sexual intercourse in the next 12 months. In addition, it assessed the behavioral, normative, and self efficacy beliefs that are assumed to underlie these constructs (see Figure 1). The question stems and wording for the direct measures of intention, attitude, perceived social pressure and self-efficacy are listed below and the measures for behavioral, normative, and self-efficacy beliefs are presented in Tables 1 and 2. With the exception of descriptive normative beliefs (which were measured on 5 point scales), all other IM items were measured on 7-point scales. The list of belief items was identified through elicitation research using open-ended questions to identify the outcomes, referents, and barriers and facilitators that were most salient in this population with respect to personally engaging in vaginal sexual behavior in the next 12 months. Alphas are not reported for behavioral or normative belief indices since these items are treated as causal indicators and causal indicators need not be unidimensional because individual beliefs may or may not be internally consistent (Jarvis, MacKenzie & Podsakoff, 2003; Streiner, 2003).
Table 1.
Outcome Expectancies | Independent Variable: Sex Content Exposure |
Independent Variable: Outcome Expectancies |
Independent Variable: Belief Index |
---|---|---|---|
Dependent Variable: Outcome Expectancies |
Dependent Variable: Belief Index |
Dependent Variable: Direct Measure Attitude Scale |
|
If I have sexual intercourse in the next 12 months, it would… | (A) Parameters | (B) Parameters | (C) Parameter |
| |||
Make me feel that someone had taken advantage of me | −.07 | .23 | .51 |
Make me feel good about myself | −.06 | .45 | R2 = .27 |
Give me HIV/AIDS | −.002 | .23 | |
Hurt my relationship with my partner | .11 | .24 | |
Increase quality of relationship | .01 | .23 | |
Increase feelings of intimacy between me and my partner | −.09 | −.01 | |
Give me pleasure | .02 | .72 | |
Make my parents mad | .06 | .07 | |
Please my partner | .03 | .15 | |
Get me or partner pregnant | −.01 | .07 | |
Gain the respect of my friends | −.03 | .04 | |
Make my friends think badly of me | .15 | .18 |
Goodness of fit indices: χ2 = 1607, df = 67, p < .05, CFI = .16, RMSEA = .22, N = 460. Bold, italic coefficients are discernable from zero at .05 or less.
Table 2.
Descriptive and Injunctive Normative Beliefs | Independent Variable: Sex Content Exposure |
Independent
Variable: Underlying Normative Beliefs |
Independent
Variable: Normative Belief Index |
---|---|---|---|
Dependent
Variable: Underlying Normative Beliefs |
Dependent Variable: Belief Index |
Dependent Variable: Direct Measure Pressure Scale |
|
(A) Parameters | (B) Parameters | (C) Parameter | |
| |||
Females my age have had sexual intercourse | .11 | .04 | .48 |
Female friends have had sexual intercourse | .15 | .40 | R2 = .23 |
Males my age have had sexual intercourse | .18 | .21 | |
Male friends have had sexual intercourse | .26 | .26 | |
Parents think I should have sexual intercourse | −.05 | .03 | |
Best friend thinks I should have sexual intercourse | −.01 | .77 | |
Sex partner thinks I should have sexual intercourse | .06 | .29 | |
Brother thinks I should have sexual intercourse | −.01 | .03 | |
Sister thinks I should have sexual intercourse | −.03 | −.04 | |
Grandmother thinks I should have sexual intercourse | −.06 | .16 |
Barriers to Having Sexual Intercourse | Independent Variable: Sex Content Exposure |
Independent
Variable: Barriers |
Independent
Variable: Barriers Index |
---|---|---|---|
| |||
Dependent
Variable: Barriers |
Dependent Variable: Barriers Index |
Dependent Variable: Direct Measure Self-efficacy Item |
|
(A) Parameters | (B) Parameters | (C) Parameter | |
| |||
No regular sex partner | −.03 | .67 | .40 |
No place to be alone | −.01 | .06 | R2 = .16 |
No condoms or birth control available | .04 | −.36 | |
Parents would be mad | .04 | .52 | |
Drinking or using drugs | −.12 | −.09 | |
You were lonely or depressed | −.13 | .31 | |
Having sexual intercourse goes against your religious beliefs | .−.08 | .18 |
Goodness of fit indices: χ2 = 1069, df = 22, p < .05, CFI = .10, RMSEA = .32, N= 460. Bold, italic coefficients are discernable from zero at .05 or less.
