Abstract
Ketamine has been reported to impair the capacity for learning and memory. This study examined whether these capacities were also altered in the offspring and investigated the role of the CREB signaling pathway in pregnant rats, subjected to ketamine-induced anesthesia. On the 14th day of gestation (P14), female rats were anesthetized for 3 h via intravenous ketamine injection (200 mg/Kg). Morris water maze task, contextual and cued fear conditioning, and olfactory tasks were executed between the 25th to 30th day after birth (B25-30) on rat pups, and rats were sacrificed on B30. Nerve density and dendritic spine density were examined via Nissl’s and Golgi staining. Simultaneously, the contents of Ca2+/Calmodulin-Dependent Protein Kinase II (CaMKII), p-CaMKII, CaMKIV, p-CaMKIV, Extracellular Regulated Protein Kinases (ERK), p-ERK, Protein Kinase A (PKA), p-PKA, cAMP-Response Element Binding Protein (CREB), p-CREB, and Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) were detected in the hippocampus. We pretreated PC12 cells with both PKA inhibitor (H89) and ERK inhibitor (SCH772984), thus detecting levels of ERK, p-ERK, PKA, p-PKA, p-CREB, and BDNF. The results revealed that ketamine impaired the learning ability and spatial as well as conditioned memory in the offspring, and significantly decreased the protein levels of ERK, p-ERK, PKA, p-PKA, p-CREB, and BDNF. We found that ERK and PKA (but not CaMKII or CaMKIV) have the ability to regulate the CREB-BDNF pathway during ketamine-induced anesthesia in pregnant rats. Furthermore, ERK and PKA are mutually compensatory for the regulation of the CREB-BDNF pathway.
Keywords: CREB pathway, ketamine, learning and memory, pregnant rats, rat offspring, Neuroscience
INTRODUCTION
Ketamine abuse causes more severe problems during pregnancy [1]. In addition, between 0.75% and 2% of pregnant women require surgery either related to the pregnancy or to unrelated medical problems [2]. Furthermore, with the popularity of minimally invasive surgery, surgery during pregnancy has become increasingly widespread [3]. Therefore, the risks of anesthetic administration (such as ketamine) for the fetus have become more important. Unfortunately, only few reports investigated the effects of generally utilized anesthesia on neurodevelopmental consequences for a fetus prior to birth [4–6].
It is well known that the hippocampus plays a central role for learning and memory processes [7], and consequently, this is a known target for drug regulation. Ketamine is a high affinity uncompetitive antagonist of voltage dependent N-Methyl-D-aspartic Acid Receptor (NMDAR) and has been used for decades as a dissociative anesthetic that also appears to be a useful tool in psychiatric research [8]. Ketamine can attenuate learning and memory impairment, particularly for short-term memory [9]. The cAMP-Response Element Binding Protein (CREB) has been reported to be involved in the learning and memory deficits, caused by ketamine [10].
Ketamine can enter the fetus through the placental barrier, where it may exert a stronger impact on the fetus since the fetal brain is still in a stage of development. Even normal use of ketamine may affect the fetus. Therefore, this study examined whether the abilities of learning and memory were altered in the offspring as well as the ketamine-induced effect on CREB signaling pathways in ketamine-induced pregnant rats on gestational day 14.
RESULTS
Nissl's staining
We selected three 104 μm2 areas to conduct a neuron count in both the CA1 (Figure 2c-2d) and CA3 (Figure 2e-2f) regions of the hippocampus. As shown in Figure 2a-2g, the cell density of K group decreased by 20.5% compared to C group (p < 0.05).
Golgi staining
Fully impregnated CA1 pyramidal cells can be detected via Golgi staining, and the spines of the apical dendrites can be analyzed under a light microscope using a 200 × oil immersion objective. We randomly selected 10 μm apical (Figure 3a-3b) and basal (Figure 3c-3d) from the same neurological level to count the number of dendritic spines. Only the density of apical and basal dendrites was determined in our study, as several different types of spines were not always clearly visible (e.g., thin, mushroom, or branched dendrites). Spine density decreased by 21.6% in the K group compared to the C group (P < 0.05, Figure 3g).
Morris water maze test
Morris water maze test data revealed that rat pups had suffered from a significant main effect on escape latency during spatial training on testing days 2-3 (Figure 4a). In contrast, the escape latency did not reveal any significant differences between the C group and the K group during place navigation trials on day 4-5 (p = 0.223) and spatial probe tests (p = 0.062, Figure 4b). It is worth noting that no significant differences in animals’ swimming speeds were detected (C group: 25.00 ± 1.96 cm/s and K group: 26.00 ± 1.33 cm/s, according to a one-way ANOVA: F = 0.191, p = 0.827) between the C and K group (Figure 4c-4d).
Contextual and cued fear conditioning
Contextual and cued fear conditioning is a standard fear conditioning task that measures the ability of rat pups to learn and remember an association between an averse experience and environmental cues. In contextual and cued fear conditioning, a significant difference was found in CS (p < 0.05), while no significant difference was found between K and C group in other tests (Figure 5b).
Olfactory tasks
As shown in Figure 6b, during the acquisition stage, no significant differences were found in the investigation time of Hole 1 and Hole 2, indicating that rats had no specific preference for carvone or limonene. During the recall stage, no significant differences were found in the investigation time of novel odor between K group and C group.
Results of the CCK-8 test
To evaluate the optimum concentration of ketamine for PC12 cells, a CCK8 assay was performed. PC12 cells were exposed to ketamine at different concentrations (0.2 μg/mL, 0.3 μg/mL, 0.4 μg/mL, 0.5 μg/mL, 0.6 μg/mL, 0.7 μg/mL, 0.8 μg/mL, 0.9 μg/mL, 1μg/mL) for 3 h. According to Table 1, the 50% cell viability of ketamine for PC12 cells was 0.6 μg/mL.
Table 1. Results of CCK-8 test.
Concentration of ketamine (μg/mL) | 1 | 0.9 | 0.8 | 0.7 | 0.6 | 0.5 | 0.4 | 0.3 | 0.2 |
Cell viability | 17% | 34% | 39% | 43% | 49% | 53% | 64% | 75% | 76% |
Ketamine exposure affects protein expression in the hippocampus
To examine whether ketamine treatment has the ability to alter the protein expression of learning and memory related proteins, we measured protein levels of CaMKII, p-CaMKII, CaMKIV, p-CaMKIV, ERK, p-ERK, PKA, p-PKA, p-CREB, and BDNF in the rat hippocampus. As shown in Figure 7 and in comparison to the values of each corresponding C group, no significant difference was found in the protein levels of CaMKII, p-CaMKII, CaMKIV, and p-CaMKIV. However, the protein levels of ERK, p-ERK, PKA, p-PKA, p-CREB, and BDNF had significantly decreased (p < 0.05) to 91.6%, 71.1%, 74.5%, 92.5%, 67.4%, and 64.2% of their original values, respectively, while the CREB protein level significantly increased (p < 0.05) to 129% (Figure 8).
Ketamine exposure affects protein expression in PC12 cells
No significant difference was found between C and D group. Compared to C group, the protein levels of ERK, p-ERK, PKA, p-PKA, CREB, p-CREB, and BDNF decreased significantly (p < 0.05): p-ERK decreased to 70.9% in S group (p < 0.05), p-PKA decreased to 74.4% in H group (p < 0.05), and the protein levels of p-ERK, p-PKA, p-CREB, and BDNF decreased to 53.7%, 68.1%, 72.7%, and 66.1% (p < 0.05). The protein levels of p-ERK decreased by 24.3% in S+H group compared to S group, while the p-PKA protein levels decreased by 8.5% in S+H group compared to H group (Figure 9).
DISCUSSION
The morris water maze task, contextual and cued fear conditioning, and olfactory tasks enable the evaluation of learning and memory of spatial, conditioned, and odor cues in rat pups (Figure 4–6). Nerve density and dendritic spine density decreased, which all influenced the nerve conduction efficiency as well as learning ability and memory capacity [6, 11]. However, during the anaesthetization process, anesthetic stress, oxygen saturation, gastrointestinal tract squeezing the uterus, change of placental blood flow, supine position, oppressing blood vessels, and other factors all impact on fetal rats. To exclude these effects from our analysis, we cultured PC12 cells, exploring the effect of ketamine on the CREB pathway.
The prevalence of substance abuse in pregnant women is similar to that of the general population, resulting in an increased fetal exposure rate during the most vulnerable period of neurodevelopment and organogenesis. Many pregnant women are exposed to various types of anesthetics for surgery or diagnostic procedures every year. Furthermore, numerous women will also undergo surgery during pregnancy, unrelated to childbirth. Consensus is that fetal exposure to alcohol is harmful. Prenatal alcohol exposure may induce abnormal brain development as well as decrease the capacity for learning and memory [12, 13]. Similar to alcohol, anticonvulsants, sedatives (such as ketamine), or narcotics can pass through the placental barrier and for ketamine in particular, researches showed its ability to impair the capacity for learning and memory [14, 15]. Evidence links early exposure to anesthesia with cognitive impairment [16]. In addition, ketamine is also one of the most commonly used drugs in pediatric clinical anesthesia and its reported influence on learning and memory has always been of clinical concern. Moreover, ketamine is a frequently abuse drug in the public, which includes pregnant women [17]. Zhang et al. suggests that repeated ketamine exposure induced long-term cognitive impairment via increased NOX2 [18]. Experimental evidence indicates that the NMDAR antagonist ketamine impairs cognition [19]. Prolonged ketamine exposure in neonates at anesthetic doses has been reported to cause long-term impairments of learning and memory [20]. Furthermore, ketamine decreased p-CREB in the hippocampus [21, 22], and decreased levels of BDNF [23]. CREB has been demonstrated to be involved in learning and memory deficits caused by ketamine [10, 22]. These findings raise concern about potential adverse effects of ketamine exposure to fetuses and infants.
In our study, the ratio of P-CREB/total CREB was decreased in the rat hippocampus. The function of CREB is dominantly regulated by phosphorylation at Ser133, which results in the activation of gene transcription [24]. The Phosphorylated CREB protein recruits the transcriptional activator CREB-binding protein (CBP), thus stimulating the transcription of CRE-regulated genes that are involved in neurogenesis and neuritogenesis [25]. We therefore hypothesize that P-CREB may be responsible for compensatory increases in CREB protein levels; however, further testing is required to confirm this hypothesis. p-CREB promotes immediate early genes such as the c-fos gene via interaction with the CRE sequence located within promotor regions (TGACGTCA) [26]. Genetic deletion of CREB selectively impaired the hippocampus-dependent spatial memory of mice subjected to the Morris water maze [27], which coincided with our results. Furthermore, CREB phosphorylation is a necessary step in the process leading to the generation of new dendritic spines [27]. In addition, the cAMP–CREB signaling cascade is critical for the generation of new neurons in the rodent hippocampus, also facilitating their subsequent morphological maturation [28].
Several findings have shown that the dysregulation of the CREB-BDNF cascade has been involved in cognitive impairment [29]. The neurons of the hippocampus of aged animals showed a down-regulation of BDNF and p-CREB expression, associated with learning and memory impairment [30, 31], which was also similar with our result. In this study, BDNF and p-CREB revealed the same tendency (Figure 9g-9h). BDNF has also been reported to elicit rapid action potentials, thus influencing neuronal excitability, and it has a demonstrated role in activity-dependent synaptic plasticity events such as long-term potentiation, learning tasks, and memory [32, 33]. BDNF is involved in structural remodeling, neuronal plasticity, and synaptic restructuring [34, 35].
Several signaling pathways, including those involving CaMKII, CaMKIV, ERK, and PKA, have been associated with the regulation of de novo protein synthesis in the context of synaptic plasticity, converging on the phosphorylation of CREB at Ser133 residue (Figure 10). It is generally accepted that ketamine blocks NMDAR, thus mediating the neurotransmission of postsynaptic receptors [36]. NMDAR in turn mediates the release of neurotransmitters (such as acetylcholine, dopamine, GABA, and NE), and regulates the levels of sodium and calcium. The increased association between CaMK II and CREB, followed by phosphorylation of CREB in response to Wnt5a stimulation was suppressed in a minimal hepatic encephalopathy rat model [37]. Ca2+ signaling not only plays a critical role in regulating apoptosis and autophagy [38], but also affects CREB phosphorylation. Cohen reported that CREB phosphorylation also proceeds with slow, sigmoid kinetics, that are rate-limited due to the paucity of CaMKIV, protecting against saturation of phospho-CREB as a response to increased firing rates and elevated Ca2+ transients [39]. Interestingly, no significance was found in the protein levels of CaMKII, p-CaMKII, CaMKIV, and p-CaMKIV in rat pups (Figure 7). Liu's research demonstrated that low-intensity pulsed ultrasound increased the intracellular concentration of calcium and enhanced protein levels of CaMKII and CaMKIV; however, it did not promote the activation of CREB [40]. CaMKII was markedly decreased following a stress-priming methamphetamine-induced conditioned place preference reinstatement test; however, p-CREB expressions in the medial prefrontal cortex were increased [41]. Guo reported that ERK and CREB phosphorylation was not mediated by CaMK [42]. Therefore, we speculate here that the effect of ketamine on CaMKII and CaMKIV can only be sustained within a window of time following anesthesia. Consequently, it did not have long-term effects on neurodevelopment, but a proving experiment is still required.
To explore whether ERK or PKA influence the phosphorylation of CREB, we cultured PC12 with both a ERK and a PKA inhibitor (SCH772984 and H89). PKA phosphorylates and activates CREB, which then binds to the CRE domain on DNA and in turn activates genes that are involved in the process of learning and memorization; however, ketamine inhibits this process [10]. N-acetylserotonin appears to partly restore the ketamine-induced decrease of ERK and BDNF to control levels [43]. Phosphorylation of CREB at Ser133 can be catalyzed via a number of protein kinases, including cAMP-dependent PKA [44]. The ERK1/2 are members of the mitogen activated protein kinase (MAPK) family and are necessary for cell growth, differentiation, survival, molecular information processing, and stabilization of structural changes in dendritic spines [45, 46]. When treated with SCH772984 or H89 alone, no change in the protein levels of p-CREB and BDNF were observed; however, these decreased when treated with the SCH772984 or H89 (Figure 9g-9h). This explains how ERK and PKA can regulate the phosphorylation of CREB. Furthermore, when ERK or PKA do not participate in this process, ERK and PKA have the ability to replace each other, thus independently regulating the phosphorylation of CREB. Lin identified the involvement of cAMP/PKA and ERK dependent CREB signaling pathways in the luteolin-mediated miR-132 expression and neuritogenesis of PC12 cells [47]. Won demonstrated that DA-9801 exerts its beneficial effects of stimulating neurite outgrowth through the ERK1/2-CREB pathway in PC12 cells [48]. Behavioral analyses of animals with altered ERK signaling have revealed a central involvement of this cascade in learning and memory [49], and it has been reported that ERK activity was decreased in dentate gyrus of aged rats, which did not sustain LTP [50, 51]. Coccomyxa gloeobotrydiformis (CGD) significantly increased ERK and CREB phosphorylation in the hippocampus, suggesting that the learning and memory-enhancing effects of CGD might be associated with the ERK/CREB pathway [52]. Li et al. reported that pretreatment with resveratrol effectively restored synaptic plasticity in chronic cerebral hypoperfusion rats both functional and structural via PKA-CREB activation [28]. The levels of PKA and cAMP were increased in the rat hippocampus following a step-down inhibitory avoidance task [53]. Furthermore, transgenic mice with the inhibitory regulatory subunit of PKA were impaired in their long-term memory abilities due to contextual fear conditioning [54].
