Skip to main content
Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY) logoLink to Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY)
. 2017 Jan 7;27(3):1709–1720. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw403

Joint Attention and Brain Functional Connectivity in Infants and Toddlers

Adam T Eggebrecht 1,*, Jed T Elison 2, Eric Feczko 3, Alexandre Todorov 4, Jason J Wolff 5, Sridhar Kandala 4, Chloe M Adams 4, Abraham Z Snyder 1, John D Lewis 6, Annette M Estes 7, Lonnie Zwaigenbaum 8, Kelly N Botteron 1,4, Robert C McKinstry 1, John N Constantino 4, Alan Evans 6, Heather C Hazlett 9, Stephen Dager 10, Sarah J Paterson 11,12, Robert T Schultz 11, Martin A Styner 9, Guido Gerig 13, Samir Das 6, Penelope Kostopoulos 6; The IBIS Network, Bradley L Schlaggar 14, Steven E Petersen 14, Joseph Piven 9,, John R Pruett Jr 4,
PMCID: PMC5452276  PMID: 28062515

Abstract

Initiating joint attention (IJA), the behavioral instigation of coordinated focus of 2 people on an object, emerges over the first 2 years of life and supports social-communicative functioning related to the healthy development of aspects of language, empathy, and theory of mind. Deficits in IJA provide strong early indicators for autism spectrum disorder, and therapies targeting joint attention have shown tremendous promise. However, the brain systems underlying IJA in early childhood are poorly understood, due in part to significant methodological challenges in imaging localized brain function that supports social behaviors during the first 2 years of life. Herein, we show that the functional organization of the brain is intimately related to the emergence of IJA using functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging and dimensional behavioral assessments in a large semilongitudinal cohort of infants and toddlers. In particular, though functional connections spanning the brain are involved in IJA, the strongest brain-behavior associations cluster within connections between a small subset of functional brain networks; namely between the visual network and dorsal attention network and between the visual network and posterior cingulate aspects of the default mode network. These observations mark the earliest known description of how functional brain systems underlie a burgeoning fundamental social behavior, may help improve the design of targeted therapies for neurodevelopmental disorders, and, more generally, elucidate physiological mechanisms essential to healthy social behavior development.

Keywords: development, enrichment, fMRI, initiating joint attention, network

Introduction

The emergence of joint attention (JA), the coordinated orienting of 2 people toward an object or event, occurs during the first 2 years of life, arguably the most dynamic and important period of early child development (Scaife and Bruner 1975). It is theorized that engaging in JA lays the foundation for prosocial cooperative behavior, from basic social-communicative functioning and language development (Premack 2004) to sophisticated forms of empathy (Mundy and Jarrold 2010) and theory of mind (Adolphs 2003). In fact, early exhibition of joint attention is strongly associated with later language ability (Morales et al. 2000; Mundy et al. 2007), and atypical development of the initiation of joint attention (IJA) is strongly indicative of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Bruinsma et al. 2004). The neural substrates underlying IJA in early childhood are poorly understood (Barak and Feng 2016), due in part to significant methodological challenges in imaging localized brain function that supports social behaviors in children during the first 2 years of life.

Developmental trajectories, evident during the first 2 years of life via directly observed behavior, physiology, or networks of genetic transcription or brain function (Lin et al. 2008; Gao et al. 2009; Fransson et al. 2011; Smyser et al. 2011; Petersen and Posner 2012; Toulmin et al. 2015), may illuminate underlying mechanisms of normative function and may be predictive of neurodevelopmental disorders (Tsodyks et al. 1999; Parikshak et al. 2015; Richiardi et al. 2015). During early childhood, the sequential emergence and consolidation of social behaviors (i.e., eye gaze following [Emery 2000], responding to a bid for joint attention [RJA] [Scaife and Bruner 1975; Elison et al. 2013], initiating joint attention [Mundy et al. 2000]) exhibit a distinctive cascading and phylogenetically preserved trajectory (Carpenter et al. 1998; Tomasello and Carpenter 2007; Okamoto-Barth et al. 2011), with an infant's capacity for IJA manifesting as a key ontogenetic watershed (Tomasello et al. 2005). Recent studies in adults using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have shown that regions recruited in acts of both RJA and IJA are distributed throughout fronto-temporal-parietal cortices and populate social-cognitive, voluntary attention, orienting, and visual networks (Redcay et al. 2012; Caruana et al. 2015), as well as subcortical and cingulate areas involved in reward processing (Schilbach et al. 2010; Pfeiffer et al. 2014; Oberwelland et al. 2016). While some of this work incorporated a developmental sample including school-aged participants (Oberwelland et al. 2016) and participants with ASD (Redcay et al. 2013b), a key question is how the emergence of IJA relates to the maturation of the brain's functional networks during typical and atypical early development (Barak and Feng 2016; Chahrour et al. 2016). Because we expect IJA behaviors to potentially serve different functions at different ages, we do not necessarily expect to observe a downward extension of the adult or school-aged participant findings to infants and toddlers. Thus, we are taking a data-driven strategy to test the brain-behavior relationships underlying IJA.

In the current study, we tested the extent of the relationship between IJA and functional connections between brain networks over the period of emergence and consolidation of IJA. We measured the frequency of spontaneous acts of IJA (Fig. 1A) by 12-month-old (mo) infants and 24 mo toddlers using a semistructured dimensional behavioral assessment. Separately, we measured brain functional networks in the same group of infants and toddlers using resting-state functional connectivity MRI (fcMRI) (Power et al. 2011). We hypothesized that IJA would be significantly correlated with a limited set of interactions between functional brain regions, and that these associations would change over time. To determine which functional networks significantly contribute to the overall brain-behavior relationship, we utilized an enrichment analysis adapted from research in large-scale genetic association studies (Backes et al. 2014), a novel strategy for analyzing neuro-behavioral relationships.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

IJA and functional connectivity in infants and toddlers. (A) An example of IJA wherein the child acts to reorient the mother's gaze from the yellow balloon (0) to the pink ball (1). (B) The proportion of testing epochs containing an act of IJA increases from 12 months (blue) to 24 months of age (red), as expected. (C) An Infomap-sorted mean fcMRI matrix derived from the correlation structure between 230 functionally defined ROIs. (D) Left lateral view of the ROIs on the brain surface with coloring representing putative functional subnetworks (see Materials and Methods for details and definition of network abbreviations).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were part of the Infant Brain Imaging Study, an ongoing longitudinal study of infants at low- (LR) and high- familial risk (HR) for ASD. Infants were recruited, screened, and assessed at each of 4 sites: University of North Carolina, University of Washington, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, and Washington University in St Louis. The research protocol was approved by the institutional review board at all sites of data acquisition for the study, and parents provided written informed consent after receiving a detailed description of the study. Behavioral assessments and MRI data were acquired at each site, and the data were used for research purposes only.

Inclusion Criteria

To maximize variance in a dimensional measure of IJA, we enrolled children who were at HR for developing ASD as well as children with LR (typically developing control participants) for developing ASD. Children at high risk for developing ASD carry a familial liability typically associated with behaviors, such as impaired IJA, subthreshold to a full autism diagnosis, but distributed along a continuum. The HR infants were defined as having at least one sibling with an ASD diagnosis, and LR infants had at least one typically developing older sibling and did not have any first- or second-degree family members with ASD or intellectual disability. Participants were excluded for comorbid medical or neurological diagnoses influencing growth, development, or cognition; prior genetic conditions; premature birth or low birth weight; maternal substance abuse during pregnancy; contraindication for MRI; or familial history of psychosis, schizophrenia, or bipolar disorder (Wolff et al. 2012; Elison et al. 2013). All infants and toddlers included in these analyses participated in a comprehensive battery of behavioral assessments and received an Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS, Gotham et al. 2007) at 24 months. ADOSs and all other testing and interview data were independently reviewed by experienced clinicians for DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association 2000) criteria for autistic disorder or pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified. This paper includes data from infants who, at 24 months of age, did and did not meet criteria for ASD according to research clinical best estimate using a DSM-IV-TR checklist. The children with a positive diagnosis were included to extend the range of observed IJA behavior at each age group. The age groups did not differ by proportion of children later diagnosed with ASD (Fisher's exact test, P = 0.29).

Demographics

Two cohorts were defined: children were assessed at visits corresponding to 12- and 24-mo age groups (ranges: 11.7–14.5 mo and 23.5–25.9 mo, respectively, Supplementary Table 1). All data sets were subject to stringent fcMRI quality control criteria and Infant Brain Imaging Study (IBIS) Network behavioral and structural MRI inclusion criteria. Data sets passing all inclusion criteria for both the Communication and Symbolic Behavioral Scale (CSBS) and MRI include N = 116 at 12 and N = 98 at 24 months of age, with 37 individuals providing data at both time points. Age groups did not significantly differ by proportion of HR participants (P = 0.99), girls (P = 0.32), or IQ (Mullen composite standard score, t = −0.92, df = 687, P = 0.18).

Assessment of IJA Behavior

IJA behavior was assessed at IBIS Network clinical sites with the Communication and Symbolic Behavioral Scales-Developmental Profile (CSBS-DP) (Wetherby et al. 2002), which is designed to elicit social and communicative behaviors in infants and toddlers between 12 and 24 months of age (Fig. 1B). The interaction between examiner and infant is divided into 6 sampling opportunities: wind-up toy, balloon, bubbles, jar, books, and play, with the complete assessment generally lasting up to 30 min. The metric used for our study is calculated as the number of sampling opportunities that included one or more acts of IJA, defined as an act used to direct another's attention to an object, event, or topic of a communicative act (Wetherby et al. 2002): “The child's goal is to get the adult to look at or notice something,” (Question #7 of the CSBS). Acts for IJA are carefully distinguished from acts used for behavioral regulation (e.g., requesting as action) and acts that are used to draw the adult's attention to the self (an act of social interaction but not explicitly IJA). Assessments are video recorded and scored by research-reliable expert raters to ensure accuracy and intersite and intrasite reliability across the IBIS network. There was no effect of site on IJA scores (F = 0.97, P = 0.41), and age-in-months was not correlated with IJA score within time point (12 mo: ρ = 0.03, P = 0.46 and 24 mo: ρ = −0.02, P = 0.68).

Image Acquisition

All scans were acquired at IBIS Network clinical sites using identical 3-T Siemens TIM Trio scanners (Siemens Medical Solutions) equipped with standard 12-channel head coils. Infants were naturally sleeping. The IBIS imaging protocol includes T1-weighted (T1W) and T2W anatomical imaging, 25-direction DTI and 65-direction HARDI DWI diffusion sequences, and resting-state fcMRI (Wolff et al. 2012). This study made use of the 3D sagittal T2W sequence (time echo [TE] = 497 ms, time repetition [TR] = 3200 ms, matrix 256 × 256 × 160, 1 mm3 voxels). Functional images were collected as a gradient-echo echo planar image (TE = 27 ms, TR = 2500 ms, voxel size 4 × 4 × 4 mm3, flip angle 90°, field of view 256 mm, matrix 64 × 64, bandwidth 1906 Hz). All included infants provided at least 2 fMRI runs, each run comprising 130 temporally contiguous frames (6.25 min).

Conditioning Infants for MRI Scanning

A multistep conditioning approach was developed for acquiring optimally reliable and successful MRI data in unsedated and naturally sleeping infants and toddlers. First, parents were sent a CD containing MRI sounds along with instructions to play the CD at an increasingly loud volume every night at the infant's bedtime. The CD was played at home for 7–10 days. On the night of the scan, a nonmagnetic crib and rocking chair were placed in the scanner suite, and all lights in the room were turned off. Child sized earplugs were placed in the infants’ ears and secured with a piece of tape. If appropriate, infants were also swaddled. The MRI CD was then streamed into the room and played during the scanning session. The parent and the infant followed their normal bedtime routine in the room, often feeding the child and rocking them to sleep. Once the infant fell naturally to sleep, the study team waited an additional 20 min before moving the infant to the MRI table. Babies, able to be scanned prone, supine, or on their side, were placed on the table in their most common sleeping position. Sandbags were placed on the sides of the infant to help stabilize and prevent rolling over. MRI compatible sound diminishing headphones were placed on the child, and the infant was allowed to sleep for 20 more minutes to enter a deeper sleep before the scan was initiated. Once the scan began, a research technician stayed in the room, at the foot of the scanner to carefully monitor the infant. If the baby awoke at any time, the scan was immediately suspended and the process was reinitiated. When feasible, MRI scan visits were scheduled in the evening as close to the infant's normal bedtime as possible and usually lasted 2–4 h. We did not track if participants arrived to the scanning session awake or alternatively for how long they had been asleep. The sleep stages of the participants while they were undergoing their functional MRI scans may be more variable than if all participants arrived awake.

fMRI Preprocessing

Initial fMRI data preprocessing followed previously described procedures (Smyser et al. 2010; Pruett et al. 2015). These procedures included 1) compensation for slice-dependent time shifts using sinc interpolation, 2) correction of systematic odd–even slice intensity differences caused by interleaved acquisition, and 3) spatial realignment to compensate for head motion within and across fMRI runs. The fMRI data were intensity scaled (one multiplicative constant over all voxels and frames) to obtain a whole brain mode value of 1000 (Ojemann et al. 1997). Such scaling facilitates the computation of variance measures for purposes of quality assessment but does not alter computed correlations. Atlas registration of functional data was achieved by a sequence of affine transforms (fMRI average volume—T2W—atlas-representative target). Age-specific (12 and 24 months) atlas-representative targets (Fonov et al. 2011) were used to account for shape differences across developmental age categories. Following fMRI—T2W—atlas transform composition, the volumetric time series were resampled in atlas space (3 mm3 voxels) including correction for head movement in a single resampling step. Each atlas-transformed functional data set was visually inspected in sagittal, transverse, and coronal views to exclude potential errors not otherwise identified.

Frame Censoring

Head motion, even of submillimeter magnitude, has been identified as a nonphysiological source of spurious variance in resting-state fMRI data (Power et al. 2012; Van Dijk et al. 2012; Satterthwaite et al. 2013). Data were subjected to rigorous frame censoring (“superscrubbing”) based on the frame-to-frame displacement (FD) measure (Power et al. 2014), which sums 3 absolute translations (X, Y, Z) and 3 absolute rotations (Pitch, Yaw, Roll) evaluated at a radius of 50 mm. Frames with FD ≥0.2 mm were marked for subsequent censoring. Temporally isolated (5 or fewer contiguous) FD <0.2 mm frames were also censored. fMRI runs with fewer than 30 uncensored frames were discarded. To control for potential biases attributable to the amount of data per cohort, 150 noncensored (retained) fMRI frames were used, prioritizing runs with the most retained frames, for correlation analysis in each subject.

fcMRI Preprocessing

Further preprocessing in preparation for computation of region-of-interest (ROI) pair time series correlations followed previously published procedures (Power et al. 2014). The data were centered and detrended at every voxel within runs, ignoring censored frames. The MRI tissue contrast properties in infants and toddlers make automatic segmentation of structural images extremely challenging (Hazlett et al. 2012). Following previously published methodology (Pruett et al. 2015), we manually defined cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) and white-matter regions in atlas-transformed T1-weighted images from 15 representative participants in each age group (including data from both high- and low-risk individuals). To reduce the risk of partial volume effects, a 2.5-mm Gaussian kernel was used to erode these regions of noninterest away from boundaries with other tissues including gray matter. The final CSF and white-matter regions of noninterest were created by computing the intersection over each age group. These regions were then used to extract nuisance regressors for fcMRI processing. Nuisance waveforms then were regressed voxelwise from the data, ignoring censored frames. Nuisance regressors included 1) time series derived from 3 translation (X, Y, Z) and 3 rotation (Pitch, Yaw, Roll) estimates derived by retrospective head motion analysis, together with Volterra expansion derivatives (24 total motion regressors) (Friston et al. 1996) and 2) time series derived from the whole brain, white matter, and CSF and their first derivatives. Following nuisance regression, data in frames marked for censoring were replaced by interpolated values computed by least-squares spectral analysis (Mathias et al. 2004; Power et al. 2014). The fMRI data were then temporally filtered to retain frequencies in the 0.009 Hz < f < 0.08 Hz band (Power et al. 2011). As a last step, the data were spatially smoothed using a Gaussian kernel (6 mm full-width at half-maximum [FWHM] isotropic). To test for a potential relationship between the resting-state motion parameters and the IJA scores, we calculated the Spearman rank correlation between the IJA scores and the mean and maximum of the FD (both before and after frame censoring at FD = 0.2) for both age groups. No significant relationship was observed (12 mo: mean FD before scrubbing ρ = 0.02, P = 0.81; max FD before scrubbing ρ = 0.02, P = 0.86; mean FD after scrubbing ρ = 0.04, P = 0.66; max FD after scrubbing ρ = 0.04, P = 0.70. 24 mo: mean FD before scrubbing ρ = −0.03, P = 0.74; max FD before scrubbing ρ = 0.06, P = 0.52; mean FD after scrubbing ρ = 0.06, P = 0.52; max FD after scrubbing ρ = 0.02, P = 0.83). The range of the number of clean frames of fcMRI data passing the FD threshold of 0.2 was by age group: 12 mo—min = 152, mean = 241.8, max = 375 and 24 mo—min = 152, mean = 247.2, max = 375. Testing with a Spearman rank correlation found no significant relationship observed in the amount of frames passing the FD threshold and IJA score (12 mo: ρ = −0.5, P = 0.57 and 24 mo: ρ = −0.1, P = 0.89). In the end, all participants contributed exactly 150 frames clean for analyses.

Definition of ROI and Functional Connectivity Computation

ROIs (n = 280) were adopted from a combination of task data and cortical functional areal parcellations (Cohen et al. 2008) obtained in healthy adults (Power et al. 2011), and meta-analyses of autism studies (Philip et al. 2012). Three viewers inspected ROI placements in age-specific atlas templates. Of the 280 ROIs, 50 were partially outside the whole brain mask or showed visible differences in gray-matter coverage at different ages. These ROIs were removed leaving 230 usable ROIs (Supplementary Fig. 1A, Supplementary Spreadsheet 1) (Pruett et al. 2015). ROI-representative time series were calculated as the average of the time series across all gray-matter voxels contained by the 10-mm-diameter sphere located at a given ROI center. Pairwise Pearson correlation values were generated for each of the 26 335 possible pairs of ROI and then Fisher-z transformed.

Derivation of Putative Functional Networks in Infants and Toddlers

A model for the organization of functional brain networks representative of the infants and toddlers in the study was generated using a set of clean fcMRI data from 48 children acquired at both the 12- and 24-month time points (Fig. 1C,D). Closely following previous methods (Power et al. 2011), the full set of ROI-pair correlations (Fisher-z values) were averaged across subjects (Fig. 1C). The resulting average set of correlations were thresholded and binarized at a variety of correlation thresholds calculated to create connection matrices with specific degrees of sparseness (ranging from 1% to 10% of all possible connections surviving the threshold, in steps of 0.01%). Connections between ROI pairs separated by an Euclidean distance of <20 mm were removed to minimize the effects of blurring in the fMRI data. At each threshold, the resulting connection matrices were evaluated using the Infomap algorithm (Rosvall and Bergstrom 2008) that assigns ROIs to communities (subnetworks) at each correlation threshold based on the maximization of within-module random walks in the connection matrix. Communities with 5 or fewer ROIs were designated “en masse” as Unassigned (US). The solutions for each threshold were then combined using an algorithmic “consensus” procedure that combined Infomap solutions across edge density thresholds by maximizing the normalized mutual information of groups of neighboring solutions and maximizing the modularity of the final consensus model. This provided a reasonable single summary view of the functional networks of the brain. This consensus model of the combined infant–toddler functional network of the brain includes 13 putative networks with naming informed by the adult set of networks: Vis (visual), tDMN (temporal default mode network), pcDMN (posterior cingulate DMN), aDMN (anterior DMN), SMN (somato-motor network), SMN2 (somato-motor network 2), DAN (dorsal attention network), pFPC (posterior frontal parietal control network), aFPC (anterior frontal parietal control network), SubCtx (subcortex), CO (cingulo-opercular), pCO (posterior CO), and Sal (salience). To generate complementary analyses using adult-functional networks, we adapted a previously published model of adult-functional networks (Power et al. 2011) (Supplementary Fig. 1C).

Statistical Analysis

Demographic characteristics were recorded at visits corresponding to the 12- and 24-mo time points (ranges: 11.7–14.5 mo and 23.5–25.9 mo, respectively). Potential group differences between age groups in proportion of HR participants, proportion of children later diagnosed with ASD and proportion of girls were tested with Fisher's exact test. Potential differences in intelligence assessed via the Mullen Scales of Early Learning composite score were tested with Welch's t-test. The potential effect of site on IJA score was tested using a one-way ANOVA. To ensure the subset of participants who contributed fcMRI data that represent the larger sample, potential differences in the IJA scores between these groups were tested with the Mann–Whitney U test. The difference in IJA score across age was calculated across the larger sample of participants who contributed behavioral data (n = 429 at 12 mo, n = 384 at 24 mo, and n = 311 with data at both time points) with the Wilcoxon signed-rank test. The potential effect of age on IJA score within each age group was tested with Spearman's correlation.