Goodness of fit indices: χ2 = 2260, df = 46, p < .05, CFI = .11, RMSEA = .32, N = 460. Bold, italic coefficients are discernable from zero at .05 or less.
Attitudes
My having sexual intercourse in the next 12 months would be… bad/good, foolish/wise, unpleasant/pleasant, not enjoyable/enjoyable, harmful/beneficial. (Scale −3 to 3; Mean=0.236 SD=1.60 Alpha=0.90)
Normative pressure
Most people who are important to me think I should not/should have sexual intercourse in the next 12 months; Most people like me will not/will have sexual intercourse in the next 12 months; Most people like me have not had/have had sexual intercourse; Most people like me have not had/have had sexual intercourse in the past 12 months. (Scale −3 to 3; Mean=−0.184 SD=1.60 Alpha=0.70)
Self Efficacy
If I really wanted to I am certain that I could not/could have sexual intercourse in the next 12 months (Answer even if you don’t have a boyfriend or girlfriend). (Scale −3 to 3; Mean=0.910 SD=2.24)
Behavioral Intentions
I am willing to have sexual intercourse in the next 12 months; I will have sexual intercourse in the next 12 months; I intend to have sexual intercourse in the next 12 months. (Scale −3 to 3; Mean=0.010 SD=2.26 Alpha=0.96)
Sexual Behavior Measures
Adolescent respondents were asked the following: “Have you ever had sexual intercourse (i.e., a penis in the vagina) with a partner of the opposite sex?” The response categories were “I have never done this;” “Yes, within the past 12 months;” “Yes, more than 12 months ago;” “Yes, within the past 12 months and more than 12 months ago;” and “Don’t want to answer.” Since our IM measures in Year 2 specifically ask about having sex in the next 12 months, our target behavior is therefore anyone who reported in Year 3 that they had sex in the past 12 months. For the purposes of analysis we created a dichotomous sexual behavior variable. Those adolescents who responded either that they had sex “within the past 12 months” or “within the past 12 months and more than 12 months ago” were assigned a value of 1. Those who had sex “more than 12 months ago,” or who never had sex were given a zero; “don’t want to answer” (n=17) were recoded into missing.
Exposure to Sexual Content Measures
A measure of exposure to sexual content in multiple media was calculated based on 2 types of variables: respondents’ self-reported exposure to selected media titles in 5 media (i.e., television, movies, music, magazines, and videogames) and the respondents’ ratings of the amount of sexual content in each of the media titles. Using an ordinal measure of exposure on a 4-point scale (never, rarely, sometimes, often) adolescents indicated how frequently within the last 12 months they watched each show, listened to each artist, read each magazine, and played each videogame. They also indicated, using a dichotomous measure, whether they had or had not seen each movie title. The lists of media titles were constructed to reflect popular titles for teenagers and/or the general public at the time of the survey. Popular titles were provided by pilot surveys conducted in the year prior to the administration of the baseline survey, website rankings (including: www.top5s.com/tvweek; www.boxofficemojo.com; www.imdb.com/boxoffice/rentals; www.billboard.com; www.gamerankings.com), and rankings from an audience research company (TRU data). The titles were designed to provide a sense of the depth and breadth of media use by teens, although we recognized that they could not possibly capture all that teens viewed, played, listen to, or read. To that end, we included opportunities for respondents to write in their “favorite” title for each medium. The number of titles coded for each media in year 2 is as follows: 52 television shows, 40 movies, 31 music artists, 26 videogames, and 14 magazines.
A team of undergraduates was trained over the course of an academic year to recognize and code the variables. Inter-rater agreement was calculated and only variables achieving a minimum of 75% agreement were used in analysis. For the present study, only the rating of “overall sex content” was defined as the prevalence of sexual talk and behavior within the media unit. Coders were asked to indicate if there was none/a little (1), some (2), or a lot (3). Inter-rater agreement on this measure was 83.3%; the Kappa coefficient was .75.