In summary, the present study investigated learning as well as spatial and conditioned memory of rat pups, following ketamine anesthesia during pregnancy. Moreover, ERK and PKA, but not CaMKII or CaMKIV, can regulate the CREB-BDNF pathway in this animal model. Furthermore, ERK and PKA in the regulation of CREB-BDNF pathway are mutually compensating.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals
Male and female Wistar rats, three months of age, weighing 200 ± 20 g, were purchased from the Animal Experimental Center of the Second Affiliated Hospital of the Harbin Medical University (Harbin, China). Prior to the experiment, rats were quarantined for two weeks at the Northeast Agricultural University (Harbin, China). All experiments were performed in accordance with the guidelines outlined by the Ethical Committee for Animal Experiments (Northeast Agricultural University, Harbin, China).
Mating and drug administration
Thirty-six Wistar rats were divided into 12 cages (one male and two females per cage) with an iron mesh at the bottom. On the next morning the vaginal suppository was investigated through the iron mesh. When sperm was detected, female rats were annotated as pregnant at day 0 (P0). The female rats were anesthetized via intravenous ketamine injection (200 mg/Kg) for 3 h on P14 [55]. The total volume of ketamine stayed below 2 mL/100 mg. Ketamine-treated offspring were recorded as K group, while individuals within the control group were recorded as C group. The first day after birth was recorded as B0. During B25-B30, Morris water maze task, contextual and cued fear conditioning, and olfactory tasks were used to test learning and memory capacity (n = 120, 5/dam, Figure 1).
Sample collections
Rat pups were sacrificed at B30 via cervical dislocation, and were recovered to collect brain tissue for Nissl staining (n = 24, 1/dam), Golgi staining (n = 24, 1/dam), and western blotting (n = 72, 3/dam). A subset of their hippocampuses were quickly dispensed on ice, put into a freezing tube, and frozen in liquid nitrogen, while other tissues were preserved in 10% formalin.
Nissl's staining
Coronal brain sections were cut in a vibratome (Leica VT1200S, Germany) after the brains were postfixed in the same fixative. To ensure matching of hippocampal sections between groups, we used anatomical landmarks provided by the brain atlas. The selected brain sections were stained with 0.5% cresyl violet and we selected three 104 μm2 areas for examination with a light microscope (Leica DFC420, Germany) to count neuron numbers in the CA1 and CA3 regions of the hippocampus.
Golgi staining
Golgi-Cox staining was utilized to obtain hippocampal dendritic spine density via the FD Rapid GolgiStainTM Kit (FD Neuro Technologies Inc), following the manufacturer's instructions. Coronal tissue sections of 150 μm thickness were cut at room temperature, using a vibratome (Leica VT1200S, Germany) and then, they were put on gelatin coated slides. Subsequently, slides were dehydrated with a gradient of 50%, 75%, 95%, to 100% ethanol and cleared in xylene, then the specimens were prepared with slide coverslips and sealed with Permount. The slides were then examined in detail with a light microscope (Leica DFC420, Germany). We analyzed the stained spine, using techniques similar to those described in previous study [56]. Five pyramidal neurons were analyzed that were well-impregnated and clearly distinguishable from others in each hippocampus (20 × objective lens). Five segments of 10 μm of apical and basal dendrites respectively, were randomly selected from each pyramidal neuron for inspection (via 200 × oil immersion lens) to quantify the density of spines. Spinal density of secondary apical and basal dendrites was analyzed at proximal segments emerging at more than 50 μm distance from the soma of the hippocampal CA1 neurons. All of these spines were required to exhibit a clearly distinguishable base or origin and were isolated from neighboring dendrites. Spine density was calculated per 10 μm of dendritic length. The open-source ImageJ 1.48 r Java image-viewing software and Adobe Photoshop CC 2015 were used to calibrate the scale and enlarge the segments of the spines. An investigator blinded to the experimental condition completed all analyses.
Morris water maze test
Place navigation trials
To test hippocampal-dependent spatial cognition, rats were trained in the standard morris water maze with a hidden platform [57]. A white escape platform (12 cm diameter) was submerged in a circular pool (160 cm diameter, at a 50 cm depth), filled with warm (23–25°C) opaque water. At B25-29, each rat pup underwent four trial sessions per day (60–70 min inter-trial interval) for five consecutive days. Each trial consisted of releasing the rat into the water, facing the outer edge of the pool at one of the quadrants (in random sequence) and permitting the animal to escape to the platform. They received four trials per day of training in search for the submerged and unmarked platform, with trial durations of 60 s on the platform at the end of trials. All trials were videotaped, and the swimming paths of rats were recorded with the ANY-maze video tracking system (Stoelting Co., IL, USA), which enabled us to measure the time taken (latency) to find the platform (s), as well as other behavioral information obtained during this spatial reference memory test. The animals were dried and placed beneath a heating lamp after completion of each test.
Spatial probe test
A probe trial was performed 1 d after the last trial at B30 where the platform was removed from the pool to assess memory retention for the location of the platform. During the 60 s test trial, we recorded and analyzed the swimming speed (cm/s), the swimming path tracks, and the number of entries into the platform quadrant zone.
Contextual and cued fear conditioning
Conditioning training on day one consisted of placing the rat pups in the chamber and exposing the animals to a mild footshock paired with an auditory cue. The rat pup was brought from the home cage to the testing room and placed into the conditioning chamber. It had 3 min to explore the novel environment. The auditory cue (a 90 dB tone) was sounded for approximately 30 sec. A stimulus light within the wall of the chamber may also be illuminated. During the last seconds of the auditory signal, an unconditioned aversive stimulus, a mild footshock in the range of 0.25 to 0.5 mA, was administered through the grid floor for 2 sec. The number of seconds spent freezing in the test chamber on the training day was considered the control measure of unconditioned fear. The rat pup was left in the conditioning chamber for 1 min after the last pairing, during which the association between the aversive stimulus and the properties of the conditioning chamber was further established. The rat pup was then returned to its home cage.
Testing on day 2 began approximately 24 hours after the conditioning session. The rat pup was returned to the same conditioning chamber and scored for bouts of freezing behavior. No footshock was administered on day two. The number of seconds spent freezing in the identical test chamber on day two was considered the measure of contextually conditioned fear, i.e., freezing within identical context. Freezing was defined as a lack of movement other than respiration. Presence or absence of freezing behavior was generally recorded by an investigator, who was blinded to the experimental condition, taking a note every 10 sec for 5 min, for a maximum total score of 30 freezing bouts. The rat pup was then returned to its home cage.
The second phase of testing began an hour later. A further testing chamber with very different properties provided the altered context. Changing the sensory cues as much as possible was essential so that the rat pup perceives the novel context as unrelated to the conditioning chamber. Such as triangle-shaped test chamber with different lighting was used and lemon juice was painted on the walls, while a different investigator wore gloves and a lab coat of different texture than on the training day. Freezing behavior was scored for 3 min. Contextual discrimination of fear conditioning was quantified by comparing the number of freezing bouts in the same contextual environment to the number of freezing bouts in the novel contextual environment.
At the end of the first 3 min, the tone that was presented on training day one (was well as the light stimulus cue if used on day one) was presented in the novel context environment. Freezing behavior was scored for the next 3 min in the presence of the sound (and light) cues. Cued conditioning was calculated via comparison of the number of freezing bouts in the novel context environment in the presence of the cue with the number of freezing bouts in the novel context environment in the absence of the cue (Figure 5a).
Olfactory task
This task was designed to investigate the olfactory learning and memory abilities [58]. For this experiment, two holes (3 cm diameter and 4.5 cm deep) were used. A polypropylene swab, embedded in a fine plastic mesh and containing 20 μL of diluted odors (1:10) was placed at the bottom of each hole and covered with wood shavings. The acquisition test (one session) consisted of one odor (either limonene or carvone, Sigma-Aldrich) being presented in both holes for a 5 min period. In a preliminary experiment, with simultaneous presentation of the same pair of odors (one odor in each hole) in a one-trial test, rat pups spent the same amount of time exploring either hole, indicating no preference for one of the two odors. The recall test consisted of a 3 min session in which one hole was odorized with the previously presented odor, while the other hole was odorized with a new odor (Figure 6a). The delay between acquisition and recall tests was 60 min. During the recall test, the cumulated exploration time of each hole was converted as the percentage of the total exploration time of both holes. Rat pups were considered to have remembered the familiar odor when they spent less time exploring the hole containing it, in relation to the time spent exploring the hole containing the new odor. Equal exploration times for both holes during the recall test were considered to indicate that rat pup did not remember the familiar odor. Both odors were used alternatively during acquisition or recall and presented randomly in each of the two holes to avoid place preference bias (Figure 6a).
Cell culture and drug treatment
PC12 cells were obtained from the Northeast Agricultural University, Harbin, China. The cells were cultured in DMEM medium (Gibco), supplemented with 10% (v/v) FBS, penicillin/streptomycin (100 U/mL; 100 μg/mL) at 37°C under an atmosphere of 5% CO2 and 95% air. The cells were seeded in 6-well plates with 2-9 × 105 cells/well or 96-well plates with 2-9 × 104 cells/well, and the culture medium was changed daily. Cells were pretreated for 3 h with Protein Kinase A (PKA) inhibitor (H89, 10 μM, H group), Extracellular Regulated Protein Kinases (ERK) inhibitor (SCH772984, 10 μM, S group), PKA inhibitor + ERK inhibitor (S+H group), DMSO (solvent of inhibitors, D group), and ketamine (K group).
Cell counting kit-8 (CCK-8) assay
Cell viability was detected via the CCK8 assay (Beyotime Institute of Biotechnology, Suzhou, Jiangsu, China). Following the indicated treatments, CCK8 solution (10 μl) was added to each well (96-well plates). Then, the cells were cultured at 37°C for one further hour. The optical density of each well was measured at 450 nm with a Bio-Tek microplate reader (Bio-Tek Instruments, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Winooski, VT).
WB
150 μg of protein were separated via 10% SDS-polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis and transferred to a nitrocellulose membrane (HybondTM-C Extra, GE Healthcare) via electroblotting. After washing, membranes were blocked with 3% (w/v) BSA (biotopped) for 4 h at room temperature and incubated overnight at 4°C in BSA with antibodies that are specific for Ca2+/Calmodulin-Dependent Protein Kinase II (CaMKII), p-CaMKII, CaMKIV, p-CaMKIV, ERK, p-ERK, PKA, CREB, p-CREB (1.5:1000, EnoGene), p-PKA, and Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF, 1:1000, abcam). Membranes were washed thrice with PBS containing 0.1% Tween and then incubated for 1 h at room temperature either with a horseradish peroxidase-conjugated secondary antibody (Goat anti-Rabbit IgG Antibody HRP (ABIN) or a goat anti-Mouse IgG Antibody HRP (Sigma)) in BSA.