To investigate the relationship between the fcMRI data and the IJA metric for each age group, we first calculated the Spearman rank correlation for each ROI-ROI fcMRI pair against the IJA score across subjects (Fig. 2A). Second, methods for enrichment analyses were adapted from those used in large-scale genomic association studies (Rivals et al. 2007; Khatri et al. 2012; Backes et al. 2014). Each network pair was tested for enrichment of strong Spearman correlation values, defined as values remaining after being thresholded and binarized at an associated uncorrected P value ≤0.05 (Supplementary Fig. 2A). Two complementary tests were conducted: a 1-degree-of-freedom χ2 test and a hypergeometric test (Supplementary Fig. 2B,C). The χ2 test compares the observed number of strong brain-behavior correlations within a functional network pair to that which would be expected if the overall number of strong correlations was uniformly distributed across all possible network pairs. The statistic is large when the number of strong correlations within a network pair is much less than (depletion) or much greater than (enrichment) expected. Empirical P values were calculated using randomization, as described below. The hypergeometric statistic, as in Fisher's exact test, assesses the likelihood of observing a given number of strong correlations in a network pair, given 1) the total number of strong correlations observed overall and 2) the total number of possible hits for that network pair (i.e., the total number or ROI pairs within a given network pair). Both the χ2 test and the hypergeometric tests had to pass the significance threshold for a given network pair's enrichment to be considered significant. Finally, we used a McNemar χ2 test to investigate differences in brain-behavior relationships between the ages of 12 mo and 24 mo. Within each network pair, the McNemar statistic uses the number of discordant tests (b = number of ROI pairs True at 12 mo but False at 24 mo, c = the number of ROI pairs False at 12 mo but True at 24 mo) between ages. The McNemar χ2 statistic is (bc)2/(b + c), if (b + c) > 25, and (|bc| − 1)2/(b + c) otherwise.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

The strongest brain-behavior relationships are concentrated in a small subset of functional network pairs. (A) Brain fc was correlated against IJA scores across subjects for each ROI pair separately for each age group. (B) Strong positive (red) and negative (blue) Spearman correlations cluster within some network pairs. (C) Quantifying the level of clustering with enrichment analyses (shown here are hypergeometric statistics, P values determined through randomization) reveals significant grouping of brain-behavior associations constrained to a minority of network pairs (●). (D) Significant differences in brain-behavior associations between age groups (McNemar χ2) were observed in a partially overlapping set of network pairs (●). (E) Only 2 functional network pairs significantly enriched at either age (Inline graphic 12 mo; Inline graphic 24 mo) also exhibit significant differences across age groups (Inline graphic).

Empirical significance levels were determined for the χ2, hypergeometric, and McNemar tests using randomization. The permutation test provides control over family-wise error rate, is nonparametric, automatically adjusts to the degree of correlation between tests, and does not make specific assumptions about the shape of the population distribution from which the observations have been derived (Backes et al. 2014). The permutation respected the correlational and missing-data patterns in the data (e.g., infants assessed at 12 months only). For each of 1 000 000 iterations, permutations—wherein complete fcMRI matrices were matched to a randomly swapped IJA score—were performed separately for the 12-mo-only group, for the 24-mo-only group, and for the set of data at both time points. The groups were then recombined for analysis. Within each iteration, asymptotic P values reflecting depletion were set to 1. This was done to explicitly focus the statistical test on the probability of observing strong enrichment. Thus, the reported brain-wide permutation-based false-positive rate of the P values reflects the empirical probability of calculating the actual observed statistical value of enrichment at the 5% false-positive rate level for the permutations. The permutation-based false-positive-rate exhibited no correlation with the number of ROIs within a network pair (Supplementary Fig. 2F,G). As an extra troubleshooting step, we also tested the within-network-pair-only false-positive rate to ensure no asymptotic P values showed up in the randomizations more than 5% of the time. Within the significantly enriched network pairs, to test whether this set of strong correlations were significantly different from zero, a 2-tailed t-test was performed. All anatomical MRI, fMRI, and fcMRI preprocessing was carried out with the Washington University in-house 4dfp-tools processing package (Pruett et al. 2015). All analyses conducted and visualizations generated were executed in MATLAB (Mathworks).

Results

This study focused on data collected in 116 infants and 98 toddlers who contributed both behavioral and imaging data at 12 and/or 24 months, of whom 37 participants contributed data at both time points (Supplementary Table 1). The 12 -mo infants and 24-mo toddlers exhibited markedly different amounts of spontaneous IJA behavior; the distribution in IJA scores reflected the emergence of IJA over the second year of life with the expected large age-related increase in IJA (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, z = −11.8, P < 3.4 × 10−32) (Fig. 1B). Thus, the relationship to brain function was assessed within and across age.

We then measured intrinsic brain functional connectivity using resting-state fcMRI in 12 mo infants and 24 mo toddlers during unsedated natural sleep. Stringent data quality control procedures minimized the presence of signal contamination due to motion-induced artifacts and ensured at least 6.25 min of high-quality MRI data. The distribution of IJA values in the participants for whom we had fcMRI data was not significantly different from that of a larger sample population (Mann–Whitney U test, 12 mo: n = 429, z = 0.60, P = 0.55; 24 mo: n = 384, z = 1.2, P = 0.24). We computed zero-lag Pearson correlation coefficients between 230 ROIs (Supplementary Fig. 1A) located throughout the cerebrum and cerebellum and adapted from task data and cortical functional parcellation studies in healthy adults (Power et al. 2011) and meta-analyses of autism studies (Philip et al. 2012).

To facilitate analyses within each age group as well as across age, we generated a cross-age network model using fcMRI data sets from n = 48 participants contributing data at both the 12- and 24-mo time points (inclusive of the 37 longitudinal subjects with IJA data; Fig. 1C,D; Supplementary Fig. 1B). This model enabled direct cross-age comparisons of brain-behavior relationships. The naming of the functional subnetworks was adapted from a previous study in healthy adults (Power et al. 2011). We also performed the brain-behavior analyses using a published adult network structure (see below; Supplementary Fig. 1C).

We tested the relationship between IJA and brain fc across subjects by calculating the Spearman correlation between IJA scores and fcMRI data for each of the 26 335 ROI pairs (Fig. 2A). Individual brain-behavior correlations were of a moderate magnitude; none passed Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. However, stronger brain-behavior correlations of matching sign generally clustered within functional network pairs (e.g., the grouping of dark blue correlation values in the Vis-DAN block at 12 mo; Fig. 2B). To quantify the level of clustering of stronger brain-behavior correlations within specific pairs of functional brain networks, we conducted the following enrichment analyses. First, strong connections were defined as ROI-pair brain-behavior correlations with uncorrected P ≤ 0.05 (Supplementary Fig. 2A). Next, 2 complementary metrics (a χ2 test and a hypergeometric test) were used to determine whether the number of strong brain-behavior correlations within each network pair was significantly greater than the number expected if the strong connections were uniformly distributed throughout the brain (Supplementary Fig. 2B,C). Empirical significance levels, using a brain-wide false-positive rejection (FPR) rate of 0.05, were determined using randomization (Supplementary Fig. 2D,E). There was no relationship between the number of ROI pairs within a network pair and the brain-wide FPR rate at 12 mo (n = 91, χ2, Pearson r = −0.15, P < 1.6 × 10−1; hypergeometric, r = −0.19, P < 7.5 × 10−2; Supplementary Fig. 2F) or at 24 mo (χ2, r = −0.05, P < 6.2 × 10−1; hypergeometric, r = −0.03, P < 7.8 × 10−1; Supplementary Fig. 2G).