To calculate the sexual content exposure measure, the cross product of a respondent’s reported exposure measure and the content analysis-based sexual content rating was summed within each type of media, resulting in sex content exposure measures specific to television, movies, music, magazines, and videogames. Each media specific measure was then standardized since there were different numbers of titles in each media. Standardizing the sums of the media-specific cross products prevents one media (e.g. television) from having more weight than another media (e.g. magazines) for which there were fewer titles. The total (multiple media) sexual content exposure measure used in this analysis was thus created by summing the 5 media-specific, standardized measures.
Statistical Analysis
The analysis was conducted in two steps. First we performed path analysis using MPlus 5.1 software because Mplus allows for models with both categorical and continuous dependent variables. When Mplus encounters ordinal or dichotomous endogenous variables it implements a weighted mean and variance estimator that has been shown to have excellent statistical qualities even with small samples (Flora & Curran, 2004). The estimator assumes a probit (i.e., Z score) metric on the unobserved latent variables. Because this estimator does not use maximum likelihood, some of the conventional goodness of fit tests are not available (e.g., confidence intervals for the RMSEA).
To assess model fit in path analytic and/or models with latent variables, the χ2 test is used to compare the predicted covariance matrix of the observed variables for the model with the actual covariance matrix. However, χ2 is usually augmented by other measures less sensitive to sample size, so this measure is augmented by the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) and the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI). The RMSEA is a measure of average fit based on the square root of the model misfit divided by the model’s degrees of freedom, where zero represents perfect fit and positive departures reflect poorer fit. Values less than or equal to .05 or .06 are usually considered acceptable (Maruyama, 1998; Kaplan, 2000) whereas values greater than .08 or .10 indicate poor fit (Kaplan, 2000; Kline, 2005). The TLI is a comparison of two fit functions: one from the covariance matrix estimated from the fitted model and one from a model that assumes no association between the observed items where positive departures from zero reflect the relative advantage of the proposed model compared with the model of no association. The TLI should be greater than .90 to reflect an adequately fitting model and some researchers argue for a .95 cutoff (Hu & Bentler, 1995; Kaplan, 2000).
In this paper, the path analysis models the effects of exposure to sexual content on sexual behavior as being mediated by intentions and its primary determinants, the direct measures of attitudes toward personally performing the behavior, perceived normative pressure, and self efficacy. Note that nowhere in the IM is there an explicit theory about the causal order of the direct measures of attitude, normative pressure, and self-efficacy on each other. Thus, an appropriate SEM approach is to estimate the correlations between the error terms of the direct measures (Preacher & Hayes, 2008, pps. 882–883). This penalizes the SEM model in terms of R2 for the mediating IM direct measures because correlated error terms do not contribute to R2. However, correlated errors do not reduce model fit because recursive causal paths between variables and correlated errors between the same variables are “equivalent models” (MacCallum et al., 1993).
The second step of the analysis demonstrates the effect of exposure on the specific underlying belief items for each of the three IM determinants. The generic model for this step in the analysis is presented in Figure 1. For each arm of the IM (e.g., attitudes, perceived normative pressure, self-efficacy), we first predict the specific underlying belief items from our measure of exposure to sexual media content. These correlations (parameter group A) reflect the sensitivity of the beliefs to exposure to sexual media content. The beliefs, in turn, are viewed as causal indicators (Bollen & Lennox, 1991) defining a composite belief index. These results are parameter group B. The belief index predicts the direct measure scale and this correlation is parameter C. We expect substantial correlations for the C parameter because in the IM, the belief index is the defining proximal cause of the direct measures. However, we are uncertain about the A parameters. If exposure to sexual media content does not affect all underlying beliefs, then the generic model applied to the three theoretical arms of the IM will show low values for some A correlations and higher values for other A correlations.