Data analysis
All data were analyzed with GraphPad Prism 7.0 (GraphPad Software Inc., USA) via one-way ANOVA, followed by Turkey's Post Hoc test or unpaired two-tailed Student t-test. Values were considered to be statistically significant for P < 0.05. Data are presented as means ± standard deviation unless otherwise noted.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31572580 and 31372491).
Footnotes
Authors contribution
XL and LG designed the study. XL, CG, and LG designed the behavioral testing. XL, CG, YL, LL, XW, and YZ collected data for behavioral testing. XL, YL, YC, and WL processed the brain tissue. XL and YL collected and analyzed the data. XL, YL, and CG interpreted the data. XL wrote and edited the manuscript. All authors critically reviewed the content and approved the final version for publication.
CONFLICTS OF INTEREST
There is no conflict of interest.
REFERENCES
- 1.Rofael HZ, Turkall RM, Abdel-Rahman MS. Immunomodulation by cocaine and ketamine in postnatal rats. Toxicology. 2003;188:101–14. doi: 10.1016/S0300-483X(03)00081-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Goodman S. Anesthesia for nonobstetric surgery in the pregnant patient. Seminars in perinatology. 2002;26:136–45. doi: 10.1053/sper.2002.32203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Cheek TG, Baird E. Anesthesia for nonobstetric surgery: maternal and fetal considerations. Clinical obstetrics and gynecology. 2009;52:535–45. doi: 10.1097/GRF.0b013e3181c11f60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kong FJ, Ma LL, Hu WW, Wang WN, Lu HS, Chen SP. Fetal exposure to high isoflurane concentration induces postnatal memory and learning deficits in rats. Biochemical pharmacology. 2012;84:558–63. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2012.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Zheng H, Dong Y, Xu Z, Crosby G, Culley DJ, Zhang Y, Xie Z. Sevoflurane anesthesia in pregnant mice induces neurotoxicity in fetal and offspring mice. The Journal of the American Society of Anesthesiologists. 2013;118:516–26. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3182834d5d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Zhao T, Li Y, Wei W, Savage S, Zhou L, Ma D. Ketamine administered to pregnant rats in the second trimester causes long-lasting behavioral disorders in offspring. Neurobiol Dis. 2014;68:145–55. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2014.02.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Squire LR. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol Rev. 1992;99:195–231. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.99.2.195. doi: org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wang LQ, Liu SZ, Wen X, Wu D, Yin L, Fan Y, Wang Y, Chen WR, Chen P, Liu Y, Lu XL, Sun HL, Shou W, et al. Ketamine-mediated afferent-specific presynaptic transmission blocks in low-threshold and sex-specific subpopulation of myelinated Ah-type baroreceptor neurons of rats. Oncotarget. 2015;6:44108–22. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.6586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Chen Q, Min S, Hao X, Peng L, Meng H, Luo Q, Chen J, Li X. Effect of Low Dose of Ketamine on Learning Memory Function in Patients Undergoing Electroconvulsive Therapy-A Randomized, Double-Blind, Controlled Clinical Study. The Journal of ECT. 2016 doi: 10.1097/YCT.0000000000000365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Peng S, Yang X, Liu GJ, Zhang XQ, Wang GL, Sun HY. From the camp pathway to search the ketamine-related learning and memory. Eur Rev Med Pharmacol Sci. 2015;19:161–4. WOS: 000352211700025. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Tang X, Wu D, Gu LH, Nie BB, Qi XY, Wang YJ, Wu FF, Li XL, Bai F, Chen XC, Xu L, Ren QG, Zhang ZJ. Spatial learning and memory impairments are associated with increased neuronal activity in 5XFAD mouse as measured by manganese-enhanced magnetic resonance imaging. Oncotarget. 2016;7:57556–57570. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.11353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Fish EW, Holloway HT, Rumple A, Baker LK, Wieczorek LA, Moy SS, Paniagua B, Parnell SE. Acute alcohol exposure during neurulation: Behavioral and brain structural consequences in adolescent C57BL/6J mice. Behav Brain Res. 2016;311:70–80. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Singer LT, Moore DG, Min MO, Goodwin J, Turner JJ, Fulton S, Parrott AC. Motor delays in MDMA (ecstasy) exposed infants persist to 2 years. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2016;54:22–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2016.01.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Zhang YH, Zhang J, Song JN, Xu X, Cai JS, Zhou Y, Gao JG. The PI3K-AKT-mTOR pathway activates recovery from general anesthesia. Oncotarget. 2016;7:40939–52. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.10172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Weed MR, Bookbinder M, Polino J, Keavy D, Cardinal RN, Simmermacher-Mayer J, Cometa FN, King D, Thangathirupathy S, Macor JE, Bristow LJ. Negative Allosteric Modulators Selective for The NR2B Subtype of The NMDA Receptor Impair Cognition in Multiple Domains. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:568–77. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Wilder RT, Flick RP, Sprung J, Katusic SK, Barbaresi WJ, Mickelson C, Gleich SJ, Schroeder DR, Weaver AL, Warner DO. Early exposure to anesthesia and learning disabilities in a population-based birth cohort. Anesthesiology. 2009;110:796–804. doi: 10.1097/01.anes.0000344728.34332.5d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rofael HZ, Turkall RM, Abdel-Rahman MS. Immunomodulation by cocaine and ketamine in postnatal rats. Toxicology. 