Functional network pairs containing a significant clustering of strong brain-behavior correlations at 12 mo (Vis-pcDMN, hypergeometric brain-wide FPR P < 1.3 × 10−2; Vis-DAN, P < 5.4 × 10−3; SMN2-aDMN, P < 7.5 × 10−3; SMN2-SMN, P < 1.9 × 10−2) did not overlap with those observed at 24 mo (aFPC-DAN, P < 2.0 × 10−2; aFPC-aFPC, P < 3.6 × 10−2; Sal-aDMN, P < 4.3 × 10−2; Fig. 2C). Additionally, only 3 (of 91 total) functional network pairs exhibited a significantly different set of brain-behavior relationships between 12 mo and 24 mo (Fig. 2D, Vis-DAN, McNemar brain-wide FPR P < 6.5 × 10−3; Vis-pcDMN, P < 2.2 × 10−2; tDMN-tDMN, P < 5.0 × 10−2). Overall, the global set of brain-behavior relationships at the ROI-pair level were different between the 2 age groups (McNemar χ2 = 4.5, P < 1.7 × 10−2), partially driven by there being nearly 8% more strong ROI-pair brain-behavior correlations at 24 mo. From the systems-level perspective, although a different and not-overlapping set of functional network pairs were implicated at 12 mo and 24 mo, only 2 of 91 network pairs were both significantly associated with IJA at a given age and significantly different in their set of brain-behavior relationships across age: Vis-DAN and Vis-pcDMN (Fig. 2E, Supplementary Table 2).

In contrast, while the other network pairs implicated at 12 mo were not significantly enriched at 24 mo (based on the χ2 and hypergeometric tests), they did not yield different enough brain-behavior relationships to pass significance across age (McNemar brain-wide FPR; SMN2-aDMN P < 1.1 × 10−1, SMN2-SMN P < 2.1 × 10−1). This reflects a more subtle difference wherein the contributions of the connections within the network pairs to IJA become more muted over the second year of life. Similarly, in those network pairs implicated at 24 mo but neither significantly enriched at 12 mo nor different across age (i.e., aFPC-DAN P < 1.5 × 10−1, aFPC-aFPC P < 2.0 × 10−1, Sal-aDMN, P < 7.0 × 10−2), the relationships of the connections within the network pairs to IJA become strengthened over the second year of life. These discovery results (significant at one age, but not statistically different from a nonsignificant result at the other age) provide potential targets for future hypothesis-driven longitudinal studies.

In 4 of the 7 significantly enriched infant–toddler functional network pairs, general consistency was present in the signs of the brain-behavior correlations (all at 12 mo, Figs 3 and 4), whether or not the correlations were significantly positive (Vis-pcDMN [t-test of sign vs. zero, t = 9.2, P < 3.5 × 10−11], SMN2-aDMN [t = 9.2, P < 1.4 × 10−11]) or negative (Vis-DAN [t = −19.9, P < 6.8 × 10−32] and SMN2-SMN [t = −9.7, P < 1.1 × 10−11]) (Fig. 4). To explain: negative brain-behavior correlation involving 12 mo Vis-DAN ROIs means that as the brain fc decreased, the frequency of IJA increased. In contrast, positive brain-behavior correlation between Vis-pcDMN means that as the brain fc increased, the frequency of IJA also increased. Additionally, within these networks, the sign of the fcMRI correlation values themselves was also generally conserved across ROI pairs (Fig. 4). For example, at 12 mo, strong positive brain-behavior correlations were apparent in connections of ROIs between the Vis-pcDMN (Fig. 3), and the fc between those ROIs was also generally positive (orange and yellow lines in Fig. 4). This means that as the fc values increased from zero to strong and positive, presumably reflecting a significant history of in-phase (coherent) co-activation (Lewis et al. 2009), the frequency of IJA increased. This finding stood in contrast to connections between the SMN2-aDMN where most ROI pairs displayed a positive brain-behavior correlation over a negative range of fc values (green lines in Fig. 4). In this case, a history of strong antiphase co-activation (i.e., strong magnitude fc but negative in sign) correlated with fewer acts of IJA.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Strong brain-behavior relationships are largely consistent within implicated network pairs. Ball color denotes the functional networks at 12 mo (top) and 24 mo (bottom). Line color joining ROI pairs denotes the sign of the brain-behavior correlation (red—positive; blue—negative). Functional network pairs that are also significantly different in their brain-behavior associations across age are shown in boxes.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

The sign of the brain-behavior correlation between ROI pairs in implicated network pairs is highly consistent at 12 mo, but is more heterogeneous at 24 mo. Each scatter plot reveals the proportion of fcMRI data that is positive for each brain-behavior correlation value separately for each of the implicated network pairs at 12 mo (A) and at 24 mo (B). The functional connectivity between ROI pairs with strong brain-behavior correlations tends to be contained within limited regimes of both sign and magnitude. The color of the lines connecting pairs of ROIs reflects the proportion of individual fcMRI values that are above zero. Light blue to dark blue to magenta colors reflect negative  brain-behavior correlation and green to yellow to red colors denote positive brain-behavior correlation (ρ). Light blue and green denote that the ROI pair contains only negative fcMRI values between the ROI pairs. Magenta and red reflect ROI pairs with only positive fcMRI. Blue and yellow reflect fcMRI values distributed across zero. The black-dotted boxes signify that the Vis-DAN and Vis-pcDMN network pairs exhibit significantly different brain-behavior relationships across age. Data are shown only for ROI pairs with a brain-behavior correlation ρ with an associated P ≤ 0.05.

These patterns were also detectable when using a brain functional network structure derived from a previously published independent fcMRI data set of typical adults (Power et al. 2011) (Supplementary Figs 1C and 3). The adult-functional network pairs displayed a qualitatively more heterogeneous set of brain-behavior relationships than those obtained using the infant–toddler networks (presumably, because our infant–toddler-derived networks better reflect the developing brain's functional architecture for these infant–toddler brain-behavior correlation analyses). Additionally, 4 of the 7 adult-functional network pairs contain significantly different brain-behavior relationships across age, including one enriched at 24 mo (VAN-DMN).

Discussion

Here, we show that IJA is intimately and differentially related to the systems-level organization of the 12-mo and 24-mo developing brain. In particular, we found that though functional connections spanning the brain are involved in IJA, the strongest brain-behavior associations are clustered within connections between a small subset of functional brain networks. We found different sets of implicated networks at each age, with the strongest associations between visual and dorsal attention networks and between the visual network and parietal aspects of the default mode network at 12 months.