Results
At wave 2, a majority (51.6%) of our adolescents indicted that they intended to have sexual intercourse in the next 12 months and at wave 3 approximately 49% of the sample reported having had vaginal intercourse in the past 12 months. At wave 2, the respondents, on average, had slightly positive attitudes toward having sex in the next 12 months, felt some social pressure to not have sex, and they believed fairly strongly that they could have sex if they wanted to (i.e., they had self-efficacy with respect to having sex).
Results of Path Analysis for IM Model
The path analysis results are shown in Figure 2 and the coefficients are fully standardized. The model fit is excellent: χ2=2.409, df=3, p=0.49; CFI=1.00; TLI=1.007; RMSEA=0.00. Exposure to sexual content in the media is significantly associated with perceived normative pressure about having sexual intercourse but not with attitudes toward having sex or with self efficacy. Attitudes, perceived normative pressure, and self-efficacy account for fifty-four percent of the variance in intentions. Attitudes towards having sexual intercourse carries the most weight (β=0.41), followed by normative pressure (β=0.35), and then self-efficacy (β=0.15). Thus intentions to have sex in the next 12 months are primarily a function of attitudes and perceived normative pressure with self-efficacy playing a relatively small (but significant) role as a determinant of these intentions. Finally it can also be seen that these intentions to have sex in the next 12 months predicted sexual intercourse one year later, explaining 29% of the variance in behavior.
Results for the Analysis of the Underlying Beliefs by IM Arm
Tables 1 and 2 show the results of the generic model for attitudes (Table 1) and norms and self-efficacy (Table 2). The entries are standardized regression coefficients, identical to correlations in the bivariate case (i.e., parameter groups A and C). For attitudes (Table 1), two of the underlying beliefs are predicted by exposure to sexual media content (“Hurt my relationship with my partner” and “Increase feelings of intimacy between mUsinge and my partner”) while six of the beliefs are the major determinants of the behavioral belief index. Note however that exposure influences only one of the six determinants of the underlying belief index (“Hurt my relationship with my partner”). The association between the belief index and the latent measure of direct attitudes is .51. For the normative beliefs (Table 2), exposure to sexual media content predicted each of the descriptive normative belief items and none of the injunctive normative beliefs. Three of the beliefs (one descriptive and two injunctive) are the major determinants of the normative belief index. In this case, one of the three major determinants are influenced by exposure. The association between the belief index and the latent measure of perceived normative pressure is .48. In the self-efficacy model (Table 2), two barriers were predicted by sexual content in the media (“Drinking or using drugs” and “You were lonely or depressed”) and only the latter was one of three statistically significant determinants of the self-efficacy belief index. The association between the belief index and the direct measure of self-efficacy is .40.
In contrast to the path analysis of the direct measures, all these underlying belief models fit poorly. This is due to the preponderance of small or zero associations/regression coefficients in the results, especially for parameter group A. However, these non-significant findings are of substantive importance because the purpose of the analysis is to identify which underlying beliefs are related to exposure to sexual media content and which are not. Thus we make no attempt to “improve the fit” by deleting non-significant parameters (Hennessy et al., 2009b). A more subtle issue refers to the choice of appropriate model-wide goodness of fit measures for IM analysis. There are multiple options here because the available measures incorporate different operational definitions of “a good fit.” For example, some comparative fit measures (e.g., the TLI) are designed to penalize the goodness of fit estimate for non-significant parameters, a feature that is justified through the quest for statistical parsimony (similar to the rationale for the difference between the adjusted and non-adjusted R2 in the case of a single regression equation).
Our preference is not to use such parsimony adjusted measures with the IM because the purpose of the analysis does not include theoretical “model trimming” based on empirical results. That is, because of the a priori nature of the IM, non-significant parameters are important substantive findings, not statistically irrelevant coefficients that should be constrained to zero. This has an important practical implication because it suggests that the CFI, for example, is a superior measure of goodness of fit than the TLI because the purpose of the SEM in a Integrative Model context is to identify if for a particular behavior and/or for a particular population group intention to perform the target behavior is a function of some or all of the IM’s three main determinants (e.g., Smith-McLallen and Fishbein, 2009).
