Abstract
Background:
Neurocognitive and functional outcome deficits have long been acknowledged in schizophrenia and neurocognition has been found to account for functional disability to a greater extent than psychopathology. Much of the variance in functional outcome however still remains unexplained and metacognition may mediate the relationship between neurocognition, functional capacity, and self-reported social and occupational function.
Method:
Eighty first episode psychosis participants were recruited and completed measures of neurocognition (memory, executive function, and intelligence quotient), metacognition (Beck Cognitive Insight Scale, Metacognitive Awareness Interview), psychopathology (PANSS), and both functional capacity (UPSA) and real-life social and occupational function (The Time Use Survey). Path analyses investigated the relationships between variables through structural equation modeling.
Results:
A series of path models demonstrated that metacognition partially mediates the relationship between neurocognition and functional capacity, and fully mediates the relationship between functional capacity and social and occupational function.
Conclusion:
The present study findings identify that metacognition may be critical to translating cognitive and functional skills into real-world contexts, and this relationship is found at early stages of illness. Understanding how individuals translate cognitive and functional skills into the real-world (the competence–performance gap) may offer valuable guidance to intervention programs. This finding is important to models of recovery as it suggests that intervention programs that focus on enhancing metacognition abilities may have a greater impact than traditional rehabilitation programs focusing on cognitive abilities, on social and occupational outcomes.
Keywords: neurocognition, metacognition, functional outcome, structural equation modeling, rehabilitation, cognitive insight
Introduction
Neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia are considered a core feature of the disorder1 and a vulnerability marker for later illness development.2 Cognitive ability may also predict functional recovery in the community.3,4 Better cognitive performance has been associated with improved self-reported quality of life,5 social and occupational outcomes6,7 and these associations persist in longitudinal designs (see Green et al8 or Lepage et al9 for a review). Cognitive and functional disabilities are also credible treatment targets.10 This association of improved cognitive skills with improved functional outcome led to the introduction of cognitive remediation programs aiming to improve an individual’s cognitive skills, and thereby functional recovery and real world community outcomes.11 However, currently the evidence for the impact on to real-world improvements in functional status is equivocal.12 Some reviews suggest that neurocognition only accounts for around 40% of the relationship between cognitive ability and functional outcome, which suggests that 60% of the relationship remains unexplained.13 The link between neurocognition and functional outcome is perhaps not direct and this has led some to the search for mediating variables to account for the relationship.13
Metacognition has been proposed as a candidate variable to explain the unaccounted variance in the relationship between neurocognition14 and functional outcome15 in schizophrenia. Metacognition is broadly defined as “thinking about thinking16 and relates to our ability to inspect cognitive products and mental states17 and objectively scrutinize them.18 This higher-order ability draws upon cognitive skills to process self-referential information and may be essential to the integration of raw cognitive processing into a complex and constantly evolving social world.19 Thus, it is what we know that we know, ie, important.20 Being unaware of erroneous decisions (possibly as a result of underlying neurocognitive deficits) may lead to inaccurate social interpretations and poor behavioral response choices fostering functional disability.
Metacognition can be both conceptualized and measured in a number of ways. One particular conceptualization (synthetic metacognition) has been to assess an individual’s ability to describe and reflect back on their own cognition, differentiate between mental state transitions, their relationship to emotion and behavior and to differentiate between one’s own and the mental states of others.21,22 Other authors have conceptualized metacognition as the insight we have into the fallibility of our own cognition (cognitive insight)23 or the appropriate level of confidence applied to cognition-based decisions (metacognitive accuracy).24 Relationships have been found between metacognition and functioning25,26 and neurocognition,27 although the findings for metacognition as cognitive insight appear more mixed.28 Metacognition has been found to mediate the relationship between neurocognition and social functioning in a chronic schizophrenia sample.26 However, it is not known if this finding would be replicated in first episode psychosis (FEP) or if metacognition predicts real world function.26 Most research in this area has been completed on chronic cohorts which introduces confounds. In studies, the use of chronic cohorts makes findings difficult to interpret as the effects on neurocognition and metacognition are difficult to disentangle from the longer-term impact of neuroleptic medication and differing access to long-term contact with psychiatric services and psychological therapies.29 Past researchers have also tended to employ only one measure of outcome (the frequency of social contacts) whereas functional recovery encompasses a variety of domains including occupational,30 social,31 and capacity skills.32
In relation to functional outcome, metacognition may offer a unique account for functional disability aside from the known relationship with cognition. Metacognition has been found to associate with both social cognition and cognition26 and factor analytic investigations have found it to load on a separate construct to both.33 Metacognition uses similar skills to mentalization but specifically addresses the application of these mentalization skills into social contexts.
Assessment of functional outcome has become increasingly sophisticated in recent years with authors beginning to acknowledge the distinction between what one can do (functional capacity) from what one actually does in real-life. Assessments of functional capacity attempt to provide objective measures of current functional ability free from the impact of social factors such as denial of opportunity.34 Functional capacity has been found to mediate the relationship between neurocognition and real-world function.35 Gupta et al36 found that a larger competence–performance gap is associated with earlier onset of illness, depressive symptoms, and greater time in hospital suggesting a complex relationship between the variables.