2003;188:101–14. doi: 10.1016/s0300-483x(03)00081-7. doi: org/10.1016/S0300-483X(03)00081-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Zhang H, Sun XR, Wang J, Zhang ZZ, Zhao HT, Li HH, Ji MH, Li KY, Yang JJ. Reactive Oxygen Species-mediated Loss of Phenotype of Parvalbumin Interneurons Contributes to Long-term Cognitive Impairments After Repeated Neonatal Ketamine Exposures. Neurotox Res. 2016;30:593–605. doi: 10.1007/s12640-016-9653-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Trevlopoulou A, Touzlatzi N, Pitsikas N. The nitric oxide donor sodium nitroprusside attenuates recognition memory deficits and social withdrawal produced by the NMDA receptor antagonist ketamine and induces anxiolytic-like behaviour in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2016;233:1045–54. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4181-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Stevens RA, Butler BD, Kokane SS, Womack AW, Lin Q. Neonatal inhibition of Na+-K+-2Cl—cotransporter prevents ketamine induced spatial learning and memory impairments. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2016.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Reus GZ, Abaleira HM, Titus SE, Arent CO, Michels M, da Luz JR, dos Santos MA, Carlessi AS, Matias BI, Bruchchen L, Steckert AV, Ceretta LB, Dal-Pizzol F, et al. Effects of ketamine administration on the phosphorylation levels of CREB and TrKB and on oxidative damage after infusion of MEK inhibitor. Pharmacol Rep. 2016;68:177–84. doi: 10.1016/j.pharep.2015.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kempf SJ, Metaxas A, Ibanez-Vea M, Darvesh S, Finsen B, Larsen MR. An integrated proteomics approach shows synaptic plasticity changes in an APP/PS1 Alzheimer’s mouse model. Oncotarget. 2016;7:33627–48. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.9092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Liu WX, Wang J, Xie ZM, Xu N, Zhang GF, Jia M, Zhou ZQ, Hashimoto K, Yang JJ. Regulation of glutamate transporter 1 via BDNF-TrkB signaling plays a role in the anti-apoptotic and antidepressant effects of ketamine in chronic unpredictable stress model of depression. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2016;233:405–15. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4128-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Montminy MR, Gonzalez GA, Yamamoto KK. Regulation of cAMP-inducible genes by CREB. Trends in neurosciences. 1990;13:184–8. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90045-C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Spencer JP, Vauzour D, Rendeiro C. Flavonoids and cognition: the molecular mechanisms underlying their behavioural effects. Archives of biochemistry and biophysics. 2009;492:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.abb.2009.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Berkowitz LA, Riabowol KT, Gilman MZ. Multiple sequence elements of a single functional class are required for cyclic AMP responsiveness of the mouse c-fos promoter. Mol Cell Biol. 1989;9:4272–81. doi: 10.1128/MCB.9.10.4272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Bourtchuladze R, Frenguelli B, Blendy J, Cioffi D, Schutz G, Silva AJ. Deficient long-term memory in mice with a targeted mutation of the cAMP-responsive element-binding protein. Cell. 1994;79:59–68. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90400-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Li H, Wang J, Wang P, Rao Y, Chen L. Resveratrol Reverses the Synaptic Plasticity Deficits in a Chronic Cerebral Hypoperfusion Rat Model. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis. 2016;25:122–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2015.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zhao Y, Xiao M, He W, Cai Z. Minocycline upregulates cyclic AMP response element binding protein and brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the hippocampus of cerebral ischemia rats and improves behavioral deficits. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015;11:507–16. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S73836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Lim YY, Villemagne VL, Laws SM, Ames D, Pietrzak RH, Ellis KA, Harrington KD, Bourgeat P, Salvado O, Darby D. BDNF Val66Met, Aβ amyloid, and cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging. 2013;34:2457–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Guerrieri D, van Praag H. Exercise-mimetic AICAR transiently benefits brain function. Oncotarget. 2015;6:18293–313. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.4715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Nacmias B, Piccini C, Bagnoli S, Tedde A, Cellini E, Bracco L, Sorbi S. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor, apolipoprotein E genetic variants and cognitive performance in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroscience letters. 2004;367:379–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.06.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Fossati P, Radtchenko A, Boyer P. Neuroplasticity: from MRI to depressive symptoms. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;14:S503–S10. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2004.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Duman R. Pathophysiology of depression: the concept of synaptic plasticity. European psychiatry. 2002;17:306–10. doi: 10.1016/S0924-9338(02)00654-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Alonso M, Medina JH, Pozzo-Miller L. ERK1/2 activation is necessary for BDNF to increase dendritic spine density in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Learning & Memory. 2004;11:172–8. doi: 10.1101/lm.67804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Rodriguez-Munoz M, Sanchez-Blazquez P, Merlos M, Garzon-Nino J. Endocannabinoid control of glutamate NMDA receptors: the therapeutic potential and consequences of dysfunction. Oncotarget. 2016;7:55840–55862. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.10095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ding S, Xu Z, Yang J, Liu L, Huang X, Wang X, Zhuge Q. The Involvement of the Decrease of Astrocytic Wnt5a in the Cognitive Decline in Minimal Hepatic Encephalopathy. Mol Neurobiol. 2016 doi: 10.1007/s12035-016-0216-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Liu F, Li ZF, Wang ZY, Wang L. Role of subcellular calcium redistribution in regulating apoptosis and autophagy in cadmium-exposed primary rat proximal tubular cells. J Inorg Biochem. 