Discussion
These results highlight the utility of using an explicit behavioral theory to explain a correlation between a background causal variable (exposure to sexual media content) and a behavioral outcome (adolescent sexual behavior). The results of the path analysis demonstrate that the theoretical IM at the global level fits the data very well and suggest that adolescent sexual behavior and intention to have sexual intercourse are primarily determined by positive attitudes toward having sex, secondarily determined by perceived normative pressure to have sex, and to a lesser extent determined by self-efficacy with respect to having sex. Exposure to sexual media content, however, has the largest correlation with perceived normative pressure. We find that in this sample of adolescents exposure to media sex content is unrelated to overall attitudes toward having sex and self efficacy to perform this behavior. What this implies is that exposure to sex content in this sample of youth has essentially no statistically significant effect on the variable that is the most important determinant of adolescents’ intentions to engage in sexual behavior. In other words, the “media effects” variable is not highly correlated with the best predictor of intentions.
The secondary analyses presented here corroborate the path analysis results. Despite strong correlations between the behavioral belief index and attitude and between the efficacy belief index and self efficacy, the changes in the belief indices that were associated with media exposure were insufficient to produce a change in neither attitudes nor self efficacy. In contrast, all of the descriptive normative beliefs were positively associated with exposure to sexual media content.
In summary, exposure to sexual media content increases adolescent sexual behavior by increasing their perceptions of social pressure to have sex. More specifically, exposure to sex content leads to increased beliefs that people like them, including their friends and peers their age, are having sex. Because perceived normative pressure is not the primary determinant of one’s intention to engage in sex, these changes produce only relatively small increments in intention and behavior. Given that one’s attitude toward having sex is the best predictor of the intention to have sex, the influence of exposure to sexual media content on adolescent sexual behavior would be significantly stronger if exposure positively increased more of the critical behavioral beliefs underlying this attitude.
Much can be learned by considering why beliefs are or are not affected by sex media content. In this instance is helpful to consider the nature of the sexual content in media to which adolescents are exposed. Sexual messages and images depicted in the media vary widely and “sex in the media” is more often implied or discussed than explicitly shown (Martino et al., 2005). For example, Kunkel and his colleagues have been tracking sexual portrayals on television for over a decade using both random sampling methods to select “average weekly” programming and targeted sampling of nationally popular shows based on Nielsen ratings. In their analysis, they differentiate between “talking about sex,” “precursory” sexual behaviors (e.g., flirting, passionate kissing, intimate touching), and sexual intercourse (either implied or depicted). In general, the former are far more common than the latter:
Nearly twice as many shows include talk about sex as contain sexual behavior. Similarly, the number of scenes per program with talk about sex is almost twice as high as the number of scenes with sexual behavior. Precursory behaviors, such as passionate kissing or intimate touching, are the most common form of sexual activity shown on television. Still, about one of every seven programs on television (excluding news, sports, and children’s programming) presents sexual intercourse either by depicting it directly or by portraying characters who are about to begin or have just finished having sex (Kunkel et al., 2003, p. 16).
It therefore follows that both implicit and explicit depictions of sexual intercourse and “precursory behaviors” could lead adolescents to perceive a meta-message that lots of people like the respondent are having sex (Sorsoli, Ward & Tolman, 2009). Such a meta-message might have a strong influence on descriptive normative beliefs and perceived normative pressure.
A limitation of our study is that our exposure measure does not take into account the specific nature of the sexual content to which the respondents were exposed. The content analysis rating used to calculate the sexual content exposure score represented the overall pervasiveness of sexual content in a particular media title; the valence of the content was not captured. To understand more precisely why sexual content exposure is associated a change in specific beliefs about having sex, it may be necessary to examine the nature of the content and media messages. That being said, we do know generally about the nature of sexual content in some media. Kunkel et al. (2005) states that messages of “risk and responsibility” with regards to having sexual intercourse are rare in television shows. In addition, the 2008 report “Managing the Media Monster” asserts that movies contain more sexually explicit content than television programming and that magazines with primarily youth audiences, compared to more general audiences, were more likely to depict couples engaged in sexual activity and to feature adolescent female models that are posed and dressed suggestively (Brown, 2008).
Additionally, we were unable to include exposure to sexual content on the internet into our exposure measures. The study was conducted prior to the emergence of social networking sites as a mainstay of adolescent internet use. During formative research for the project, elicitation of frequently visited sites yielded mostly the web addresses of search engines which could not be content analyzed. Therefore, we did not include internet in the media captured by our exposure measure.
Another limitation of the current study is the absence of an analysis on how these results may differ by race and/or ethnicity. African-American youth spend more time with media compared to their White counterparts (Kunkel et al, 2005), and findings from Brown et al. (2005) demonstrated that the effect of sexual content exposure on sexual initiation was limited to white youth; the effects were not statistically significant among African-American youth. However, a report that is based on the same data as this manuscript (Hennessy et al., 2009a) showed that the sexual behavior and exposure to sexual media content trajectories are different by both gender and race. In our sample of adolescents it is necessary to look simultaneously at race/gender interactions, and a larger sample is needed to produce stable estimates. To examine differences only by race with this data would be misleading, but future studies should investigate if such differences are present. The magnitude and direction of the effect of sexual content exposure on specific beliefs vary and which beliefs are affected by exposure to sexual media content, if any at all, may be different by gender and race groups.
Implications for Practice
As discussed earlier, the utility of using the Integrative Model helps those who work with youth to understand the primary determinants of their intention to engage in sexual intercourse. In this sample, the behavior was primarily driven by attitudes, but was a combination of attitudinal, normative, and efficacy considerations. Perhaps more importantly, specific beliefs associated with increased intentions to have sex, and their relationship to sexual content exposure, were identified. Communication campaigns and health educators can work to counter these beliefs.
In conclusion, our findings offer an explanation of the mechanism by which exposure to sexual content in the media affects adolescents’ sexual activity. Exposure to media sex increases normative pressure to have sex. However, it is equally important that there is an absence of an effect of exposure on attitudes toward having sex and self efficacy with regards to performing the behavior. Because the primary determinant of intention to have sexual intercourse is attitudes, our findings help to clarify why the effect of exposure to sexual content in media on sexual behavior is often weak in research samples of adolescents. Future research using the Integrative Model should be conducted with larger samples of adolescents and should examine racial differences to determine if these findings hold across different racial and ethnic groups.
References
- Ajzen I, Albarracín D. Predicting and changing behavior: a reasoned action approach. In: Ajzen I, Albarracín D, Hornik R, editors. Prediction and Change of Health Behavior. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2007. pp. 1–22. [Google Scholar]
- Annenberg Media Exposure Research Group (AMERG) Linking measures of media exposure to sexual cognitions and behaviors: A review. Communication Methods and Measures. 2008;2:23–42. [Google Scholar]
- Ashby S, Arcari C, Edmonson B. Television viewing and risk of sexual initiation by young adolescents. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. 2006;160:375–380. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.160.4.375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aubrey J, Harrison K, Kramer L, Yellin J. Variety versus timing: Gender differences in college students’ sexual expectations as predicted by exposure to sexually oriented television. Communication Research. 2003;30:432–460. [Google Scholar]
- Bleakley A, Hennessy M, Fishbein M, Jordan A. How source of sexual information relates to adolescents’ belief about sex. American Journal of Health Behavior. 2009;33:37–48. doi: 10.5993/ajhb.33.1.4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bleakley A, Hennessy M, Fishbein M, Jordan A. It works both ways: the relationship between exposure to sexual content in the media and adolescent sexual behavior. Media Psychology. 2008a;11:443–461. doi: 10.1080/15213260802491986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bleakley A, Fishbein M, Hennessy M, Jordan A, Chernin A, Stevens R. Developing respondent based multi-media measures of exposure to sexual content. Communication Methods and Measures. 2008b;2:43–64. doi: 10.1080/19312450802063040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bollen K, Lennox R. Conventional wisdom on measurement: a structural equation perspective. Psychological Bulletin. 1991;110:305–314. [Google Scholar]