In summary metacognition, having the ability to reflect back and learn, may be critical to the initiation, integrating, and application of cognitive skills into real-world situations. These metacognitive skills may aid the learning of new information, improve social and occupational recovery and promote self-management in the community. Incorporating metacognitive processing into cognitive remediation programs has already begun in trials with positive results thus far.37 However, research is needed to clarify how metacognition impacts on the relationship between neurocognition and both functional capacity and social and occupational function. To date few designs have differentiated between these variables.
The present study investigated the association between metacognition, neurocognition, and both functional capacity and social and occupational functioning in a cohort of people with FEP using structural equation modeling. We predicted that metacognition would mediate the relationship between neurocognition and functional capacity (assessed by performance skills assessment UPSA) and secondly that metacognition will mediate the relationship between functional capacity and real-world functioning (assessed by Time Use). This is the first study of its type with FEP.
Methods
Participants
Participants were recruited from Early Intervention in Psychosis (EIP) services in Sussex, UK. Ethical approval was obtained (Ref:11/LO/1877, project ID 72141). All participants gave informed written consent to enter the research study. The study inclusion criteria were: A current diagnosis of FEP and being over the age of 18. Exclusion criteria were: A primary diagnosis of substance misuse or organic neurological impairment or insufficient English language skills to complete the assessments. Demographic and medication information was recorded and medication converted to olanzapine atypical equivalents using conversion tables from Leucht et al.38
Measures
Neurocognition.
Several specific domains of neurocognitive function known to be impaired in schizophrenia and psychosis were selected as previous research identifies the need for more comprehensive and more standardized39 measurement of neurocognitive impairment.
Verbal and working memory were assessed using the Logical Memory and Letter-Number Sequencing subscales from the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-III).40 Executive function was assessed through phonological and semantic41 Verbal Fluency, and the Trailmaking Task. Intelligence quotient (IQ) was captured through the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence.42
Metacognition.
Metacognition was assessed through two measures: the Metacognitive Assessment Interview (MAI)43 and the Beck Cognitive Insight Scale (BCIS).23 These measures were selected for their frequency of use as metacognitive measures in psychosis and schizophrenia and shared loading in a factor analysis investigation.33 The MAI is an adaptation of the Metacognitive Assessment Scale (MAS)22 and based on the same theoretical framework43 but is less time consuming to administer as metacognitive function is directly questioned as opposed to assessing a standardized psychiatric interview retrospectively. The MAS upon which the MAI is based has been validated across numerous clinical populations17 including FEP44 and schizophrenia45 and the MAI has been validated in a clinical population.46
The MAI is a semi-structured clinical interview designed to assess 4 domains of metacognition; monitoring, integration, differentiation, and decentralization. A manualized set of interview questions are conducted, with the participant response guiding the interview sequence to assess the aforementioned domains. The monitoring subsection is comprised of questions that capture the interviewee’s ability to identify and monitor the thoughts and emotions that make up their own mental state. The integration subscale assesses the individual’s ability to reflect back on the transitions between their own mental states and identify causal reasons behind the transitions. The differentiation subscale assesses the individual’s ability to consider their point of view as subjective and fallible and distinguish between fantasies, beliefs, and assumptions about reality in relation to factual events. The final decentralization subscale requires the participant to describe and interpret the mental state of another person and to understand how that person’s beliefs, values, and perspectives are separate from their own.
The BCIS captures the participant’s self-reported ability to reflect on their own cognitive products, distance themselves from and re-evaluate thoughts, beliefs, and subjective interpretations.23 It comprises 2 subscales: the self-reflectiveness scale assesses the individual’s willingness to reflect upon and be objective about thoughts. The self-certainty subscale relates to the individual’s certainty about being right and their resistance to correction. This information is measured through 15 self-report questionnaire items rated from 0 (do not agree at all) to 3 (agree completely). The BCIS has been assessed for validity and reliability23,47 and employed in FEP.48
Symptoms.
Symptoms were measured by the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS).49 The PANSS has been well validated in research50 and employed in FEP.44
Functioning.
Function was measured through two methods: functional capacity and self-reported social and occupational functioning (SOF). Functional capacity was measured by the UCSD Performance-based Skills Assessment (UPSA)51 and SOF was captured by the Time Use Survey.52 The UPSA is an instrument to assess capacity to complete everyday tasks across 5 domains: finance, communication, comprehension and planning, transportation, and household chores. The UPSA has been assessed for reliability and validated in schizophrenia53 and employed in FEP before.54,55
The Time Use Survey51 is a semi-structured interview in which the participant is asked about the amount of time spent undertaking a variety of activities in the preceding month. The activities capture a host of functional domains including employment, education, voluntary work, leisure and sport, childcare and household maintenance. The total time spent in structured activities was calculated from the aforementioned domains and included in analysis as a measure of SOF. The Time Use survey has been used in healthy individuals, FEP and CHR groups56 in previous research.