2016;164:99–109. doi: 10.1016/j.jinorgbio.2016.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Cohen SM, Ma H, Kuchibhotla KV, Watson BO, Buzsaki G, Froemke RC, Tsien RW. Excitation-Transcription Coupling in Parvalbumin-Positive Interneurons Employs a Novel CaM Kinase-Dependent Pathway Distinct from Excitatory Neurons. Neuron. 2016;90:292–307. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Liu SH, Lai YL, Chen BL, Yang FY. Ultrasound Enhances the Expression of Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor in Astrocyte Through Activation of TrkB-Akt and Calcium-CaMK Signaling Pathways. Cereb Cortex. 2016 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Han WY, Du P, Fu SY, Wang F, Song M, Wu CF, Yang JY. Oxytocin via its receptor affects restraint stress-induced methamphetamine CPP reinstatement in mice: Involvement of the medial prefrontal cortex and dorsal hippocampus glutamatergic system. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2014;119:80–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Guo Y, Feng P. OX2R activation induces PKC-mediated ERK and CREB phosphorylation. Exp Cell Res. 2012;318:2004–13. doi: 10.1016/j.yexcr.2012.04.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Choudhury A, Singh S, Palit G, Shukla S, Ganguly S. Administration of N-acetylserotonin and melatonin alleviate chronic ketamine-induced behavioural phenotype accompanying BDNF-independent and dependent converging cytoprotective mechanisms in the hippocampus. Behav Brain Res. 2016;297:204–12. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.10.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hagiwara M, Brindle P, Harootunian A, Armstrong R, Rivier J, Vale W, Tsien R, Montminy M. Coupling of hormonal stimulation and transcription via the cyclic AMP-responsive factor CREB is rate limited by nuclear entry of protein kinase A. Molecular and cellular biology. 1993;13:4852–9. doi: 10.1128/MCB.13.8.4852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Ivanov A, Pellegrino C, Rama S, Dumalska I, Salyha Y, Ben-Ari Y, Medina I. Opposing role of synaptic and extrasynaptic NMDA receptors in regulation of the extracellular signal-regulated kinases (ERK) activity in cultured rat hippocampal neurons. The Journal of physiology. 2006;572:789–98. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.105510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Sindreu CB, Scheiner ZS, Storm DR. Ca 2+-stimulated adenylyl cyclases regulate ERK-dependent activation of MSK1 during fear conditioning. Neuron. 2007;53:79–89. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Lin LF, Chiu SP, Wu MJ, Chen PY, Yen JH. Luteolin induces microRNA-132 expression and modulates neurite outgrowth in PC12 cells. PLoS One. 2012;7:e43304. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Won JH, Ahn KH, Back MJ, Ha HC, Jang JM, Kim HH, Choi SZ, Son M, Kim DK. DA-9801 promotes neurite outgrowth via ERK1/2-CREB pathway in PC12 cells. Biol Pharm Bull. 2015;38:169–78. doi: 10.1248/bpb.b14-00236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Atkins CM, Selcher JC, Petraitis JJ, Trzaskos JM, Sweatt JD. The MAPK cascade is required for mammalian associative learning. Nature neuroscience. 1998;1:602–9. doi: 10.1038/2836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.McGahon B, Maguire C, Kelly A, Lynch M. Activation of p42 mitogen-activated protein kinase by arachidonic acid and trans-1-amino-cyclopentyl-1, 3-dicarboxylate impacts on long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus in the rat: analysis of age-related changes. Neuroscience. 1999;90:1167–75. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(98)00528-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Drulis-Fajdasz D, Wojtowicz T, Wawrzyniak M, Wlodarczyk J, Mozrzymas JW, Rakus D. Involvement of cellular metabolism in age-related LTP modifications in rat hippocampal slices. Oncotarget. 2015;6:14065–81. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.4188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Sun L, Jin Y, Dong L, Sui HJ, Sumi R, Jahan R, Hu D, Li Z. Coccomyxa Gloeobotrydiformis Improves Learning and Memory in Intrinsic Aging Rats. Int J Biol Sci. 2015;11:825–32. doi: 10.7150/ijbs.10861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Bernabeu R, Bevilaqua L, Ardenghi P, Bromberg E, Schmitz P, Bianchin M, Izquierdo I, Medina JH. Involvement of hippocampal cAMP/cAMP-dependent protein kinase signaling pathways in a late memory consolidation phase of aversively motivated learning in rats. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1997;94:7041–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.13.7041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Abel T, Nguyen PV, Barad M, Deuel TA, Kandel ER, Bourtchouladze R. Genetic demonstration of a role for PKA in the late phase of LTP and in hippocampus-based long-term memory. Cell. 1997;88:615–26. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81904-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Clancy B, Darlington RB, Finlay BL. Translating developmental time across mammalian species. Neuroscience. 2001;105:7–17. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00171-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Xiao H, Liu B, Chen Y, Zhang J. Learning, memory and synaptic plasticity in hippocampus in rats exposed to sevoflurane. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. 2016;48:38–49. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2015.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Morris R, Garrud P, Rawlins J, O’Keefe J. Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature. 1982;297:681–3. doi: 10.1038/297681a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Mandairon N, Sacquet J, Garcia S, Ravel N, Jourdan F, Didier A. Neurogenic correlates of an olfactory discrimination task in the adult olfactory bulb. European journal of neuroscience. 2006;24:3578–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05235.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]