In general, a highly similar pattern emerges from both the infant–toddler and adult network models: IJA is correlated with brain fc between regions distributed throughout the cerebrum, with the strongest brain-behavior correlations clustered into a specific and small number of functional network pairs that are different across the age groups. Previous foundational task-based studies in infants using positron emission tomography (PET) (Caplan et al. 1993) or electroencephalography (EEG) (Mundy et al. 2000; Henderson et al. 2002), indicated that IJA may be supported by prefrontal areas, while groundbreaking studies in adults using novel fMRI and eye tracking paradigms (Redcay et al. 2012; Caruana et al. 2015) implicated a distributed set of regions throughout the prefrontal, temporal, and parietal cortices. These regions reflect a broad integrated set of regions that support the coordinated functioning of vision, attention, task control, and—along with subcortical and cingulate regions (Schilbach et al. 2010; Pfeiffer et al. 2014; Oberwelland et al. 2016)—motivation and reward signals. The current study expands on these observations with a brain-wide systems perspective utilizing fcMRI at time points within the period of emergence of IJA. Because of near complete absence of extant data about brain-behavior associations during this epoch of development, we chose to use data-driven methods with the power to identify unanticipated relationships between IJA and functional connectivity. The current results reveal that specific network pairs exhibiting the strongest associations with IJA within age as well as the strongest differences across age include the Vis-DAN and the Vis-pcDMN in the 12-mo age group. At 24 mo, though 8% more ROI pairs have strong correlations between fc and IJA, these strong correlations are more distributed throughout the brain, reflecting a different balance of brain function underlying the complex behavior of IJA. The present findings of strong brain-behavior relationships at either 12 mo or 24 mo (but not significantly different across age) are discovery results to be used as features for model generation for future studies.

These observed differences in functional network associations with increasing frequency of IJA across 12–24 months may reflect changes in fc driven by social experience-dependent learning that occurs over the second year of life (Tomasello et al. 2005; Canals et al. 2009; Lewis et al. 2009; Fransson et al. 2011) and the unfolding, later transition from nonverbal to verbal communication (Morales et al. 2000; Mundy et al. 2007; Kuhl 2010; Igualada et al. 2015). Additionally, these observed varying brain-behavior relationships may relate to the toddlers’ increasing capacity to use multiple strategies to initiate joint attention. Infants around 12 mo tend to use eye gaze shifts, pointing gestures, or a simple vocalization. However, by 24 mo as IJA becomes a more salient aspect of behavior, toddlers integrate these communicative domains into an actionable unit, combining eye gaze shifts, pointing gestures, and vocalizations into a single multifaceted gesture. This shift in behavioral strategies may reflect maturation of connections between default mode network regions, known to be involved in hetero-modal integration as well as processing of socially relevant stimuli and interactions, with primary visual and sensory-motor regions along with higher association areas distributed throughout the prefrontal and parietal cortex (Buckner et al. 2008; Fransson et al. 2011).

The ROIs used in the current study are well defined and have been used in the literature in typical and autism-related studies (Pruett et al. 2015), and offer a foundation for the current analysis. One limitation is that the ROIs derive from work in older subjects. In the future, the analyses described in this paper may be extended to alternate ROIs such as infant and toddler functional areal parcellations. There are significant challenges, however, to generating these, and this idea constitutes an exciting future direction.

Another limitation of the current study is that the fcMRI data were acquired while the participants were naturally sleeping and sleep stage was not monitored with EEG polysomnography. Potential differences in sleep stage may exist both within and between the age groups, complicating interpretation of the fcMRI data. Acquiring fMRI data during natural sleep provides potential insight into a population that generally is challenging to study (Dehaene-Lambertz et al. 2002; Redcay et al. 2007; Graham et al. 2015; Pruett et al. 2015). Future studies using combined fcMRI-EEG present exciting opportunities to better interpret brain function in sleeping infants and toddlers. Alternatively, given that IJA is a fundamentally interactive communication behavior, advances in wearable and minimally invasive neuroimaging technology (Hassanpour et al. 2015; Lloyd-Fox et al. 2015; Begus et al. 2016; Ferradal et al. 2016) may facilitate further advancements in study designs that use awake and directly interacting participants (Redcay et al. 2013a; Oberwelland et al. 2016). An additional limitation of the study is the potential for differences in brain-behavior relationships between high- and low-risk groups both within and across age, including the possibility that the network architecture underlying IJA may differ in children who develop ASD. However, our study was not designed or powered to address these questions. The underlying psychological and behavioral features of ASD are continuously distributed in the population (e.g., Constantino and Todd 2000; Constantino 2011; Constantino and Charman 2016). Infants with an older sibling with an ASD diagnosis (HR) are >10× more likely to be diagnosed with the disorder than infants without an older sibling or immediate relative with a positive ASD diagnosis (LR). In addition, those HR infants who do not develop autism will eventually show rates of >30% with other traits subthreshold (for autism) psychopathology. As such, this sample provided the range of behavior required to adequately test the brain-IJA relationships that we report. Though the sample is too small to test for differences within age between risk or diagnostic groups (Supplementary Table 1), this work sets the stage for future studies comparing brain-behavior relationships in larger age-specific, risk and outcome subgroups.

In summary, these observations offer a first glimpse into the functional brain structure underlying the emergence and consolidation of IJA across the second year of life. These results may inform clinical therapies by providing further physiological foundation for JA-based behavior interventions that have been shown to significantly improve outcome in children with ASD (Kasari et al. 2012; Chahrour et al. 2016). Important future directions include the examination of relationships between brain-IJA correlations and those for specific behavioral and cognitive domains of function suspected (or known) to be intimately related to IJA at different points in development (Conboy et al. 2015; Igualada et al. 2015). Our methods would enable explorations of potential overlap and nonoverlap of brain-behavior relationships for IJA and RJA (aspects of which are evolutionarily conserved [Adolphs 2003]), language (Premack 2004), and Theory of Mind (Adolphs 2003; Penn and Povinelli 2007; Krupenye et al. 2016). Related experiments could characterize the brain-behavior relationships for what may be “building blocks” of IJA-specific motor processes (pointing, head turning, gaze shifting), visuo-motor integration (pointing at what you are looking at), and aspects of visual attention (selecting the target object in the visual field). Additionally, studies utilizing dense longitudinal designs could employ these methods, using brain data taken as early as 6 months, to elucidate within-subject trajectories for normal socio-cognitive maturation that may help differentiate typical from atypical development.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Data
Supplementary Data

Notes

We wish to thank the families and children for their time and participation. We thank Sarah Hoertel for key early analyses and for guiding the brain-behavior analysis strategy. We also thank Leigh MacIntyre for managing the full IBIS database. We thank Lisa Flake and Meghan Campbell for helpful discussion about interpreting the behavior assessments. We also thank Dr Natasha Marrus, Dr Patrick Wright, Claire McKinnon, and Dr Muriah Wheelock for helpful comments on the manuscript. Conflict of Interest: The authors declare the following conflicts of interests: R.C.M. receives acting, modeling, and speaking fees from Siemens Healthcare and A.C.E. is a founder and a member of the Board of Directors of Biospective Inc. All other authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary material can be found at Cerebral Cortex Journal .