Data Analysis
First, descriptive statistics for all variables were computed using SPSS version 22 and inspected for normality and suitability for factor analysis. Second, preliminary relationships between variables were investigated through correlation analysis. The factor structure of observed variables was next explored using Mplus version 6. Factor structure was assessed through confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) where pre-existing data was available and exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM) where no existing research had assessed factor structure in the population.
Third, mediation analyses were conducted using Mplus (version 6), to investigate the relationships between the key factors of neurocognition, functional capacity, and SOF, with metacognition as a mediator. The role of negative symptoms was also explored. The mediation model was investigated through indirect pathway statistics and confidence intervals (CI) derived through bootstrapping as suggested by Preacher and Hayes.56 To support the direction of the relationships investigated, reverse models were also assessed. Lastly, to rule out the role of medication, the relationship between olanzapine equivalent and metacognitive variables and function was investigated through correlation analysis.
Results
Sample Statistics
Eighty FEP patients were recruited in total, mean age 26.08 (SD = 5.53, range = 18–40) and the sample was comprised of 49 male and 31 female patients (see table 1 for demographic information).
Table 1.
Sample Characteristics
Sample Characteristics | Mean (SD) |
---|---|
Age | 26.08 (5.53) |
Gender (M/F) | 49/31 |
Symptoms (positive) | 12.01 (3.5) |
Symptoms (negative) | 13.6 (4.92) |
Symptoms (general) | 28.35 (6.7) |
Prescribed anti-psychotic medication (Y/N) | 48/32 |
Olanzapine equivalent dose (of those prescribed medication) (mg/day) | 12.77 (7.79) |
The means and standard deviations of the neurocognitive, metacognitive, and functional capacity and outcome variables are available in table 2. Detailed test statistics are provided in tables 3 and 4. In addition, appropriate summary test statistics (r values) have been included throughout. Missing data were treated as missing at random (MAR) therefore a maximum likelihood model estimation was used to address missing data issues as the default in Mplus.
Table 2.
Study Variable Mean Scores and Range
Measure | Raw Score Mean (SD) | Range |
---|---|---|
Neurocognition | ||
Logical memory I(0–75) | 27.7 (10.9) | 10–55 |
Logical memory II(0–50) | 16.28 (8.26) | 0–35 |
Letter-number sequencing(0–21) | 8.64 (2.46) | 4–15 |
Verbal fluency (phonetic) | 32.28 (10.27) | 5–56 |
Verbal fluency (semantic) | 18.49 (4.77) | 9–29 |
Matrix reasoning(0–35) | 25.82 (4.13) | 13–34 |
Vocabulary(0–80) | 52.8 (11.67) | 11–73 |
Trailmaking task (B-A) | 37.99 (33.88) | |
Metacognition | ||
(MAI) Monitoring(0–5) | 2.93 (1.25) | 0–5 |
(MAI) Differentiation(0–5) | 2.77 (1.17) | 0–5 |
(MAI) Integration(0–5) | 2.83 (1.22) | 0–5 |
(MAI) Decentralism(0–5) | 2.57 (1.43) | 0–5 |
(BCIS) Self-reflectivity(0–27) | 14.3 (5.06) | 0–5 |
(BCIS) Self-certainty(0–18) | 5.88 (2.88) | 0–5 |
Functional outcome | ||
(UPSA) Finance(0–20) | 15.68 (3.06) | 7.27–20 |
(UPSA) Communication(0–20) | 12.99 (3.69) | 5–18.33 |
(UPSA) Comprehension and planning(0–20) | 12.48 (4.65) | 0–20 |
(UPSA) Transport(0–20) | 15.12 (3.03) | 8.89–20 |
(UPSA) Household(0–20) | 15.75 (4.42) | 0–20 |
(Time Use) Structured activity (total hours per week) | 24.97 (23.09) | 2.3– 96.74 |
Note: Scale total range indicated in brackets(x–x).
Table 3.
Bivariate Correlations Between Variables
Education | Neurocognition | BCIS Self-reflectivity | MAI | UPSA | Time Use | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Neurocognition | .56*** | 1 | ||||
Beck Cognitive Insight Scale: self-reflectivity | .37** | .19 | 1 | |||
Synthetic metacognition (MAI) | .56*** | .61*** | .43*** | 1 | ||
Functional capacity (UPSA) | .57*** | .70*** | .31** | .81*** | 1 | |
Self-report social and occupational function (Time Use) | .43** | .48*** | .33** | .84*** | .64*** | 1 |
***P < .001, **P < .01, *P < .05.
Table 4.
