Abstract
Brains are constantly flooded with sensory information that needs to be filtered at the pre-attentional level and integrated into endogenous activity in order to allow for detection of salient information and an appropriate behavioral response. People with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) or Fragile X Syndrome (FXS) are often over- or under-reactive to stimulation, leading to a wide range of behavioral symptoms. This altered sensitivity may be caused by disrupted sensory processing, signal integration and/or gating, and is often being neglected. Here, we review translational experimental approaches that are used to investigate sensory processing in humans with ASD and FXS, and in relevant rodent models. This includes electroencephalographic measurement of event related potentials, neural oscillations and mismatch negativity, as well as habituation and pre-pulse inhibition of startle. We outline robust evidence of disrupted sensory processing in individuals with ASD and FXS, and in respective animal models, focusing on the auditory sensory domain. Animal models provide an excellent opportunity to examine common mechanisms of sensory pathophysiology in order to develop therapeutics.
Keywords: Autism, Sensory filtering, Habituation, Fragile-x, Sensory processing, Startle, EEG, Animal model, Translation
1. Introduction
We are continuously bombarded with sensory stimuli in daily life. The appropriate pre-attentive filtering of this sensory information and its integration into other cognitive processes are critical for healthy brain function. This is exemplified in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), in which abnormal filtering and the accompanying hyper-sensitivity or hypo-sensitivity to sensory stimuli are cardinal symptoms which have a debilitating impact on social interactions and daily functioning (Crane et al., 2009; Elison et al., 2013; Elsabbagh et al., 2013; Hirstein et al., 2001; Leekam et al., 2007; Minshew et al., 2002; Zwaigenbaum et al., 2005). ASD impacts approximately 1 in 68 children in the US, with nearly five times higher incidence in boys than girls (data from 8 year old children, Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring (ADDM) Network – CDC, 2010). Individuals with ASD display a range of symptoms potentially related to sensory disruptions, including difficulties with social communication/interactions, fixated interests, inflexible routines and motor stereotypies.
Fragile X Syndrome (FXS) is the most common monogenic form of ASD that affects 1 in 4000 males and 1 in 8000 females. FXS occurs due to a mutation in the Fmr1 gene that results in the down-regulation of fragile × mental retardation protein (FMRP). FMRP is a translation regulator and its absence causes abnormal protein synthesis particularly those associated with activity dependent synaptic plasticity. The symptoms of FXS include intellectual disability, repetitive behaviors, social communication deficits, increased anxiety and arousal, and abnormal sensory processing. Between one quarter and one third of individuals with FXS are also diagnosed with ASD (Bailey et al., 2008; Rogers et al., 2001). Several studies suggest the existence of similar pathophysiological and anatomical mechanisms in ASD and FXS, particularly in the sensory processing domain (Belmonte and Bourgeron, 2006; Feinstein and Reiss, 1998; Hagerman, 2006; Pickett and London, 2005; Reiss et al., 1995).
Of all the symptom domains characterizing ASD and FXS, the sensory domain arguably holds the most promise for revealing mechanisms central to the pathogenesis of the spectrum of disorders. Sensory processing difficulties in ASD are manifested in hypersensitivity, avoidance of sensory stimuli, diminished responses to sensory stimulation, and/or sensory seeking behavior. These symptoms can impact multiple sensory systems including the visual, auditory, gustatory, olfactory and tactile systems (Ben-Sasson et al., 2008; Cascio et al., 2015; Foss-Feig et al., 2012; Klintwall et al., 2011; Lane et al., 2014; Liss et al., 2006; Marco et al., 2012; O’Connor, 2012). Similar sensory system deficits are also seen in humans with FXS (Baranek et al., 2008; Castrén et al., 2003; Frankland et al., 2004; Kogan et al., 2004; Miller et al., 1999; Rotschafer and Razak, 2014; Schneider et al., 2013; Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b). The symptoms can be evaluated by clinical assessment or parent report (Baranek et al., 2008; Ben-Sasson et al., 2007; Tavassoli et al., 2016) and have recently been included as a criterion for ASD diagnosis in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition (DSM-5). Sensory abnormalities in ASD and FXS are not only among the most replicable features of these disorders (Ben-Sasson et al., 2008; Donkers et al., 2015; Klintwall et al., 2011; Liss et al., 2006), they are also present early in childhood (Baranek et al., 2008; Elison et al., 2013; Elsabbagh et al., 2013; Germani et al., 2014; Zwaigenbaum et al., 2005) and are strong predictors of some later-emerging symptoms, such as anxiety (Green et al., 2012; Sullivan et al., 2014). Importantly, sensory deficits may be accompanied by neurophysiological abnormalities of sensory processing which can be quantified objectively and non-invasively using electroencephalography (EEG) (Brandwein et al., 2015; Castrén et al., 2003; Ethridge et al., 2016; Lepisto et al., 2005; Machado et al., 2013; Orekhova et al., 2007, 2008; Stroganova et al., 2007; Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b; van Diessen et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2013) or electromyography (EMG) and/or behavioral measures of pre-pulse inhibition and habituation of the startle reflex (Perry et al., 2007).
In the fields of ASD and FXS research, it is critical to develop translation-relevant outcome measures which bridge human and animal studies. Experimental approaches which measure sensory processing are emerging as powerful translational tools in this regard. Studies using EEG, magnetoencephalography (MEG) or startle/EMG point to analogous sensory processing deficits in humans and animal models (Castrén et al., 2003; Connolly et al., 2004; Ehrlichman et al., 2008; Ethridge et al., 2016; Harms et al., 2014; Lovelace et al., 2016; Maxwell et al., 2004; Nakamura et al., 2011; Siegel et al., 2003; Umbricht et al., 2004) and have facilitated examination of underlying mechanisms of sensory disorders in rodent models relevant to ASD/FXS. Furthermore, such studies take advantage of the properties of the neural circuitry underlying basic sensory processing, which is better characterized and may be more conserved across species than neural circuitry underlying complex social and communication behaviors.
In this review, we outline recent advances in understanding the sensory processing disruptions underpinning sensory features of ASD and FXS, focusing on the auditory sensory domain. Rather than exhaustively reviewing the literature as others have done (e.g. O’Connor, 2012; Orekhova and Stroganova, 2014), we highlight similarities and differences between auditory sensory processing deficits in ASD and FXS to highlight aspects of their shared and distinct pathophysiology. We then describe emerging evidence from rodent models which sheds light on the possible neurobiological underpinnings of sensory deficits in ASD, FXS and associated neurodevelopmental disorders.
2. Objective measurement of sensory processing
Auditory stimuli elicit stereotypical changes in electrical activity across the brain, which are reproducibly detected by EEG and can be accompanied by predictable behavioral responses. At a given location on the scalp (humans) or in the brain (rodents), changes in the electrical field represent the summation of extracellular currents generated by postsynaptic neurotransmission within neuronal networks (Creutzfeldt et al., 1966a,b), particularly those in closer proximity to the electrode. Measurement of these electrical field changes by EEG enables quantification of the amplitude, latency, frequency and power of neural oscillations, including those induced by sensory stimulation. An analogous technique, MEG, measures magnetic fields rather than electric fields in the brain and can also be used to quantify neural electrical activity (Cohen, 1972). Sound-evoked changes in electrical activity occur primarily in the brainstem/midbrain, thalamus, auditory cortex, amygdala, frontal cortex and temporal cortex, regions which are involved in processing of auditory sensory information. Auditory processing advances through multiple stages, including pre-attentive stimulus recognition, sensory perception, active attentional shifting, salience detection and task-dependent activity (Picton and Hillyard, 1974; Picton et al., 1974). Below we describe a number of key experimental approaches which utilize EEG and/or behavioral measures in order to investigate auditory processing and sensory filtering in ASD.
2.1. Event-related potentials (ERPs)
Individual auditory stimuli each elicit a stereotypical pattern of voltage deflections in the EEG signal, which are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) and can be reliably quantified when responses to multiple auditory stimuli are averaged. For instance, the ERP following simple auditory stimuli, such as sounds with a particular intensity, pitch and duration, can be divided into discrete components. In humans, these components are termed the P50, P200, P300, N100 and N200 and consist of positive deflections at 50, 200 and 300 ms post-stimulus and negative deflections at 100 and 200 ms post-stimulus (Fig. 1; Duncan et al., 2009; Picton et al., 1974; Sutton et al., 1965). In rodents, analogous ERP components are seen, but are termed the P1, P2, P3, N1 and N2. They arise with shorter latencies, with P1, N1 and P2 components observed at approximately 20, 40 and 80 ms post-stimulus, respectively (Fig. 1; Broberg et al., 2010; Maxwell et al., 2004; Siegel et al., 2003; Umbricht et al., 2004; Witten et al., 2014). For consistency in this review, the rodent nomenclature will be used in both species. Individual ERP components have been linked to a number of aspects of sensory processing. The P1, N1 and P2 components are considered obligatory, in that they are evoked regardless of task or attentive state (Picton and Hillyard, 1974). The P1 is thought to reflect sensory registration and pre-attentive stimulus processing, the N1 cortical activation and sensory perception, and the P2 salience detection and stimulus evaluation. The P3, usually split into an earlier and more frontally located P3a and a later, more posterior (parietal) P3b component, is an index of novelty and attentional orienting elicited by infrequent or unpredictable elements introduced into otherwise predictable trains of stimuli (Polich, 2007; Polich and Criado, 2006; Sutton et al., 1965). Importantly, ERPs change across human and rodent postnatal development. In humans, there is evidence that P1 amplitude decreases and N1 and P2 amplitudes increases with age, although these changes differ depending on electrode location and vary between studies (Ponton et al., 2000; Wunderlich and Cone-Wesson, 2006; Wunderlich et al., 2006). In particular, children up to the age of 16 years display a prolonged, obligatory negative component which is less discernible in adulthood (Ponton et al., 2000; Sussman et al., 2008; Wunderlich et al., 2006). Latencies also change across development, with P1 and N1 latencies decreasing until adulthood (Ponton et al., 2000). In rodents, the opposite developmental patterns in ERP amplitude have been reported, with higher P1 amplitude and lower N1 and P2 amplitude in juvenile mice (7 weeks old) relative to adults (12 weeks; Featherstone et al., 2014; Nagy et al., 2015).
Fig. 1.
Schematic representation of human and rodent EEG electrode placement, ERPs and gamma oscillations.
A) Human scalp EEG array using the International 10–20 System for electrode placement; B) example of rodent EEG using implanted tripolar electrodes; C) schematic representation of stereotypical auditory-evoked ERP with characteristic positive and negative voltage deflections. ERP latencies and peaks can vary according to electrode placement, experimental paradigm and species; D) example of neural oscillations-gamma oscillations at baseline and following auditory stimulus, contrasting wildtype and GluN1 knockout mice. S – stimulus, KO – knockout, WT – wildtype. Panel B adapted from Connolly et al. (2003) with permission, panel C adapted from Gandal et al. (2012b) with permission.
2.2. P1 (P50) suppression
Auditory stimuli not only induce ERPs and changes in neural oscillations, but also trigger the engagement of filtering mechanisms that modify responses to subsequent stimuli. At a neural level, this ‘gating’ phenomenon primarily involves the subconscious dampening of P1 responses to a second stimulus which occurs 0.5 s after the initial stimulus (Nagamoto et al., 1991; Oranje et al., 2006). Abnormal P1 suppression involves a failure to diminish the P1 ERP response to the second click in a paired-click experimental paradigm (Freedman et al., 1983).
2.3. Acoustic startle and pre-pulse inhibition of startle (PPI)
A useful measure of general sensitivity to sensory auditory stimuli is the magnitude of the startle response to an acoustic stimulus (Braff et al., 1992; Swerdlow et al., 1994). This startle response can also be used to assess sensory filtering and sensorimotor gating. In a behavioral test called pre-pulse inhibition of startle (PPI), a loud tone or noise (around 115 dB) is presented either alone or preceded by a quieter tone/noise of approximately 72–85 dB, 30–500 ms beforehand. Maximum PPI is generally occurring at around 120 ms in humans (Braff et al., 1992; Graham, 1975), and at around 50 ms in rats. The extent of the acoustic startle response (ASR) is measured either by EMG measurement of eye muscle contraction (m. orbicularis oculi) in humans, or in rodents by the whole body twitch, sensed by a motion sensitive platform (Valsamis and Schmid, 2011). Humans or animals with normal PPI display strongly attenuated startle responses (by up to 70–90%) to the loud acoustic stimulus if it is preceded by the quieter pre-pulse stimulus, whereas individuals with diminished sensorimotor gating display less attenuation of startle responses by the pre-pulse (Braff et al., 1992; Swerdlow et al., 1994). The neural circuitry mediating acoustic startle responses is well-described and seems to be highly conserved (Schmid et al., 2014; Swerdlow et al., 1999). Secondary auditory neurons in the cochlear nucleus (cochlear root in mice and rats) innervate giant neurons in the caudal pontine reticular formation that directly activate cranial and spinal motorneurons (Koch, 1999; Nodal and Lopez, 2003; Swerdlow et al., 1999). PPI is thought to be mediated by a feed-forward inhibitory loop from the cochlear nucleus to the inferior and superior colliculi and the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus, which in turn sends descending fibers to the startle mediating neurons in the reticular formation (Fendt et al., 2001; Gomez-Nieto et al., 2014; Swerdlow and Geyer, 1993; Yeomans et al., 2006).
2.4. Habituation, sensitization and refractory processes
Trains of stimuli induce additional changes in EEG and behav-ior, which either reflect refractory processes or the processes of habituation and sensitization. An example of refractory processes is the dampening of the N1 amplitude, following repeated high frequency stimulation: it may take up to 10 s for the N1 amplitude to fully recover after initial stimulation (Budd et al., 1998; Davis et al., 1966; Oranje et al., 2006). Habituation is the exponential decrement of a response to an initially novel stimulus that is presented repeatedly over time at lower frequencies, and it is different from refractory processes. Sensitization describes the opposite, an increment in response to the same stimulus over time. Habituation and sensitization are thought to be independent processes, modulating the same behavioural response in opposite ways (Groves and Thompson, 1970; Rankin et al., 2009; Schmid et al., 2014). An overall sensitization is often found in the first 2–4 trials of a block of startle-eliciting stimuli, followed by a gradual decline in responses (habituation) to the remainder of presented trials (Aggernaes et al., 2010; Meincke et al., 2004). Habituation (short-term) is assumed to be caused by synaptic mechanisms intrinsic to sensory pathways (Davis et al., 1982; Leaton et al., 1985; Simons-Weidenmaier et al., 2006). Specifically, short-term habituation of startle has been proposed to be caused by activity-induced synaptic depression at the sensorimotor synapses in the pontine reticular formation, mediated by the activation of voltage- and calcium dependent large conductance potassium channels, called Maxi K, KCa1.1, or BK channels (Simons-Weidenmaier et al., 2006; Typlt et al., 2013b; Weber et al., 2002). Sensitization and long-term habituation are caused through an extrinsic modulation of the startle pathway by structures including the pedunculopontine tegmentum, the cerebellar vermis, the amygdala and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (Davis et al., 1997; Gonzalez-Lima et al., 1989; Koch, 1999; Leaton and Supple, 1986, 1991). The fact that these circuits are relatively well described, highly preserved, and that they can be studied in animal models, provides a unique opportunity for exploring potential mechanisms underlying sensory filtering disruptions in ASD and FXS, as well as for identifying potential drug targets to enhance sensory filtering.
2.5. Mismatch negativity (MMN)
Mismatch negativity (MMN) is another neural phenomenon reflecting underlying sensory filtering which is revealed by studying EEG responses to trains of auditory stimuli in humans (Näätänen et al., 2007) and rodents (Ehrlichman et al., 2008; Harms et al., 2014; Nakamura et al., 2011; Siegel et al., 2003; Witten et al., 2014). Unlike P1 suppression, PPI and habituation, which all dampen responses to less relevant stimuli, MMN involves the pre-attentive identification of more relevant stimuli among trains of less relevant ones. In the context of repeated stimuli of consistent pitch, duration and intensity, a deviant tone will elicit a negative deflection of the ERP at fronto-central and central electrodes (Näätänen et al., 2007), with a latency dependent on the paradigm used. Because it reflects the differences between evoked responses to two tones (standard and deviant), MMN is usually expressed as a difference wave.
2.6. Neural oscillations
Changes to the power and synchrony of neural oscillations also occur in response to auditory stimulation. These changes occur in the context of, and can be influenced by, the patterns of oscillations in resting state, where subjects are asked to think of nothing in particular. Both at rest and following stimulation, information about neural oscillations across a range of frequencies can be extracted from within the EEG signal using spectral decomposition approaches such as fast Fourier transformation or wavelet analysis (Fig. 1). Neural oscillations are important because, when divided into discrete frequency bands, they share physiological properties. Delta frequency oscillations (1–3 Hz) are particularly relevant to deep sleep, theta oscillations (4–7 Hz) to top-down cognitive control/memory, beta oscillations (13–30 Hz) to task engagement/motor behavior and gamma oscillations (30–80 Hz) to sensory and cognitive inhibition. In addition, changes in phase synchronization of the alpha frequency band (8–12 Hz) are thought to generate some components of the ERP, such as P1 and N2 (Gruber et al., 2005). Neural oscillations in rodents change across postnatal development, with increases in baseline power (Featherstone et al., 2014) and induced power (time-locked but not phase-locked to stimulus; Nagy et al., 2015) occurring during adolescence.
3. Sensory processing in individuals with ASD and FXS
3.1. ERPs
Abnormal auditory ERPs, consistent with impaired sensory processing, have been revealed by EEG in individuals with ASD. Decreased amplitude of the P1 peak at fronto-central electrodes has been described using both a classical P1 suppression paradigm in children aged 7–12 years with Asperger’s syndrome (Madsen et al., 2015) and in children aged 7–12 years with ASD by presenting speech and non-speech sounds (Lepisto et al., 2005). This latter finding was replicated in a more recent study which reported decreased amplitude of a later component of the P1 in the central region in children aged 3–8 years with ASD (Stroganova et al., 2013). Increased variability of P1 amplitude and latency between trials may also occur in individuals with ASD (Milne, 2011). In a MEG study, increased latency of the M50 (equivalent to ERP P1) predicted language deficits in ASD (Oram Cardy et al., 2008). Decreased amplitude of the N1 component of the ERP has been observed in children with ASD aged 4–16 years (Bruneau et al., 1999; Gandal et al., 2010; Seri et al., 1999). Consistent with a role for early sensory processing abnormalities underpinning symptoms in ASD, a negative correlation between N1 amplitude (N1a at temporal electrodes and N1b at fronto-central electrodes) and autism symptom severity in children and adolescents 6–17 years has been reported (Brandwein et al., 2015). Increased N1 latency in ASD has been reported in some studies (Gage et al., 2003; Gandal et al., 2010; Korpilahti et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2010) but not others (Bruneau et al., 1999; Ceponiene et al., 2003; Madsen et al., 2015). This may be explained in part by differences in mean age of subjects between studies, given that two studies using MEG have shown that normal age-related decreases in M100 between 8 and 16 years were absent in ASD individuals (Gage et al., 2003; Roberts et al., 2010). Such developmental differences may make detection of N1 latency differences in ASD more difficult in cohorts of younger children with a mean age <8 years (Bruneau et al., 1999; Ceponiene et al., 2003; Madsen et al., 2015) than older children with a mean age >9 years (Gage et al., 2003; Gandal et al., 2010; Korpilahti et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2010). In addition to decreased amplitude of N1, decreased amplitude of N2 fronto-centrally for non-speech sounds has also been described (Lepisto et al., 2005). There is evidence that sensory processing deficits may be identifiable prior to ASD diagnosis. Increased P1 amplitude during standard repetitive speech stimuli in infants at high risk of ASD relative to children at low risk has been described (Seery et al., 2014), as has a failure of normal later-alization of ERPs during development in infants at high risk (Seery et al., 2013). These findings suggest a possible role for altered sensory processing as a risk factor for ASD and/or an early emerging symptom of the disorders.
Abnormalities of attentional orienting, indexed by P3 have also been investigated in ASD. The P3 consists of the P3a and P3b components, which are both elicited by infrequent or unpredictable elements introduced into otherwise predictable trains of stimuli. The P3a component occurs at 250–280 ms, and is present when infrequent or unpredictable shifts occur during a train of otherwise predictable stimuli regardless of where the participant is asked to direct his or her attention (Hruby and Marsalek, 2003; Squires et al., 1975). The P3a is therefore often described as a ‘novelty detector’ (Comerchero and Polich, 1999; Hruby and Marsalek, 2003). The P3b is also evoked by oddball tasks, and is observed at 250–500 ms (Polich, 2007). However, the amplitude of the P3b is dependent upon how improbable a stimulus is, with more improbable stimuli resulting in larger amplitude responses (Polich, 2007; Sutton et al., 1965). Both are elicited by infrequent or unpredictable elements introduced into otherwise predictable trains of stimuli (Hruby and Marsalek, 2003; Squires et al., 1975). Age dependent abnormalities of the P3a have been described in ASD, with increased P3a amplitude to pure tone deviants evident in childhood (less than 8 years of age) but decreased P3a amplitude evident in young adulthood in a small study (Ferri et al., 2003). Decreased P3a amplitude has been observed at younger ages (mean age 9 years) for vowel sound and speech-like frequency deviants in children with ASD and Asperger’s syndrome (Ceponiene et al., 2003; Lepisto et al., 2005, 2006). Impairment of P3a in ASD is consistent with evidence of attentional deficits in ASD (for review see Orekhova and Stroganova, 2014), and suggests a common underpinning neurobiological mechanism.
Abnormal auditory ERPs have also been described in FXS. The published work is primarily on temporal and amplitude properties of the auditory ERPs (Knoth and Lippe, 2012; Rotschafer and Razak, 2014). A consistent observation across studies is that the N1 component has larger average amplitude and shows reduced habituation to repeated sounds in humans with FXS compared to control subjects (Castrén et al., 2003; Ethridge et al., 2016; Rojas et al., 2001; St Clair et al., 1987; Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b). A study using MEG also revealed enlargement and reduced latency of the N100 m (the MEG equivalent of the N1 in EEG) in adults with FXS (Rojas et al., 2001). Interestingly, the direction of N1 amplitude and latency changes in FXS is opposite that of ASD described above. Typically, FXS-related N1 enhancement is accompanied by P2 enhancement (Castrén et al., 2003; St Clair et al., 1987; Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b). Treatment with candidate pharmacotherapy minocycline can decrease N1 and P2 amplitudes in individuals with FXS (Schneider et al., 2013), suggesting that auditory ERP abnormalities can be used as outcome measures in drug treatment in human studies. Since both N1 and P2 components stem from temporal lobe activity, the altered N1 and P2 amplitudes are consistent with structural deficits in temporal lobe regions in humans with FXS, implicating this region in some of the behavioral abnormalities associated with FXS. These structural deficits include decreased size of superior temporal gyrus (Reiss et al., 1994), and white matter enlargement localized specifically to the temporal lobe (Hazlett et al., 2012). Additionally, fMRI research shows that the superior temporal gyrus, along with the medial frontal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, cerebellum, and pons display higher levels of activation in individuals with FXS, consistent with the larger N1 component (Hall et al., 2009). Enhancement of the P2 component also suggests abnormal activation of the mesencephalic reticular activating system. Structures linked to P2 generation are also responsible for early auditory processing. Alteration of P2-associated structures may create an incorrect memory trace of the target stimulus, which may decrease performance on stimulus detection tasks (Näätänen et al., 2011, 2007).
The amplitude of the P3 component is also consistently reduced in humans with FXS (St Clair et al., 1987; Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b). Specifically, Van der Molen et al. (2012b,a) revealed reduced P3a and P3b components in individuals with FXS. Decreased P3b amplitude, specifically, may reflect a failure to identify a stimulus as improbable (Sutton et al., 1965), possibly resulting from improper stimulus representation at lower levels of processing, or from short term memory impairments (Polich, 2007).
In summary, various components of the auditory ERP are altered in FXS, with the N1/P2 changes predicting reduced habituation to a continuous acoustic environment. This may underlie the hypersensitivity phenotype observed behaviorally in humans with FXS. The changes in the longer latency components indicate improper memory trace formation that may result in reduced auditory change detection. Whether such deficits are present in early development has not been directly tested. However, if present, these deficits will leave an indelible mark on maturation of language and cognitive functions.
3.2. P1 (P50) suppression
Despite the phenotypic variation within the autistic spectrum, abnormal perception and processing of sensory information appears to be a shared phenomenon. Adequate processing of sounds and appropriate attention oriented towards them are considered prerequisites for accurate higher order processing. Similar to individuals with schizophrenia, it has been suggested that deficient inhibitory control of sensory stimuli in ASD may cause sensory overload and disruption of higher order processing, leading to avoidance of external stimulation altogether (Kootz et al., 1982). P1 suppression is a valuable index of inhibitory control of sensory stimuli, yet in ASD, research on P1 suppression has been sparse. There is some evidence for reduced P1 suppression in very young (3–8 years) autistic children with intellectual disability (IQ < 72), but this seemed to improve with age (Orekhova et al., 2008). Studies on older ASD subjects without intellectual disability showed comparable P1 suppression with typically developing children (Kemner et al., 2002), similar to that observed in the PDD-NOS subgroup Multiple Complex Developmental Disorder (MCDD) (Oranje et al., 2013b). Neither were P1 suppression deficits found in a study on adult males with ASD (Magnee et al., 2009). Healthy P1 suppression was also found in two recent studies on 8–12 year old children with ASD (Madsen et al., 2015), although a subpopulation of these children (those with Asperger’s syndrome) showed attenuated P1 amplitude (Madsen et al., 2015). In summary, there is currently not much evidence for a P1 suppression deficit in autism, although on amplitude level the P1 ERP appears smaller in children with Asperger’s syndrome. Future studies should include larger sample sizes not only in order to increase power, but also to allow grouping based on sub-diagnoses. Although inhibitory control of auditory sensory processing is also of interest in FXS, P1 suppression has not been investigated in FXS to our knowledge.
3.3. Acoustic startle and PPI
Acoustic startle reactivity has been used in ASD and FXS research as an objective measure of sensitivity to auditory stimulation. Increased startle magnitude in ASD, indicating higher auditory sensitivity, has been reported in both adults (Kohl et al., 2014) and children (Chamberlain et al., 2013; Takahashi et al., 2016). Other studies, however, have not found a general increase of startle magnitude in subjects with autism (Bernier et al., 2005; McAlonan et al., 2002; Salmond et al., 2003; Sterling et al., 2013; Yuhas et al., 2011). Because startle reactivity in individuals with ASD may be influenced by their level of functioning, these divergent results may have occurred in part due to differences in the severity and/or range of ASD symptoms in different cohorts. In a study which took into account ASD symptom severity, increased startle magnitude was only seen in higher functioning individuals (Kohl et al., 2014). Choice of auditory stimulus in the experimental paradigms used may also have influenced the ability of some studies to detect startle abnormalities, since it has been shown that adult ASD subjects exhibit increased startle responses to pleasant stimuli, but not to neutral or unpleasant stimuli (Dichter et al., 2010; Wilbarger et al., 2009). Alongside possible increases in startle magnitude, increases in startle latency have also been reported in ASD (Ornitz et al., 1993; Takahashi et al., 2016; Yuhas et al., 2011). Like startle mag nitude, startle latency may be influenced by the severity of ASD symptoms. Whereas high-functioning individuals with ASD may display greater startle magnitude, they have been reported to display no change in startle latency (Bernier et al., 2005). Normal startle magnitude and latency have been reported in FXS (Frankland et al., 2004; Yuhas et al., 2011). Overall, although these studies of acoustic startle in ASD are consistent with increased sensitivity to auditory stimulation and altered sensory processing, further work is required to clarify the influence of stimulus valence and other experimental parameters on startle measurements. A greater understanding of whether startle reactivity correlates with specific ASD symptom profiles will also shed light on the translational and diagnostic utility of these measures.
PPI is an additional startle measure which has been investigated in ASD and FXS and takes advantage of the acoustic startle response to quantify sensorimotor gating. Reports on disruptions of PPI in ASD and FXS have not been consistent to date. For ASD, many studies on both adults as well as children with autism found no change in PPI (Kohl et al., 2014; Oranje et al., 2013b; Ornitz et al., 1993; Takahashi et al., 2016; Yuhas et al., 2011), while others report reduced PPI at least in adults (McAlonan et al., 2002; Perry et al., 2007). One study found slightly higher PPI levels at 76 dB pre-pulses in 8–12 year old children with autism (Madsen et al., 2014). Although it is possible that experimental variability or cohort heterogeneity is preventing reliable detection of subtle PPI deficits in ASD, it is currently not possible to conclude that altered PPI is a robust feature of ASD.
In contrast, individuals with FXS have robustly disrupted PPI compared to normal controls and individuals with ASD only (Frankland et al., 2004; Yuhas et al., 2011). Interestingly, although PPI deficits have been identified in FXS subjects regardless of their comorbid ASD diagnosis, they have also been correlated with the severity of autistic traits (Frankland et al., 2004), repetitive behavior (Perry et al., 2007) and several subscales of Social Responsiveness Scale and the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (Takahashi et al., 2016). This suggests that the PPI deficits in FXS reflect pathological sensory processing which also underpins the emergence of autism-related phenotypes in FXS. It also suggests that, although abnormal PPI has not been conclusively demonstrated in ASD, it may still be an informative indicator of abnormal sensorimotor gating in ASD and related disorders.
3.4. Habituation
Baseline startle reactivity is greatly influenced by short-term and long-term habituation (Valsamis and Schmid, 2011), which occurs independently of PPI and has been investigated in ASD, but not (to our knowledge) in FXS. Habituation is a form of sensory filtering and there is evidence that short-term habituation is slower at least in subpopulations of autistic individuals (Ornitz et al., 1993; Perry et al., 2007). Other studies which did not find differences in short-term habituation levels in ASD (Kohl et al., 2014; McAlonan et al., 2002; Takahashi et al., 2016) may not have been well equipped to reveal any differences in the time scale of habituation, since blocks of responses before and after PPI testing were compared. Both short-term and long-term habituation deserve further investigation in children and adults with ASD and FXS.
3.5. MMN
MMN and other changes elicited by deviant auditory stimuli have also been investigated in ASD and FXS, in order to assess the integrity of pre-attentive auditory change detection mechanisms. Reports on MMN in ASD are highly inconsistent. Some studies report larger MMN amplitude in children with ASD (Ferri et al., 2003; Kujala, 2007; Lepisto et al., 2005, 2006), whereas others report smaller (Abdeltawwab and Baz, 2015; Ludlow et al., 2014) or equal amplitudes compared to typically developing children (Dunn et al., 2008; Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2003; Roberts et al., 2011; Seri et al., 1999; Weismuller et al., 2015). Given the scope of this review, only MMN amplitude data are considered here. For information on MMN latency we refer the reader to other recent reviews (Kujala, 2007; Näätänen et al., 2011; Orekhova and Stroganova, 2014; Seri et al., 2007).
One likely contributor to disparities in the MMN studies described above is experimental differences between MMN paradigms, particularly at the level of the auditory stimuli used. Therefore, when considering here whether conclusions can be drawn from the MMN literature to date, we have subdivided MMN studies based on a) whether auditory stimuli consisted of pure tones (i.e. non speech-like) or more complicated, speech-like stimuli, and b) whether frequency or duration deviants were used.
In pure tone MMN paradigms, children with ASD have been reported to show either less (Abdeltawwab and Baz, 2015; Dunn et al., 2008), more (Ferri et al., 2003) or normal MMN to frequency deviants (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2003; Weismuller et al., 2015). Attention may impact consistency of findings with pure tone frequency deviants, given that one study reported reduced MMN amplitude to changes in frequency of pure tones in children with ASD in unattended conditions, but equal amplitudes in attended conditions (Dunn et al., 2008). Although the above mentioned studies were typically conducted in unattended conditions, the nature of the distractor stimulus differed substantially between studies (e.g. animated silent movie, picture book, etc.), and may have had different salience for individuals of different ages within each study. Electrode placement may also play a role, given that in some work, MMN amplitude deficits to pure tone frequency deviants could only be detected in children with ASD when the topography of the electrodes was taken into account, since maximal MMN amplitude was observed at the frontal Fz electrode in controls and more posterior C3/4 electrodes in individuals with ASD (Gomot et al., 2002). While some studies only record at Fz (Abdeltawwab and Baz, 2015; Seri et al., 1999), others use the International 10–20 System to record at multiple sites (Weismuller et al., 2015). Inconsistent results found with pure tone paradigms may also have been caused by differences in composition of ASD cohorts. Overall, the data seems to suggest a decrease in MMN to pure tone deviants, although further work is required to confirm this effect.
In MMN paradigms employing speech-like stimuli, a disparate picture is also seen. There is some evidence indicating that MMN to complex speech-like deviants or vowel/word change is reduced in children with ASD compared to typically developing children (Kuhl et al., 2005; Kujala et al., 2010; Ludlow et al., 2014). However, other studies have not reported such diagnostic differences (Ceponiene et al., 2003; Kasai et al., 2005; Oram Cardy et al., 2005; Weismuller et al., 2015), or have reported enhanced MMN amplitude to frequency deviants or affective prosody in ASD or Asperger’s syndrome compared to typically developing children (Korpilahti et al., 2007; Lepisto et al., 2005, 2006). In the studies of Lepisto and colleagues, increased MMN in ASD and Asperger’s syndrome was not found at the usual frontal electrodes (Fz) but rather only at parietal electrodes, possibly reflecting the MMN in primary auditory cortex and reinforcing that differences in the locations of recording electrodes may impact the ability to detect MMN abnormalities in ASD. In circumstances where there may be increased MMN to frequency deviants in ASD, this may not be accompanied by concordant increases for duration deviants since studies reporting increased MMN to frequency deviants in ASD also report normal (Lepisto et al., 2005) or decreased (Lepisto et al., 2006) MMN to duration deviants.
Despite the disparity of MMN findings in both pure tone and speech-like stimulus paradigms, there are two areas in which a more consistent picture is emerging. Firstly, studies employing a range of paradigms have reported that normal lateralization of MMN is diminished or absent in ASD subjects compared to healthy controls (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2003; Korpilahti et al., 2007; Kuhl et al., 2005; Lepisto et al., 2006; Weismuller et al., 2015). Unfortunately, due to the placement of electrodes, many studies are not designed to compare MMN in both hemispheres. Secondly, multiple studies have reported a relationship between MMN and measures of ASD symptom severity (such as sensory profiles, auditory preference etc.). These studies, which found decreased MMN in ASD, reported that greater MMN deficits were associated with increased sensory sensitivity (Ludlow et al., 2014), decreased auditory preference for speech (Kuhl et al., 2005) and increased CARS score (Abdeltawwab and Baz, 2015). These observations suggest that, in some cohorts of individuals with ASD, impairment of sensory filtering and auditory change detection are associated with, and may underlie, sensory hypersensitivity and other autism-related phenotypes.
In summary, the results of studies on MMN in ASD are inconsistent, although there seems to be some evidence for deficient MMN amplitudes in children with ASD and FXS. It is challenging to determine which factors contribute most to this variability, given that studies differ in the nature of auditory stimulus and deviant, EEG recording locations, ages of subjects and severity of subjects’ ASD symptoms and comorbidities. Future studies should include larger sample sizes, so that comorbidity and levels of anxiety can be taken into account and the ASD cohort split in meaningful (DSM-V based) subcategories. Furthermore, studies should use standardized paradigms for measurements and data analyses, or at least report on a standard set of electrodes (in combination with interesting results from other electrodes), in order to facilitate comparisons of results across studies.
In FXS, MMN and other changes elicited by deviant auditory stimuli have also been investigated. In addition to the abnormally high amplitude, an inability of the N1 amplitude to distinguish between a standard tone of high probability of occurrence and an ‘oddball’ tone of low probability of occurrence has been described in FXS (Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b). In control subjects, the average N1 amplitude was smaller for the standard tone compared to the oddball tone, while in individuals with FXS, the standard and oddball tones elicited similar N1 responses (Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b). Enlargement of the N2b wave (Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b) and increased N2 latency (St Clair et al., 1987; Van der Molen et al., 2012a) have also been seen in individuals with FXS. However, despite a general increase in N2 amplitude, decreased MMN to pure tone frequency deviants has been reported in individuals with FXS (Van der Molen et al., 2012b). As was observed in ASD (Gomot et al., 2002), the maximal MMN amplitude was identified at different electrodes in FXS and control individuals (Van der Molen et al., 2012b). Given the scarcity of studies of MMN in FXS, further work is required to confirm increased MMN and altered sensory filtering in FXS.
3.6. Baseline and evoked changes in neural oscillations
Abnormal ‘baseline’ neural oscillations have been reported in ASD. In the resting state, increased power of low frequency (delta and theta) and high frequency (beta, gamma) oscillations, alongside decreased power of mid-range (alpha) oscillations, have been described in children and adults with ASD (Machado et al., 2013; Orekhova et al., 2007, 2008; van Diessen et al., 2014), (reviewed in Wang et al., 2013). In children aged 3–8 years with ASD, increased baseline gamma was negatively correlated with P1 suppression, such that increased baseline gamma was associated with greater impairment of P1 suppression (Orekhova et al., 2008).
Altered neural oscillatory responses to sensory stimulation have also been reported in ASD. Increased gamma power and decreased phase-locking of gamma oscillations in response to auditory stimuli have been described in individuals with ASD and/or their parents, as detected by MEG (McFadden et al., 2012; Rojas et al., 2011, 2008). Abnormal synchrony of oscillations across the theta and gamma bands in response to speech in ASD has also been described (Jochaut et al., 2015). The observed failure of theta activity in auditory cortex to downregulate gamma oscillations in response to speech was correlated with verbal impairment and general autism symptoms (Jochaut et al., 2015). These few above-mentioned studies are consistent with other studies using visual cues (such as faces) that have also revealed abnormal evoked oscillations in ASD, particularly in the gamma frequency range (Grice et al., 2001; Keehn et al., 2015; Wright et al., 2012). Overall, studies of baseline and evoked neural oscillations in ASD support a role for impaired resting state and auditory-evoked cortical activity in sensory processing deficits in ASD.
Although patterns of neural oscillations have not been extensively investigated In FXS, two studies have indicated that resting (baseline) power of neural oscillations, and underlying functional connectivity, are impaired in FXS. As observed in ASD, increased baseline theta (4–8 Hz) power occur has been described in FXS (Van der Molen and Van der Molen, 2013). Also consistent with ASD, decreased alpha power (in this case, in the upper alpha 10–12 Hz range) is also seen in individuals with FXS (Van der Molen and Van der Molen, 2013). In that study, oscillations in other frequency bands (beta 13–30 Hz and gamma 30–80 Hz) were not investigated. Finally, functional connectivity computed from the resting EEG phase lag index is reportedly altered in FXS, with increased resting theta functional connectivity and decreased resting alpha and beta functional connectivity mirroring diagnostic differences in baseline neural oscillations (van der Molen et al., 2014). These abnormalities in baseline neural oscillations suggests that, as in ASD, impaired resting state cortical activity may play a role in altered sensory processing and cognitive function in FXS.
A summary of auditory sensory processing abnormalities in ASD and FXS is provided in Table 1.
Table 1.
Comparison of sensory processing deficits in Autism Spectrum Disorder and Fragile X Syndrome, as measured by EEG, MEG and EMG. Refer to the text for discussion of possible reasons for divergent findings; *to our knowledge.
Objective measure of auditory sensory processing | Autism Spectrum Disorder | Fragile X syndrome |
---|---|---|
Event-related potentials | ||
P1 amplitude | Decreased P1 amplitude (Ceponiene et al., 2003; Lepisto et al., 2005; Stroganova et al., 2013); (Asperger’s syndrome- Madsen et al., 2015) | Not investigated/described* |
latency | Increased M50 (MEG P1) latency (Oram Cardy et al., 2008) | Not investigated/described* |
N1 amplitude | Decreased N1 amplitude (Brandwein et al., 2015; Bruneau et al., 1999; Gandal et al., 2010; Seri et al., 1999) Normal N1 amplitude (Ceponiene et al., 2003; Madsen et al., 2015) | Increased N1/M100 (MEG N1) amplitude (Castrén et al., 2003; Rojas et al., 2001; St Clair et al., 1987; Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b) |
latency | Increased N1/M100 (MEG N1) latency (Gage et al., 2003; Gandal et al., 2010; Korpilahti et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2010) Normal N1 latency (Bruneau et al., 1999; Ceponiene et al., 2003; Madsen et al., 2015) | Decreased M100 latency (MEG N1) (Rojas et al., 2001) Normal N1 latency (Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b) |
P2 amplitude | Not investigated/described* | Increased P2 amplitude (Van der Molen et al., 2012b) Normal P2 amplitude (Van der Molen et al., 2012a) |
latency | Not investigated/described* | Normal P2 latency (Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b) |
N2 amplitude | Decreased N2 amplitude (Lepisto et al., 2005) | Increased N2b amplitude (Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b) |
latency | Normal N2 latency (Lepisto et al., 2005) | Increased N2b latency (Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b) |
P3a,b amplitude (pure tones) | Increased P3a amplitude (childhood- Ferri et al., 2003) Decreased P3a amplitude (young adult- Ferri et al., 2003) Normal P3a amplitude (Lepisto et al., 2005, 2006) | Decreased P3a,b amplitude (Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b) |
amplitude (speech-like stimuli) | Decreased P3a amplitude (Ceponiene et al., 2003; Lepisto et al., 2005, 2006) | Not investigated/described* |
P1 (P50) suppression | Decreased P1 suppression (Orekhova et al., 2008); (Madsen et al., 2015) Normal P1 suppression (Kemner et al., 2002; Madsen et al., 2015; Magnee et al., 2009) | Not investigated/described* |
Startle reactivity, prepulse inhibition and startle habituation | ||
Acoustic startle response magnitude | Increased startle magnitude (Chamberlain et al., 2013; Dichter et al., 2010; Kohl et al., 2014; Takahashi et al., 2016; Wilbarger et al., 2009) Normal startle magnitude (Bernier et al., 2005; McAlonan et al., 2002; Salmond et al., 2003; Sterling et al., 2013; Yuhas et al., 2011) | Normal startle magnitude (Frankland et al., 2004) |
latency | Increased startle latency (Ornitz et al., 1993; Takahashi et al., 2016; Yuhas et al., 2011) Normal startle latency (Bernier et al., 2005) | Normal startle latency (Yuhas et al., 2011) |
PPI | Decreased PPI (adults (McAlonan et al., 2002; Perry et al., 2007)) Increased PPI (children (Madsen et al., 2014)) Normal PPI (adults and children (Kohl et al., 2014; Oranje et al., 2013b; Ornitz et al., 1993; Takahashi et al., 2016; Yuhas et al., 2011)) | Decreased PPI (Frankland et al., 2004; Yuhas et al., 2011) |
Habituation | Decreased habituation of ASR (Ornitz et al., 1993; Perry et al., 2007) | Not investigated/described* |
Mismatch negativity | ||
Pure tone-frequency deviant | Decreased MMN (Abdeltawwab and Baz, 2015; Dunn et al., 2008) Increased MMN (Ferri et al., 2003) Normal MMN (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2003; Weismuller et al., 2015) | Decreased MMN (pure tone frequency deviant) (Van der Molen et al., 2012b) |
Pure tone-duration deviant | Decreased MMN (Lepisto et al., 2005, 2006) Normal MMN (Weismuller et al., 2015) | Not investigated/described* |
Speech-like stimuli-frequency deviant, word/vowel change | Decreased MMN (Kuhl et al., 2005; Kujala et al., 2010; Ludlow et al., 2014) Increased MMN (Korpilahti et al., 2007; Lepisto et al., 2005, 2006) Normal MMN (Ceponiene et al., 2003; Kasai et al., 2005; Oram Cardy et al., 2005; Weismuller et al., 2015) | Not investigated/described* |
Speech-like stimuli-duration deviant | Decreased MMN (Lepisto et al., 2006) Normal MMN (Lepisto et al., 2005). | Not investigated/described* |
MMN-all stimuli | Decreased MMN lateralization (Korpilahti et al., 2007; Kuhl et al., 2005; Weismuller et al., 2015) Abnormal MMN lateralization (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2003; Lepisto et al., 2006) | Not investigated/described* |
Neural oscillations | ||
Baseline power of oscillations | Increased baseline delta (1–3 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz) and gamma (30–80 Hz) power (Machado et al., 2013; Orekhova et al., 2007, 2008; van Diessen et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2013) | Increased baseline theta power (4–8 Hz) (Van der Molen and Van der Molen, 2013) |
Decreased baseline alpha (8–12 Hz) power (Machado et al., 2013; Orekhova et al., 2007, 2008; van Diessen et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2013) | Decreased baseline upper-alpha power (10–12 Hz) (Van der Molen and Van der Molen, 2013) | |
Not investigated/described* | Increased resting theta functional connectivity (van der Molen et al., 2014) | |
Not investigated/described* | Decreased resting upper alpha and beta functional connectivity (van der Molen et al., 2014) | |
Auditory-evoked power/synchrony of oscillations | Increased auditory-evoked gamma power (McFadden et al., 2012; Rojas et al., 2001, 2008) Decreased evoked synchrony across theta and gamma frequency bands (Jochaut et al., 2015) |
Not investigated/described* Not investigated/described* |
4. Sensory processing in rodent models relevant to FXS and ASD
4.1. Animal models relevant to FXS
The fragile X mental retardation 1 (Fmr1) knock out (KO) mouse is a pre-clinical animal model to study the pathophysiology underlying FXS with well-characterized construct and face validity (Bakker et al., 1994). Fmr1 KO mice lack the Fragile X Mental Retardation Protein (FMRP) and show several FXS-and ASD-like symptoms (Bernardet and Crusio, 2006), including social impairments (Spencer et al., 2005), repetitive behaviors (Crawley, 2004, 2007), heightened anxiety (Bilousova et al., 2009), hyperactivity that may be related to increased arousal in a novel environment (Kramvis et al., 2013) and altered ultrasonic vocalization communication by both pups and adults (Rotschafer et al., 2012; Roy et al., 2012). Physical alterations seen in humans such as macro-orchidism are also seen in the Fmr1 KO mouse (Lachiewicz and Dawson, 1994; Sidhu et al., 2014). One caveat to consider in examining predictive validity in the mouse model include the fact that in the Fmr1 KO mouse, the protein FMRP is virtually absent, whereas in humans with FXS, the number of CGG repeats influences FMRP levels and the range of resulting deficits. It is also important to note that there are mouse strain specific differences that interfere with different measures (Bernardet and Crusio, 2006). Nevertheless, the Fmr1 KO mouse recapitulates several FXS-like symptoms and is a commonly used pre-clinical model (Kooy et al., 1996). Most encouragingly, Fmr1 KO mice also exhibit sensory system deficits. These deficits have been measured in terms of increased susceptibility to audiogenic seizures, increased response of sensory cortex to stimuli (Rotschafer and Razak, 2014; Zhang et al., 2014), increased UP states in sensory cortex (Hays et al., 2011), increased spontaneous and correlated activity in sensory cortex (Goncalves et al., 2013) and reduced habituation of sensory stimulus evoked responses (Lovelace et al., 2016). Some of these deficits may arise due to abnormal critical period plasticity during development (Contractor et al., 2015; Kim et al., 2013).
Prominent deficits appear consistently in auditory sensitivity. FMRP is expressed across multiple levels of the auditory neuraxis (Wang et al., 2014). Genetic removal of FMRP in the mouse model results in increased propensity for audiogenic seizures, suggesting hypersensitive auditory responses. In Fmr1 KO mice, auditory stimuli with sound levels >100 dB SPL can cause a period of wild running, tonic/clonic seizing, and death (Chen and Toth, 2001; Musumeci et al., 2000, 2007). Reintroduction of FMRP to Fmr1 KO mice significantly reduced audiogenic seizure susceptibility (Musumeci et al., 2007). In terms of acoustic startle responses and their modulations, Fmr1 KO mice seemed to have an opposite phenotype to what was found in human studies: they display lower startle responses, and higher PPI (Chen and Toth, 2001; Frankland et al., 2004; Olmos-Serrano et al., 2011; Yun et al., 2006), although one group found PPI to be normal in these mice PPI (Uutela et al., 2012). However, Nielsen et al. (2002) showed that startle responses were decreased only at higher amplitude stimuli, and actually increased at lower startle stimulus amplitudes, resembling individuals with FXS in their increased sensitivity to low intensity auditory stimuli. It would be interesting to see if PPI in Fmr1 KO mice also depends on stimuli intensity and is increased at lower prepulse/pulse combinations.
Recent studies have revealed an exciting potential connection between the molecular alterations in FXS and startle habituation mechanisms: it has been shown that FMRP directly interacts with synaptic BK channels, which in turn have been shown to be crucial for short-term habituation (Deng et al., 2013; Hebert et al., 2014; Typlt et al., 2013b; Zhang et al., 2014). Unfortunately, startle habituation is not well studied in humans with FXS or Fmr1 ko mice, however, the allosteric BK channel activator BMS-204352 was shown to reverse behavioural and cellular FXS symptoms in Fmr1 ko mice (Hebert et al., 2014), highlighting the potential of studying sensory filtering mechanisms in animal models for developing pharmaceutical treatments. More potent and specific BK channels modulators are currently developed and in clinical trials (mostly for stroke and/or epilepsy treatment), however, the abundant BK channel expression in the periphery, e.g. in the myocardium, vasculature, and bladder, pose a major challenge.
To address the mechanisms that may underlie the differences in auditory processing in FXS, (Rotschafer and Razak, 2014) performed in vivo extracellular single neuron recordings from the auditory cortex of the Fmr1 ko and wildtype (WT) mice. Fmr1 ko mouse cortex showed four main processing deficits in the primary auditory cortex (A1) at the single neuron level: (1) Hyper-excitability: cortical neurons in KO mice respond with more action potentials to tones than WT neurons. The increased response was due to prolonged spiking indicating that the KO neurons failed to shut down their responses after sound offset. This may underlie the hyper-excitability phenotype in both humans with FXS and the mouse model (2) Increased variability: the first spike latency in response to tones in KO mice neurons was more variable indicating that the neurons were not providing consistent information for repetitions of the same sound. (3) Broader receptive fields: the excitatory frequency tuning curves are broader in the KO mice neurons. This indicates that for a given tone, more neurons will be activated. This may explain the larger amplitude and more widespread activation of the auditory cortex in FXS. (4) Reduced spectrotemporal selectivity: reduction in selectivity for the rate of frequency modulated (FM) sweeps. This was because the KO neurons responded better to much shorter duration sounds and faster changes in frequency that WT neurons. The robust responses to sounds as short as <3 ms in KO neurons is typically not seen in WT neurons and indicates that the KO neurons have a different temporal integration window and are easier to stimulate than WT neurons.
The in vivo single neuron electrophysiology data suggest deficits in spectral and temporal processing in auditory cortex of KO mice that may relate to auditory deficits in humans with FXS. Preliminary data based on auditory ERPs from the Fmr1 KO mouse cortex suggests a more directly translation relevant phenotype in the mouse (Lovelace et al., 2016). Auditory ERPs were measured using electrodes placed between 200 and 400 μm deep in the primary auditory cortex of anesthetized Fmr1 KO and WT mice. Repeated presentation of a sound stimulus (tones or noise) at various repetition rates (0.25–5 Hz) showed a robust reduction in habituation of the N1 component for repetition rates faster than 1 Hz in the Fmr1 KO mice. The similar habituation deficit in auditory ERP habituation in humans and mice provides a useful translation relevant functional marker to include in drug development for FXS. The deficit in early sensory processing will also allow an examination of circuit and cellular deficits (Gibson et al., 2008; Hays et al., 2011; Selby et al., 2007) that may be more tractable than complex social behaviors and importantly conserved across species.
4.2. Animal models of possible etiologic factors in idiopathic ASD
There is a huge range of transgenic mouse lines which model possible etiologic factors in ASD and show analogies to the diagnostic symptoms of autism, including low social interactions, reduced vocalizations in social settings, and high levels of repetitive self-grooming. Table 2 lists relevant rodent strains and their respective ERP, MMN, startle reactivity, PPI and neural oscillation abnormalities. Most of them show distinct alterations in startle reactivity and/or PPI, which has been the focus of sensory processing characterization in rodent models of ASD even though findings in humans with ASD are not consistent. In contrast, ERPs, MMN, habituation and neural oscillations have been less commonly assessed even though abnormalities of these measures in ASD are more replicable. Some strains have been studied further for sensory processing abnormalities. For example, contactin associated like protein 2 (Cntnap2) was first associated with Specific Language Impairment (SLI) and has been linked to ASD. Cntnap2 homozygous null mice have shown distinct alterations in auditory processing, i.e. in silent gap detection and pitch discrimination (Rendall et al., 2015; Truong et al., 2015), and increased PPI (Brunner et al., 2015). Of special interest is also the BK-channel knock-out mouse that shows disruptions in short-term habituation of startle, along with repetitive self-grooming, mild PPI deficits, and specific cognitive deficits (Typlt et al., 2013a,b). As mentioned above, BK channels are crucial for short-term habituation, Fragile x mental retardation protein directly interacts with synaptic BK channels (Deng et al., 2013; Hebert et al., 2014; Typlt et al., 2013b; Zhang et al., 2014).
Table 2.
Changes in sensory processing measures in rodent models that show analogies to the diagnostic symptoms of autism, including low social interactions, reduced vocalizations in social settings, and/or high levels of repetitive self-grooming. Findings in ASD and FXS are provided at the top of the table for comparison purposes.
ERPs | MMN | Startle | PPI | Neural oscillations | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Human disorders (refer to Table 1 for references) | |||||
ASD | Decreased P1, N1, N2 and P3a amplitudes Increased N1 latency | Decreased (?) | Increased | ? | Increased baseline theta and gamma Decreased baseline alpha power |
FXS | Increased N1, P2 and N2b amplitudes | Decreased | = | Decreased | Increased baseline theta power Decreased baseline alpha power |
ASD- and FXS-relevant mouse models | |||||
Fmr1 KO | =Decreased habituation of N1 amplitude (Lovelace et al., 2016) | ? | Decreased (Chen and Toth, 2001; Olmos-Serrano et al., 2011) | Increased (Chen and Toth, 2001; Frankland et al., 2004; Olmos-Serrano et al., 2011) | ? |
Prenatal VPA | Increased N1 latency (Gandal et al., 2010) | ? | =(Schneider and Przewlocki, 2005; Schneider et al., 2006) | Decreased (Schneider and Przewlocki, 2005; Schneider et al., 2006) | ? |
MeCP2 T158A | Decreased N1 and P2 amplitudes Increased P1, N1 and P2 latencies (Goffin et al., 2011). | ? | ? | ? | Increased baseline gamma power Decreased evoked theta, alpha and gamma power/ITC (Goffin et al., 2011) |
MeCP2 +/− | Increased N1 amplitude Increased P2 latency (Liao et al., 2012) | ? | Decreased (Chao et al., 2010; Samaco et al., 2008; Samaco et al., 2013) | Increased (Chao et al., 2010; Samaco et al., 2008; Samaco et al., 2013) | Increased evoked gamma power/ITC (Liao et al., 2012) |
MeCP2 (interneuron KO) | Decreased (Chao et al., 2010) | Increased (Chao et al., 2010) | Decreased evoked theta, alpha and gamma power/ITC (Goffin et al., 2014) | ||
C58/J | ? | ? | Decreased (Moy et al., 2014) | Increased (Moy et al., 2014) | ? |
Ephrin-A2/-A3 KO | ? | ? | Decreased (Wurzman et al., 2015) | Increased (Wurzman et al., 2015) | ? |
Gabrb3 +/− | ? | ? | Decreased (DeLorey et al., 2011) | Increased (DeLorey et al., 2011) | ? |
BK channel KO | ? | ? | Decreased (Typlt et al., 2013a) | Decreased (Typlt et al., 2013a) | ? |
P2 × 4 receptor KO | ? | ? | Decreased (Wyatt et al., 2013) | Decreased (Wyatt et al., 2013) | ? |
NL-3 mutant (R451C) | ? | ? | Decreased (Chadman et al., 2008) | =(Chadman et al., 2008) | ? |
NL-3 KO | ? | ? | =(Radyushkin et al., 2009) | =(Radyushkin et al., 2009) | ? |
BTBR T+ Itpr3 tf/J | ? | ? | =(Silverman et al., 2010) | =(Silverman et al., 2010) | ? |
16p11.2 +/− | ? N/A | ? N/A | =(Brunner et al., 2015) mice deaf (Portmann et al., 2014; Yang et al., 2015) | =(Brunner et al., 2015) mice deaf (Portmann et al., 2014; Yang et al., 2015) | ? N/A |
PDGFR-β KO | ? | ? | =(Nakamura et al., 2015; Nguyen et al., 2011) | Decreased (Nakamura et al., 2015; Nguyen et al., 2011) | Decreased gamma power and ITC (Nakamura et al., 2015; Nguyen et al., 2011) |
Neurexin-1α KO | ? | ? | =(mouse) (Etherton et al., 2009) Increased (rat) (Esclassan et al., 2015) | Decreased (mouse) (Etherton et al., 2009) =(rat) (Esclassan et al., 2015) | ? |
PWS-IC(+/−) | ? | ? | Increased (Relkovic et al., 2010) | Decreased (Relkovic et al., 2010) | ? |
Th(tk-)/th (tk-) | ? | ? | Increased (Klejbor et al., 2009) | Decreased (Klejbor et al., 2009) | ? |
Npas4-KO | ? | ? | Increased (Coutellier et al., 2012) | Decreased (Coutellier et al., 2012) | ? |
Cntnap2 KO | ? | ? | =(Brunner et al., 2015) | Increased (Brunner et al., 2015) | ? |
Nrcam-KO | ? | ? | =(Moy et al., 2009) | Decreased (males) (Moy et al., 2009) | ? |
Shank3 | ? | ? | =(Kouser et al., 2013) | =(Kouser et al., 2013) | ? |
KO – knockout, ITC – inter-trial coherence, N/A – not applicable.
Apart from genetic factors, maternal infections and exposure to valproate (VPA) during pregnancy have been shown to greatly increase the risk for a child to develop autism. This has led to the establishment of respective animal models. One of the best established ASD models are intraperitoneal injections of 600 mg VPA at gestation day 12.5 in rats which causes cardinal symptoms of ASD in the offspring (reviewed by Roullet et al., 2013). Prenatal VPA treatment increases ERP N1 latency, decreases evoked gamma power and decreases gamma phase locking (Gandal et al., 2010). It also reduces PPI in young rodents, and to a lesser extent in adult rodents, while startle response amplitudes seem to be unchanged (Schneider and Przewlocki, 2005; Schneider et al., 2006). Postnatal treatment with 150 mg/kg VPA for six days reduces startle responses and impairs PPI in adult rats as well (Reynolds et al., 2012). Interestingly, offspring of a male rat of advanced paternal age (another risk factor for ASD identified in humans; Sandin et al., 2015) has also shown to reduce startle reactivity and impair PPI (Milekic et al., 2015).
Maternal infections during pregnancy can be mimicked by injections of Lipopolysaccharides (LPS) or Polyinosinic:polycytidylic acid (poly-IC). Prenatal LPS seems to increase startle reactivity at least in male offspring (Foley et al., 2015) and induces PPI deficits (Fortier et al., 2007). Prenatal immune challenge by poly-IC leads to either less startle reactivity in rats (Vorhees et al., 2015), or unchanged startle responses (Howland et al., 2012; Meyer et al., 2008), but consistent with VPA models disrupts PPI in the offspring (Howland et al., 2012; Meyer et al., 2008; Vorhees et al., 2015). Likewise, influenza virus injection at gestational day 9.5 in mice leads to PPI deficits in adolescence (Shi et al., 2003).
In conclusion, a range of different rodent models show some or all of the cardinal symptoms of ASD, have face and/or construct validity, and show changes in startle reactivity and/or PPI, indicating that sensory processing and sensory filtering is affected in these models. Changes in startle responsiveness, habituation, and PPI depend greatly on the specific parameters used, such as stimulus intensities and temporal properties. Unfortunately, parameters vary substantially between studies. This might explain some of the variability in startle modulations between different animal models. Like in human studies, some variability might be inherent, due to the fact that different perturbations all lead to autistic symptoms, while having different (even opposite) effects on sensory processing and sensory filtering. Nevertheless, these animal models provide the unique opportunity to study cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying the emergence of ASD and FXS symptoms.
4.3. Animal models of circuit changes that recapitulate ASD phenotypes
Abnormal ERPs and neural oscillations, akin to deficits in ASD, are evident in a number of rodent models of disrupted excitatory/inhibitory balance. Three such models, of constitutive pan NMDA receptor hypofunction (Gandal et al., 2012a,b), cell-type specific NMDA receptor ablation (Saunders et al., 2013) and transient NMDA receptor blockade (Saunders et al., 2012), have shed light on mechanisms which may underlie the emergence of electrophysiological and sensory processing deficits in ASD (Table 3).
Table 3.
Changes in sensory processing measures in rodent models of disrupted excitatory/inhibitory balance that recapitulate ASD phenotypes. Findings in ASD and FXS are provided at the top of the table for comparison purposes.
ERPs | MMN | Startle | PPI | Neural oscillations | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Human disorders (refer to Table 1 for references) | |||||
ASD | Decreased P1, N1, N2 and P3a amplitudes Increased N1 latency |
Decreased (?) | Increased | ? | Increased baseline theta and gamma Decreased baseline alpha power |
FXS | Increased N1, P2 and N2b amplitudes | Decreased | = | Decreased | Increased baseline theta power Decreased baseline alpha power |
ASD- and FXS-relevant mouse models | |||||
GluN1neo−/− | Increased N1 latency (Gandal et al., 2012a) | ? | Increased (Duncan et al., 2004; Gandal et al., 2012a,b; Moy et al., 2014) | Decreased (Duncan et al., 2004; Gandal et al., 2012a,b; Moy et al., 2014) | Increased baseline theta and gamma Decreased evoked gamma power/ITC (Gandal et al., 2012b) |
PV-selective GluN1 KO | Increased N1 latency (Billingslea et al., 2014; Saunders et al., 2013) | ? | ? | = (Carlén et al., 2012) | Increased baseline gamma (Billingslea et al., 2014; Carlén et al., 2012) |
NMDAR blockade | Increased N1 latency (Saunders et al., 2012) | Decreased (Ehrlichman et al., 2008) | Increased (Bakshi and Geyer, 1998; Bast et al., 2000) | Decreased (Bakshi and Geyer, 1998; Bast et al., 2000) | Increased baseline gamma power Decreased evoked gamma power/ITC (Saunders et al., 2012) |
KO – knockout, ITC – inter-trial coherence.
Mice which have widespread, constitutively decreased expression of the obligatory GluN1 subunit of the NMDA receptor (GluN1neo−/−) display disrupted sensory processing and other ASD relevant phenotypes. In the hippocampus, GluN1neo−/− mice exhibit delayed N1 latency, increased baseline gamma power, decreased auditory evoked gamma power and decreased evoked gamma phase synchrony/inter-trial coherence (ITC) (Gandal et al., 2012a,b; Halene et al., 2009), reminiscent of ASD (Bruneau et al., 1999; Gage et al., 2003; Gandal et al., 2010; Machado et al., 2013; Orekhova et al., 2007, 2008; van Diessen et al., 2014). ERP and gamma frequency abnormalities were accompanied by decreased PPI, decreased premating ultrasonic vocalizations, diminished social preference, decreased anxiety-like behavior and stereotyped behaviors (Gandal et al., 2012a,b; Halene et al., 2009). A number of other impairments in GluN1neo−/− mice point to generalized neuronal hyper-excitability due to dysfunction of fast spiking par-valbumin (PV)-positive interneurons as the central pathology in these mice, consistent with a number of other ASD mouse models including the neuroligin-3 R451C, PDGFR-β KO and prenatal VPA models which also show PV interneuron pathology (Gogolla et al., 2009; Nakamura et al., 2015). This is also similar to the PV neuron dysfunction observed in the cortex of Fmr1 KO mice (Gibson et al., 2008; Hays et al., 2011). Impairments in the GluN1neo−/− mice included decreased protein expression of the GABA-A receptor and parvalbumin, and increased pyramidal neuron intrinsic excitability (Gandal et al., 2012b). Abnormal PV protein expression has also been observed in ASD but in contrast to GluN1neo−/− mice, PV immunoreactivity was increased in postmortem ASD tissue (Lawrence et al., 2010). Interestingly, the constellation of EEG abnormalities and behavioral impairments were remediated by treatment with baclofen, a GABA-B agonist (Gandal et al., 2012b), lending preclinical support to a therapeutic approach trialed in ASD and highlighting the utility of EEG in rodent studies as a measure of treatment responsiveness.
Cell-type specific NMDA receptor ablation also results in ASD-relevant circuit changes and behavioral deficits (Carlén et al., 2012; Saunders et al., 2013), but to a lesser extent than the constitutive knockdown described above. Knockout of GluN1 selectively in PV-positive interneurons increased N1 latency in auditory cortex and deceased social preference (Billingslea et al., 2014; Saunders et al., 2013). Decreased premating ultrasonic vocalizations in PV-selective GluN1 KO mice were also described, but other behaviors such as spatial working memory were intact. Increased baseline gamma power and decreased light-evoked gamma power have also been also identified in PV-selective GluN1 KO mice using EEG (Billingslea et al., 2014) and also local field potential measurement and optogenetic stimulation (Carlén et al., 2012), but baseline gamma increases were less prominent when recorded in awake animals using EEG. These electrophysiological abnormalities were accompanied by impaired habituation and subtle spatial working memory deficits (Carlén et al., 2012). Consistent with these studies, and supporting the presence of an ASD-relevant phenotype in mice lacking the NMDA receptor in interneurons, mice expressing Cre recombinase under the Ppplr2 promoter (expressed predominantly in interneurons) displayed impaired neuronal synchrony (measured by single unit recordings), alongside decreased PPI and spatial working memory (Belforte et al., 2010). These findings further strengthen the validity of rodent models of NMDA receptor hypofunction, particularly in PV-positive interneurons, for investigation of sensory processing in ASD.
Finally, transient blockade of NMDA receptors can also mimic sensory processing deficits seen in ASD. Treatment of mice with NMDA receptor antagonist MK801 increases N1 ERP latency, disrupts PPI and baseline gamma power, but decreases evoked gamma power and phase synchrony (ITC) and (Bakshi and Geyer, 1998; Bast et al., 2000; Saunders et al., 2012), resulting in a phenotype similar to that seen in ASD (Gandal et al., 2010; McFadden et al., 2012; Orekhova et al., 2008; Rojas et al., 2011, 2008). Treatment with ketamine, an NMDA receptor antagonist which also acts at other sites, also increases baseline gamma power and decreases MMN (Ehrlichman et al., 2009; Ehrlichman et al., 2008; Lazarewicz et al., 2010; Long et al., 2015). However, it decreases rather than increases N1 latency (Connolly et al., 2004; Maxwell et al., 2006) and increases rather than decreases evoked gamma power (Lazarewicz et al., 2010). Interestingly, chronic ketamine exposure in juvenile mice rather than adults induces different deficits in ERPs and neural oscillations (Featherstone et al., 2014). Juvenile ketamine exposure (from post-natal days 28–42) does not increase baseline gamma oscillations over and above the normal developmental increase in gamma power, but results in the delayed emergence of decreased total event-related gamma power and decreased P20 amplitude (Featherstone et al., 2014). Broadly speaking, the deficits of baseline and auditory-evoked neural oscillations arising from pharmacologically-induced NMDA receptor hypofunction mirror deficits seen in constitutively GluN1-underexpressing GluN1neo−/− mice (Gandal et al., 2012b), but likely depend on age of exposure and the pharmacological properties of the antagonist. Overall, these models suggest that transgenic rodents whose neurological and behavioral abnormalities recapitulate deficits in ASD may be useful tools in understanding underlying cellular and circuit pathologies in the disorders.
5. Key similarities and differences in sensory processing measures between ASD and FXS
As the most common known genetic cause of ASD, FXS can be used powerfully as a window into pathophysiological mechanisms in ASD. However, only one quarter and one third of individuals with FXS are also diagnosed with ASD (Bailey et al., 2008; Rogers et al., 2001), and individuals with FXS who do not have ASD display sensory processing abnormalities similar to, but not the same as, those seen in ASD. It follows, therefore, that some objective sensory processing measures which are similarly dysregulated in FXS and ASD may shed light on mechanisms underlying shared sensory processing deficits, while other measures which display different patterns of dysregulation may illuminate aspects of sensory processing which are distinct in the two disorders.
The sensory processing measures which appear to be most consistent between FXS and ASD are sensory filtering and attentional orienting measures, MMN and the P3a respectively. These measures are both evaluated in paradigms which feature rare, deviant stimuli and can be tailored to target filtering of speech or non-speech auditory stimuli. Although findings with MMN in ASD are variable, many studies have reported decreased MMN (Abdeltawwab and Baz, 2015; Dunn et al., 2008; Kuhl et al., 2005; Kujala et al., 2010; Lepisto et al., 2005, 2006; Ludlow et al., 2014), which is negatively correlated with symptom severity (Abdeltawwab and Baz, 2015; Kuhl et al., 2005; Ludlow et al., 2014). Decreased MMN is also reported in FXS (Van der Molen et al., 2012b). Similarly, a consistent picture of decreased P3a amplitude, particularly with speech-like stimuli in individuals greater than 10 years of age, has emerged (Ceponiene et al., 2003; Ferri et al., 2003; Lepisto et al., 2005, 2006), consistent with the decreased P3a amplitude observed in FXS (Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b). It is plausible that sensory filtering and attentional orienting mechanisms underlying MMN and the P3a represent points of convergence for sensory processing deficits in FXS and ASD. In contrast, ERP abnormalities such as N1 amplitude, N1 latency and N2 amplitude consistently display opposite patterns of dysregulation in FXS and ASX (Brandwein et al., 2015; Bruneau et al., 1999; Castrén et al., 2003; Gage et al., 2003; Gandal et al., 2010; Korpilahti et al., 2007; Lepisto et al., 2005; Roberts et al., 2010; Rojas et al., 2001; Seri et al., 1999; St Clair et al., 1987; Van der Molen et al., 2012a,b). As a result, it is possible that early auditory processing mechanisms supporting generation of early ERP components are dysregulated in different ways in FXS and ASD, resulting in divergent sensory processing abnormalities in some areas. It is important to note that more work needs to be done in FXS to examine spectral characteristics of EEGs to determine if similarities with ASD exist in these measurements.
One way in which future studies of FXS could further strengthen understanding of ASD would be for such studies to subgroup FXS individuals into those with and without ASD. Subgrouping of those with comorbid intellectual disability and those without would also be beneficial. Although challenging due to the low incidence of FXS, such an approach would enable disentangling of the mechanisms underlying the emergence of ASD phenotypes from those underlying other symptom features of FXS.
The models described above also have cross-diagnostic utility for understanding impairments of sensory processing. Although the main focus of this review is on autism and FSX, it is important to realize that many of the electrophysiological measures that are described in this review have also been implicated in schizophrenia. Similarities such as increased baseline gamma power, decreased evoked gamma power, decreased P1 suppression and decreased PPI (Braff et al., 1992; Freedman et al., 1996, 1983; Hanlon et al., 2005; Kwon et al., 1999; Spencer et al., 2003), as well as decreased MMN amplitude in both disorders (Atkinson et al., 2012; Baldeweg et al., 2002) have been reported in both ASD and schizophrenia. However, other studies indicate that there might be marked differences, such as that some studies found normal levels of both PPI and P1 suppression in children with autism (Kemner et al., 2002; Kohl et al., 2014; Madsen et al., 2015; Oranje et al., 2013b; Ornitz et al., 1993), while deficient levels are robustly found in schizophrenia (Aggernaes et al., 2010; Braff et al., 2001; Oranje et al., 2013a; Thibaut et al., 2015). There seems to be at least some overlap between schizophrenia and autism symptoms: in two Danish register based studies it was shown that, dependent on the specific form of autism, up to 30% of individuals with a childhood diagnose of autism develop a schizophrenia spectrum disorder later in life (Mouridsen et al., 2008a,b), while parental reports suggest that up to 60% of patients with schizophrenia have had a history with autistic symptoms (Unenge Hallerback et al., 2012).
6. Overlapping sensory processing deficits in ASD and schizophrenia
Although the main focus of this review is on autism and FSX, it is important to realize that most of the electrophysiological measures that are described in this review have also been implicated in schizophrenia. Similarities such as increased baseline gamma power, decreased evoked gamma power, decreased P1 suppression and decreased PPI (Braff et al., 1992; Freedman et al., 1996, 1983; Hanlon et al., 2005; Kwon et al., 1999; Spencer et al., 2003), as well as decreased MMN amplitude in both disorders (Atkinson et al., 2012; Baldeweg et al., 2002) have been reported in both ASD and schizophrenia. However, other studies indicate that there might be marked differences, such as that some studies found normal levels of both PPI and P1 suppression in children with autism (Kemner et al., 2002; Kemner et al., 1995; Kohl et al., 2014; Madsen et al., 2015; Oranje et al., 2013b; Ornitz et al., 1993), while deficient levels are robustly found in schizophrenia (Aggernaes et al., 2010; Braff et al., 2001; Oranje et al., 2013a; Thibaut et al., 2015). There seems to be at least some overlap between schizophrenia and autism symptoms: in two Danish register based studies it was shown that, dependent on the specific form of autism, up to 30% of individuals with a childhood diagnose of autism develop a schizophrenia spectrum disorder later in life (Mouridsen et al., 2008a,b), while parental reports suggest that up to 60% of patients with schizophrenia have had a history with autistic symptoms (Unenge Hallerback et al., 2012).
7. Concluding discussion
Sensory dysfunction, particularly in the auditory domain, is consistently seen in ASD and FXS. Basic sensory processing circuitry may be relatively more tractable compared to circuits involved in social communication and cognitive aspects of ASD. Circuitry involved in basic sensory processing may also be more conserved across humans and mice compared to circuits involved in cognitive and social communication. These observations suggest that sensory processing offers a unique opportunity to understand the patho-physiology of ASD/FXS at a circuit and cellular level. Future studies are required to elucidate the molecular mechanisms of altered auditory subcortical and cortical processing and how these differences correlate with auditory behaviors in ASD and FXS.
ASD and FXS are neurodevelopmental disorders, but little is known about how the auditory processing deficits develop, and how these deficits impact the further development of the brain. Therefore, additional behavioural and anatomical studies in humans and animals, as well as in vivo single neuron and ERP recordings in animals, are required. Such studies may identify correlations between developmental hyper-excitability, habituation deficits and responses to treatments, enabling identification of specific patient subgroups suited to specific therapeutic approaches. In rats and mice, it will be important to study how proteins implicated in ASD and FXS, such as CNTNAP2 and FMRP, contribute to the normal developmental maturation of neurons, auditory circuits, and e.g. ERP responses. Ultimately, studies of sensory processing in ASD and FXS may reveal mechanisms underlying developmental disruptions in ASD and FXS, offering hope for individually targeted, age-specific therapeutic approaches in the future.
Acknowledgments
DS is supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council CJ Martin Fellowship (#1072878). SJS relevant work supported by NIH R01 MH075916-05, and NIH 5P50MH096891-03. KAR is supported by NIH Center for Collaborative Research on Fragile X Syndrome grant (1U54HD082008-01). SS is supported by CIHR (operating grant), and NSERC.
References
- Abdeltawwab MM, Baz H. Automatic pre-attentive auditory responses: MMN to tone burst frequency changes in autistic school-age children. J Int Adv Otol. 2015;11:36–41. doi: 10.5152/iao.2014.438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggernaes B, Glenthoj BY, Ebdrup BH, Rasmussen H, Lublin H, Oranje B. Sensorimotor gating and habituation in antipsychoticnaive, first-episode schizophrenia patients before and after 6 months’ treatment with quetiapine. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;13:1383–1395. doi: 10.1017/S1461145710000787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atkinson RJ, Michie PT, Schall U. Duration mismatch negativity and P3a in first-episode psychosis and individuals at ultra-high risk of psychosis. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71:98–104. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.08.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey DB, Raspa M, Olmsted M, Holiday DB. Co-occurring conditions associated with FMR1 gene variations: findings from a national parent survey. Am J Med Genet A. 2008;146:2060–2069. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.32439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bakker CE, Verheij C, Willemsen R, Vanderhelm R, Oerlemans F, Vermey M, Bygrave A, Hoogeveen AT, Oostra BA, Reyniers E, Deboulle K, Dhooge R, Cras P, Vanvelzen D, Nagels G, Martin JJ, Dedeyn PP, Darby JK, Willems PJ. Fmr1 knockout mice: a model to study fragile X mental retardation. Cell. 1994;78:23–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bakshi VP, Geyer MA. Multiple limbic regions mediate the disruption of prepulse inhibition produced in rats by the noncompetitive NMDA antagonist dizocilpine. J Neurosci. 1998;18:8394–8401. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-20-08394.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldeweg T, Klugman A, Gruzelier JH, Hirsch SR. Impairment in frontal but not temporal components of mismatch negativity in schizophrenia. Int J Psychophysiol. 2002;43:111–122. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(01)00183-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baranek GT, Roberts JE, David FJ, Sideris J, Mirrett PL, Hatton DD, Bailey DB., Jr Developmental trajectories and correlates of sensory processing in young boys with fragile X syndrome. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr. 2008;28:79–98. doi: 10.1300/j006v28n01_06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bast T, Zhang W, Feldon J, White IM. Effects of MK801 and neuroleptics on prepulse inhibition: re-examination in two strains of rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2000;67:647–658. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(00)00409-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belforte JE, Zsiros V, Sklar ER, Jiang Z, Yu G, Li Y, Quinlan EM, Nakazawa K. Postnatal NMDA receptor ablation in corticolimbic interneurons confers schizophrenia-like phenotypes. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:76–83. doi: 10.1038/nn.2447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belmonte MK, Bourgeron T. Fragile X syndrome and autism at the intersection of genetic and neural networks. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:1221–1225. doi: 10.1038/nn1765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Sasson A, Cermak SA, Orsmond GI, Tager-Flusberg H, Carter AS, Kadlec MB, Dunn W. Extreme sensory modulation behaviors in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Am J Occup Ther. 2007;61:584–592. doi: 10.5014/ajot.61.5.584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Sasson A, Cermak SA, Orsmond GI, Tager-Flusberg H, Kadlec MB, Carter AS. Sensory clusters of toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: differences in affective symptoms. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 2008;49:817–825. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01899.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernardet M, Crusio WE. Fmr1 KO mice as a possible model of autistic features. The Scientific World Journal. 2006;6:1164–1176. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2006.220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernier R, Dawson G, Panagiotides H, Webb S. Individuals with autism spectrum disorder show normal responses to a fear potential startle paradigm. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005;35:575–583. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0002-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Billingslea EN, Tatard-Leitman VM, Anguiano J, Jutzeler CR, Suh J, Saunders JA, Morita S, Featherstone RE, Ortinski PI, Gandal MJ, Lin R, Liang Y, Gur RE, Carlson GC, Hahn CG, Siegel SJ. Parvalbumin cell ablation of NMDA-R1 causes increased resting network excitability with associated social and self-care deficits. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:1603–1613. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bilousova TV, Dansie L, Ngo M, Aye J, Charles JR, Ethell DW, Ethell IM. Minocycline promotes dendritic spine maturation and improves behavioural performance in the fragile X mouse model. J Med Genet. 2009;46:94–102. doi: 10.1136/jmg.2008.061796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braff DL, Grillon C, Geyer MA. Gating and habituation of the startle reflex in schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49:206. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820030038005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braff DL, Geyer MA, Swerdlow NR. Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology. 2001;156:234–258. doi: 10.1007/s002130100810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brandwein AB, Foxe JJ, Butler JS, Frey HP, Bates JC, Shulman LH, Molholm S. Neurophysiological indices of atypical auditory processing and multisensory integration are associated with symptom severity in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:230–244. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2212-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broberg BV, Oranje B, Glenthoj BY, Fejgin K, Plath N, Bastlund JF. Assessment of auditory sensory processing in a neurodevelopmental animal model of schizophrenia-gating of auditory-evoked potentials and prepulse inhibition. Behav Brain Res. 2010;213:142–147. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruneau N, Roux S, Adrien JL, Barthélémy C. Auditory associative cortex dysfunction in children with autism: evidence from late auditory evoked potentials (N1 wave-T complex) Clin Neurophysiol. 1999;110:1927–1934. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00149-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunner D, Kabitzke P, He D, Cox K, Thiede L, Hanania T, Sabath E, Alexandrov V, Saxe M, Peles E, Mills A, Spooren W, Ghosh A, Feliciano P, Benedetti M, Luo Clayton A, Biemans B. Comprehensive analysis of the 16p11.2 deletion and null cntnap2 mouse models of autism spectrum disorder. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0134572. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Budd TW, Barry RJ, Gordon E, Rennie C, Michie PT. Decrement of the N1 auditory event-related potential with stimulus repetition: habituation vs. refractoriness. Int J Psychophysiol. 1998;31:51–68. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(98)00040-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlén M, Meletis K, Siegle JH, Cardin JA, Futai K, Vierling-Claassen D, Rühlmann C, Jones SR, Deisseroth K, Sheng M, Moore CI, Tsai LH. A critical role for NMDA receptors in parvalbumin interneurons for gamma rhythm induction and behavior. Mol Psychiatry. 2012;17:537–548. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cascio CJ, Gu C, Schauder KB, Key AP, Yoder P. Somatosensory event-related potentials and association with tactile behavioral responsiveness patterns in children with ASD. Brain Topogr. 2015;28:895–903. doi: 10.1007/s10548-015-0439-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castrén M, Pääkkönen A, Tarkka IM, Ryynänen M, Partanen J. Augmentation of auditory N1 in children with fragile X syndrome. Brain Topogr. 2003;15:165–171. doi: 10.1023/a:1022606200636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ceponiene R, Lepistö T, Shestakova A, Vanhala R, Alku P, Näätänen R, Yaguchi K. Speech-sound-selective auditory impairment in children with autism: they can perceive but do not attend. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:5567–5572. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0835631100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chadman KK, Gong S, Scattoni ML, Boltuck SE, Gandhy SU, Heintz N, Crawley JN. Minimal aberrant behavioral phenotypes of neuroligin-3 R451C knockin mice. Autism Res. 2008;1:147–158. doi: 10.1002/aur.22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chamberlain PD, Rodgers J, Crowley MJ, White SE, Freeston MH, South M. A potentiated startle study of uncertainty and contextual anxiety in adolescents diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Mol Autism. 2013;4:31. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-4-31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chao HT, Chen H, Samaco RC, Xue M, Chahrour M, Yoo J, Neul JL, Gong S, Lu HC, Heintz N, Ekker M, Rubenstein JL, Noebels JL, Rosenmund C, Zoghbi HY. Dysfunction in GABA signalling mediates autism-like stereotypies and Rett syndrome phenotypes. Nature. 2010;468:263–269. doi: 10.1038/nature09582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen L, Toth M. Fragile X mice develop sensory hyperreactivity to auditory stimuli. Neuroscience. 2001;103:1043–1050. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(01)00036-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen D. Magnetoencephalography: detection of the brain’s electrical activity with a superconducting magnetometer. Science. 1972;175:664–666. doi: 10.1126/science.175.4022.664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Comerchero MD, Polich J. P3a and P3b from typical auditory and visual stimuli. Clin Neurophysiol. 1999;110:24–30. doi: 10.1016/s0168-5597(98)00033-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connolly PM, Maxwell CR, Kanes SJ, Abel T, Liang Y, Tokarczyk J, Bilker WB, Turetsky BI, Gur RE, Siegel SJ. Inhibition of auditory evoked potentials and prepulse inhibition of startle in DBA/2 : J and DBA/2Hsd inbred mouse substrains. Brain Res. 2003;992:85–95. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2003.08.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connolly PM, Maxwell C, Liang Y, Kahn JB, Kanes SJ, Abel T, Gur RE, Turetsky BI, Siegel SJ. The effects of ketamine vary among inbred mouse strains and mimic schizophrenia for the P80 but not P20 or N40 auditory ERP components. Neurochem Res. 2004;29:1179–1188. doi: 10.1023/b:nere.0000023605.68408.fb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Contractor A, Klyachko VA, Portera-Cailliau C. Altered neuronal and circuit excitability in fragile X syndrome. Neuron. 2015;87:699–715. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coutellier L, Beraki S, Ardestani PM, Saw NL, Shamloo M. Npas4: a neuronal transcription factor with a key role in social and cognitive functions relevant to developmental disorders. PLoS One. 2012;7:e46604. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crane L, Goddard L, Pring L. Sensory processing in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Autism. 2009;13:215–228. doi: 10.1177/1362361309103794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crawley JN. Designing mouse behavioral tasks relevant to autistic-like behaviors. Mental Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2004;10:248–258. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crawley JN. Mouse behavioral assays relevant to the symptoms of autism. Brain Pathol. 2007;17:448–459. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2007.00096.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creutzfeldt OD, Watanabe S, Lux HD. Relations between EEG phenomena and potentials of single cortical cells. I. Evoked responses after thalamic and epicortical stimulation. Electroencephalogr. Clin Neurophysiol. 1966a;20:1–18. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(66)90136-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creutzfeldt OD, Watanabe S, Lux HD. Relations between EEG phenomena and potentials of single cortical cells. II. Spontaneous and convulsoid activity. Electroencephalogr. Clin Neurophysiol. 1966b;20:19–37. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(66)90137-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis H, Mast T, Yoshie N, Zerlin S. The slow response of the human cortex to auditory stimuli: recovery process. Electroencephalogr. Clin Neurophysiol. 1966;21:105–113. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(66)90118-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis M, Parisi T, Gendelman DS, Tischler M, Kehne JH. Habituation and sensitization of startle reflexes elicited electrically from the brainstem. Science. 1982;218:688–690. doi: 10.1126/science.7134967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis M, Walker DL, Lee Y. Roles of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in fear and anxiety measured with the acoustic startle reflex. Possible relevance to PTSD. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1997;821:305–331. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb48289.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeLorey TM, Sahbaie P, Hashemi E, Li WW, Salehi A, Clark DJ. Somatosensory and sensorimotor consequences associated with the heterozygous disruption of the autism candidate gene, Gabrb3. Behav Brain Res. 2011;216:36–45. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.06.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deng PY, Rotman Z, Blundon JA, Cho Y, Cui J, Cavalli V, Zakharenko SS, Klyachko VA. FMRP regulates neurotransmitter release and synaptic information transmission by modulating action potential duration via BK channels. Neuron. 2013;77:696–711. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dichter GS, Benning SD, Holtzclaw TN, Bodfish JW. Affective modulation of the startle eyeblink and postauricular reflexes in autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40:858–869. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0925-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donkers FCL, Schipul SE, Baranek GT, Cleary KM, Willoughby MT, Evans AM, Bulluck JC, Lovmo JE, Belger A. Attenuated auditory event-related potentials and associations with atypical sensory response patterns in children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:506–523. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1948-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan GE, Moy SS, Perez A, Eddy DM, Zinzow WM, Lieberman JA, Snouwaert JN, Koller BH. Deficits in sensorimotor gating and tests of social behavior in a genetic model of reduced NMDA receptor function. Behav Brain Res. 2004;153:507–519. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan CC, Barry RJ, Connolly JF, Fischer C, Michie PT, Näätänen R, Polich J, Reinvang I, Van Petten C. Event-related potentials in clinical research: guidelines for eliciting, recording, and quantifying mismatch negativity, P300, and N400. Clin Neurophysiol. 2009;120:1883–1908. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.07.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunn MA, Gomes H, Gravel J. Mismatch negativity in children with autism and typical development. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:52–71. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0359-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehrlichman RS, Maxwell CR, Majumdar S, Siegel SJ. Deviance-elicited changes in event-related potentials are attenuated by ketamine in mice. J Cognit Neurosci. 2008;20:1403–1414. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehrlichman RS, Gandal MJ, Maxwell CR, Lazarewicz MT, Finkel LH, Contreras D, Turetsky BI, Siegel SJ. N-Methyl-D-aspartic acid receptor antagonist-induced frequency oscillations in mice recreate pattern of electrophysiological deficits in schizophrenia. Neuroscience. 2009;158:705–712. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.10.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elison JT, Paterson SJ, Wolff JJ, Reznick JS, Sasson NJ, Gu H, Botteron KN, Dager SR, Estes AM, Evans AC, Gerig G, Hazlett HC, Schultz RT, Styner M, Zwaigenbaum L, Piven J. White matter microstructure and atypical visual orienting in 7-month-olds at risk for autism. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170:899–908. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12091150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elsabbagh M, Fernandes J, Jane Webb S, Dawson G, Charman T, Johnson MH. Disengagement of visual attention in infancy is associated with emerging autism in toddlerhood. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74:189–194. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esclassan F, Francois J, Phillips KG, Loomis S, Gilmour G. Phenotypic characterization of nonsocial behavioral impairment in neurexin 1alpha knockout rats. Behav Neurosci. 2015;129:74–85. doi: 10.1037/bne0000024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Etherton MR, Blaiss CA, Powell CM, Sudhof TC. Mouse neurexin-1alpha deletion causes correlated electrophysiological and behavioral changes consistent with cognitive impairments. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:17998–18003. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910297106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ethridge LE, White SP, Mosconi MW, Wang J, Byerly MJ, Sweeney JA. Reduced habituation of auditory evoked potentials indicate cortical hyperexcitability in Fragile X Syndrome. Transl Psychiatry. 2016;6:e787. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Featherstone RE, Nagy LR, Hahn CG, Siegel SJ. Juvenile exposure to ketamine causes delayed emergence of EEG abnormalities during adulthood in mice. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;134:123–127. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feinstein C, Reiss AL. Autism: the point of view from fragile X studies. J Autism Dev Disord. 1998;28:393–405. doi: 10.1023/a:1026000404855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fendt M, Li L, Yeomans JS. Brain stem circuits mediating prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex. Psychopharmacology. 2001;156:216–224. doi: 10.1007/s002130100794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferri R, Elia M, Agarwal N, Lanuzza B, Musumeci SA, Pennisi G. The mismatch negativity and the P3a components of the auditory event-related potentials in autistic low-functioning subjects. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003;114:1671–1680. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00153-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foley KA, MacFabe DF, Kavaliers M, Ossenkopp KP. Sexually dimorphic effects of prenatal exposure to lipopolysaccharide, and prenatal and postnatal exposure to propionic acid, on acoustic startle response and prepulse inhibition in adolescent rats: relevance to autism spectrum disorders. Behav Brain Res. 2015;278:244–256. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fortier ME, Luheshi GN, Boksa P. Effects of prenatal infection on prepulse inhibition in the rat depend on the nature of the infectious agent and the stage of pregnancy. Behav Brain Res. 2007;181:270–277. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.04.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foss-Feig JH, Heacock JL, Cascio CJ. Tactile responsiveness patterns and their association with core features in autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2012;6:337–344. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frankland PW, Wang Y, Rosner B, Shimizu T, Balleine BW, Dykens EM, Ornitz EM, Silva AJ. Sensorimotor gating abnormalities in young males with fragile X syndrome and Fmr1-knockout mice. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9:417–425. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freedman R, Adler LE, Waldo MC, Pachtman E, Franks RD. Neurophysiological evidence for a defect in inhibitory pathways in schizophrenia: comparison of medicated and drug-free patients. Biol Psychiatry. 1983;18:537–551. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freedman R, Adler LE, Myles-worsley M, Nagamoto HT, Miller C, Kisley M, Mcrae K, Cawthra E, Waldo M. Inhibitory gating of an evoked response to repeated auditory stimuli in schizophrenic and normal subjects: human recordings, computer simulation, and an animal model. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53:1114–1121. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830120052009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gage NM, Siegel B, Roberts TPL. Cortical auditory system maturational abnormalities in children with autism disorder: an MEG investigation. Dev Brain Res. 2003;144:201–209. doi: 10.1016/s0165-3806(03)00172-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gandal MJ, Edgar JC, Ehrlichman RS, Mehta M, Roberts TPL, Siegel SJ. Validating (oscillations and delayed auditory responses as translational biomarkers of autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68:1100–1106. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gandal MJ, Anderson RL, Billingslea EN, Carlson GC, Roberts TPL, Siegel SJ. Mice with reduced NMDA receptor expression: more consistent with autism than schizophrenia? Genes Brain Behav. 2012a;11:740–750. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2012.00816.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gandal MJ, Sisti J, Klook K, Ortinski PI, Leitman V, Liang Y, Thieu T, Anderson R, Pierce RC, Jonak G, Gur RE, Carlson G, Siegel SJ. GABAB-mediated rescue of altered excitatory-inhibitory balance, gamma synchrony and behavioral deficits following constitutive NMDAR-hypofunction. Transl Psychiatry. 2012b;2:e142. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Germani T, Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Brian J, Smith I, Roberts W, Szatmari P, Roncadin C, Sacrey LAR, Garon N, Vaillancourt T. Brief report: assessment of early sensory processing in infants at high-risk of autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44:3264–3270. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2175-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson JR, Bartley AF, Hays SA, Huber KM. Imbalance of neocortical excitation and inhibition and altered UP states reflect network hyperexcitability in the mouse model of fragile X syndrome. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100:2615–2626. doi: 10.1152/jn.90752.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goffin D, Allen M, Zhang L, Amorim M, Wang ITJ, Reyes ARS, Mercado-Berton A, Ong C, Cohen S, Hu L, Blendy JA, Carlson GC, Siegel SJ, Greenberg ME, Zhou Z. Rett syndrome mutation MeCP2 T158A disrupts DNA binding, protein stability and ERP responses. Nat Neurosci. 2011;15:274–283. doi: 10.1038/nn.2997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goffin D, Brodkin ES, Blendy JA, Siegel SJ, Zhou Z. Cellular origins of auditory event-related potential deficits in Rett syndrome. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17:804–806. doi: 10.1038/nn.3710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gogolla N, Leblanc JJ, Quast KB, Sudhof TC, Fagiolini M, Hensch TK. Common circuit defect of excitatory-inhibitory balance in mouse models of autism. J Neurodev Disord. 2009;1:172–181. doi: 10.1007/s11689-009-9023-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gomez-Nieto R, Sinex DG, Horta-Junior Jde A, Castellano O, Herrero-Turrion JM, Lopez DE. A fast cholinergic modulation of the primary acoustic startle circuit in rats. Brain Struct Funct. 2014;219:1555–1573. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0585-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gomot M, Giard MH, Adrien JL, Barthelemy C, Bruneau N. Hypersensitivity to acoustic change in children with autism: electrophysiological evidence of left frontal cortex dysfunctioning. Psychophysiology. 2002;39:577–584. doi: 10.1017.S0048577202394058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goncalves JT, Anstey JE, Golshani P, Portera-Cailliau C. Circuit level defects in the developing neocortex of Fragile X mice. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:903–909. doi: 10.1038/nn.3415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gonzalez-Lima F, Finkenstadt T, Ewert JP. Neural substrates for long-term habituation of the acoustic startle reflex in rats: a 2-deoxyglucose study. Neurosci Lett. 1989;96:151–156. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(89)90049-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graham FK. The more or less startling effects of weak prestimulation. Psychophysiology. 1975;12:238–248. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1975.tb01284.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green SA, Ben-Sasson A, Soto TW, Carter AS. Anxiety and sensory over-responsivity in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: bidirectional effects across time. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42:1112–1119. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1361-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grice SJ, Spratling MW, Karmiloff-Smith A, Halit H, Csibra G, de Haan M, Johnson MH. Disordered visual processing and oscillatory brain activity in autism and Williams syndrome. Neuroreport. 2001;12:2697–2700. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200108280-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groves PM, Thompson RF. Habituation: a dual-process theory. Psychol Rev. 1970;77:419–450. doi: 10.1037/h0029810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gruber WR, Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Doppelmayr M. Alpha phase synchronization predicts P1 and N1 latency and amplitude size. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15:371–377. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagerman RJ. Lessons from fragile X regarding neurobiology, autism, and neurodegeneration. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006;27:63–74. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200602000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halene TB, Ehrlichman RS, Liang Y, Christian EP, Jonak GJ, Gur TL, Blendy JA, Dow HC, Brodkin ES, Schneider F, Gur RC, Siegel SJ. Assessment of NMDA receptor NR1 subunit hypofunction in mice as a model for schizophrenia. Genes Brain Behav. 2009;8:661–675. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2009.00504.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall SS, Walter E, Sherman E, Hoeft F, Reiss AL. The neural basis of auditory temporal discrimination in girls with fragile X syndrome. J Neurodev Disord. 2009;1:91–99. doi: 10.1007/s11689-009-9007-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanlon FM, Miller GA, Thoma RJ, Irwin J, Jones A, Moses SN, Huang M, Weisend MP, Paulson KM, Edgar JC, Adler LE, Cañive JM. Distinct M50 and M100 auditory gating deficits in schizophrenia. Psychophysiology. 2005;42:417–427. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00299.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harms L, Fulham WR, Todd J, Budd TW, Hunter M, Meehan C, Penttonen M, Schall U, Zavitsanou K, Hodgson DM, Michie PT. Mismatch negativity (MMN) in freely-moving rats with several experimental controls. PLoS One. 2014;9:e110892. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hays SA, Huber KM, Gibson JR. Altered neocortical rhythmic activity states in Fmr1 KO mice are due to enhanced mGluR5 signaling and involve changes in excitatory circuitry. J Neurosci. 2011;31:14223–14234. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3157-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hazlett HC, Poe MD, Lightbody AA, Styner M, Macfall JR, Reiss AL, Piven J. Trajectories of early brain volume development in fragile x syndrome and autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51:921–933. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hebert B, Pietropaolo S, Meme S, Laudier B, Laugeray A, Doisne N, Quartier A, Lefeuvre S, Got L, Cahard D, Laumonnier F, Crusio WE, Pichon J, Menuet A, Perche O, Briault S. Rescue of fragile X syndrome phenotypes in Fmr1 KO mice by a BKCa channel opener molecule. Orphanet J Rare Dis. 2014;9:124. doi: 10.1186/s13023-014-0124-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirstein W, Iversen P, Ramachandran VS. Autonomic responses of autistic children to people and objects. Proc R Soc London B. 2001;268:1883–1888. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2001.1724. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howland JG, Cazakoff BN, Zhang Y. Altered object-in-place recognition memory, prepulse inhibition, and locomotor activity in the offspring of rats exposed to a viral mimetic during pregnancy. Neuroscience. 2012;201:184–198. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.11.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hruby T, Marsalek P. Event-related potentials–the P3 wave. Acta Neurobiol Exp. 2003;63:55–63. doi: 10.55782/ane-2003-1455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jansson-Verkasalo E, Ceponiene R, Kielinen M, Suominen K, Jantti V, Linna SL, Moilanen I, Näätänen R. Deficient auditory processing in children with Asperger syndrome, as indexed by event-related potentials. Neurosci Lett. 2003;338:197–200. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(02)01405-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jochaut D, Lehongre K, Saitovitch A, Devauchelle AD, Olasagasti I, Chabane N, Zilbovicius M, Giraud AL. Atypical coordination of cortical oscillations in response to speech in autism. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015;9:171. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kasai K, Hashimoto O, Kawakubo Y, Yumoto M, Kamio S, Itoh K, Koshida I, Iwanami A, Nakagome K, Fukuda M, Yamasue H, Yamada H, Abe O, Aoki S, Kato N. Delayed automatic detection of change in speech sounds in adults with autism: a magnetoencephalographic study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2005;116:1655–1664. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keehn B, Vogel-Farley V, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. Atypical hemispheric specialization for faces in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2015;8:187–198. doi: 10.1002/aur.1438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemner C, Verbaten MN, Cuperus JM, Camfferman G, van Engeland H. Auditory event-related brain potentials in autistic children and three different control groups. Biol Psychiatry. 1995;38:150–165. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)00247-Z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemner C, Oranje B, Verbaten MN, van Engeland H. Normal P50 gating in children with autism. J Clin Psychiatry. 2002;63:214–217. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v63n0307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim H, Gibboni R, Kirkhart C, Bao SW. Impaired critical period plasticity in primary auditory cortex of fragile X model mice. J Neurosci. 2013;33:15686–15692. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3246-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klejbor I, Kucinski A, Wersinger SR, Corso T, Spodnik JH, Dziewiatkowski J, Morys J, Hesse RA, Rice KC, Miletich R, Stachowiak EK, Stachowiak MK. Serotonergic hyperinnervation and effective serotonin blockade in an FGF receptor developmental model of psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2009;113:308–321. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klintwall L, Holm A, Eriksson M, Carlsson LH, Olsson MB, Hedvall Å, Gillberg C, Fernell E. Sensory abnormalities in autism. Res Dev Disabil. 2011;32:795–800. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knoth IS, Lippe S. Event-related potential alterations in fragile X syndrome. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;6:264. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koch M. The neurobiology of startle. Prog Neurobiol. 1999;59:107–128. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(98)00098-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kogan CS, Bertone A, Cornish K, Boutet I, Der Kaloustian VM, Andermann E, Faubert J, Chaudhuri A. Integrative cortical dysfunction and pervasive motion perception deficit in fragile X syndrome. Neurology. 2004;63:1634–1639. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000142987.44035.3b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohl S, Wolters C, Gruendler TO, Vogeley K, Klosterkotter J, Kuhn J. Prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex in high functioning autism. PLoS One. 2014;9:e92372. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0092372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kootz JP, Marinelli B, Cohen DJ. Modulation of response to environmental stimulation in autistic children. J Autism Dev Disord. 1982;12:185–193. doi: 10.1007/BF01531308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kooy RF, DHooge R, Reyniers E, Bakker CE, Nagels G, DeBoulle K, Storm K, Clincke G, DeDeyn PP, Oostra BA, Willems PJ. Transgenic mouse model for the fragile X syndrome. Am J Med Genet. 1996;64:241–245. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19960809)64:2<241::AID-AJMG1>3.0.CO;2-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korpilahti P, Jansson-Verkasalo E, Mattila ML, Kuusikko S, Suominen K, Rytky S, Pauls DL, Moilanen I. Processing of affective speech prosody is impaired in Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1539–1549. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0271-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kouser M, Speed HE, Dewey CM, Reimers JM, Widman AJ, Gupta N, Liu S, Jaramillo TC, Bangash M, Xiao B, Worley PF, Powell CM. Loss of predominant Shank3 isoforms results in hippocampus-dependent impairments in behavior and synaptic transmission. J Neurosci. 2013;33:18448–18468. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3017-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kramvis I, Mansvelder HD, Loos M, Meredith R. Hyperactivity, perseveration and increased responding during attentional rule acquisition in the Fragile X mouse model. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013;7:172. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhl PK, Coffey-Corina S, Padden D, Dawson G. Links between social and linguistic processing of speech in preschool children with autism: behavioral and electrophysiological measures. Dev Sci. 2005;8:F1–F12. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00384.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kujala T, Kuuluvainen S, Saalasti S, Jansson-Verkasalo E, von Wendt L, Lepisto T. Speech-feature discrimination in children with Asperger syndrome as determined with the multi-feature mismatch negativity paradigm. Clin Neurophysiol. 2010;121:1410–1419. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kujala TM. The role of early auditory discrimination deficits in language disorders. J Psychophysiol. 2007;21:239–250. [Google Scholar]
- Kwon JS, O’Donnell BF, Wallenstein GV, Greene RW, Hirayasu Y, Nestor PG, Hasselmo ME, Potts GF, Shenton ME, McCarley RW. Gamma frequency-range abnormalities to auditory stimulation in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:1001–1005. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.11.1001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lachiewicz AM, Dawson DV. Do young boys with fragile-X-syndrome have macroorchidism. Pediatrics. 1994;93:992–995. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lane AE, Molloy CA, Bishop SL. Classification of children with autism spectrum disorder by sensory subtype: a case for sensory-based phenotypes. Autism Res. 2014;7:322–333. doi: 10.1002/aur.1368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence YA, Kemper TL, Bauman ML, Blatt GJ. Parvalbumin-, calbindin-, and calretinin-immunoreactive hippocampal interneuron density in autism. Acta Neurol Scand. 2010;121:99–108. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2009.01234.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lazarewicz MT, Ehrlichman RS, Maxwell CR, Gandal MJ, Finkel LH, Siegel SJ. Ketamine modulates theta and gamma oscillations. J Cognit Neurosci. 2010;22:1452–1464. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leaton RN, Supple WF., Jr Cerebellar vermis: essential for long-term habituation of the acoustic startle response. Science. 1986;232:513–515. doi: 10.1126/science.3961494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leaton RN, Supple WF., Jr Medial cerebellum and long-term habituation of acoustic startle in rats. Behav Neurosci. 1991;105:804–816. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.105.6.804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leaton RN, Cassella JV, Borszcz GS. Short-term and long-term habituation of the acoustic startle response in chronic decerebrate rats. Behav Neurosci. 1985;99:901–912. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.99.5.901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leekam SR, Nieto C, Libby SJ, Wing L, Gould J. Describing the sensory abnormalities of children and adults with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:894–910. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0218-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lepisto T, Kujala T, Vanhala R, Alku P, Huotilainen M, Näätänen R. The discrimination of and orienting to speech and non-speech sounds in children with autism. Brain Res. 2005;1066:147–157. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.10.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lepisto T, Silokallio S, Nieminen-von Wendt T, Alku P, Näätänen R, Kujala T. Auditory perception and attention as reflected by the brain event-related potentials in children with Asperger syndrome. Clin Neurophysiol. 2006;117:2161–2171. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.06.709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liao W, Gandal MJ, Ehrlichman RS, Siegel SJ, Carlson GC. MeCP2+/mouse model of RTT reproduces auditory phenotypes associated with Rett–syndrome and replicate select EEG endophenotypes of autism spectrum disorder. Neurobiol Dis. 2012;46:88–92. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.12.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liss M, Saulnier C, Fein D, Kinsbourne M. Sensory and attention abnormalities in autistic spectrum disorders. Autism. 2006;10:155–172. doi: 10.1177/1362361306062021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Long LE, Anderson P, Frank E, Shaw A, Liu SJ, Huang XF, Pinault D, Karl T, O’Brien TJ, Weickert CS, Jones NC. Neuregulin 1 expression and electrophysiological abnormalities in the neuregulin 1 transmembrane domain heterozygous mutant mouse. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0124114. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lovelace J, Wen T, Reinhard S, Hsu M, Sidhu H, Ethell I, Binder D, Razak K. Matrix metalloproteinase-9 deletion rescues auditory evoked potential habituation deficit in a mouse model of Fragile X Syndrome. Neurobiol Dis. 2016;89:126–135. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2016.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ludlow A, Mohr B, Whitmore A, Garagnani M, Pulvermuller F, Gutierrez R. Auditory processing and sensory behaviours in children with autism spectrum disorders as revealed by mismatch negativity. Brain Cogn. 2014;86:55–63. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Machado C, Estévez M, Leisman G, Melillo R, Rodríguez R, DeFina P, Hernández A, Pérez-Nellar J, Naranjo R, Chinchilla M, Garófalo N, Vargas J, Beltrán C. QEEG spectral and coherence assessment of autistic children in three different experimental conditions. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;45:406–424. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1909-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madsen GF, Bilenberg N, Cantio C, Oranje B. Increased prepulse inhibition and sensitization of the startle reflex in autistic children. Autism Res. 2014;7:94–103. doi: 10.1002/aur.1337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madsen GF, Bilenberg N, Jepsen JR, Glenthoj B, Cantio C, Oranje B. Normal P50 gating in children with autism, yet attenuated P50 amplitude in the Asperger subcategory. Autism Res. 2015;8:371–378. doi: 10.1002/aur.1452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magnee MJ, Oranje B, van Engeland H, Kahn RS, Kemner C. Cross-sensory gating in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorder: EEG evidence for impaired brain connectivity? Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:1728–1732. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marco EJ, Khatibi K, Hill SS, Siegel B, Arroyo MS, Dowling AF, Neuhaus JM, Sherr EH, Hinkley LNB, Nagarajan SS. Children with autism show reduced somatosensory response: an MEG study. Autism Research. 2012;5:340–351. doi: 10.1002/aur.1247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maxwell CR, Liang Y, Weightman BD, Kanes SJ, Abel T, Gur RE, Turetsky BI, Bilker WB, Lenox RH, Siegel SJ. Effects of chronic olanzapine and haloperidol differ on the mouse N1 auditory evoked potential. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:739–746. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maxwell CR, Ehrlichman RS, Liang Y, Trief D, Kanes SJ, Karp J, Siegel SJ. Ketamine produces lasting disruptions in encoding of sensory stimuli. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2006;316:315–324. doi: 10.1124/jpet.105.091199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAlonan GM, Daly E, Kumari V, Critchley HD, van Amelsvoort T, Suckling J, Simmons A, Sigmundsson T, Greenwood K, Russell A, Schmitz N, Happe F, Howlin P, Murphy DG. Brain anatomy and sensorimotor gating in Asperger’s syndrome. Brain. 2002;125:1594–1606. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McFadden KL, Hepburn S, Winterrowd E, Schmidt GL, Rojas DC. Abnormalities in gamma-band responses to language stimuli in first-degree relatives of children with autism spectrum disorder: an MEG study. BMC Psychiatry. 2012;12:213. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meincke U, Light GA, Geyer MA, Braff DL, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E. Sensitization and habituation of the acoustic startle reflex in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2004;126:51–61. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer U, Nyffeler M, Yee BK, Knuesel I, Feldon J. Adult brain and behavioral pathological markers of prenatal immune challenge during early/middle and late fetal development in mice. Brain Behav Immun. 2008;22:469–486. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milekic MH, Xin Y, O’Donnell A, Kumar KK, Bradley-Moore M, Malaspina D, Moore H, Brunner D, Ge Y, Edwards J, Paul S, Haghighi FG, Gingrich JA. Age-related sperm DNA methylation changes are transmitted to offspring and associated with abnormal behavior and dysregulated gene expression. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20:995–1001. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller LJ, McIntosh DN, McGrath J, Shyu V, Lampe M, Taylor AK, Tassone F, Neitzel K, Stackhouse T, Hagerman RJ. Electrodermal responses to sensory stimuli in individuals with fragile X syndrome: a preliminary report. Am J Med Genet. 1999;83:268–279. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milne E. Increased intra-participant variability in children with autistic spectrum disorders: evidence from single-trial analysis of evoked EEG. Front Psychol. 2011;2:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minshew NJ, Sweeney J, Luna B. Autism as a selective disorder of complex information processing and underdevelopment of neocortical systems. Mol Psychiatry. 2002;7(Suppl 2):S14–S15. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mouridsen SE, Rich B, Isager T. Psychiatric disorders in adults diagnosed as children with atypical autism: a case control study. J Neural Trans (Vienna) 2008a;115:135–138. doi: 10.1007/s00702-007-0798-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mouridsen SE, Rich B, Isager T, Nedergaard NJ. Psychiatric disorders in individuals diagnosed with infantile autism as children: a case control study. J Psychiatr Pract. 2008b;14:5–12. doi: 10.1097/01.pra.0000308490.47262.e0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moy SS, Nonneman RJ, Young NB, Demyanenko GP, Maness PF. Impaired sociability and cognitive function in Nrcam-null mice. Behav Brain Res. 2009;205:123–131. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.06.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moy SS, Riddick NV, Nikolova VD, Teng BL, Agster KL, Nonneman RJ, Young NB, Baker LK, Nadler JJ, Bodfish JW. Repetitive behavior profile and supersensitivity to amphetamine in the C58/: J mouse model of autism. Behav Brain Res. 2014;259:200–214. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.10.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Musumeci SA, Bosco P, Calabrese G, Bakker C, De Sarro GB, Elia M, Ferri R, Oostra BA. Audiogenic seizures susceptibility in transgenic mice with fragile X syndrome. Epilepsia. 2000;41:19–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.2000.tb01499.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Musumeci SA, Calabrese G, Bonaccorso CM, D’Antoni S, Brouwer JR, Bakker CE, Elia M, Ferri R, Nelson DL, Oostra BA, Catania MV. Audiogenic seizure susceptibility is reduced in fragile X knockout mice after introduction of FMR1 transgenes. Exp Neurol. 2007;203:233–240. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2006.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Näätänen R, Paavilainen P, Rinne T, Alho K. The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: a review. Clin Neurophysiol. 2007;118:2544–2590. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Näätänen R, Kujala T, Kreegipuu K, Carlson S, Escera C, Baldeweg T, Ponton C. The mismatch negativity: an index of cognitive decline in neuropsychiatric and neurological diseases and in ageing. Brain. 2011;134:3435–3453. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagamoto HT, Adler LE, Waldo MC, Griffith J, Freedman R. Gating of auditory response in schizophrenics and normal controls: effects of recording site and stimulation interval on the P50 wave. Schizophr Res. 1991;4:31–40. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(91)90007-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagy LR, Featherstone RE, Hahn CG, Siegel SJ. Delayed emergence of behavioral and electrophysiological effects following juvenile ketamine exposure in mice. Transl Psychiatry. 2015;5:e635. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakamura T, Michie PT, Fulham WR, Todd J, Budd TW, Schall U, Hunter M, Hodgson DM. Epidural auditory event-related potentials in the rat to frequency and duration deviants: evidence of mismatch negativity? Front Psychol. 2011;2:1–17. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakamura T, Matsumoto J, Takamura Y, Ishii Y, Sasahara M, Ono T, Nishijo H. Relationships among parvalbumin-immunoreactive neuron density, phase-locked gamma oscillations, and autistic/schizophrenic symptoms in PDGFR-beta knock-out and control mice. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0119258. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0119258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nguyen PT, Nakamura T, Hori E, Urakawa S, Uwano T, Zhao J, Li R, Bac ND, Hamashima T, Ishii Y, Matsushima T, Ono T, Sasahara M, Nishijo H. Cognitive and socio-emotional deficits in platelet-derived growth factor receptor-beta gene knockout mice. PLoS One. 2011;6:e18004. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nielsen DM, Derber WJ, McClellan DA, Crnic LS. Alterations in the auditory startle response in Fmr1 targeted mutant mouse models of fragile X syndrome. Brain Res. 2002;927:8–17. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(01)03309-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nodal FR, Lopez DE. Direct input from cochlear root neurons to pontine reticulospinal neurons in albino rat. J Comp Neurol. 2003;460:80–93. doi: 10.1002/cne.10656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor K. Auditory processing in autism spectrum disorder: a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:836–854. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olmos-Serrano JL, Corbin JG, Burns MP. The GABA(A) receptor agonist THIP ameliorates specific behavioral deficits in the mouse model of fragile X syndrome. Dev Neurosci. 2011;33:395–403. doi: 10.1159/000332884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oram Cardy JE, Flagg EJ, Roberts W, Roberts TPL. Delayed mismatch field for speech and non-speech sounds in children with autism. Neuroreport. 2005;16:521–525. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200504040-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oram Cardy JE, Flagg EJ, Roberts W, Roberts TPL. Auditory evoked fields predict language ability and impairment in children. Int J Psychophysiol. 2008;68:170–175. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oranje B, Geyer MA, Bocker KB, Leon Kenemans J, Verbaten MN. Prepulse inhibition and P50 suppression: commonalities and dissociations. Psychiatry Res. 2006;143:147–158. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oranje B, Aggernaes B, Rasmussen H, Ebdrup BH, Glenthoj BY. P50 suppression and its neural generators in antipsychotic-naive first-episode schizophrenia before and after 6 months of quetiapine treatment. Schizophr Bull. 2013a;39:472–480. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oranje B, Lahuis B, van Engeland H, Jan van der Gaag R, Kemner C. Sensory and sensorimotor gating in children with multiple complex developmental disorders (MCDD) and autism. Psychiatry Res. 2013b;206:287–292. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orekhova EV, Stroganova TA. Arousal and attention re-orienting in autism spectrum disorders: evidence from auditory event-related potentials. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:34. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orekhova EV, Stroganova TA, Nygren G, Tsetlin MM, Posikera IN, Gillberg C, Elam M. Excess of high frequency electroencephalogram oscillations in boys with autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:1022–1029. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.12.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orekhova EV, Stroganova TA, Prokofyev AO, Nygren G, Gillberg C, Elam M. Sensory gating in young children with autism: relation to age, IQ, and EEG gamma oscillations. Neurosci Lett. 2008;434:218–223. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.01.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ornitz EM, Lane SJ, Sugiyama T, de Traversay J. Startle modulation studies in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 1993;23:619–637. doi: 10.1007/BF01046105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry W, Minassian A, Lopez B, Maron L, Lincoln A. Sensorimotor gating deficits in adults with autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:482–486. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickett J, London E. The neuropathology of autism: a review. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2005;64:925–935. doi: 10.1097/01.jnen.0000186921.42592.6c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Picton TW, Hillyard SA. Human auditory evoked potentials. II. Effects of attention. Electroencephalogr. Clin Neurophysiol. 1974;36:191–199. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(74)90156-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Picton TW, Hillyard SA, Krausz HI, Galambos R. Human auditory evoked potentials. I. Evaluation of components. Electroencephalogr. Clin Neurophysiol. 1974;36:179–190. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(74)90155-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polich J, Criado JR. Neuropsychology and neuropharmacology of P3a and P3b. Int J Psychophysiol. 2006;60:172–185. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polich J. Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol. 2007:2128–2148. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ponton CW, Eggermont JJ, Kwong B, Don M. Maturation of human central auditory system activity: evidence from multi-channel evoked potentials. Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;111:220–236. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00236-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Portmann T, Yang M, Mao R, Panagiotakos G, Ellegood J, Dolen G, Bader PL, Grueter BA, Goold C, Fisher E, Clifford K, Rengarajan P, Kalikhman D, Loureiro D, Saw NL, Zhou ZQ, Miller MA, Lerch JP, Henkelman RM, Shamloo M, Malenka RC, Crawley JN, Dolmetsch RE. Behavioral abnormalities and circuit defects in the basal ganglia of a mouse model of 16p11.2 deletion syndrome. Cell Rep. 2014;7:1077–1092. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2014.03.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radyushkin K, Hammerschmidt K, Boretius S, Varoqueaux F, El-Kordi A, Ronnenberg A, Winter D, Frahm J, Fischer J, Brose N, Ehrenreich H. Neuroligin-3-deficient mice: model of a monogenic heritable form of autism with an olfactory deficit. Genes Brain Behav. 2009;8:416–425. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2009.00487.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rankin CH, Abrams T, Barry RJ, Bhatnagar S, Clayton DF, Colombo J, Coppola G, Geyer MA, Glanzman DL, Marsland S, McSweeney FK, Wilson DA, Wu CF, Thompson RF. Habituation revisited: an updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2009;92:135–138. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiss AL, Lee J, Freund L. Neuroanatomy of fragile X syndrome: the temporal lobe. Neurology. 1994;44:1317–1324. doi: 10.1212/wnl.44.7.1317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiss AL, Abrams MT, Greenlaw R, Freund L, Denckla MB. Neurodevelopmental effects of the FMR-1 full mutation in humans. Nat Med. 1995;1:159–167. doi: 10.1038/nm0295-159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Relkovic D, Doe CM, Humby T, Johnstone KA, Resnick JL, Holland AJ, Hagan JJ, Wilkinson LS, Isles AR. Behavioural and cognitive abnormalities in an imprinting centre deletion mouse model for Prader–Willi syndrome. Eur J Neurosci. 2010;31:156–164. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.07048.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rendall AR, Truong DT, Castelluccio BC, Eigsti IM, Fitch RH. Language deficits in autism and assessment of the Cntnap2 mouse. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2015;47:93. [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds S, Millette A, Devine DP. Sensory and motor characterization in the postnatal valproate rat model of autism. Dev Neurosci. 2012;34:258–267. doi: 10.1159/000336646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts TP, Khan SY, Rey M, Monroe JF, Cannon K, Blaskey L, Woldoff S, Qasmieh S, Gandal M, Schmidt GL, Zarnow DM, Levy SE, Edgar JC. MEG detection of delayed auditory evoked responses in autism spectrum disorders: towards an imaging biomarker for autism. Autism Res. 2010;3:8–18. doi: 10.1002/aur.111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts TPL, Cannon KM, Tavabi K, Blaskey L, Khan SY, Monroe JF, Qasmieh S, Levy SE, Edgar JC. Auditory magnetic mismatch field latency: a biomarker for language impairment in autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70:263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers SJ, Wehner DE, Hagerman R. The behavioral phenotype in fragile X: symptoms of autism in very young children with fragile X syndrome, idiopathic autism, and other developmental disorders. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2001;22:409–417. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200112000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rojas DC, Benkers TL, Rogers SJ, Teale PD, Reite ML, Hagerman RJ. Auditory evoked magnetic fields in adults with fragile X syndrome. Neuroreport. 2001;12:2573–2576. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200108080-00056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rojas DC, Maharajh K, Teale P, Rogers SJ. Reduced neural synchronization of gamma-band MEG oscillations in first-degree relatives of children with autism. BMC Psychiatry. 2008;8:66. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-8-66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rojas D, Teale P, Maharajh K, Kronberg E, Youngpeter K, Wilson L, Wallace A, Hepburn S. Transient and steady-state auditory gamma-band responses in first-degree relatives of people with autism spectrum disorder. Mol Autism. 2011;2:11. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-2-11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rotschafer SE, Razak KA. Auditory processing in fragile x syndrome. Front Cell Neurosci. 2014;8:19. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2014.00019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rotschafer SE, Trujillo MS, Dansie LE, Ethell IM, Razak KA. Minocycline treatment reverses ultrasonic vocalization production deficit in a mouse model of Fragile X Syndrome. Brain Res. 2012;1439:7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.12.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roullet FI, Lai JK, Foster JA. In utero exposure to valproic acid and autism—a current review of clinical and animal studies. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2013;36:47–56. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2013.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roy S, Watkins N, Heck D. Comprehensive analysis of ultrasonic vocalizations in a mouse model of fragile x syndrome reveals limited, call type specific deficits. PLoS One. 2012;7:e44816. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salmond CH, de Haan M, Friston KJ, Gadian DG, Vargha-Khadem F. Investigating individual differences in brain abnormalities in autism. Philos Trans R Soc London B: Biol Sci. 2003;358:405–413. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samaco RC, Fryer JD, Ren J, Fyffe S, Chao HT, Sun Y, Greer JJ, Zoghbi HY, Neul JL. A partial loss of function allele of methyl-CpG-binding protein 2 predicts a human neurodevelopmental syndrome. Hum Mol Genet. 2008;17:1718–1727. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddn062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samaco RC, McGraw CM, Ward CS, Sun Y, Neul JL, Zoghbi HY. Female Mecp2(+/−) mice display robust behavioral deficits on two different genetic backgrounds providing a framework for pre-clinical studies. Hum Mol Genet. 2013;22:96–109. doi: 10.1093/hmg/dds406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandin S, Schendel D, Magnusson P, Hultman C, Suren P, Susser E, Gronborg T, Gissler M, Gunnes N, Gross R, Henning M, Bresnahan M, Sourander A, Hornig M, Carter K, Francis R, Parner E, Leonard H, Rosanoff M, Stoltenberg C, Reichenberg A. Autism risk associated with parental age and with increasing difference in age between the parents. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;21:693–700. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders JA, Gandal MJ, Roberts TP, Siegel SJ. NMDA antagonist MK801 recreates auditory electrophysiology disruption present in autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders. Behav Brain Res. 2012;234:233–237. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.06.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders JA, Tatard-Leitman VM, Suh J, Billingslea EN, Roberts TP, Siegel SJ. Knockout of NMDA receptors in parvalbumin interneurons recreates autism-like phenotypes. Autism Res. 2013;6:69–77. doi: 10.1002/aur.1264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmid S, Wilson DA, Rankin CH. Habituation mechanisms and their importance for cognitive function. Front Integr Neurosci. 2014;8:97. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider T, Przewlocki R. Behavioral alterations in rats prenatally exposed to valproic acid: animal model of autism. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:80–89. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider T, Turczak J, Przewlocki R. Environmental enrichment reverses behavioral alterations in rats prenatally exposed to valproic acid: issues for a therapeutic approach in autism. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:36–46. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider A, Leigh MJ, Adams P, Nanakul R, Chechi T, Olichney J, Hagerman R, Hessl D. Electrocortical changes associated with minocycline treatment in fragile X syndrome. J Psychopharmacol. 2013;27:956–963. doi: 10.1177/0269881113494105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seery AM, Vogel-Farley V, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. Atypical lateralization of ERP response to native and non-native speech in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Dev Cognit Neurosci. 2013;5:10–24. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2012.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seery A, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. Event-related potentials to repeated speech in 9-month-old infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. J Neurodev Disord. 2014;6:43. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-6-43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Selby L, Zhang C, Sun QQ. Major defects in neocortical GABAergic inhibitory circuits in mice lacking the Fragile X mental retardation protein. Neurosci Lett. 2007;412:227–232. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.11.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seri S, Cerquiglini A, Pisani F, Curatolo P. Autism in tuberous sclerosis: evoked potential evidence for a deficit in auditory sensory processing. Clin Neurophysiol. 1999;110:1825–1830. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00137-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seri S, Pisani F, Thai JN, Cerquiglini A. Pre-attentive auditory sensory processing in autistic spectrum disorder. Are electromagnetic measurements telling us a coherent story? Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;63:159–163. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shi L, Fatemi SH, Sidwell RW, Patterson PH. Maternal influenza infection causes marked behavioral and pharmacological changes in the offspring. J Neurosci. 2003;23:297–302. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-01-00297.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sidhu H, Dansie LE, Hickmott PW, Ethell DW, Ethell IM. Genetic removal of matrix metalloproteinase 9 rescues the symptoms of Fragile X syndrome in a mouse model. J Neurosci. 2014;34:9867–9879. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1162-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel SJ, Connolly P, Liang Y, Lenox RH, Gur RE, Bilker WB, Kanes SJ, Turetsky BI. Effects of strain, novelty, and NMDA blockade on auditory-evoked potentials in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:675–682. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silverman JL, Yang M, Turner SM, Katz AM, Bell DB, Koenig JI, Crawley JN. Low stress reactivity and neuroendocrine factors in the BTBR T + tf/J mouse model of autism. Neuroscience. 2010;171:1197–1208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.09.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons-Weidenmaier NS, Weber M, Plappert CF, Pilz PK, Schmid S. Synaptic depression and short-term habituation are located in the sensory part of the mammalian startle pathway. BMC Neurosci. 2006;7:38. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-7-38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spencer KM, Nestor PG, Niznikiewicz MA, Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, McCarley RW. Abnormal neural synchrony in schizophrenia. J Neurosci. 2003;23:7407–7411. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-19-07407.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spencer CM, Alekseyenko O, Serysheva E, Yuva-Paylor LA, Paylor R. Altered anxiety-related and social behaviors in the Fmr1 knockout mouse model of Fragile X syndrome. Genes Brain Behav. 2005;4:420–430. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00123.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squires NK, Squires KC, Hillyard SA. Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1975;38:387–401. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(75)90263-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- St Clair DM, Blackwood DH, Oliver CJ, Dickens P. P3 abnormality in Fragile X syndrome. Biol Psychiatry. 1987;22:303–312. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(87)90148-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sterling L, Munson J, Estes A, Murias M, Webb SJ, King B, Dawson G. Fear-potentiated startle response is unrelated to social or emotional functioning in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 2013;6:320–331. doi: 10.1002/aur.1289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stroganova TA, Nygren G, Tsetlin MM, Posikera IN, Gillberg C, Elam M, Orekhova EV. Abnormal EEG lateralization in boys with autism. Clin Neurophysiol. 2007;118:1842–1854. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stroganova TA, Kozunov VV, Posikera IN, Galuta IA, Gratchev VV, Orekhova EV. Abnormal pre-attentive arousal in young children with autism spectrum disorder contributes to their atypical auditory behavior: an ERP study. PLoS One. 2013;8:e69100. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan JC, Miller LJ, Nielsen DM, Schoen SA. The presence of migraines and its association with sensory hyperreactivity and anxiety symptomatology in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism. 2014;18:743–747. doi: 10.1177/1362361313489377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sussman E, Steinschneider M, Gumenyuk V, Grushko J, Lawson K. The maturation of human evoked brain potentials to sounds presented at different stimulus rates. Hear Res. 2008;236:61–79. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2007.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton S, Braren M, Zubin J, John ER. Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty. Science. 1965;150:1187–1188. doi: 10.1126/science.150.3700.1187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA. Prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in rats after lesions of the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus. Behav Neurosci. 1993;107:104–117. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.107.1.104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swerdlow NR, Braff DL, Taaid N, Geyer MA. Assessing the validity of an animal model of deficient sensorimotor gating in schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51:139–154. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950020063007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swerdlow NR, Braff DL, Geyer MA. Cross-species studies of sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;877:202–216. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09269.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi H, Komatsu S, Nakahachi T, Ogino K, Kamio Y. Relationship of the acoustic startle response and its modulation to emotional and behavioral problems in typical development children and those with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46:534–543. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2593-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tavassoli T, Bellesheim K, Siper PM, Wang AT, Halpern D, Gorenstein M, Grodberg D, Kolevzon A, Buxbaum JD. Measuring sensory reactivity in autism spectrum disorder: application and simplification of a clinician-Administered sensory observation scale. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46:287–293. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2578-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thibaut F, Boutros NN, Jarema M, Oranje B, Hasan A, Daskalakis ZJ, Wichniak A, Schmitt A, Riederer P, Falkai P, Marker WTFB. Consensus paper of the WFSBP task force on biological markers: criteria for biomarkers and endophenotypes of schizophrenia part I: neurophysiology. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2015;16:280–290. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2015.1050061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Truong DT, Rendall AR, Castelluccio BC, Eigsti IM, Fitch RH. Auditory processing and morphological anomalies in medial geniculate nucleus of Cntnap2 mutant mice. Behav Neurosci. 2015;129:731–743. doi: 10.1037/bne0000096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Typlt M, Mirkowski M, Azzopardi E, Ruettiger L, Ruth P, Schmid S. Mice with deficient BK channel function show impaired prepulse inhibition and spatial learning, but normal working and spatial reference memory. PLoS One. 2013a;8:e81270. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Typlt M, Mirkowski M, Azzopardi E, Ruth P, Pilz PK, Schmid S. Habituation of reflexive and motivated behavior in mice with deficient BK channel function. Front Integr Neurosci. 2013b;7:79. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Umbricht D, Vyssotky D, Latanov A, Nitsch R, Brambilla R, D’Adamo P, Lipp H-P. Midlatency auditory event-related potentials in mice: comparison to midlatency auditory ERPs in humans. Brain Res. 2004;1019:189–200. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.05.097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Unenge Hallerback M, Lugnegard T, Gillberg C. Is autism spectrum disorder common in schizophrenia? Psychiatry Res. 2012;198:12–17. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uutela M, Lindholm J, Louhivuori V, Wei H, Louhivuori LM, Pertovaara A, Akerman K, Castrén E, Castrén ML. Reduction of BDNF expression in Fmr1 knockout mice worsens cognitive deficits but improves hyperactivity and sensorimotor deficits. Genes Brain Behav. 2012;11:513–523. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2012.00784.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valsamis B, Schmid S. Habituation and prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in rodents. J Visualized Exp. 2011;55:e3446. doi: 10.3791/3446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Molen MJ, Stam CJ, van der Molen MW. Resting-state EEG oscillatory dynamics in Fragile X syndrome: abnormal functional connectivity and brain network organization. PLoS One. 2014;9:e88451. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Molen MJ, Van der Molen MW. Reduced alpha and exaggerated theta power during the resting-state EEG in Fragile X syndrome. Biol Psychol. 2013;92:216–219. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.11.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Molen MJ, Van der Molen MW, Ridderinkhof KR, Hamel BC, Curfs LM, Ramakers GJ. Crown 2011. Elsevier Ireland Ltd; Netherlands: 2012a. Auditory and Visual Cortical Activity During Selective Attention in Fragile X Syndrome: A Cascade of Processing Deficiencies, Clin Neurophysiol; pp. 720–729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Molen MJ, Van der Molen MW, Ridderinkhof KR, Hamel BC, Curfs LM, Ramakers GJ. International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Elsevier Ireland Ltd; Netherlands: 2012b. Auditory change detection in Fragile X syndrome males: a brain potential study, Clin Neurophysiol. 2011; pp. 1309–1318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Diessen E, Senders J, Jansen F, Boersma M, Bruining H. Increased power of resting-state gamma oscillations in autism spectrum disorder detected by routine electroencephalography. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;265:537–540. doi: 10.1007/s00406-014-0527-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vorhees CV, Graham DL, Braun AA, Schaefer TL, Skelton MR, Richtand NM, Williams MT. Prenatal immune challenge in rats: effects of polyinosinic-polycytidylic acid on spatial learning, prepulse inhibition, conditioned fear, and responses to MK-801 and amphetamine. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2015;47:54–65. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2014.10.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang J, Barstein J, Ethridge LE, Mosconi MW, Takarae Y, Sweeney JA. Resting state EEG abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders. J Neurodev Disord. 2013;5:24. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-5-24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y, Sakano H, Beebe K, Brown MR, de Laat R, Bothwell M, Kulesza RJ, Rubel EW. Intense and specialized dendritic localization of the Fragile X mental retardation protein in binaural brainstem neurons: a comparative study in the alligator, chicken, gerbil, and human. J Comp Neurol. 2014;522:2107–2128. doi: 10.1002/cne.23520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weber M, Schnitzler HU, Schmid S. Synaptic plasticity in the acoustic startle pathway: the neuronal basis for short-term habituation? Eur J Neurosci. 2002;16:1325–1332. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02194.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weismuller B, Thienel R, Youlden AM, Fulham R, Koch M, Schall U. Psychophysiological correlates of developmental changes in healthy and autistic boys. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:2168–2175. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2385-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilbarger JL, McIntosh DN, Winkielman P. Startle modulation in autism: positive affective stimuli enhance startle response. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:1323–1331. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witten L, Oranje B, Mork A, Steiniger-Brach B, Glenthoj BY, Bastlund JF. Auditory sensory processing deficits in sensory gating and mismatch negativity-like responses in the social isolation rat model of schizophrenia. Behav Brain Res. 2014;266:85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.02.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright B, Alderson-Day B, Prendergast G, Bennett S, Jordan J, Whitton C, Gouws A, Jones N, Attur R, Tomlinson H, Green G. Gamma activation in young people with autism spectrum disorders and typically-developing controls when viewing emotions on faces. PLoS One. 2012;7:e41326. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wunderlich JL, Cone-Wesson BK. Maturation of CAEP in infants and children: a review. Hear Res. 2006;212:212–223. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2005.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wunderlich JL, Cone-Wesson BK, Shepherd R. Maturation of the cortical auditory evoked potential in infants and young children. Hear Res. 2006;212:185–202. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2005.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wurzman R, Forcelli PA, Griffey CJ, Kromer LF. Repetitive grooming and sensorimotor abnormalities in an ephrin—a knockout model for autism spectrum disorders. Behav Brain Res. 2015;278:115–128. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wyatt LR, Godar SC, Khoja S, Jakowec MW, Alkana RL, Bortolato M, Davies DL. Sociocommunicative and sensorimotor impairments in male P2X4-deficient mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38:1993–2002. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.98. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang M, Mahrt EJ, Lewis F, Foley G, Portmann T, Dolmetsch RE, Portfors CV, Crawley JN. 16p11.2 deletion syndrome mice display sensory and ultrasonic vocalization deficits during social interactions. Autism Res. 2015;8:507–521. doi: 10.1002/aur.1465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeomans JS, Lee J, Yeomans MH, Steidl S, Li L. Midbrain pathways for prepulse inhibition and startle activation in rat. Neuroscience. 2006;142:921–929. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.06.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yuhas J, Cordeiro L, Tassone F, Ballinger E, Schneider A, Long JM, Ornitz EM, Hessl D. Brief report: sensorimotor gating in idiopathic autism and autism associated with Fragile X syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41:248–253. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1040-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yun SW, Platholi J, Flaherty MS, Fu W, Kottmann AH, Toth M. Fmrp is required for the establishment of the startle response during the critical period of auditory development. Brain Res. 2006;1110:159–165. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Y, Bonnan A, Bony G, Ferezou I, Pietropaolo S, Ginger M, Sans N, Rossier J, Oostra B, LeMasson G, Frick A. Dendritic channelopathies contribute to neocortical and sensory hyperexcitability in Fmr1(-/y) mice. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17:1701–1709. doi: 10.1038/nn.3864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Rogers T, Roberts W, Brian J, Szatmari P. Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2005;23:143–152. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]