- Brown J, editor. Managing the Media Monster: The Influence of Media (from Television to Text Messages) on Teen Sexual Behavior and Attitudes. Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent Teen and Unplanned Pregnancy; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Brown JD, Halpern CT, L’Engle KL. Mass media as a sexual super peer for early maturing girls. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2005;36:420–427. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown JD, L’Engle KL, Pardun CJ, Guo G, Kenneavy K, Jackson C. Sexy media matter: Exposure to sexual content in music, movies, television, and magazines predicts back and white adolescents’ sexual behavior. Pediatrics. 2006;117:1018–1027. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-1406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown J, Newcomer S. Television viewing and adolescents’ sexual behavior. Journal of Homosexuality. 1991;21:77–91. doi: 10.1300/J082v21n01_07. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buhi E, Goodson P. Predictors of adolescent sexual behavior and intention: a theory-guided systematic review. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2007;40:4–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.09.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chandra A, Martino SC, Collins RL, Elliot MN, Berry SH, Kanouse DE, Miu A. Does watching sex on television predict teen pregnancy? Findings from a national longitudinal survey of youth. Pediatrics. 2008;122:1047–1054. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-3066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chia S, Gunther A. How media contribute to misperceptions of social norms about sex. Mass Communication & Society. 2006;9:301–320. [Google Scholar]
- Cialdini R, Reno R, Kallgren C. A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990;58:1015–1026. [Google Scholar]
- Collins R. Sex on television and its impact on American youth: Background and results from the RAND Television and Adolescent Sexuality Study. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2005;14:371–385. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2005.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins RL, Elliot MN, Berry SH, Kanouse DE, Kunkel D, Hunter SB, Miu A. Watching sex on television predicts adolescent initiation of sexual behavior. Pediatrics. 2004;114:e280–e289. doi: 10.1542/peds.2003-1065-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins R, Elliot M, Miu A. Linking media content to media effects: The RAND television and adolescent sexuality study. In: Jordan A, Kunkel D, Manganello J, Fishbein M, editors. Media Messages and Public Health: A Decisions Approach to Content Analysis. New York: Routledge; 2009. pp. 154–172. [Google Scholar]
- Escobar-Chaves SL, Tortolero SR, Markham CM, Low BJ, Eitel P, Thickstun P. Impact of media on sexual attitudes and behaviors. Pediatrics. 2005;116:303–326. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fishbein M. A reasoned action approach to health promotion. Medical Decision Making. 2008;28:834–844. doi: 10.1177/0272989X08326092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fishbein M. The role of theory in HIV prevention. AIDS Care. 2000;12:273–278. doi: 10.1080/09540120050042918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fishbein M, Yzer M. Using theory to design effective health behavior interventions. Communication Theory. 2003;13:164–183. [Google Scholar]
- Fishbein M, Ajzen I. Predicting and Changing Behavior. New York: Taylor Francis; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Flora D, Curran P. An empirical evaluation of alternative methods of estimation for confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data. Psychological Methods. 2004;9:466–491. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.9.4.466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hennessy M, Bleakley A, Fishbein M, Jordan A. Estimating the longitudinal association between adolescent sexual behavior and exposure to sexual media content. Journal of Sex Research. 2009a doi: 10.1080/00224490902898736. Forthcoming. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hennessy M, Bleakley A, Fishbein M, Brown L, DiClemente R, Romer D, Vanable P, Carey M, Salazar L. Differentiating between precursor and control variables when analyzing reasoned action theories. AIDS and Behavior, available online. 2009b doi: 10.1007/s10461-009-9560-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hennessy M, Bleakley A, Fishbein M, Busse P. What is the appropriate response to wardrobe malfunctions? Fining stations for TV sex and violence. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media. 2008;52:387–407. [Google Scholar]
- Hu L, Bentler P. Evaluating model fit. In: Hoyle RH, editor. Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues and applications. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1995. pp. 76–99. [Google Scholar]
- Jarvis C, MacKenzie S, Podsakoff P. A critical review of construct indictors and measurement model misspecification in marketing and consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research. 2003;30:199–218. [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan D. Structural equation modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Kline R. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. New York: Guilford; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Kunkel D, Biely E, Eyal K, Cope-Ferrer K, Donnerstein E, Fandrich R. Sex on TV3: A biennial report to the Kaiser Family Foundation. Menlo Park, CA: A Kaiser Family Foundation; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Kunkel D, Cope K, Colvin C. Sexual messages on family hour television: Content and context. Oakland and Menlo Park, CA: Children Now and the Kaiser Family Foundation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Kunkel D, Cope-Ferrer K, Biely E, Donnerstein E. Sex on TV2: A biennial report to the Kaiser Family Foundation. Menlo Park, CA: A Kaiser Family Foundation; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Kunkel D, Eyal K, Finnerty K, Biely E, Donnerstein E. Sex on TV 2005. Menlo Park, CA: A Kaiser Family Foundation; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- L’Engle KL, Jackson C, Brown JD. Early adolescents’ cognitive susceptibility to initiating sexual intercourse. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health. 2006;38:97–105. doi: 10.1363/psrh.38.097.06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacCallum R, Wegener D, Uchino B, Fabrigar L. The problem of equivalent models in applications of covariance structure analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 1993;114:185–199. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martino S, Collins R, Kanhouse D, Elliot M, Berry S. Social cognitive processes mediating the relationship between exposure to television’s sexual content and adolescent’s sexual behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005;89:914–924. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.914. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martino S, Collins R, Elliott M, Strachman A, Kanouse D, Berry S. Exposure to degrading versus nondegrading music lyrics and sexual behavior among youth. Pediatrics. 2006;118:e430–441. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-0131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maruyama G. Basics of structural equation modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Pardun C, L’Engle K, Brown J. Linking exposures to outcomes: Early adolescents’ consumption of sexual content in six media. Mass Communication & Society. 2005;8:75–91. [Google Scholar]
- Peterson J, Moore K, Furstenberg F. Television viewing and early initiation of sexual intercourse: is there a link? In: Wolf MA, Kielwasser AP, editors. Gay people, sex, and the media. Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press; 1991. pp. 93–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preacher K, Hayes A. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods. 2008;40:879–891. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.3.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts DF, Foehr UG, Rideout V. Generation M: Media in the lives of 8–18 year olds. Menlo Park, CA: Kaiser Family Foundation; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Schumacker R, Lomax R. A beginner’s guide to structural equation modeling. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Somers C, Tynan J. Consumption of sexual dialogue and content on television and adolescent sexual outcomes: multiethnic findings. Adolescence. 2006;41:15–38. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sorsoli L, Ward M, Tolman D. (De)coding television’s representation of sexuality. In: Jordan A, Kunkel D, Manganello J, Fishbein M, editors. Media Messages and Public Health: A Decisions Approach to Content Analysis. New York: Routledge; 2009. pp. 137–153. [Google Scholar]
- Strasburger V. Why do adolescent health researchers ignore the impact of a media? Journal of Adolescent Health. 2009;44:203–205. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strouse J, Buerkel-Rothfuss N. Media exposure and the sexual attitudes and behaviors of college students. Journal of Sex Education and Therapy. 1987;13:43–51. doi: 10.1080/01614576.1987.11074908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward L. Does television exposure affect emerging adults’ attitudes and assumptions about sexual relationships? Correlational and experimental confirmation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2002;31:1–15. [Google Scholar]
- Ward LM. Understanding the role of entertainment media in the sexual socialization of American youth: A review of empirical research. Developmental Review. 2003;23:347–388. [Google Scholar]
- Ward LM. Understanding the role of entertainment media in the sexual socialization of American youth: A review of empirical research. Developmental Review. 2003;23:347–388. [Google Scholar]
- Ward LM, Friedman K. Using TV as a guide: Associations between television viewing and adolescents’ sexual attitudes and behavior. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2006;16:133–156. [Google Scholar]
- Ward LM, Friedman K. Using TV as a guide: Associations between television viewing and adolescents’ sexual attitudes and behavior. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2006;16:133–156. [Google Scholar]