Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2017 May 26;7:2444. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-02569-4

Differences in selectivity to natural images in early visual areas (V1–V3)

David D Coggan 1, Luke A Allen 1, Oliver R H Farrar 1, Andre D Gouws 1, Antony B Morland 1, Daniel H Baker 1, Timothy J Andrews 1,
PMCID: PMC5446401  PMID: 28550282

Abstract

High-level regions of the ventral visual pathway respond more to intact objects compared to scrambled objects. The aim of this study was to determine if this selectivity for objects emerges at an earlier stage of processing. Visual areas (V1–V3) were defined for each participant using retinotopic mapping. Participants then viewed intact and scrambled images from different object categories (bottle, chair, face, house, shoe) while neural responses were measured using fMRI. Our rationale for using scrambled images is that they contain the same low-level properties as the intact objects, but lack the higher-order combinations of features that are characteristic of natural images. Neural responses were higher for scrambled than intact images in all regions. However, the difference between intact and scrambled images was smaller in V3 compared to V1 and V2. Next, we measured the spatial patterns of response to intact and scrambled images from different object categories. We found higher within-category compared to between category correlations for both intact and scrambled images demonstrating distinct patterns of response. Spatial patterns of response were more distinct for intact compared to scrambled images in V3, but not in V1 or V2. These findings demonstrate the emergence of selectivity to natural images in V3.

Introduction

Visual areas involved in object perception form a ventral processing pathway that projects from the occipital toward the temporal lobe. Physiological studies have shown that neurons in early visual areas show selectivity for low-level visual properties such as orientation, spatial frequency and retinal position1. Information from early visual areas eventually reaches high-level visual cortex in the temporal lobe, where neurons are tuned to combinations of low-level visual features2, 3. Indeed, neurons in different regions of high-level visual cortex have properties that are selective for different categories of objects4. For example, some regions respond more to images of faces58. Other regions show selectivity for images of places9, body parts10 and visually presented words11. Other studies have shown that the spatial pattern of response in these regions is able to discriminate a larger range of object categories12, 13.

A characteristic of these high-level regions is their selectivity for intact images1416. At early stages of processing (V1), there are greater responses to scrambled compared to intact images15. In contrast, the responses in high-level visual cortex are greater for intact compared to scrambled images. The selectivity for intact images is also evident in the spatial pattern of response of high-level regions of the ventral pathway. More distinct patterns of neural response (defined by higher within- compared to between-category correlations) are found to intact compared to scrambled images17. An important feature of intact images is the strong statistical dependencies between features, such as location-specific combinations of orientation and spatial frequency. Indeed, the behavioural sensitivity to the regularities that occur in intact objects suggests that these properties are critical for differentiating between different classes of images1822.

The aim of this study is to determine at what stage these statistical properties of intact objects emerge in the ventral visual pathway. Recent studies have shown that neurons in V2, but not in V1, respond selectively to synthetic textures that are based on the higher-order statistical properties found in natural images23, 24. Patterns of response in V2 are better able discriminate these naturalistic textures than control textures that are not based on natural images25. In the current study, we compare the response to images of objects and to scrambled versions of objects in early visual areas (V1–V3). Our aim was to determine whether these early visual areas showed selectivity to the statistical properties found in natural images. First, we asked whether the magnitude of response in these regions was greater for intact compared to scrambled images. Second, we asked whether the spatial pattern of response was more distinct for intact compared to scrambled images. Our hypothesis was that, if neurons in early visual areas are selective for statistical regularities found in intact objects, there should be either a greater response or a more distinct pattern of response to intact compared to scrambled images.

Materials and Methods

Stimuli

180 images of five object categories (bottle, chair, face, house, shoe) were taken from an object image stimulus set26. All images were gray-scale, superimposed on a mid-gray background, and had a resolution of 680 × 680 pixels. Faces were taken from the Radboud face database (http://www.socsci.ru.nl:8180/RaFD2/RaFD?p=main), which can be used freely for non-commercial scientific research. A scrambled version of each image was created by applying a Fourier phase-scramble to different spatial regions of the image17. This involved windowing each image into an 8 × 8 grid and phase-scrambling the contents of each window independently. This process preserves the spatial extent of the images. It also preserves the overall spatial frequency and orientation information (amplitude spectrum). However, the phase scrambling disrupts the specific combinations of image properties that are characteristic and perhaps diagnostic of particular semantic categories. Images subtended a maximum retinal angle of approximately 15° and were viewed on a screen at the rear of the scanner via a mirror placed immediately above the participant’s head. Examples of the stimuli are shown in Fig. 1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Exemplars of intact and scrambled images from the different object categories.

Participants

Twenty-one participants took part in the fMRI experiment (10 male, mean age = 26.3, SD = 6.0 years). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Each gave their informed, written consent and the study was approved by the York Neuroimaging Centre (YNiC) Ethics Committee and adhered to the Declaration of Helsinki.

Design and Procedure

There were 10 conditions: 5 categories (bottle, chair, face, house, shoe) × 2 image types (intact, scrambled). Images were presented in 6 s blocks. In each block, 6 images from a condition were presented individually for 800 ms, with a 200 ms inter-stimulus-interval. This was followed by a fixation cross lasting 9 s. There were 6 repetitions of each condition in the scan. To maintain attention participants were instructed to press a button on a response box whenever a red dot appeared on an image, which occurred once in each block. On average, subjects responded to 99.3% (SEM = 0.04%) of red dot images, with a mean reaction time of 420 msec (SEM = 14 msec). There was no significant difference in the number of hits between intact (mean = 99.4%, SEM = 0.3%) and scrambled (mean = 99.2%, SEM = 0.5%) conditions (t(20) = 0.37, ns). There was also no significant difference in the response latencies between intact (mean = 417 msec, SEM = 15 msec) and scrambled (mean = 425 msec, SEM = 14 msec) conditions (t(20) = 1.56, ns).

Data Acquisition

fMRI data were acquired with a General Electric 3 T HD Excite MRI scanner at YNiC at the University of York, fitted with an eight-channel, phased-array, head coil. A gradient-echo echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence was used to collect data from 38 contiguous axial slices (TR = 3000 ms, TE = 32.7 ms, FOV = 288 × 288 mm, matrix size = 128 × 128, slice thickness = 3 mm). The fMRI data were initially analyzed with FEAT v5.98 (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). In all scans, the initial 9 s of data was removed to reduce the effects of magnetic saturation. Motion correction (MCFLIRT, FSL) and slice-timing correction were applied followed by temporal high-pass filtering (Gaussian-weighted least-squares straight line fitting, sigma = 50 s). Gaussian spatial smoothing was applied at 6 mm FWHM.

Region of Interest Localization

Visual areas were defined in a separate scan session (TR, 3000 ms; TE, 30 ms; voxel size, 2 × 2 × 2 mm3; flip angle, 90°; matrix size, 96 × 96 × 39; FOV, 19.2 cm) with a 16-channel head coil to improve signal-to-noise in the occipital lobe using either ring and wedge type stimuli27 or population receptive field techniques28. Wedges rotated counterclockwise about a red fixation cross. Ring stimuli expanded about fixation. Both wedges and rings were high contrast checkerboard stimuli that flickered at a rate of 6 Hz. Each scan contained eight cycles of wedges/rings, with 36 s per cycle, traversing a circular region of radius 14.3°. Participants maintained fixation throughout the scan. Visual area boundaries between V1/V2 and V2/V3 (dorsal and ventral) were defined by the phase reversals in the polar angle representations on inflated representations of the visual cortex (Fig. 2, Supplementary Figure 1). Visual field eccentricity representations were used to restrict the ROI to the location of the stimulus, i.e. the central 15° of visual angle. Functional data from the main experimental scan were aligned to a high-resolution T1-anatomical image that was segmented into gray matter and white matter.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Early visual cortical regions for a representative participant. Visual areas are superimposed onto the occipital lobe – see red insert on the posterior view of the inflated brain. Colour maps indicate the preferred polar angle.

Data Analysis

To compare the magnitude of response to different stimulus conditions, parameter estimates were generated for each condition by regressing the hemodynamic response of each voxel against a boxcar function convolved with a single-gamma HRF. The responses from each voxel were then averaged within each ROI and converted from units of image intensity to % signal change. A repeated measures ANOVA was then used to determine the effect of ROI (V1, V2, V3) and Image Type (intact, scrambled).

To compare the spatial patterns of neural response, parameter estimates were generated for odd and even runs of each condition by regressing the hemodynamic response of each voxel against a box-car function convolved with a single-gamma HRF. Parameter estimates were normalized by subtracting the mean response per voxel across all conditions (odd and even, intact and scrambled). These data were then submitted to a within-subjects, correlation-based multivariate pattern analysis12, 29 (MVPA) implemented using the PyMVPA toolbox30 (http://www.pymvpa.org/). This allowed us to compare spatial patterns of response to all combinations of objects. For within-category comparisons, the correlation between responses in odd and even runs was used. For between-category comparisons, the mean correlation across odd-even and even-odd contrasts was used. A Fisher’s Z-transformation was then applied to the correlations prior to further statistical analyses. If there are distinct patterns for each object category, there should be a higher correlation in the spatial pattern of response for within-category compared to between-category comparisons.

Results

First, we asked whether the overall neural response in V1, V2 and V3 could distinguish intact and scrambled images. To address this question, we measured the % signal change in each region to intact and scrambled images (Fig. 3). We then performed a 2-way ANOVA with Region (V1, V2, V3) and Image Type (intact, scrambled) as factors. There was a significant main effect of Image Type (F(1,20) = 54.67, p < 0.0001) and a significant interaction between Region and Image Type (F(2,40) = 10.83, p = 0.0002). Pairwise comparisons revealed that scrambled images evoked more activity than intact images in V1 (t(20) = 6.46, p < 0.0001), V2 (t(20) = 7.55, p < 0.0001) and V3 (t(20) = 5.28, p = 0.0001). However, this difference was significantly smaller in V3 compared to both V1 (t(20) = 3.13, p = 0.0079) and V2 (t(20) = 5.54, p < 0.0001) (see Fig. 3B). This is the cause of the interaction detected by the ANOVA analysis as there was no difference between V1 and V2 (t(20) = 0.97, ns).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

(A) Magnitude of response to intact and scrambled images. Scrambled images evoked more activity than intact images in each visual area. (B) Differences in response to intact and scrambled images for each visual region. V3 showed a smaller difference in response to intact and scrambled images compared to V1 and V2. Error bars show ±1 SEM. *p < 0.05, FDR corrected.

Next, we asked whether there were differences in the spatial patterns of response in V1, V2 and V3 to intact and scrambled images. To address this question, we first tested whether different intact and scrambled object categories evoked distinct and reliable patterns of fMRI response in regions V1, V2 and V3 (Fig. 4 and Supplementary Figure 2). We compared the similarity of patterns of response to images from the same category (e.g. bottle vs. bottle) with the similarity of patterns to images of different categories (e.g. bottle vs. chair). Distinct, category-specific patterns of response are indicated by the within-category correlations being significantly greater than the between-category correlations (see Fig. 4b).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

(A) Similarity matrices showing the correlation in patterns of neural response to all within-category and between-category comparisons. Within-category comparisons (e.g. bottle-bottle) are shown on the diagonal. (B) Bar graph showing the mean within-category and between-category correlations for intact and scrambled images across participants. There was a significant interaction between Comparison, Image Type and Region, which was due to more distinct (within > between) patterns of neural response to intact relative to scrambled images in V3. Error bars show ±1 SEM. *p < 0.05, FDR corrected.

A 3-way repeated-measures ANOVA was performed with Comparison (within-category, between-category), Region (V1, V2, V3) and Image Type (intact, scrambled) as factors. There were main effects of Comparison (F(1,20) = 383.15, p < 0.0001) and Region (F(2,40) = 10.40, p = 0.0002). Although there was no effect of Image Type (F(1,20) = 1.24, p = 0.28), there was a significant three-way interaction between Comparison, Region and Image Type (F(2,40) = 5.28, p = 0.0093). This indicated that the distinctiveness of the category-specific patterns of response reflected by the effect of Comparison (within-category - between-category) differed across intact and scrambled images, depending on the visual region. Pairwise comparisons revealed that intact images evoked more distinct category-specific patterns than scrambled images in V3 (t(20) = 2.99, p = 0.020). This difference in the spatial pattern of response was not seen in V1 (t(20) = 1.17, ns) or V2 (t(20) = 0.71, ns). This shows that the spatial pattern of response to different object categories is more distinct for intact compared to scrambled images in V3.