Full List of Mediation Models With Fit Statistics
Predictor | Mediator | Outcome | Model Fit (χ2 (df), p) | CFI/TFI/RMSEA | Indirect (a × b, [95% CI]) | R 2 med | R 2 out |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
NC | MC | FX | .03 (1), .86 | 1.00, 1.04, 0.00 | 0.40 (.29)** [.07, .50] | .313 | .673 |
NC | MC | FO | .11 (1), .74 | 1.00, 1.04, 0.00 | 18.55 (.41)*** [.31, .64] | .317 | .594 |
NC | FX | FO | .00 (0), .00a | 1.00, 1.00, 0.00 | 18.74 (.40)*** [.26, .55] | .495 | .413 |
FX | MC | FO | .17 (1), .68 | 1.00, 1.00, 0.00 | 24.14 (.41)*** [.31, .64] | .314 | .66 |
NC | MC, Neg | FX | .22 (3), .00 | 0.89, 0.63, 0.00 | 25.68 (.60)** [.24, .95] | .595 | .71 |
Note: NC, neurocognition; MC, metacognition; FX, functional capacity; FO, self-report structured activity; Neg, negative symptoms. Parenthesis indicates the standardized values.
aJust identified model.
***P < .001, **P < .01, *P < .05.
Model selection and assessment was made using the criteria outlined by Preacher and Hayes57 where bootstrapping and confidence intervals are advised to assess the presence of mediation rather than the more traditional Baron and Kenny approach.
Factor Analysis
A CFA confirmed that neurocognition was a 1-factor solution containing verbal and working memory, executive function, verbal, and performance IQ. The model demonstrated an acceptable fit to the observed data [χ2(29) = 29.55, P = .06, CFI = 0.96, TLI = 0.95, RMSEA = 0.08]. An ESEM analysis suggested that metacognition was best captured by the BCIS self-reflectivity and MAI total score. The CFA for functional capacity suggested that a 1-factor solution offered acceptable model fit statistics test [χ2(5) = 10.59, P = .06, CFI = 0.94, TLI = 0.89, RMSEA = 0.13). Self-reported social and occupational function was included a single item observed variable in mediation analysis.
Correlation Analysis
Table 3 reveals that, as suggested by previous research, neurocognitive ability is significantly associated with one’s ability to conduct everyday tasks (functional capacity) and the amount of time spent in structured activities (SOF). Higher neurocognitive ability is associated with better individual functioning. The analysis also demonstrates that metacognitive ability, both measured by the MAI and the BCIS self-reflectivity scale, is positively associated with both functional capacity and SOF. This suggests that better metacognitive function is associated with greater capacity to complete daily living tasks and spending more time in structured activities. Due to the high correlation between neurocognition and education level, neurocognition alone was selected for further analysis. No significant relationship was found between medication dose and metacognition (P = .687) or functional capacity (P = .407) or SOF (P = .369) so excluded from further analysis.
Path Analysis
To replicate existing work, the role of functional capacity in mediating the relationship between neurocognition and SOF was investigated and a significant mediation pathway was found. In order to test the mediating effect of metacognition on the relationship between neurocognition and functional capacity, model 1 (figure 1) was tested. A significant direct pathway was found between neurocognition and both functional capacity (P < .001) and metacognition (P = .02) and between metacognition and functional capacity (P = .005). The mediation model was then run and metacognition significantly mediated the relationship between neurocognition and functional capacity (β = .29, P = .009, ±95% CI [0.07, 0.50]). As the direct pathway remained significant in the mediation model the data is consistent with partial mediation. The mediation model accounted for 67% (R2 = .67) of the variation in capacity to complete simulated daily living tasks and 31% of the variation in metacognitive ability (R2 = .31). The reverse model (X and Y reversed and Y and M reversed) was not significant. Additional models were run with a significant secondary mediation pathway through negative symptoms and negative symptoms as an individual mediator of cognition and functional capacity. However introducing negative symptoms into the model led to the model fit statistics (CFI, TFI, and chi-square model fit) deteriorated below what is considered acceptable model fit therefore the simple model with only metacognition was selected.
Fig. 1.
Mediation of the effect of neurocognition on functional capacity through metacognition. ***P < .001, **P < .01, *P < .05.
Next, the relationship between functional capacity and SOF was assessed, with metacognition as a mediator (figure 2). Metacognition significantly mediated the relationship between functional capacity and SOF as evidenced by the indirect path (β = .41, P = .001, 95% CI [0.31, 0.64]). Interestingly, as the direct path is not significant, this suggests the presence of full mediation and the model accounted for 66% of the variance in SOF (R2 = .66). The reverse model was not significant. Alternative models were also run and metacognition was found to significantly mediate the relationship between neurocognition and SOF (available in table 4. Table 4 contains the indirect (mediation) path statistics, CIs, and overall model fit statistics). This confirms that the ability to conduct daily tasks and the ability to scrutinize cognitive products are implicated in translating cognitive abilities into real-life contexts.
Fig. 2.
Mediation of the effect of functional capacity on self-reported social and occupational functioning through metacognition. ***P < .001, **P < .01, *P < .05.
Discussion
The present study offers two important findings; (a) that metacognition partially mediates the relationship between neurocognition and functional capacity, and (b) that metacognition fully mediates the relationship between functional capacity and SOF. All models reported demonstrated acceptable model fit statistics (table 4) and the effect sizes (R2) are large adding to the importance of the present work. In relation to the hypotheses, hypothesis 1 that metacognition would significantly mediate the relationship between neurocognition and functional capacity, was partially accepted as a significant partial mediation relationship was found. Hypothesis 2 that metacognition would significantly mediate the relationship between functional capacity and SOF was fully supported.
Neurocognition and Functional Capacity
The mediation analysis found that metacognition partially mediates the relationship between neurocognition and functional capacity explaining 67% of the overall variance in functional capacity. This suggests that raw cognitive ability may not be sufficient in itself to successfully complete everyday tasks but rather the metacognitive ability to accurately reflect back on cognition may be a key ingredient in equipping those recovering from psychosis to apply these cognitive process to function effectively.
Functional Capacity and SOF
In terms of the relationship between functional capacity and SOF, metacognition was found to fully mediate this relationship. This is an important finding as this suggests that, while individuals may possess the capacity to complete tasks essential to community function, without sufficient metacognitive abilities, these skills may not be initiated, integrated, and applied successfully to support real-world function and recovery. This finding suggest that metacognition and the ability to reflect back on cognitive products and accurately identify their relationship to emotions and behavior in both the self and others, is critical to translating cognitive skills into real-world behavior.
In terms of the relationship between neurocognition and metacognition, the present study corroborates the work of Hamm et al,58 Nicolò et al,14 and Abu-Akel and Bo,59 and a host of papers by Lysaker17,21,26 in which improved neurocognition is associated with better metacognition. The present sample report similar scores to a paper by Lepage28 in FEP on both self-reflectivity (mean 13 vs the present study 15.88) and self-certainty subscales (7.9 vs the present study 5.88). The work available on synthetic metacognition is more difficult to compare as the other studies in the area29,45 used a slightly different measure. The present study does however offer corollary support in that deficits in metacognition are present in FEP however goes further by offering an account for the relationship between neurocognition and functional outcome. The findings that self-reflectivity but not self-certainty loaded onto an overall metacognitive factor is interesting and can be accounted for by known work in the area. Gilleen et al60 found a relationship between self-reflectivity and awareness but not with self-certainty. Self-certainty may be more related to pathology and delusional thinking than higher-order synthetic metacognition. The present study also confirms that metacognition is an important determinant of community function as suggested by Giusti et al25 and Brüne et al.18 These studies were predominantly obtained in chronic samples however and this is the first study in FEP.
The present study builds on Lysaker et al’s26 study which found that metacognition mediated the relationship between cognition and social function. The present study extends this finding going on to demonstrate that this mediation effect may also be found using a measure of real life function (Time Use), as well as functional capacity (UPSA) and using a richer overall construct of metacognition. The present study was conducted in FEP which minimizes the problems of interpretation associated with findings from chronic participants due to confounding with chronicity of illness and associated medication and treatment therapy exposure. The present article adds to our understanding of the competence–performance gap35 by suggesting that metacognition may account for the disparity in the ability to complete daily living tasks, and actually performing them in the community. The present findings may imply that that the relationship between cognition and functional outcome may be a multi-step pathway, with metacognition as well as social cognitive and belief driven causal mechanisms. While Green et al61 have discussed this possibility, they suggest that a single pathway through social cognition is the most parsimonious solution.
The role of social cognition was not investigated in the present study although has been investigated at length elsewhere.8,30 However, some shared relationship between the MAI domain understanding the mind of others and social cognitive domains such as theory of mind would be expected as both require the individual to adopt the perspective of others which may be key to a host of functional domains such as social relationships and occupational functioning. A future study investigating the relationship between social cognitive and metacognitive variables on different aspects of functional outcome would be valuable to understand the unique contribution each offers different aspects of community functioning. Our findings suggest that metacognition may be a key mediating variable which is able to offer an account for how these pathways interact.
This study had methodological strengths in contrast to previous studies using chronic samples. No significant relationships were found between medication and metacognitive domains or outcome domains in this first episode sample thus the impact of medication may be minimized. Furthermore, although the sample was still predominantly male, the ratio was much more even (61% male versus 39% female) than in past studies. This is also the first study to incorporate cognitive insight into this analysis and suggests that self-reflectivity may contribute to community function in addition to cognitive ability.
In addition to work with CRT, the present study adds to the evidence base supporting treatment initiatives such as the Metacognitive Reflection and Insight Therapy (MERIT) and Neurocognitive Enhancement Therapy (NET) which also directly target the mechanisms discussed in the present article for development in psychotherapy. MERIT seeks to assist those in treatment with developing a more complex narrative of their own mental lives and to be able to reflect on interpersonal events to better assist reducing social disability.62 The present study offers a clear link between these abilities and their importance to improving functional outcome at early stage of illness. One of the key outcomes for NET has been identified as translating cognitive skills into real-world benefits63 and the present study demonstrates that metacognitive ability may be key to facilitating this.
Engaging in normative social and occupational activity64 and participating in social and meaningful activity65 have been identified as key to personal recovery.66 Thus, a greater understanding of factors that allow individuals to maintain or regain these structured activities is crucial to treatments to improve functional outcome.
The study findings are of clinical relevance as they suggests that cognitive abilities are an important determinant of the capacity to complete everyday tasks, however, metacognition is also required to navigate the complexities of daily life. When it comes to translating these skills into a real-world setting, targeting metacognition is potentially at least as important as a focus on raw cognitive ability which may be less amenable to intervention. In summary, one’s ability to reflect back on cognition and monitor the transitions between mental states may improve both the capacity to complete daily tasks and apply these competencies to real-world situations.
Limitations
Although the present study attempted to address many of the identified issues with known research, a number of limitations must be held in mind when considering the findings. Firstly, the present study is cross-sectional so the impact of changes in neurocognition and metacognition and how these may impact on functional levels in the individual are not known. The reverse model testing offers support to the direction of effects, however, Maxwell and Cole67 warn against claims of causation when employing cross-sectional data. Future studies tracking the changes in all three domains across time would offer greater insight into the mechanisms of the temporal relationship between variables.
The MAI, as a measure of synthetic metacognitive ability, relies on self-report of internal mechanisms. Language disturbances have been identified as a primary feature of schizophrenia68 or as a result of current symptoms.69 As a self-report interview measure reliant upon verbalization of internal experiences, deficits may be due to linguistic deficits or descriptive impairment rather than metacognition per se. However, as one of the measures of neurocognition is verbal IQ, logical memory, and verbal fluency also draw on these abilities, both neurocognition and metacognition measurement would have been impacted on by language deficits in the study.
Secondly, the present study was potentially underpowered, however, sample size was acceptable for simple path analysis to detect mediation effects according to Fritz and MacKinnon.70 In addition to maximize power, the models assessed were restricted; ideally the path models would have been measurement models with all indicators included, however, the present study had to approach this in a stepwise manner and run through multiple models to maintain a respectable case to parameter ratio.71 Finally, due to the poor factor loading of self-certainty, the latent variable of metacognition was only measured by two indicators. Although Kenny suggests this as a minimum number, 3 is a safer option72 and under estimation may lead to biased parameter estimates.73 The significant correlation however between the two indicators helps to justify the use of only two indicators.
Implications
Despite these restrictions, the present study implicates metacognition in both functional capacity and real-world functioning. This confirms the relationship between neurocognition and function being mediated by metacognition. This also demonstrates that this relationship is present at early stages of illness. Metacognition may be a critical ingredient in initiating, integrating, and applying a host of skills into occupational and social situations and this may suggests that treatment programs may wish to target this ability in care pathways. Cognitive remediation programs have already started to incorporate metacognitive content to traditional cognitive remediation exercises36 and the present study suggests that this is a valuable addition. By improving both cognition and metacognition rather than solely the former, recovery from psychosis after symptoms have subsided may be maximized. The effect sizes (variance accounted for) were also larger than previous studies investigating the relationship between neurocognition and function accounting for a large proportion of the overall variance in outcome. This offers far more explanation of functioning and highlights the importance of metacognition in functional outcome.
References
- 1. Reichenberg A, Harvey PD. Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia: integration of performance-based and brain imaging findings. Psychol Bull. 2007;133:833–858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Nuechterlein KH, Subotnik KL, Ventura J, Green MF, Gretchen-Doorly D, Asarnow RF. The puzzle of schizophrenia: tracking the core role of cognitive deficits. Dev Psychopathol. 2012; 24:529–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Green MF. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153:321–330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, Mintz J. Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the “right stuff”? Schizophr Bull. 2000;26:119–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Addington J, Addinton J. Neurocognitive and social functioning in schizophrenia: a 2.5 year follow-up study. Schizophr Res. 2000;44:47–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Stirling J, Stirling C, White S., et al. Neurocognitive function and outcome in first-episode schizophrenia: a 10-year follow-up of an epidemiological cohort. Schizophr Res. 2003;65:75–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Jaeger J, Berns S, Czobar P. The multidimensional scale of independent functioning: a new instrument for measuring functional disability in psychiatric populations. Schizophr Bull. 2003;29:153–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Green MF, Kern RS, Heaton RK. Longitudinal studies of cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: implications for MATRICS. Schizophr Res. 2004;72:41–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Lepage M, Bodnar M, Bowie CR. In review neurocognition: Clinical and functional outcomes in schizophrenia. Can J Psychiatry. 2014;59:5.–. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Keefe RS, Haig GM, Marder SR, et al. Report on ISCTM consensus meeting on clinical assessment of response to treatment of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42:19–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Wykes T, Huddy V, Cellard C, McGurk SR, Czobor P. A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: methodology and effect sizes. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168:472–485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Wykes T, Reeder C, Huddy V, et al. Developing models of how cognitive improvements change functioning: mediation, moderation and moderated mediation. Schizophr Res. 2012;138:88–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Schmidt SJ, Mueller DR, Roder V. Social cognition as a mediator variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: empirical review and new results by structural equation modeling. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(suppl 2):S41–S54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Nicolò G, Dimaggio G, Popolo R., et al. Associations of metacognition with symptoms, insight, and neurocognition in clinically stable outpatients with schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2012;200:644–647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Lysaker PH, Mccormick BP, Snethen G, et al. Metacognition and social function in schizophrenia : associations of mastery with functional skills competence. Schizophr Res. 2011;131:214–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Frith CD. The role of metacognition in human social interactions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2012;367:2213–2223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Lysaker PH, Carcione A, Dimaggio G, et al. Metacognition amidst narratives of self and illness in schizophrenia: associations with neurocognition, symptoms, insight and quality of life. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2005;112:64–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Brüne M, Dimaggio G, Linkage PH. Metacognition and social functioning in schizophrenia: evidence, mechanisms of influence and treatment implications. Curr Psychiatry Rev. 2011;7:239–247. [Google Scholar]
- 19. Lysaker PH, Dimaggio G, Daroyanni P, et al. Assessing metacognition in schizophrenia with the Metacognition Assessment Scale: associations with the Social Cognition and Object Relations Scale. Psychol Psychother Theory Res Pract. 2010; 83:303–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Koriat A. Easy comes, easy goes? The link between learning and remembering and its exploitation in metacognition. Mem Cogn. 2008; 36:416–428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Lysaker PH, Warman DM, Dimaggio G, et al. Metacognition in schizophrenia: associations with multiple assessments of executive function. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2008;196:384–389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Semerari A, Carcione A, Dimaggio G, et al. How to evaluate metacognitive functioning in psychotherapy? The Metacognition Assessment scale and its applications. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2003;10:238–261. [Google Scholar]
- 23. Beck AT, Baruch E, Balter JM, Steer RA, Warman DM. A new instrument for measuring insight: the Beck Cognitive In- sight Scale. Schizophr Res. 2004;68:319–329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Bruno N, Sachs N, Demily C, Franck N, Pacherie E. Delusions and metacognition in patients with schizophrenia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2012;17:1–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Giusti L, Mazza M, Pollice R, Casacchia M, Roncone R. Relationship between self‐reflectivity, Theory of Mind, neurocognition, and global functioning: an investigation of schizophrenic disorder. Clin Psychol. 2013;17:67–76. [Google Scholar]
- 26. Lysaker PH, Shea AM, Buck KD, et al. Metacognition as a mediator of the effects of impairments in neurocognition on social function in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2010;122:405–413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Lysaker PH, Dimaggio G, Buck KD, Carcione A, Nicolò G. Metacognition within narratives of schizophrenia: associations with multiple domains of neurocognition. Schizophr Res. 2007;93:278–287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Lepage M, Buchy L, Bodnar M, Bertrand MC, Joober R, Malla A. Cognitive insight and verbal memory in first episode of psychosis. Eur Psychiatry. 2008;23:368–374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Vohs JL, Hummer TA, Yung MG, Francis MM, Lysaker PH, Breier A. Metacognition in early phase psychosis: Toward understanding neural substrates. Int J Mol Sci. 2015;16:14640–14654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Bell MD, Tsang HW, Greig TC, Bryson GJ. Neurocognition, social cognition, perceived social discomfort, and vocational outcomes in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:738–747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Robertson DA, Hargreaves A, Kelleher EB, et al. Social dysfunction in schizophrenia: an investigation of the GAF scale’s sensitivity to deficits in social cognition. Schizophr Res. 2013;146:363–365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Leifker FR, Patterson TL, Heaton RK, Harvey PD. Validating measures of real-world outcome: the results of the VALERO expert survey and RAND panel. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37:334–343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Lysaker PH, Gumley A, Luedtke B, et al. Social cognition and metacognition in schizophrenia: evidence of their independence and linkage with outcomes. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2013;127:239–247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Patterson TL, Mausbach BT. Measurement of functional capacity: a new approach to understanding functional differences and real-world behavioral adaptation in those with mental illness. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6:139–154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Bowie CR, Reichenberg A, Patterson TL, Heaton RK, Harvey PD. Determinants of real-world functioning performance in schizophrenia: correlations with cognition, functional capacity, and symptoms. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:418–425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Gupta M, Bassett E, Iftene F, Bowie CR. Functional out-comes in schizophrenia: understanding the competence-performance discrepancy. J Psychiatr Res. 2012;46:205–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Cella M, Reeder C, Wykes T. Group cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: exploring the role of therapist support and metacognition. Psychol Psychother Theory Res Pract. 2016;89:1–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Leucht S, Samara M, Heres S, Patel MX, Woods SW, Davis JM. Dose equivalents for second-generation antipsychotics: the minimum effective dose method. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40:314–326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Kern RS, et al. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, Part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:203–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Memory Scale-Revised. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- 41. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological Assessment.4th ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 42. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Abbreviated Intelligence Scale (WASI). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 43. Semerari A, Cucchi M, Dimaggio G, et al. The development of the Metacognition Assessment interview: instrument description, factor structure and reliability in a non-clinical sample. Psychiatry Res. 2012;200:890–895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. McLeod HJ, Gumley AI, MacBeth A, Schwannauer M, Lysaker PH. Metacognitive functioning predicts positive and negative symptoms over 12 months in first episode psychosis. J Psychiatr Res. 2014;54:109–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Lysaker PH, Vohs JL, Ballard R, et al. Metacognition, self-reflection and recovery in schizophrenia. Future Neurol. 2013;8:103–115. [Google Scholar]
- 46. Pellecchia G, Moroni F, Carcione A, et al. Metacognition assessment interview: instrument description and factor structure. Clin Neuropsychiatry. 2015;12:157–165. [Google Scholar]
- 47. Riggs SE, Grant PM, Perivoliotis D, Beck AT. Assessment of cognitive insight: a qualitative review. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:338–350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Tranulis C, Lepage M, Malla A. Insight in first episode psychosis: who is measuring what? Early Interv Psychiatry. 2008;2:34–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13:261–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Peralta Martín V, Cuesta Zorita MJ. [Validation of positive and negative symptom scale (PANSS) in a sample of Spanish schizophrenic patients]. Actas Luso Esp Neurol Psiquiatr Cienc Afines. 1994;22:171–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Patterson TL, Goldman S, McKibbin CL, Hughs T, Jeste DV. UCSD performance-based skills assessment: development of a new measure of everyday functioning for severely mentally ill adults. Schizophr Bull. 2001;27:235–245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Short S. Review of the UK 2000 Time Use Survey.London: Office for National Statistics; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 53. Velligan DI, Fredrick M, Mintz J, et al. The reliability and validity of the MATRICS functional assessment battery. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40:1047–1052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Vesterager L, Christensen TØ, Olsen BB, et al. Cognitive and clinical predictors of functional capacity in patients with first episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2012;141:251–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Mendella PD, Burton CZ, Tasca GA, Roy P, Louis LS, Twamley EW. Compensatory cognitive training for people with first-episode schizophrenia: results from a pilot randomized controlled trial. Schizophr Res. 2015;162:108–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Hodgekins J, French P, Birchwood M, et al. Comparing time use in individuals at different stages of psychosis and a non-clinical comparison group. Schizophr Res. 2015;161:188–193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods. 2008;40:879–891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Hamm JA, Renard SB, Fogley RL, et al. Metacognition and social cognition in schizophrenia: stability and relationship to concurrent and prospective symptom assessments. J Clin Psychol. 2012;68:1303–1312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Abu-Akel A, Bo S. Superior mentalizing abilities of female patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2013;210:794–799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Gilleen J, David A, Greenwood K. Self-reflection and set-shifting mediate awareness in cognitively preserved schizophrenia patients. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2016;21:185–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Green MF, Llerena K, Kern RS. The “right stuff” revisited: what have we learned about the determinants of daily functioning in schizophrenia? Schizophr Bull. 2015;41:781–785. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Hillis JD, Leonhardt BL, Vohs JL, et al. Metacognitive reflective and insight therapy for people in early phase of a schizophrenia spectrum disorder. J Clin Psychol. 2015;71:125–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Bell MD, Zito W, Greig T, Wexler BE. Neurocognitive enhancement therapy with vocational services: work outcomes at two-year follow-up. Schizophr Res. 2008;105:18–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Lam MM, Pearson V, Ng RM, Chiu CP, Law CW, Chen EY. What does recovery from psychosis mean? Perceptions of young first-episode patients. Int J Social Psychiatry. 2011;57:580–587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Salzmann-Erikson M. An integrative review of what contributes to personal recovery in psychiatric disabilities. Issues Ment Health Nurs. 2013;34:185–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Repper J, Perkins R. Recovery and social inclusion. In: Callaghan P Playle J Cooper L, eds. Mental Health Nursing Skill. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2009: 85–96. [Google Scholar]
- 67. Maxwell SE, Cole DA. Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation. Psychol Methods. 2007;12:23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Kuperberg G, Caplan DA. Language dysfunction in schizophrenia. In: Schiffer RB, Rao SM, Fogel BS, eds. Neuropsychiatry. 2nd ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 2003: 444–466. [Google Scholar]
- 69. Stirling J, Hellewell J, Blakey A, Deakin W. Thought disorder in schizophrenia is associated with both executive dysfunction and circumscribed impairments in semantic function. Psychol Med. 2006;36:475–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Fritz MS, MacKinnon DP. Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychol Sci. 2007;18:233–239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Bentler PM, Chou CP. Practical issues in structural modeling. Sociol Methods Res. 1987;16:78–117. [Google Scholar]
- 72. Raubenheimer J. An item selection procedure to maximize scale reliability and validity. SA J Ind Psychol. 2004;30:59–64. [Google Scholar]
- 73. Iacobucci D. Structural equations modeling: fit indices, sample size, and advanced topics. J Consum Psychol. 2010;20:90–98. [Google Scholar]