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (grant numbers K01-MH103594 to A.T.E., R01-MH093510 to J.R.P., R01-HD055741 to J.P., K01-MH101653 to J.J.W., P30-NS048056 to A.Z.S., and U54-HD087011 to J.N.C. and B.L.S.), the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience, Autism Speaks (grant number #6020 to J.P.), and the Simons Foundation (grant number #140209 to J.P.).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: J.R.P., J.T.E., J.J.W., and J.P.; Data Curation: J.T.E., J.J.W., A.M.E., L.Z., J.N.C., C.M.A., A.Z.S, S.K., K.N.B., M.A.S., G.G., R.C.M., S.D., P.K., and J.D.L.; Formal Analysis: A.T.E. and A.T.; Investigation: A.T.E., J.T.E., A.T., J.R.P., E.F., and S.K.; Methodology: A.T.E, S.E.P., E.F., A.T., J.R.P., B.L.S., and S.K.; Project Administration: J.R.P., J.T.E., J.P., K.N.B., R.C.M., J.N.C., L.Z., A.C.E., H.C.H., S.J.P., R.T.S., S.D., A.M.E., and M.A.S.; Resources: K.N.B., A.C.E., H.C.H., S.D., S.J.P., R.T.S., J.P., and J.R.P.; Software: A.T.E., A.T., J.R.P.; Supervision: K.N.B., A.C.E., H.C.H., S.D., S.J.P., R.T.S., J.P., and J.R.P.; Visualization: A.T.E.; Writing—Original Draft: A.T.E., J.T.E., J.R.P.; Writing—Review and Editing: A.T.E., J.T.E., E.F., A.T., J.J.W., C.M.A., J.D.L., A.M.E., L.Z., K.N.B., R.C.M., J.N.C., H.C.H., S.D., S.J.P., R.T.S., M.A.S., G.G., P.K., B.L.S., S.E.P., J.P., and J.R.P.; Funding Acquisition: J.P., J.R.P., A.T.E., and J.J.W.

References

  1. Adolphs R. 2003. Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci. 4:165–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association 2000. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed., Text Revision Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
  3. Backes C, Ruhle F, Stoll M, Haas J, Frese K, Franke A, Lieb W, Wichmann HE, Weis T, Kloos W, et al. . 2014. Systematic permutation testing in GWAS pathway analyses: identification of genetic networks in dilated cardiomyopathy and ulcerative colitis. BMC Genomics. 15:622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barak B, Feng G. 2016. Neurobiology of social behavior abnormalities in autism and Williams syndrome. Nat Neurosci. 19:647–655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Begus K, Lloyd-Fox S, Halliday D, Papademetriou M, Darboe MK, Prentice AM, Moore SE, Elwell CE. 2016. Using fNIRS to study working memory of infants in rural Africa. Adv Exp Med Biol. 876:273–279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bruinsma Y, Koegel RL, Koegel LK. 2004. Joint attention and children with autism: a review of the literature. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 10:169–175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. 2008. The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1124:1–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Canals S, Beyerlein M, Merkle H, Logothetis NK. 2009. Functional MRI evidence for LTP-induced neural network reorganization. Curr Biol. 19:398–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Caplan R, Chugani HT, Messa C, Guthrie D, Sigman M, de Traversay J, Mundy P. 1993. Hemispherectomy for intractable seizures: presurgical cerebral glucose metabolism and post-surgical non-verbal communication. Dev Med Child Neurol. 35:582–592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Carpenter M, Nagell K, Tomasello M. 1998. Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 63:i–vi., 1–143. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Caruana N, Brock J, Woolgar A. 2015. A frontotemporoparietal network common to initiating and responding to joint attention bids. Neuroimage. 108:34–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chahrour M, O'Roak BJ, Santini E, Samaco RC, Kleiman RJ, Manzini MC. 2016. Current perspectives in autism spectrum disorder: from genes to therapy. J Neurosci. 36:11402–11410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cohen AL, Fair DA, Dosenbach NU, Miezin FM, Dierker D, Van Essen DC, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. 2008. Defining functional areas in individual human brains using resting functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage. 41:45–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Conboy BT, Brooks R, Meltzoff AN, Kuhl PK. 2015. Social interaction in infants’ learning of second-language phonetics: an exploration of brain-behavior relations. Dev Neuropsychol. 40:216–229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Constantino JN. 2011. The quantitative nature of autistic social impairment. Pediatr Res. 69:55R–62R. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Constantino JN, Charman T. 2016. Diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: reconciling the syndrome, its diverse origins, and variation in expression. Lancet Neurol. 15:279–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Constantino JN, Todd RD. 2000. Genetic structure of reciprocal social behavior. Am J Psychiatry. 157:2043–2045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dehaene-Lambertz G, Dehaene S, Hertz-Pannier L. 2002. Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science. 298:2013–2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Elison JT, Wolff JJ, Heimer DC, Paterson SJ, Gu HB, Hazlett HC, Styner M, Gerig G, Piven J, IBIS_Network . 2013. Frontolimbic neural circuitry at 6 months predicts individual differences in joint attention at 9 months. Dev Sci. 16:186–197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Emery NJ. 2000. The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 24:581–604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Ferradal SL, Liao SM, Eggebrecht AT, Shimony JS, Inder TE, Culver JP, Smyser CD. 2016. Functional imaging of the developing brain at the bedside using diffuse optical tomography. Cereb Cortex. 26:1558–1568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fonov V, Evans AC, Botteron K, Almli CR, McKinstry RC, Collins DL. 2011. Unbiased average age-appropriate atlases for pediatric studies. Neuroimage. 54:313–327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fransson P, Aden U, Blennow M, Lagercrantz H. 2011. The functional architecture of the infant brain as revealed by resting-state fMRI. Cereb Cortex. 21:145–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Friston KJ, Williams S, Howard R, Frackowiak RS, Turner R. 1996. Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magn Reson Med. 35:346–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gao W, Zhu H, Giovanello KS, Smith JK, Shen D, Gilmore JH, Lin W. 2009. Evidence on the emergence of the brain's default network from 2-week-old to 2-year-old healthy pediatric subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 106:6790–6795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gotham K, Risi S, Pickles A, Lord C. 2007. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: revised algorithms for improved diagnostic validity. J Autism Dev Disord. 37:613–627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Graham AM, Pfeifer JH, Fisher PA, Lin W, Gao W, Fair DA. 2015. The potential of infant fMRI research and the study of early life stress as a promising exemplar. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 12:12–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hassanpour MS, Eggebrecht AT, Culver JP, Peelle JE. 2015. Mapping cortical responses to speech using high-density diffuse optical tomography. Neuroimage. 117:319–326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hazlett HC, Gu H, McKinstry RC, Shaw DW, Botteron KN, Dager SR, Styner M, Vachet C, Gerig G, Paterson SJ, et al. . 2012. Brain volume findings in 6-month-old infants at high familial risk for autism. Am J Psychiatry. 169:601–608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Henderson LM, Yoder PJ, Yale ME, McDuffie A. 2002. Getting the point: electrophysiological correlates of protodeclarative pointing. Int J Dev Neurosci. 20:449–458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Igualada A, Bosch L, Prieto P. 2015. Language development at 18 months is related to multimodal communicative strategies at 12 months. Infant Behav Dev. 39:42–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kasari C, Gulsrud A, Freeman S, Paparella T, Hellemann G. 2012. Longitudinal follow-up of children with autism receiving targeted interventions on joint attention and play. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 51:487–495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Khatri P, Sirota M, Butte AJ. 2012. Ten years of pathway analysis: current approaches and outstanding challenges. PLoS Comput Biol. 8:e1002375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Krupenye C, Kano F, Hirata S, Call J, Tomasello M. 2016. Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science. 354:110–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kuhl PK. 2010. Brain mechanisms in early language acquisition. Neuron. 67:713–727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lewis CM, Baldassarre A, Committeri G, Romani GL, Corbetta M. 2009. Learning sculpts the spontaneous activity of the resting human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 106:17558–17563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lin W, Zhu Q, Gao W, Chen Y, Toh CH, Styner M, Gerig G, Smith JK, Biswal B, Gilmore JH. 2008. Functional connectivity MR imaging reveals cortical functional connectivity in the developing brain. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 29:1883–1889. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lloyd-Fox S, Szeplaki-Kollod B, Yin J, Csibra G. 2015. Are you talking to me? Neural activations in 6-month-old infants in response to being addressed during natural interactions. Cortex. 70:35–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Mathias A, Grond F, Guardans R, Seese D, Canela M, Diebner HH. 2004. Algorithms for spectral analysis of irregularly sampled time series. J Stat Softw. 11:1–27. [Google Scholar]
  40. Morales M, Mundy P, Delgado CEF, Yale M, Messinger D, Neal R, Schwartz HK. 2000. Responding to joint attention across the 6- through 24-month age period and early language acquisition. J Appl Dev Psychol. 21:283–298. [Google Scholar]
  41. Mundy P, Block J, Delgado C, Pomares Y, Van Hecke AV, Parlade MV. 2007. Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Dev. 78:938–954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Mundy P, Card J, Fox N. 2000. EEG correlates of the development of infant joint attention skills. Dev Psychobiol. 36:325–338. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Mundy P, Jarrold W. 2010. Infant joint attention, neural networks and social cognition. Neural Netw. 23:985–997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Oberwelland E, Schilbach L, Barisic I, Krall SC, Vogeley K, Fink GR, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Konrad K, Schulte-Ruther M. 2016. Look into my eyes: Investigating joint attention using interactive eye-tracking and fMRI in a developmental sample. Neuroimage. 130:248–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Ojemann JG, Akbudak E, Snyder AZ, McKinstry RC, Raichle ME, Conturo TE. 1997. Anatomic localization and quantitative analysis of gradient refocused echo-planar fMRI susceptibility artifacts. Neuroimage. 6:156–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Okamoto-Barth S, Moore C, Barth J, Subiaul F, Povinelli DJ. 2011. Carryover effect of joint attention to repeated events in chimpanzees and young children. Dev Sci. 14:440–452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Parikshak NN, Gandal MJ, Geschwind DH. 2015. Systems biology and gene networks in neurodevelopmental and neurodegenerative disorders. Nat Rev Genet. 16:441–458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Penn DC, Povinelli DJ. 2007. On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a ‘theory of mind’. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 362:731–744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Petersen SE, Posner MI. 2012. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu Rev Neurosci. 35:73–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Pfeiffer UJ, Schilbach L, Timmermans B, Kuzmanovic B, Georgescu AL, Bente G, Vogeley K. 2014. Why we interact: on the functional role of the striatum in the subjective experience of social interaction. Neuroimage. 101:124–137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Philip RC, Dauvermann MR, Whalley HC, Baynham K, Lawrie SM, Stanfield AC. 2012. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the fMRI investigation of autism spectrum disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 36:901–942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. 2012. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 59:2142–2154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Barnes KA, Church JA, Vogel AC, Laumann TO, Miezin FM, Schlaggar BL, et al. . 2011. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron. 72:665–678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Power JD, Mitra A, Laumann TO, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. 2014. Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. 84:320–341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Premack D. 2004. Psychology. Is language the key to human intelligence. Science. 303:318–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Pruett JR Jr, Kandala S, Hoertel S, Snyder AZ, Elison JT, Nishino T, Feczko E, Dosenbach NUF, Nardos B, Power JD, et al. . 2015. Accurate age classification of 6 and 12 month-old infants based on resting-state functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging data. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 12:123–133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Redcay E, Dodell-Feder D, Mavros PL, Kleiner M, Pearrow MJ, Triantafyllou C, Gabrieli JD, Saxe R. 2013. a. Atypical brain activation patterns during a face-to-face joint attention game in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. 34:2511–2523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Redcay E, Kennedy DP, Courchesne E. 2007. fMRI during natural sleep as a method to study brain function during early childhood. Neuroimage. 38:696–707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Redcay E, Kleiner M, Saxe R. 2012. Look at this: the neural correlates of initiating and responding to bids for joint attention. Front Hum Neurosci. 6:169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Redcay E, Moran JM, Mavros PL, Tager-Flusberg H, Gabrieli JD, Whitfield-Gabrieli S. 2013. b. Intrinsic functional network organization in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Front Hum Neurosci. 7:573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Richiardi J, Altmann A, Milazzo AC, Chang C, Chakravarty MM, Banaschewski T, Barker GJ, Bokde AL, Bromberg U, Buchel C, et al. . 2015. BRAIN NETWORKS. Correlated gene expression supports synchronous activity in brain networks. Science. 348:1241–1244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Rivals I, Personnaz L, Taing L, Potier MC. 2007. Enrichment or depletion of a GO category within a class of genes: which test. Bioinformatics. 23:401–407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Rosvall M, Bergstrom CT. 2008. Maps of random walks on complex networks reveal community structure. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 105:1118–1123. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov.beckerproxy.wustl.edu/pubmed/18216267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Satterthwaite TD, Wolf DH, Ruparel K, Erus G, Elliott MA, Eickhoff SB, Gennatas ED, Jackson C, Prabhakaran K, Smith A, et al. . 2013. Heterogeneous impact of motion on fundamental patterns of developmental changes in functional connectivity during youth. Neuroimage. 83:45–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Scaife M, Bruner JS. 1975. The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature. 253:265–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Schilbach L, Wilms M, Eickhoff SB, Romanzetti S, Tepest R, Bente G, Shah NJ, Fink GR, Vogeley K. 2010. Minds made for sharing: initiating joint attention recruits reward-related neurocircuitry. J Cogn Neurosci. 22:2702–2715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Smyser CD, Inder TE, Shimony JS, Hill JE, Degnan AJ, Snyder AZ, Neil JJ. 2010. Longitudinal analysis of neural network development in preterm infants. Cereb Cortex. 20:2852–2862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Smyser CD, Snyder AZ, Neil JJ. 2011. Functional connectivity MRI in infants: exploration of the functional organization of the developing brain. Neuroimage. 56:1437–1452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Tomasello M, Carpenter M. 2007. Shared intentionality. Dev Sci. 10:121–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H. 2005. Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav Brain Sci. 28:675–691. discussion 691-735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Toulmin H, Beckmann CF, O'Muircheartaigh J, Ball G, Nongena P, Makropoulos A, Ederies A, Counsell SJ, Kennea N, Arichi T, et al. . 2015. Specialization and integration of functional thalamocortical connectivity in the human infant. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 112:6485–6490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Tsodyks M, Kenet T, Grinvald A, Arieli A. 1999. Linking spontaneous activity of single cortical neurons and the underlying functional architecture. Science. 286:1943–1946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Van Dijk KR, Sabuncu MR, Buckner RL. 2012. The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage. 59:431–438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Wetherby AM, Allen L, Cleary J, Kublin K, Goldstein H. 2002. Validity and reliability of the communication and symbolic behavior scales developmental profile with very young children. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 45:1202–1218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Wolff JJ, Gu H, Gerig G, Elison JT, Styner M, Gouttard S, Botteron KN, Dager SR, Dawson G, Estes AM, et al. . 2012. Differences in white matter fiber tract development present from 6 to 24 months in infants with autism. Am J Psychiatry. 169:589–600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Data
Supplementary Data

Articles from Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY) are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES