Abstract
It has been 30 years since Holzman introduced a special issue of the Schizophrenia Bulletin entitled “Thought Disorder in Schizophrenia.” He pointed out in his Editor’s Introduction that in contrast to the explosion of interest at that time in the biological aspects of schizophrenia, there were important areas of study that represented “... relatively neglected aspects of the psychopathology of schizophrenia, namely the varieties of thinking disorders (emphasis added) characteristic of schizophrenic patients and their possible underlying mechanisms.” Perhaps presciently, he ended his introduction by expressing hope that the articles included in that issue would lead to further intensive study of the cognitive (emphasis added) dysfunctions in schizophrenia. There has, indeed, been extensive research conducted in further understanding cognitive dysfunctions in schizophrenia, but considerably less so in understanding thought disorder.
Keywords: thought disorder, schizophrenia, categorical, dimensional
We emphasize in this introduction, as did Holzman1 30 years ago, the importance of thought disorder and hope this issue of the Schizophrenia Bulletin will stimulate further interest in what is still a relatively neglected area. Unlike Holzman, however, we suggest that thought disorder may ultimately be best studied as a dimensional, cross-diagnostic phenomenon that will have broad implications for understanding and treating psychopathology.
Rethinking Thought Disorder
Thought is a complex phenomenon, through which our understanding of self, world, and reality is constructed. Connected to our capacity for language, thought is critical to communication and plays an important role in how we understand others and make our own experiences known. Breakdowns in the thought system (eg, how one perceives, interprets, structures, and responds to information) is intrinsically linked to difficulties with psychological and social wellbeing and the ability to function adaptively in the world. At their extreme, these disturbances are the core of psychotic experiences.
Thought disorder (TD) is a multidimensional construct, reflecting peculiarities in thinking, language, and communication. Broadly, TD is defined as any disturbance that affects the form of thinking, including the organization, control, processing, or expression of thoughts. Given the breadth of the construct, TD has been defined and classified in a number of different ways. Different perspectives have placed varying emphasis on features related to the contextual appropriateness of ideas, the way in which they are organized, and the language used to express them.
History of Thought Disorder
Disturbances in thought have been central to the conceptualization of psychosis since Kraepelin2 introduced the diagnostic construct, dementia praecox, which categorized psychoses that presented relatively early in life and resulted in progressive deterioration of mental functions. For Kraepelin, this deterioration was evidenced by “derailments” and “incoherence” of thought processes and was observable in the speech of patients.
When Bleuler3 reformulated the dementia praecox construct, he emphasized “splitting” of mental functions as the conceptual core of schizophrenia, describing a process in which the psychological force that holds together facets of the psyche—perception, affect, memory, thought, behavior—breaks down. A primary manifestation of splitting was disturbance of thought, which he inferred through observing the speech of patients. He described subtle phenomenological differences in the form of thought disturbances, describing processes in which “the most important determinant of the associations is lacking the concept of purpose3 (p. 15)” and others in which “associations do not become entirely senseless, but they still appear odd, bizarre, distorted3 (p. 19).” For Bleuler, clinical heterogeneity, and the dimensionality it suggested, was critical to understanding the nature of psychosis. He believed that the accurate measurement and classification of thought disturbances would elucidate the mechanisms underlying psychosis.
With the evolution of our modern diagnostic system, other symptoms displaced the centrality of TD to psychosis. This shift was hastened by publication of the DSM-III, which redefined schizophrenia in terms of observable, polythetic criteria, namely hallucinations and delusions. The complexity and conceptual significance of thought disturbances was further reduced with the revision of schizophrenia in the DSM-IV, referred to as simply “disorganized speech.” In short, TD was reduced by fiat to a subsidiary role in schizophrenia and categorical diagnosis was established as the gold standard.
Contemporary Approach to Diagnosis and Thought Disorder
The movement toward a dimensional approach to diagnosis, the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC)4 is consistent with TD as a dimensional, multifaceted set of features that present both transdiagnostically and in psychologically well individuals. Although TD is the central defining feature of psychosis, it does not map neatly onto specific diagnostic categories, but may have greater specificity in terms of etiology, course, and treatment. Disturbances in thinking represent a unique and promising pathway through which to understand the nature of psychotic phenomena as well as their subclinical representations. Exploring this from a dimensional perspective has the potential to contribute to a more comprehensive and integrative model of psychosis, which has implications for both science and clinical applications.
Theories of Thought Disorder
In line with the associationist paradigm that predominated psychology at the turn of the last century, Kraepelin and Bleuler conceptualized TD as a result of disruptions in the interconnectivity of ideas. Over the past century, efforts to explain the nature of TD and its significance in psychosis have been theoretically diverse.
Cognitive-Developmental
Language and thought are abilities that we develop over time and thus, it is not surprising that disturbances in these functions have been explained from a developmental perspective. Piaget 5 saw schizophrenia as a result of disruptions in the process by which children acquire knowledge and construct an understanding of the world. He believed normative cognitive development occurred over 4 consecutive stages, namely sensorimotor, preoperational, operational, and concrete operational. For Piaget, in schizophrenia, the establishment of object permanence and differentiation from the mother was disrupted in the preoperational stage, leaving the child unable to move past primary egocentricity. As a result, boundaries between self and other, subjective and objective are never fully established. Further, this affected development in later stages, which Piaget believed was evident in a disrupted ability to categorize, common in those diagnosed with schizophrenia.
Vygotsky’s6 view of schizophrenia paralleled that of Piaget, but emphasized the sociocultural context of cognitive development. He saw the development of a child’s cognition as a largely social process, founded in cooperative and collaborative dialogue with caregivers. This was at the core of his theory of psychosis, in which he described “the fragmentation and the breaking of that part of the psyche which is involved in the process of formation of concepts is just as characteristic of schizophrenia as the development of the function of formation of concepts is characteristic of adolescence”6 (p. 1063).
Psychodynamic
TD from a psychodynamic perspective (drive theory, ego psychology, object relations, and self-psychology) is largely inaccessible as it represents an unspecified failure of a complex array of mental processes that form representations or “internal working models”7 of self and other in the context of affect. Emerging early in an individual’s development, these internal cognitive models affect, among other functions, language patterns.8
Examined most frequently in the context of personality disorder, TD from this perspective can be construed as an impairment in a cognitive process that leads to fragmented aspects of the self. That is, TD is a loss of cognitive stability underlying the integrated self, such as a failure to inhibit primary process thinking, resulting in an intrusion of uninhibited drive material (eg, libidinal, aggressive, etc.) Some believe this represents regression to an earlier form of thinking, in which internal representations of the self and other become blurred and fragmented, an idea that could be construed as an interpretation of the earlier cognitive hypotheses.9,10 Of particular note here is that TD relied on a broad range of behavior observed and interpreted by clinicians.
Social Learning
The origins of TD have also been conceptualized from a social-learning perspective, focused on how interpersonal transactions shape the way in which we perceive and make sense of surroundings, form concepts, process and modulate affect, and construct an understanding of the self and others. When early relationships do not provide the opportunity to establish a shared perspective or present inaccurate or inconsistent feedback about perceptual and emotional experience, the child may not develop an organized, stable representation of the self and world.
Singer and Wynne11 posited the role of familial communication patterns in shaping the development of TD, and psychosis more broadly. Their work was based on a social-transactional hypothesis of TD; as explained by Wynne et al12 “the fragmentation of experience, the identity diffusion, the disturbed modes of perception and communication, and certain other characteristics of the acute reactive schizophrenic’s personality structure are to a significant extent derived, by processes of internalization, from characteristics of the family social organization. Also internalized are the ways of thinking and of deriving meaning, the points of anxiety, and the irrationality, confusion, and ambiguity that were expressed in the shared mechanisms of the family social organization.”
Their research explored how unusual ways in which parents perceived, interpreted, and reasoned about the world interfered with their ability to establish shared attention and construct mutual meaning with their children. Wynne and Singer posited that these disturbed social interactions compromised the child’s development of cohesive, stable mental representations of the self and world, thereby putting them at increased risk of psychosis. They identified a pattern of communication that reliably predicted the later emergence and severity of psychosis in children, which was characterized as vague, fragmented, and contradictory, features recognized in contemporary measures of TD (reviewed below). Interestingly, subtle hindrances to shared meaning were more psychologically detrimental to the listener than overt disruptions.
Familial Basis of Thought Disorder
The familial basis of TD has been well evidenced, not only from a social-learning perspective, but from a genetic perspective. This has been supported by evidence of an over-represented aggregation of TD within families.13,14 Familial-high risk studies have documented TD in children and adolescents who go on to develop psychotic disorders.15–19 Further, TD is common in clinically unaffected relatives of manic and schizophrenic patients, differentiating them from relatives of healthy controls.20–22 Interestingly, the type and severity of TD has been shown to cluster in families.22,23
Adoption studies have helped to disentangle the genetic and environmental contributions to TD. Wahlberg and colleagues24 examined the likelihood of TD in adoptees, based on genetic-risk (presence or absence of psychotic disorder in biological mother) and communication deviance (CD) in the adoptive parents. Results suggested a significant interaction effect, with high-risk adoptees in high CD environments exhibiting the greatest TD. Interestingly, high-risk adoptees in low CD families had significantly lower TD, suggesting potential protective effects of environment. Results have also demonstrated the co-familiality of TD independent of shared environmental factors. In a subsequent study, Wahlberg and colleagues25 replicated these results using the TDI, finding that the interaction between high-risk status and CD in the adoptive parents distinctly predicted idiosyncratic thinking in adoptees. Taken together, results from family and adoption studies suggest that both genetic and environmental influences are likely involved in the development of TD.
Classification and Measurement
Bleuler used the sentence, “The mountains which are outlined in the swellings of oxygen are beautiful” as a clear depiction of deviant use of language (ref.14). While the peculiarity of this statement is largely agreed upon, how to describe and define it is a more complex issue. The term TD reflects a broad construct, encompassing diverse conceptual and nosological perspectives cutting across domains of thought, language, and speech. While the study of TD has a rich and lengthy history, consensus has not been reached on what constitutes disordered TD or how it should be classified and measured. A variety of approaches have been used to elicit and assess thought disturbances, including interviews with rating scales, psychodiagnostic tests, and count-based metrics. With this, questions have been raised as to the degree of intricacy with which to measure TD, specifically related to capturing the complexity of language phenomena vs maximizing inter-rater reliability, minimizing subjectivity, burden of learning, administering, and scoring.
Thought, Language, and Communication
The study of “TD” assumes that thought processes can be accurately inferred from speech. This assumption has been challenged by the view that thinking can occur independently of language, suggesting that they are separate domains.26 Studies of language acquisition have supported this; eg, the ability for conceptual thinking has been found to exist in deaf children prior to the attainment of language. The linguist Chaika and Lambe27 has held that psychotic speech is solely reflective of language deficits and does not imply a disturbance in underlying thought processes. This view is supported by observations of lexical and syntactical errors in schizophrenic patients that are also seen in healthy individuals at a lower level.28 Furthermore, individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia who exhibit delusional and bizarre thinking are often able to articulate these thoughts using clear and conventional language.29 This point raises additional questions about the interplay between thought and language, while introducing further complexities to assessment modalities.
Assessment Methods
Rorschach and Rapaport.
A variety of approaches have been developed to measure TD, including reasoning tasks, clinician rating scales, and self-report measures. Perhaps the most widely used medium has been the Rorschach inkblot test, which has a rich history in the measurement of disordered thinking, has been the basis of several scoring systems, and has, paradoxically, even received strong support for this usage from a major critic of the Rorschach.30 The Rorschach lends itself well to TD assessment, as it is a relatively standardized technique that allows examinees to interpret and structure the task freely, minimizing administration variability while maximizing individual response variability.21 Its projective nature offers a glimpse into how an individual perceives, interprets, and responds to ambiguous stimuli.
The use of the Rorschach in assessing thinking disturbances can be traced back to Rorschach himself, who observed a characteristic style of responding reflected in the protocols of those with psychosis. He described a tendency for these individuals to formulate responses based on absurd, narrow details of the blot, ignoring typical determinants of form, color, and shading, and often ascribing idiosyncratic or personal meaning to perceptual features.31 Building on Rorschach’s work, Rapaport32 sought to develop a more formal system for classifying thought disturbances, which he referred to as “deviant verbalizations.” He believed that adaptive thinking was a function of the integration of perceptual (eg, blot features) and associative (eg, internal ideas, memories, feelings) processes. The relative pull of one process over the other resulted in either a loss or increase of appropriate “distance” from the task, signifying thought disturbances. A loss of perceptual distance reflected a tendency to see the blot as too real, while an increase in perceptual distance was associated with an overly symbolic view of percepts.
Thought Disorder Index.
Rapaport’s system became the basis for the Delta Index,33 the first standardized assessment to exclusively measure disordered thinking. The Delta Index included 15 of Rapaport’s 21 scoring categories, each of which was assigned a 4-point level of severity. The Delta Index was innovative in that it captured the multifaceted and continuous nature of TD. For this reason, Johnston and Holzman, who developed the Thought Disorder Index, revised the scale decades later.
The Thought Disorder Index21 (TDI) provides a system for identifying, categorizing, and evaluating the severity of disordered thinking as expressed in language. TDI scoring can be based on any verbal sample, including the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), but is most commonly derived from verbatim responses to the Rorschach Inkblot Test.21 As suggested previously, the Rorschach is believed to elicit greater instances of TD than more structured methods,21 such as interviews (eg, the Scale for the Assessment of Thought, Language, and Communication26) and nonprojective tests (eg, WAIS).
The TDI specifies 23 categories of thought disturbances, most of which are based on Rappaport’s original classification. Because TD is understood as existing on a continuum, each category is assigned to a level of severity, ranging from mild to severe (0.25, 0.50, 0.75, and 1.00), mirroring the structure of the Delta Index. The 0.25 level reflects subtle instances of cognitive slippage, which are occasionally observed in healthy individuals, particularly in times of anxiety, stress, and fatigue.21 Disturbances at the 0.50 level “convey[s] an impression of loss of mooring, shaky reality contact, emotional overreaction, and distinct oddness”21 (p. 490). Significant instability in thinking and perceiving is represented at the 0.75 level, while responses at the 1.0 level indicate a complete break from reality.
In developing the TDI, Johnston and Holzman21 distinguished 4 qualitative dimensions of TD based on the conceptual relatedness of the individual categories. These include: associative looseness, in which responses appear to be driven by internal processes instead of demands of the task; combinatory thinking, in which percepts, ideas, or images are joined in an inappropriate, incongruous, or unrealistic manner; disorganized responses, in which a lack of clarity of thought and sense of confusion are displayed; deviant verbalizations, in which word usage is odd, idiosyncratic, or undecipherable.
A subsequent factor analysis yielded 6 discrete factors, many of which overlapped with the original, conceptually derived dimensions.22 These “empiric factors” included: (1) combinatory thinking, (2) idiosyncratic verbalizations, (3) autistic thinking, (4) fluid thinking, (5) absurdity, and (6) confusion.
The TDI is a highly sensitive measure of TD and thus is able to detect subtle disturbances in language that may be overlooked by other methods. This is facilitated by the scoring protocol, as ratings are based on written transcriptions of verbatim samples, which allows for systematic analysis of thought disturbances, in terms of qualitative form and severity.
Bizarre-Idiosyncratic Thinking.
Harrow and Quinlan34 introduced the notion of “bizarre-idiosyncratic thinking” to describe a diverse set of language behaviors associated with formal TD. They defined bizarre-idiosyncratic thinking as: (1) unique to the particular subject; (2) deviant with respect to conventional social norms; (3) frequently hard to understand, or to empathize with, in the context from which the response arose; (4) may appear confused, contradictory, or illogical; (5) may involve sudden or unexpected contrasts; and (6) are usually inappropriate in relation to the task at hand. Initially, Harrow and Quinlan rated these domains of disordered thinking based on responses to the Rorschach,35,36 but later moved away from this due to the demanding and time-consuming nature of Rorschach administration, opting instead for verbal samples derived from free-verbalization techniques.
Scale for the Assessment of Thought, Language, and Communication.
Alternative approaches to assessing TD have been developed that are not as methodologically rigorous as the TDI. Of these measures, the Scale for the Assessment of Thought, Language, and Communication26,37,38 (TLC) is perhaps the most widely used clinician-rated assessment of TD in both research and clinical practice. Its development was undertaken as part of the broader objective to establish a standard set of TD subtypes for inclusion in the glossary of the DSM-III. Subtypes were identified and defined based strictly on clinical observation, with no assumptions of underlying etiological mechanisms. This atheoretical approach was assumed deliberately in the service of designing an instrument with high inter-rater reliability and clinical utility.26 The original definitions were piloted in a small sample of patients (n = 44) and subsequently revised to improve clarity. The severity of each item is rated on a 4-or 5-point scale, ranging from “absent” to “severe.” These anchor points are defined quantitatively (eg, speech behavior occurs 5–10 times during interview) and are item specific. In addition to the relative severity of each item, TD subtypes are identified as “more pathological” (eg, poverty of speech, pressure of speech, clanging) or “less pathological” (eg, circumstantiality, perseveration, blocking).39 Thus, items are not equally weighted in the determination of global TD severity.
Several studies have examined the factor structure of the TLC, with mixed results. In her early examination of the TLC, Andreasen37 conducted an exploratory factor analysis of 12 TLC items, which yielded a single “verbosity” factor, on which derailment, illogicality, loss of goal, perseveration, incoherence, and pressure of speech loaded positively and poverty of speech loaded negatively. These results were interpreted as evidence for distinct “positive” (florid) and “negative” (diminished) dimensions of TD. In a subsequent factor analysis of the complete TLC, Andreasen and Grove39 reported 3 distinct domains, fluent disorganization, emptiness, and linguistic control. A comparable 3-factor model was generated from an exploratory factor analysis of 8 TLC items23 and later replicated through confirmatory factor analysis.40 Interestingly, however, the inclusion of all 18 TLC items has typically revealed a more complex factor structure, with 6–7 distinct dimensions.41–43
Communication Disturbances Index.
The Communication Disturbances Index (CDI) assesses communication disturbance in natural speech in terms of categorization and severity without attempting to separate language from thought. The CDI is based in the assumption that both thought and language are cognitive processes that occur together and, therefore, can be measured as such.44,45 Unlike some measures of formal TD, the CDI is sensitive to low levels of disturbances, including minor communicative errors. This increases the CDI’s utility in identifying those in premorbid and prodromal phases of schizophrenia.
The theoretical basis for the CDI came from linguistic concepts pioneered in the works Cohesion in English46 and Crazy Talk,47 which focused on the link between speech and “real-world” referents.28,44 Prominent in both compositions is the idea of cohesion, which drives successful communication through the effective synthesis of speech and its accurate reference to the outside world.48 Additionally, earlier studies on linguistics by Docherty and colleagues44,49–51 helped to inform the construction of the CDI. Each of these 4 studies looked at speech samples from a schizophrenic sample and assessed variables such as unclear and incompetent references in addition to administering established measures of formal TD. Using the aforementioned theoretical framework and research-based information, 6 categories of communication failure were created as part of the CDI; they are: vague references, confused references, missing information references, ambiguous word meanings, wrong word references, and grammatical unclarities.44,52
Thought Disorder Measurement: Dimensional Vs Categorical.
It is important to distinguish between the qualitative nature of the various scales/ratings and how those scales/ratings are used in data analyses. All of the measures reviewed above are dimensional in nature. Whether based on a clinician’s observation or the counting of instances of a form of TD, numbers are assigned that represent ordinal differences among levels of TD. A “1” reflects less TD than a “3” which in turn reflects less TD than a “5.” That is, the numbers in all measurement systems capture a dimensional severity scaling. The data used to generate the numbers can vary widely, for example, from global ratings of alogia to the number of peculiar instances of word usage. It is important, therefore, to remember that while all measures provide dimensional numerical indices of TD severity, the underlying behavioral data upon which those numbers are based can be vastly different. Finally, it is often the case that dimensional scores will be used to create categorical distinctions. It is not uncommon to find that schizophrenia patients are divided into those “with” and “without” TD for purposes of statistical analysis. This can lend a deceptive sense of differentness between groups that belies the continuous nature of the TD measurement and the “fuzziness” of boundaries among groups.53
Diagnostic Specificity of Thought Disorder
For much of the 20th century, TD was widely accepted as a schizophrenia-specific feature and studied within this context. This perspective shifted in the 1970s, with recognition that thought disturbances were also prevalent in mania, spurring considerable efforts to identify patterns of TD that differentiated among diagnostic groups. The earliest of these studies, which assessed TD using abstract reasoning tasks (eg, Gorham’s Proverbs Test), demonstrated that under-inclusive thinking tended to characterize schizophrenia, while over-inclusive thinking was more typical of mania.54,55 Using this approach, depressed patients have been shown to exhibit deficits characterized by under-inclusive and concrete thinking compared to controls, although to a lesser degree than schizophrenia patients.56,57
Similar patterns were found in a series of later studies using the TLC, with poverty of speech and speech content (ie, negative TD) common to schizophrenia, and pressured speech, tangentiality, incoherence, illogicality, and loss of goal characterizing mania (ie, positive TD).37,39,49,58,59 However, inconsistent evidence has also been reported, with several studies finding greater tangentiality, looseness, and illogicality in schizophrenia compared to mania.41,60,61 Schizoaffective patients were included in only 2 studies, both of which evidenced TD profiles comparable to manic patients.39,61 Examining differences by schizophrenia subtype, Andreasen and Grove39 reported a significantly higher prevalence of poverty of speech content in patients with hebephrenic vs paranoid schizophrenia.39
The presence of negative TD has also been observed in patients with primary depression, although overall TLC severity tends to be considerably lower than that in schizophrenia and mania.37,61,62 Further, patients diagnosed with psychotic depression tend to exhibit significantly greater alogia, poverty of content, blocking, and perseveration than their nonpsychotic counterparts.62
A series of studies by Holzman and colleagues examined the form and severity of TD in schizophrenia, mania, and schizoaffective disorder using the TDI.21,22,63,64 Results revealed disorder-specific patterns of TD, in which schizophrenia was characterized by “fluid thinking, interpenetrations of one idea by another, unstable verbal referents, and overly concise and contracted communications which give the impression of inner turmoil and confusion”63 (p. 369). Conversely, manic thought processes were described as “loosely tied together ideas that are excessively and immoderately combined and elaborated” with “a playful, mirthful, and breezy quality to their productions”63 (p. 369). Contrary to the findings of Andreasen and Grove,39 Johnston and Holzman21 reported no significant differences in TDI scores between paranoid and nonparanoid patients.
The pattern of TD in schizoaffective patients was less consistent, as significant differences were found between those in manic and depressed states. Schizoaffective manic patients exhibited a high level of combinatory thinking comparable to manic patients. However, they lacked the characteristic flippancy and humor of the manic patients and resembled the schizophrenic patients in terms of the frequency of idiosyncratic verbalizations, confusion, and autistic logic. The schizoaffective-depressed patients were similar to the schizophrenic group in terms of frequency of absurd responses and relatively constricted protocol length. However, they had very low levels of total TD, with their overall TDI scores resembling those of a healthy comparison group. In contrast, their rate of absurd responses was similar to that of the schizophrenic group.
Despite evidence of differences in qualitative form and severity across disorders, the variability in TD presentation is not fully explained by diagnostic groupings. Comparative studies of TD consistently report significant within-group variance (ie, large standard deviations) in TDI total and factor scores, across diagnostic and control groups. This point was empirically demonstrated using discriminant-function analysis, which found that TDI total scores correctly classified only 63.0% of manic and schizophrenic patients.64 Qualitative factors were shown to more accurately differentiate groups, as a subset of 5 empirically derived categories (irrelevant intrusions, combinatory thinking, fluid thinking, confusion, and idiosyncratic verbalization) correctly classified 76.5% of the sample. However, when re-examined in a subsequent study that also included a schizoaffective subsample, the accuracy rate of the same factors dropped to 57.7%.22
Course and Chronicity.
The course of TD is highly variable across individuals. Diagnosis has been shown to account for a portion of this variance, with TD in schizophrenia being more stable and persistent than in schizoaffective disorder and affective psychoses.65–67 TD in mania has been described as reversible,39 given the observation that disturbances are typically severe during acute phases of illness (eg, hospital admission) and remit completely following treatment.39,49,66,68 However, remission of positive TD has also been observed in schizophrenic patients,39 suggesting that some subtypes tend to follow an episodic course, independent of diagnosis. Forms of TD that are characteristic of schizophrenia have been shown to have greater stability over time, including more severe level disturbances (ie, 0.50, 0.75) on the TDI17,67 and idiosyncratic verbalizations.17,18,57,68
Summary.
Research has documented differences in the form and severity of TD across diagnostic groups. Broadly, thinking in schizophrenia has been characterized by odd, impoverished, and internally driven speech. Classically “schizophrenic” TD tends to have greater stability over time, with increases in severity during acute phases of illness. The disconnected and elaborated thinking disturbances seen in mania tend to be episodic, emerging during acute phases and after remitting partially or completely with treatment. In contrast, measures of thought disorganization (eg, vagueness, perseveration, inappropriate distance, confusion, looseness, fluidity, absurd responses, and incoherence) appear to reflect a more stable trait. It has been posited that the stable thought disturbances associated with schizophrenia are pathognomonic of underlying pathophysiology, while state-related forms are secondary to clinical and situational factors.14,25
Despite the high degree of within-subject variability across studies, these data demonstrate a clear association between TD and diagnosis. We would note, however, that the association between TD and diagnosis is heavily influenced by the course and/or reversibility of the thought disturbance. That a type of TD may remit does not mean it is ipso facto TD. To the extent that we rely on the course of a symptom to determine if it should qualify as a symptom reflects a logical error (post hoc ergo propter hoc.)
Given that TD can vary over time, it is possible that the studies of TD reviewed above reflect an association between TD and certain clinical or personal features related to diagnosis, such as symptoms, medication, or social functioning. In this case, diagnosis contains important information about the nature of TD that warrants further study. We review below some of the potential influences on TD.
Implications and Future Directions
Bleuler saw TD as the core defining feature of psychotic phenomena, reflective of the “splitting of the psychic functions” that occurred when, in the process of thinking, one’s ideas and feelings disconnect, becoming fragmented and competing functions. This view was echoed by Meehl,69 who called TD the “diagnostic bell ringer” for schizophrenia, which for him, was exemplified by the comment, “naturally I’m growing my father’s hair.” While Meehl cautioned that the presence of a single symptom is inadequate grounds on which to infer a nosology, he saw TD as a “rare exception,” which itself was pathognomonic of schizophrenia.
Interest in TD as the conceptual core of psychosis diminished with the rise of the modern DSM system, shifting focus to more clearly defined and readily observed indicators that would enhance the reliability of psychodiagnosis. The last several decades have seen mounting criticism of the DSM framework, prompting a movement to reform our scientific paradigm. At current, this pendulum appears to be swinging back toward a dimension-based approach to classification and study.
With this shift, revisiting the significance of TD as a core dimension in the study of psychosis is not only warranted, but timely. Theoretical and empirical foundations of TD have supported it as a construct with a high degree of specificity (eg, compared to hallucinations, which are highly diverse in terms of etiology). Thus, research in this area has the potential to elucidate robust etiological links, which, in turn, could inform individualized, effective intervention approaches.
Acknowledgement
This article is adapted from the first author’s doctoral dissertation: Exploring a Multifactorial, Clinical Model of Thought Disorder: Application of a Dimensional, Transdiagnostic Approach (2016). The authors would like to thank Alison Sommers for her helpful feedback on an earlier version of the manuscript. Finally, the authors would like to express their appreciation for the feedback from and collaboration with the reviewers of this manuscript.
References
- 1. Holzman P. Thought disorder in schizophrenia: editor’s introduction. Schizophr Bull. 1986;12:342–347. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Kraepelin E. Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia. Translated by Barclay R. Mary; edited by Robertson George M. Huntington, NY: R. E. Krieger Pub. Co; 1971. [Google Scholar]
- 3. Bleuler E. Dementia Praecox; or the Group of Schizophrenias. Translated by Zinkin Joseph; foreword by Nolan D.C. Lewis. New York: International Universities Press; 1952. [Google Scholar]
- 4. National Institute of Mental Health. RDoC Draft 3.1 June 2011 (Internet). http://www.nimh. nih.gov/research-priorities/rdoc/nimh-research-domain-criteria-rdoc.shtml [Google Scholar]
- 5. Piaget J. The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York: Basic Books; 1954. [Google Scholar]
- 6. Vygotsky LS. Thought in schizophrenia. Arch Neurol Psychiatr. 1934;31:1062–1077. [Google Scholar]
- 7. Blatt S, Auerbach J, Levy K. Mental representations in personality development, psychopathology, and the therapeutic process. Rev Gen Psychol. 1997;1:351–374. [Google Scholar]
- 8. Kernberg O, Caligor E. A psychoanalytic theory of personality disorder. InLenzenweger M, Clarkin J, eds. Major Theories of Personality Disorder. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2005:114–156. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Holt RR. Freud’s theory of the primary process: present status. Freud Reappraised: A Fresh Look at Psychoanalytic Theory. Reprinted. New York: Guilford Press; 1989:280–301. [Google Scholar]
- 10. Meloy JR. Thought organization and primary process in the parents of schizophrenics. Br J Med Psychol. 1984;57:279–281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Singer MT, Wynne LC. Thought disorder and family relations of schizophrenics. Iv. Results and implications. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1965;12:201–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Wynne LC, Ryckoff IM, Day J, Hirsch SI. Pseudo-mutuality in the family relations of schizophrenics. Psychiatry. 1958;21:205–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Docherty NM, Hall MJ, Gordinier SW. Affective reactivity of speech in schizophrenia patients and their nonschizophrenic relatives. J Abnorm Psychol. 1998;107:461–467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Levy DL, Coleman MJ, Sung H, et al. The genetic basis of thought disorder and language and communication disturbances in schizophrenia. J Neurolinguist. 2010;23:176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Bearden C, Wu K, Caplan R, Cannon T. Thought disorder and communication deviance as predictors of outcome in youth at clinical high risk for psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;50:1–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Gooding DC, Coleman MJ, Roberts SA, Shenton ME, Levy DL, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L. Thought disorder in offspring of schizophrenic parents: findings from the New York High-Risk Project. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:263–271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Metsänen M, Wahlberg KE, Hakko H, Saarento O, Tienari P. Thought Disorder Index: a longitudinal study of severity levels and schizophrenia factors. J Psychiatr Res. 2006;40:258–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Metsänen M, Wahlberg KE, Saarento O, et al. Early presence of thought disorder as a prospective sign of mental disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2004;125:193–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Ott SL, Roberts S, Rock D, Allen J, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L. Positive and negative thought disorder and psychopathology in childhood among subjects with adulthood schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2002;58:231–239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Hain C, Maier W, Hoechst-Janneck S, Franke P. Subclinical thought disorder in first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients. Results from a matched-pairs study with the Thought Disorder Index. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1995;92:305–309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Johnston MH, Holzman PS. Assessing Schizophrenia Thinking. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- 22. Solovay MR, Shenton ME, Holzman PS. Comparative studies of thought disorders. I. Mania and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44:13–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Berenbaum H, Oltmanns TF, Gottesman II. Formal thought disorder in schizophrenics and their twins. J Abnorm Psychol. 1985;94:3–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Wahlberg KE, Wynne LC, Oja H, et al. Gene–environment interaction in vulnerability to schizophrenia: findings from the Finnish Adoptive Family Study of Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154:355–362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Wahlberg KE, Wynne LC, Oja H, et al. Thought disorder index of Finnish adoptees and communication deviance of their adoptive parents. Psychol Med. 2000;30:127–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Andreasen NC. Thought, language, and communication disorders. I. Clinical assessment, definition of terms, and evaluation of their reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1979;36:1315–1321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Chaika E, Lambe R. The locus of dysfunction in schizophrenic speech. Schizophr Bull. 1985;11:8–15. [Google Scholar]
- 28. Kuperberg GR. Language in schizophrenia. Lang Linguist Compass. 2010;4:576–589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Docherty NM. On identifying the processes underlying schizophrenic speech disorder. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:1327–1335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Lilienfeld SO, Wood JM, Garb HN. The scientific status of projective techniques. Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2000;1:27–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Kleiger JH. Disordered Thinking and the Rorschach: Theory, Research, and Differential Diagnosis. New York: Routledge Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 32. Rapaport D, Gill MM, Schafer R. (1945–1946). Diagnostic Psycho-logical Testing. 2 vols. Rev. ed. New York: International Universities Press; 1968. [Google Scholar]
- 33. Watkins JG, Stauffacher JC. An index of pathological thinking in the Rorschach. J Proj Tech. 1952;16:276–286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Harrow M, Quinlan D. Is disordered thinking unique to schizophrenia? Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1977;34:15–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Quinlan DM, Harrow M, Tucker G, Carlson K. Varieties of “disordered” thinking on the Rorschach: findings in schizophrenic and nonschizophrenic patients. J Abnorm Psychol. 1972;79:47–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Quinlan DM, Harrow M. Boundary disturbances in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1974;83:533–541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Andreasen NC. Thought, language, and communication disorders. II. Diagnostic significance. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1979;36:1325–1330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Andreasen NC. Scale for the assessment of thought, language, and communication (TLC). Schizophr Bull. 1986;12:473–482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Andreasen NC, Grove WM. Thought, language, and communication in schizophrenia: diagnosis and prognosis. Schizophr Bull. 1986;12:348–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Harvey PD, Lenzenweger MF, Keefe RS, Pogge DL, Serper MR, Mohs RC. Empirical assessment of the factorial structure of clinical symptoms in schizophrenic patients: formal thought disorder. Psychiatry Res. 1992;44:141–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Cuesta MJ, Peralta V. Does formal thought disorder differ among patients with schizophrenic, schizophreniform and manic schizoaffective disorders? Schizophr Res. 1993;10:151–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Cuesta MJ, Peralta V. Thought disorder in schizophrenia. Testing models through confirmatory factor analysis. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;249:55–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Peralta V, Cuesta MJ, de Leon J. Formal thought disorder in schizophrenia: a factor analytic study. Compr Psychiatry. 1992;33:105–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Docherty NM, Grosh ES, Wexler BE. Affective reactivity of cognitive functioning and family history in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 1996;39:59–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Nienow TM, Docherty NM. Internal source monitoring and communication disturbance in patients with schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2005;35:1717–1726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Halliday MAK, Hasan R. Cohesion in English. London, UK: Longman; 1976. [Google Scholar]
- 47. Rochester S, Martin JR. Crazy Talk: A Study of the Discourse of Schizophrenic Speakers. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- 48. De Smet-D’Hondt JJ. Cohesion in English (Book). English Stud. 1977;58:470. [Google Scholar]
- 49. Docherty N, Schnur M, Harvey PD. Reference performance and positive and negative thought disorder: a follow-up study of manics and schizophrenics. J Abnorm Psychol. 1988;97:437–442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Docherty NM, Evans IM, Sledge WH, Seibyl JP, Krystal JH. Affective reactivity of language in schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1994;182:98–102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Docherty NM. Linguistic reference performance in parents of schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry. 1995;58:20–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Gordinier SW, Docherty NM. Factor analysis of the Communication Disturbances Index. Psychiatry Res. 2001;101:55–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Meehl PE. Toward an integrated theory of schizotaxia, schizotypy, and schizophrenia. J Pers Disord. 1990;4:1–99. [Google Scholar]
- 54. Andreasen NJ, Powers PS. Overinclusive thinking in mania and schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 1974;125:452–456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Breakey WR, Goodell H. Thought disorder in mania and schizophrenia evaluated by Bannister’s grid test for schizophrenic thought disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 1972;120:391–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Braff DL, Glick ID, Griffin P. Thought disorder and depression in psychiatric patients. Compr Psychiatry. 1983;24:57–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Braff DL, Glick ID, Johnson MH, Zisook S. The clinical significance of thought disorder across time in psychiatric patients. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1988;176:213–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Andreasen NC, Olsen S. Negative v positive schizophrenia. Definition and validation. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1982;39:789–794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Harvey PD, Earle-Boyer EA, Wielgus MS. The consistency of thought disorder in mania and schizophrenia. An assessment of acute psychotics. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1984;172:458–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Jampala VC, Taylor MA, Abrams R. The diagnostic implications of formal thought disorder in mania and schizophrenia: a reassessment. Am J Psychiatry. 1989;146:459–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Ragin AB, Oltmanns TF. Communicability and thought disorder in schizophrenics and other diagnostic groups. A follow-up study. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150:494–500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Wilcox JA, Ramirez AL, Baida-Fragoso N. The prognostic value of thought disorder in psychotic depression. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2000;12:1–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Holzman PS, Shenton ME, Solovay MR. Quality of thought disorder in differential diagnosis. Schizophr Bull. 1986;12:360–371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Solovay MR, Shenton ME, Gasperetti C, et al. Scoring manual for the Thought Disorder Index. Schizophr Bull. 1986;12:483–496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Harvey PD. Speech competence in manic and schizophrenic psychoses: the association between clinically rated thought disorder and cohesion and reference performance. J Abnorm Psychol. 1983;92:368–377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Marengo JT, Harrow M. Longitudinal courses of thought disorder in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Schizophr Bull. 1997;23:273–285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Spohn HE, Coyne L, Larson J, Mittleman F, Spray J, Hayes K. Episodic and residual thought pathology in chronic schizophrenics: effect of neuroleptics. Schizophr Bull. 1986;12:394–407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Harrow M, Marengo JT. Schizophrenic thought disorder at followup: its persistence and prognostic significance. Schizophr Bull. 1986;12:373–393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Meehl PE. Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. Am Psychol. 1962;17:827–838. [Google Scholar]