Finally, we investigated the patterns of response similarity to different object categories across image type and region. Figure 5A shows all pairwise correlations across the different similarity matrices shown in Fig. 4. This appears to show higher correlations to the same image type (intact or scrambled). For example, in V1 the correlation with V2 and V3 for intact images was 0.99 and 0.96, respectively. In contrast, the correlation between intact and scrambled images in V1 was 0.63. To assess whether the higher correlation for the same image type was statistically significant, we generated two models: Image Type and Region (5B). These models were regressed onto each subject’s matrix, generating a distribution of beta-weights for each model (5C). These weights were significantly above zero for Image Type (t(20) = 8.45, p < 0.0001), but not for Region (t(20) = 0.73, ns). Weights for Image Type were also significantly higher than those for Region (t(20) = 8.26, p < 0.0001). This shows that representational distances between the object categories across all visual areas was different for intact and scrambled images.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

(A) Correlations between the similarity matrices shown in Fig. 4A. (B) Matrix predictions based on representations of image type and region. (C) Models were used in a regression analysis across participants. Performance was determined by the regression coefficients for each model. The results show that patterns of response were predicted significantly more by the image type than region. Error bars show ±1 SEM. *p < 0.05.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to determine whether early stages of the ventral visual pathway are selective for objects. To address this issue, we compared both the magnitude and the pattern of response to intact and scrambled images from different object categories in V1, V2 and V3. Our results reveal that all regions showed greater overall neural response to scrambled images relative to intact images. However, this difference was smaller in V3 compared to V1 and V2. We also found that the spatial pattern of response in V3, but not in V1 or V2, was more distinct for intact objects compared to scrambled objects. Our results show that this selectivity for the properties of natural objects begins to emerge in the response properties of V3.

The majority of studies of the human object-recognition pathway have focused on the initial (V1) or the final (category-selective regions) stages of processing, while the intermediate stages have received less attention31. Neurons in V1 are known to be selective for low-level features of the image1. Further downstream, neurons are tuned to properties that appear to combine features encoded in earlier visual areas that are statistically characteristic of natural images24, 29. To determine where selectivity emerges, we compared responses to intact and scrambled objects in early visual areas (V1–V3). Scrambled images contain the same visual elements as intact images, but lack the statistical regularities between elements that are important for object perception2022. Previous studies have shown differential responses to intact and scrambled objects along the ventral visual pathway14, 15. For example, Grill-Spector and colleagues used a box-scrambling method to progressively change the degree of scrambling. In early visual areas (V1–V3), they found a higher response to all the scrambled conditions compared to intact images. V4 showed a maximal response to intermediate levels of scrambling and higher visual areas responded most strongly to intact images. These findings suggest that V4 is an important intermediate region in the neural representation of objects3234. However, previous studies have either not reported the responses in V3 or have not distinguished between the response properties of V3 and V1/V2. We found that all regions (V1–V3) showed higher responses to scrambled compared to intact images, but that this difference was attenuated in V3. This suggests that some of the selectivity in V4 to higher order properties of the image may emerge from V3.

To further probe the selectivity to objects in V3, we compared the spatial pattern of response to intact and scrambled images. This analysis is based on a comparison of the within-category similarity in spatial response with the between-category similarity. In a recent study, we found that category-selective patterns of response in high-level regions of the ventral pathway to scrambled images are less distinct than for intact images17. We found that the distinctiveness of the pattern of response to different object categories in V3 was greater for intact compared to scrambled images. This reliability in the spatial pattern of response to intact objects is consistent with other studies that have found that temporal patterns of neural response are also more reliable with natural images35. These findings complement the univariate analysis and show that a bias toward natural images begins at an early stage of processing. It is important to note, however, that these findings only show the emergence of selectivity rather than the full selectivity found in higher visual areas. In our opinion, it is likely that selectivity for objects is an emergent property, rather than a binary property, of the visual system.

Although neurons in V2 receive most of their input from V1 and have similar selectivity for orientation and spatial scale36, a number of studies have shown differences in the response to conjunctions of image features in V23739. Recent studies have found that neurons in V2 show larger and more reliable responses to synthetic textures that have properties based on natural images compared to control textures2325. Given this sensitivity to the higher order structure of more naturalistic stimuli, the lack of difference between V1 and V2 in our study was unexpected. One possibility is that the objects used in the current study lack the regularity in structure found in the textures generated by Simoncelli and colleagues40. Nevertheless, these findings fit with a recent study showing responses to different texture patterns could be differentiated in V3, but not in V241.

Our final analysis involved comparing the representational distances between object categories for intact and scrambled images. Despite the fact that the low-level features were matched between the intact and scrambled images, the representational similarity was more similar for the same image type (intact or scrambled) across regions than for different image types within the same region. For example, the similarity in the representational similarity between V1 and V2 for intact images was greater that the representational similarity between intact and scrambled images in V1. This suggests early visual areas are sensitive to the statistical regularities found in intact natural images.

Anatomical observations have shown that neuronal density decreases along the posterior-anterior axis of the primate visual system42. This is accompanied by a corresponding reduction in the surface area of regions in higher visual areas43. Taken together, these findings indicate that there is a reduction in the amount of information encoded at higher levels of the ventral stream44. This places constraints on the number of feature conjunctions that can encoded45. One solution to this combinatorial problem is to only encode combinations of low-level features that are commonly found in natural objects46, 47. We found that the difference between the patterns of response to intact and scrambled images was a reduction in the distinctiveness of the scrambled images. This suggests that the adaptive encoding that is necessary for successful object perception begins at an early stage of processing.

In conclusion, the ventral visual pathway comprises a sequence of cortical areas in which successively more complex visual attributes are extracted, beginning with contour orientations in V1 and resulting in representations of objects at the highest levels. In contrast, high-level regions of the ventral visual pathway produce greater or more reliable responses to natural, intact images relative to artificial, scrambled images. Previous studies have been unclear at which stage in the processing stream this selectivity to natural images emerges. Here, we show a preference for natural images can be found at early stages of processing in extrastriate visual cortex.

Electronic supplementary material

Acknowledgements

This work Supported in part by the Wellcome Trust (ref: 105624) through the Centre for Chronic Diseases and Disorders (C2D2) at the University of York and by a BBSRC grant (BB/L007770/1) to Antony Morland.

Author Contributions

D.D.C., L.A.A. & O.R.H.F. collected the data for the main experiment. A.D.G. and A.B.M. analysed and defined the regions of interest. D.D.C., L.A.A., O.R.H.F., D.H.B. & T.J.A. analysed the data from the main experiment. D.D.C., D.H.B. & T.J.A. wrote the paper. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Competing Interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at doi:10.1038/s41598-017-02569-4

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Receptive Fields and Functional Architecture of monkey striate cortex. J. Physiol. 1968;195:215–243. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1968.sp008455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Tanaka K. Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1996;19:109–139. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.19.030196.000545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Connor CE, Brincat SL, Pasupathy A. Transformation of shape information in the ventral pathway. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2007;17:140–147. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kanwisher N. Functional specificity in the human brain: a window into the functional architecture of the mind. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:11163–70. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1005062107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Rolls ET. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying face processing within and beyond the temporal cortical visual areas. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 1992;335:11–21. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1992.0002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM. The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:4302–11. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.McCarthy G, Puce A, Gore JC, Truett A. Face-Specific Processing in the Human Fusiform Gyrus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1997;9:605–610. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Tsao DY, Freiwald WA, Tootell RBH, Livingstone MS. A cortical region consisting entirely of face-selective cells. Science (80-.). 2006;311:670–674. doi: 10.1126/science.1119983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Epstein R, Kanwisher N. A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature. 1998;392:598–601. doi: 10.1038/33402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Downing PE, Jiang Y, Shuman M, Kanwisher N. A cortical area selective for visual processing of the human body. Science. 2001;293:2470–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1063414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Cohen L, et al. The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain. 2000;123:291–307. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.2.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Haxby JV, et al. Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science. 2001;293:2425–30. doi: 10.1126/science.1063736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kriegeskorte N, et al. Matching Categorical Object Representations in Inferior Temporal Cortex of Man and Monkey. Neuron. 2008;60:1126–1141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Malach R, et al. Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. Neurobiology. 1995;92:8135–8139. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.18.8135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Edelman S, Itzchak Y, Malach R. Cue-invariant activation in object-related areas of the human occipital lobe. Neuron. 1998;21:191–202. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80526-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Andrews TJ, Clarke A, Pell P, Hartley T. Selectivity for low-level features of objects in the human ventral stream. Neuroimage. 2010;49:703–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Coggan DD, Liu W, Baker DH, Andrews TJ. Category-selective patterns of neural response in the ventral visual pathway in the absence of categorical information. Neuroimage. 2016;135:107–114. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Sigman, M., Cecchi, G. A., Gilbert, C. D. & Magnasco, M. O. On a common circle: Natural scenes and Gestalt rules. 2000, 1935–1940 (2001). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 19.Geisler WS. Visual perception and the statistical properties of natural scenes. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2008;59:167–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Oppenheim AV, Lim JS. Importance of Phase in Signals. Proc. IEEE. 1981;69:529–541. doi: 10.1109/PROC.1981.12022. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Thomson, M. G. A. Higher-order structure in natural scenes. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1549–1553 (1999).
  • 22.Vogels R. Categorization of complex visual images by rhesus monkeys. Part 2: single-cell study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1999;11:1239–1255. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00531.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Freeman J, Simoncelli EP. Metamers of the ventral stream. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:1195–1201. doi: 10.1038/nn.2889. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Freeman J, Ziemba CM, Heeger DJ, Simoncelli EP, Movshon JA. A functional and perceptual signature of the second visual area in primates. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:974–81. doi: 10.1038/nn.3402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ziemba, C. M., Freeman, J., Movshon, J. A. & Simoncelli, E. P. Selectivity and tolerance for visual texture in macaque V2. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 201510847, doi:10.1073/pnas.1510847113 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 26.Rice GE, Watson DM, Hartley T, Andrews TJ. Low-Level Image Properties of Visual Objects Predict Patterns of Neural Response across Category-Selective Regions of the Ventral Visual Pathway. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:8837–44. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5265-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Vernon RJW, Gouws AD, Lawrence SJD, Wade AR, Morland AB. Multivariate Patterns in the Human Object-Processing Pathway Reveal a Shift from Retinotopic to Shape Curvature Representations in Lateral Occipital Areas, LO-1 and LO-2. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:5763–5774. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3603-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Dumoulin SO, Wandell BA. Population receptive field estimates in human visual cortex. Neuroimage. 2008;39:647–660. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Haxby, J. V., Connolly, A. C. & Guntupalli, J. S. Decoding Neural Representational Spaces Using Multivariate Pattern Analysis. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 435–456, 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170325 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 30.Hanke M, et al. PyMVPA: A python toolbox for multivariate pattern analysis of fMRI data. Neuroinformatics. 2009;7:37–53. doi: 10.1007/s12021-008-9041-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Peirce JW. Understanding mid-level representations in visual processing vision. J. Vis. 2015;15:1–9. doi: 10.1167/15.7.5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gallant JL, Braun J, Essen DCVan. Selectivity for Polar, Hyperbolic, and Cartesian Gratings in Macaque Visual Cortex. Science (80-.). 1993;259:100–103. doi: 10.1126/science.8418487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Pasupathy A, Connor CE. Population coding of shape in area V4. Nat. Neurosci. 2002;5:1332–1338. doi: 10.1038/972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.David SV, Hayden BY, Gallant JL. Spectral receptive field properties explain shape selectivity in area V4. J. Neurophysiol. 2006;96:3492–3505. doi: 10.1152/jn.00575.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hasson U, Malach R, Heeger DJ. Reliability of cortical activity during natural stimulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010;14:40–48. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Levitt J, Kiper D, Movshon JA. Receptive Fields Anf Functional Architecture of Macaque V2. J. Neurophysiol. 1994;71:2517–2542. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.71.6.2517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Ito M, Komatsu H. Representation of Angles Embedded within Contour Stimuli in Area V2 of Macaque Monkeys. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:3313–3324. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4364-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Anzai A, Peng X, Van Essen DC. Neurons in monkey visual area V2 encode combinations of orientations. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:1313–21. doi: 10.1038/nn1975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.El-Shamayleh Y, Movshon JA. Neuronal Responses to Texture-Defined Form in Macaque Visual Area V2. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:8543–8555. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5974-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Portilla J, Simoncelli EP. Aparametric texture model based on joint statistics of complex wavelet coefficients. Int. J. Comput. Vis. 2000;40:49–71. doi: 10.1023/A:1026553619983. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kohler PJ, Clarke A, Yakovleva A, Liu Y, Norcia AM. Representation of Maximally Regular Textures in Human Visual Cortex. J Neurosci. 2016;36:714–729. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2962-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Cahalane DJ, Charvet CJ, Finlay BL. Systematic, balancing gradients in neuron density and number across the primate isocortex. Front. Neuroanat. 2012;6:28. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2012.00028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Van Essen DC, Anderson CH, Felleman DJ. Information Processing in the Primate Visual System: An Integrated Systems Perspective. Science (80-.). 1992;255:419–424. doi: 10.1126/science.1734518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Lehky SR, Kiani R, Esteky H, Tanaka K. Dimensionality of object representations in monkey inferotemporal cortex. Neural Comput. 2014;1872:1840–1872. doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wilson HR, Wilkinson F. From orientations to objects: Configural processing in the ventral stream. J. Vis. 2015;15:1–10. doi: 10.1167/15.7.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kourtzi Z, Connor CE. Neural Representations for Object Perception: Structure, Category, and Adaptive Coding. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2011;34:45–67. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kourtzi Z, Welchman AE. Adaptive shape coding for perceptual decisions in the human brain. J. Vis. 2015;15:1–9. doi: 10.1167/15.7.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES