Abstract
Lrfn2/SALM1 is a PSD-95-interacting synapse adhesion molecule, and human LRFN2 is associated with learning disabilities. However its role in higher brain function and underlying mechanisms remain unknown. Here, we show that Lrfn2 knockout mice exhibit autism-like behavioural abnormalities, including social withdrawal, decreased vocal communications, increased stereotyped activities and prepulse inhibition deficits, together with enhanced learning and memory. In the hippocampus, the levels of synaptic PSD-95 and GluA1 are decreased. The synapses are structurally and functionally immature with spindle shaped spines, smaller postsynaptic densities, reduced AMPA/NMDA ratio, and enhanced LTP. In vitro experiments reveal that synaptic surface expression of AMPAR depends on the direct interaction between Lrfn2 and PSD-95. Furthermore, we detect functionally defective LRFN2 missense mutations in autism and schizophrenia patients. Together, these findings indicate that Lrfn2/LRFN2 serve as core components of excitatory synapse maturation and maintenance, and their dysfunction causes immature/silent synapses with pathophysiological state.
Lrfn2/SALM1 is a synaptic adhesion molecule, and is known to interact with PSD-95. Here the authors show that Lrfn2 regulates excitatory synapse maturation and maintenance, and that Lrfn2 knockout mice exhibit autism-like behaviours as well as enhanced learning and memory.
Synapse dysfunction is associated with the pathophysiology of various neurodevelopmental disorders such as autism, intellectual disability and schizophrenia1,2. Genetic studies suggest that the components of excitatory synapses are involved in the pathogenesis2,3. Mutations in NRXN1, NLGN3/4 and SHANK1/2/3 alter synaptic function and lead to autism spectrum disorders (ASD), intellectual disability or schizophrenia2,4,5,6,7. Neurexins (NRXN) and Neuroligins (NLGN) are presynaptic and postsynaptic synaptic adhesion proteins, respectively6. Shanks are scaffold proteins in the postsynaptic density (PSD)4.
NLGN and Shanks are linked by PSD-95, an abundant PDZ domain–containing scaffold protein8,9. PSD-95 is a critical postsynaptic regulator that facilitates the formation of the PSD and has multiple protein–protein interaction domains to organize a molecular network linking extracellular to intracellular components. The PDZ domains of PSD-95 associate with the PDZ-binding motif located at the carboxy-termini of transmembrane molecules such as the glutamate receptors10,11, NLGN and other adhesion molecules8,9. Recruitment of PSD-95 and other PSD-95-interacting membrane molecules to the synapse has been proposed to lead to trafficking and clustering of AMPA receptors (AMPAR) and NMDA receptors (NMDAR), consequently affecting synaptic morphology, function and plasticity12,13. Mice with genetic deletion of PSD-95 show enhanced long-term potentiation (LTP) and more silent synapse population by the dampened AMPA function, as well as longer spines and impaired learning14,15.
Although many postsynaptic adhesion molecules can physically interact with PSD-95 in vitro, the major regulator of PSD-95 in vivo is unclear. For example, Neuroligin1/2/3 triple-knockout (KO) mice show normal synaptogenesis and synaptic morphology of hippocampal and neocortical neurons16. Furthermore, to our knowledge, none of the other PDZ-binding postsynaptic synaptic adhesion proteins, including Neuroligin4 (ref. 17), Lrrtm2 (ref. 18), NGL3 (ref. 19) and SynCAM1 (ref. 20), have been shown to alter the amount or distribution of PSD-95 in vivo. It is essential to clarify the postsynaptic protein that regulates PSD-95 in physiological contexts, given its importance in AMPAR regulation.
Lrfn2 (also known as SALM1) is a neuronal single-pass transmembrane protein that physically interacts with PSD-95 through a C-terminal PDZ-binding motif21,22,23. In human, LRFN2 is associated with autism24, learning disabilities25, and antisocial personality disorder26. Lrfn2 overexpression promotes neurite outgrowth27, recruits NMDAR, and enhances the surface expression of transfected GluN2A (NR2A, a glutamate receptor subunit)23. Lrfn2 can bind the NMDA receptor GluN1 (NR1) subunit through their extracellular or transmembrane domains23. Although previous studies addressed the molecular function of Lrfn2, its physiological role at the synapse is little understood, presumably because no loss-of-function analysis has been performed in vivo.
Here, we show that Lrfn2 is a major regulator of PSD-95 in hippocampal neurons and is required for excitatory synapse maturation. Lrfn2 KO mice showed ASD-like behaviours with enhanced cognitive function. We also carried out resequencing analysis of LRFN2 in autism and schizophrenia patients and identified functionally impaired missense mutations. These results suggest that Lrfn2 is an integral component of the excitatory postsynaptic assembly, and defects in it could lead to developmental psychiatric illnesses.
Results
Characterization and expression pattern of Lrfn proteins
A previous study detected Lrfn mRNAs in pyramidal neurons in the cerebral cortex and hippocampus of mouse brain22. In the present work, we assessed the developmental expression profiles of Lrfn proteins using subtype-specific antibodies (Supplementary Fig. 1a). In whole-brain lysates (Fig. 1a), Lrfn1/SALM2 and Lrfn3/SALM4 were detected from embryonic day 14 to 56 and peaked between postnatal day 4 and 14. Lrfn2/SALM1 and Lrfn5/SALM5 proteins increased in the first postnatal week. Lrfn5 levels decreased after postnatal day 28, whereas Lrfn2 levels were maintained. The majority of Lrfn4/SALM3 expression was observed during embryonic stages, and it became undetectable after birth. Lrfn2 expression increased in the postnatal cerebral cortex and hippocampus, (Fig. 1b), and Lrfn2 was the only Lrfn family member that prominently increased in postnatal hippocampus.
In adulthood, Lrfn2 protein was abundant in the cerebral cortex and hippocampus (Fig. 1c). Mouse Lrfn2 protein was detected in the synaptosomal plasma membrane and PSD fractions (Supplementary Fig. 1b), similar to the distribution of rat Lrfn2 (ref. 23). Epitope tagged mammalian Lrfn2 proteins localize to both dendrites and axons and can be detected in postsynapse, overlapping with PSD-95 (Supplementary Fig. 1c; ref. 23). In an immunoprecipitation experiment using the mouse brain synaptosomal fraction, Lrfn2 co-precipitated not only with PSD-95 (Supplementary Fig. 1d; ref. 23), but also with the GluA1 and GluA2 subunits of AMPAR and the GluN2A and GluN2B subunits of NMDAR (Supplementary Fig. 1d).
Lrfn2 KO mice exhibit social withdrawal
To understand the role of Lrfn2, we generated Lrfn2 KO mice (Lrfn2−/−). In the Lrfn2 null mutation allele (Lrfn2−), Lrfn2 exon 2, which encodes most of the extracellular region from the amino-terminus to the middle of the FnIII domain, was replaced with a single loxP sequence (Supplementary Fig. 2a–c), and the absence of Lrfn2 protein was confirmed by immunoblotting (Supplementary Fig. 2d). Lrfn2 KO was not lethal (Lrfn2+/+, 25.9%; Lrfn2+/−, 46.0%; Lrfn2−/−, 28.1% in 913 3- to 8-week-old progenies from interheterozygote mating; no significant differences from the expected mendelian ratio in χ2 test), and Lrfn2 KO mice were fertile and displayed no gross defects in brain anatomy (Supplementary Fig. 2e).
However, Lrfn2 KO mice showed mild (6–9%) body weight loss in a conventional group housing environment of 4–5 mice per cage (mean body weight±s.e.m. of 8-week-old male wild types (WT), 24.1±0.43 g; male KO, 22.7±0.39 g; female WT, 19.7±0.25 g; female KO, 17.9±0.24 g; n=10–19; P=0.02 [male] and P=0.02 [female] by t-test). The body weight difference started 6 weeks postnatally and became obvious at 7–10 weeks (Fig. 2a left). In contrast, when the mice were reared in an isolated housing condition from 3 weeks of age, there were no obvious differences in body weight between WT and Lrfn2 KO (Fig. 2a right). We speculated that juvenile Lrfn2 KO mice have altered behavioural responses to sustained social stimuli.
To investigate the social behaviour, we conducted the reciprocal social interaction test (Fig. 2b–d)28,29. In three trials carried out on 3 consecutive days, Lrfn2 KO mice showed remarkably reduced social investigation such as approach and sniffing (Fig. 2c), and less physical contact with a DBA2 male mouse (Fig. 2d). Interestingly, Lrfn2 KO mice frequently hid themselves under the bedding material instead of social investigation (Fig. 2b and Supplementary Movies 1 and 2). This withdrawn behaviour from the social peer was notably enhanced day by day (Fig. 2c). In addition, social avoidance behaviour that includes freezing, escaping and hiding was much more time in Lrfn2 KO mice than WT mice (insets of Fig. 2c). Together, these observations revealed that Lrfn2 KO mice show a marked tendency to social withdrawal and avoidance.
We next performed a 3-chambered social approach test as a measure of one-way social activity30. WT mice showed normal sociability with a significant preference for a caged stranger mouse (DBA2 male), whereas Lrfn2 KO mice did not show a significant preference in terms of either time spent in each chamber or number of contacts with the stranger's cage (Fig. 2e).
Having observed the social behaviour abnormalities, we checked the ultrasonic vocalizations (USV) of nursing infants and mating adults. Although there was no significant difference in the properties of isolation-induced USV of nursing infants (Supplementary Fig. 3a), mature Lrfn2 KO males encountering oestrous females emitted fewer USV than WT males (Fig. 2f). Thus, communication skills are impaired in the Lrfn2 KO, consistent with the tendency for social withdrawal.
We also checked if some physical or mental changes associate with the Lrfn2 social phenotype. As a result, there were no abnormalities in tests of olfaction (Supplementary Fig. 3b), locomotion (Supplementary Fig. 3c,e), spontaneous motor activities in home cages (Supplementary Fig. 3d) and motor coordination on an accelerating rotarod (Supplementary Fig. 3f), or in conventional anxiety-related behavioural tasks such as light/dark box test (Supplementary Fig. 3g), elevated plus-maze test (Supplementary Fig. 3h), and open field test (the time spent in the centre area, Supplementary Fig. 3c).
Lrfn2 KO mice exhibit repetitive behaviours and impaired PPI
To determine whether the withdrawn Lrfn2 KO mice also avoid inanimate or not, we performed marble burying assay, which are deemed to be rodent-specific repetitive and perseverative behaviours31,32. Lrfn2 KO mice buried significantly more marbles than WT mice during the observation period (Fig. 3a). Because digging behaviour observed during the reciprocal social interaction test did not depend on genotype (Fig. 2c), the increased burying behaviour in KO mice indicates they had higher sensitivity to the marbles than did WT mice.
Similar tendency of Lrfn2 KO was observed in wheel-running, which is considered a spontaneous and pleasurable activity with highly repetitive and stereotyped behaviour33. Compared to WT mice, Lrfn2 KO mice exhibited excess repetitive running (Fig. 3b), and this hyperactivity was constantly observed over a week (Fig. 3c).
Lrfn2 KO mice also exhibited an enhanced startle response to an acoustic stimulus (120 dB; Fig. 3d), and prepulse inhibition (PPI) of the auditory startle response was decreased in KO mice (Fig. 3e). The results indicated that Lrfn2 KO mice have defective sensory information processing.
Enhanced spatial learning and fear memory in Lrfn2 KO mice
Intelligence quotient varies widely in ASD patients; it may be normal or above average34. We noticed enhanced memory in Lrfn2 KO mice. In the classical fear conditioning test, the mice received two electrical footshock-tone pairings. Lrfn2 KO mice showed robust freezing behaviour associated with contextual fear 1 h and 1 day after training, but a level comparable to that of WT mice in cue test that was carried out 2 days after training (Fig. 3f and Supplementary Fig. 3i). This led us to examine the duration of the enhanced contextual fear memory retention by measuring the freezing responses in the training box and in a differently textured novel box 3, 7 and 28 days after the training. Lrfn2 KO mice froze numerically (but not significantly) more often than WT mice in the training box until day 7 but not on day 28 (Fig. 3g). When we compared the freezing responses in the two boxes, KO mice tended to discriminate the two boxes until day 7 (Fig. 3g and Supplementary Fig. 3i). By contrast, WT mice failed to discriminate them even at 3 days. There were no differences in basal or footshock-induced movement velocity (Supplementary Fig. 3i), indicating the intact nociception and motor function of Lrfn2 KO mice. Taken together, these results indicate that fear memory is enhanced in Lrfn2 KO mice.
Improved memory was also indicated by performance on the Morris water maze test. We carried out 2 days of massed learning consisting of four blocks of two training trials and a probe test that was carried out 1 day after the training trials (Fig. 3h,i). In the second probe test (Probe 2) after day 2 of training, Lrfn2 KO mice reached the hidden platform faster than WT mice (Fig. 3j), although the time spent in the target quadrant did not differ between the genotypes (Fig. 3k). There were no differences in visual recognition, motor skills or motivation, as indicated by control experiments with visual platforms (Supplementary Fig. 3j–l). Thus, spatial learning performance is increased in Lrfn2 KO mice.
Altered excitatory postsynaptic proteins in Lrfn2 KO mice
Because both spatial learning and contextual fear memory retention depend on hippocampal function35,36, we next addressed the hippocampal phenotype in Lrfn2 KO.
First, we used immunoblotting to examine the amounts of synaptic proteins in the hippocampus from 8-week-old mice, including GluA1, GluA2, GluN1, GluN2A, GluN2B, PSD-95, a presynaptic protein (synaptophysin) and other Lrfn proteins (Fig. 4a), and found that PSD-95 was significantly decreased and GluN2A was significantly increased. Immunofluorescence staining of the synapse markers showed lower PSD-95 in the CA1, CA3, and dentate gyrus regions of the KO (Fig. 4b,c) and no significant differences between the groups in synaptophysin, GluA1, GluA2 and GluN1 in these regions (Supplementary Fig. 4a–d). In the dorsal CA1 radial layer, both the intensity and size of the PSD-95–positive puncta were decreased by 20% (Fig. 4d,e), indicating that the absence of Lrfn2 resulted in reduced synaptic accumulation of PSD-95. In addition, the number of GluN2A puncta was comparable between WT and KO (Supplementary Fig. 4g), but the number of GluN2A puncta overlapping with PSD-95 signals was decreased (Supplementary Fig. 4h). Although the altered PSD-95 and GluN2A protein levels or localization, their mRNA levels were comparable between WT and KO (Supplementary Fig. 4i). These results raised the possibility that the loss of Lrfn2 causes abnormal structure and impaired function of excitatory synapses, because PSD-95 is an essential constituent of excitatory PSDs.
By contrast to the altered postsynaptic constituents, there were no significant differences in the intensity, size or number of Vglut1 (excitatory presynapse marker) or Vgat (inhibitory presynapse marker)-derived punctate immunopositive signals between WT and KO hippocampal CA1 radial layer (Supplementary Fig. 4e,f). The results indicated that the gross synapse numbers were comparable between WT and KO.
Abnormal spine and synapse shapes in Lrfn2 KO mice
Next we investigated the morphology of the spines on the apical dendrites of dorsal CA1 neurons. Golgi staining revealed that the shapes of spines in Lrfn2 KO mice were clearly distinguishable from those in WT mice (Fig. 5a). When the total spine-like protrusions were counted, Lrfn2 KO mice had a slightly but significantly lower number than WT mice (Fig. 5b). In contrast, we noticed a dramatic increase in the number of oddly shaped spines (Fig. 5c) with thin protrusions from the spine heads (arrowheads in Fig. 5a), reminiscent of ‘spinules'37. Morphometrical analysis of each neuron (Fig. 5d) confirmed that the mean length of spines was longer and the mean head width of spines was smaller in Lrfn2 KO mice than in WT mice (Fig. 5e).
In parallel, we assessed the ultrastructure of the synapses by electron microscopy (EM) of the CA1 radial layer of the dorsal hippocampus (Fig. 5f). The ratio of perforated synapses to total asymmetrical synapses increased (P<0.01; t-test; Fig. 5g). We further quantified the morphology of the simple PSD synapses. The population of synapses in Lrfn2 KO mice was shifted towards shorter PSD length (P<0.05; Kolomogorov–Smirnov [K–S] test; Fig. 5h), but PSD thickness was not changed compared to WT synapses (Fig. 5i). A cumulative plot of the width of the synaptic cleft showed that clefts in Lrfn2 KO mice were shifted towards the wider direction (P<0.001; K–S test; Fig. 5j). Because the size of PSD is thought to correlate with the size of the spine head, the results of the EM analysis seemed to be consistent with the thin spines shown in the Golgi staining. Furthermore, the frequent appearance of oddly shaped, spinule-like spines and perforated PSD synapses implied an unstable spine structure, although no clear abnormalities were detected at the presynaptic terminal (Fig. 5f). Taken together, these results demonstrate that Lrfn2 plays an important role in maintaining normal spine and synapse structures in vivo.
We also investigated dendritic length and dendritic arborization in cultured hippocampal neurons stained with MAP2 after 10 days of culture (DIV10). Tracings of MAP2-immunoreactive dendrites (Fig. 5k) revealed that loss of Lrfn2 increased total dendritic length (Fig. 5l) and branching complexity as indicated by a Sholl analysis (Fig. 5m).
Enhanced LTP and increased silent synapses in Lrfn2 KO mice
Hippocampal synaptic plasticity is important for contextual memory and spatial learning38,39. Spine morphology is believed to reflect molecular composition, synaptic function and plasticity40,41,42. We next addressed whether the Lrfn2 KO hippocampus shows electrophysiological changes in synaptic plasticity such as LTP. LTP of field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSP) at Schaffer collateral/CA1 synapses was significantly enhanced in Lrfn2 KO mice compared to WT littermates (WT, 160.5%±10.9%; KO, 204.3%±9.9% of baseline at 60 min after tetanus; P<0.01; t-test; Fig. 6a,b). No significant differences were observed for either the paired-pulse (Supplementary Fig. 5a) or post-tetanic potentiation ratio (Supplementary Fig. 5b) between the two genotypes, suggesting that the probability of transmitter release in Lrfn2 KO mice was normal. To further examine the postsynaptic alterations, we assessed the ratio of AMPAR- to NMDAR-mediated excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) of CA1 pyramidal neurons. Lrfn2 KO mice showed significantly lower AMPAR/NMDAR ratios than WT mice (WT, 3.74±0.51; KO, 2.42±0.26; P<0.05; t-test; Fig. 6c).
When we evaluated spontaneous miniature EPSCs (mEPSCs), the mEPSC frequency was strongly reduced in Lrfn2 KO hippocampus (WT, 1.75±0.10 Hz; KO, 0.89±0.13 Hz; P<0.001; t-test; Fig. 6d). Together with the decreased AMPAR/NMDAR ratios and intact excitatory synapse numbers, we hypothesized that a part of the excitatory synapses may have become non-functional at normal resting potential (silent synapse) in Lrfn2 KO hippocampus. To test this idea, we carried out minimal stimulation experiments and assessed silent synapses, the ones that have only NMDARs43,44. Minimal stimulation evoked AMPAR-EPSCs at −70 mV and NMDAR- (plus AMPAR-) EPSCs at +40 mV with failures in both genotypes (Fig. 6e), however, the failure rate at +40 mV was significantly lower than that at −70 mV only in KO mice (WT, 0.43±0.05 at −70 mV, 0.37±0.07 at +40 mV; KO, 0.54±0.05 at −70 mV, 0.33±0.05 at +40 mV; P<0.01; Fig. 6f). We also calculated the percentage of the silent synapses by using a formula reported previously43 (see extended experimental procedures). As a result, the silent synapse was markedly increased in Lrfn2 KO hippocampus (WT, 23.2±6.7%; KO, 50.0±6.8%; P<0.01; t-test; Fig. 6f).
Trafficking of AMPAR at postsynaptic sites is involved in LTP45. We therefore quantified the amount of hippocampal GluA1, GluA2 and GluN1 proteins in the PSD-enriched fraction (Triton X-100 insoluble) by immunoblot analysis. The GluA1 content was significantly reduced in Lrfn2 KO hippocampus without affecting the GluN1 content (Fig. 7 and Supplementary Table 1), whereas the content in the total cell lysate was not reduced. This finding indicates that an Lrfn2 deficit results in lower basal synaptic expression of GluA1-containing AMPAR. It seems that Lrfn2-deficient spines provide potential space for the addition of GluA1-containing AMPAR in the postsynaptic membrane during LTP.
Lrfn2 enhances synaptic AMPAR incorporation with PSD-95
To gain more insight into the effects of Lrfn2 on AMPAR dynamics, we analysed surface expression of AMPAR and AMPAR-mediated currents in rat hippocampal neurons transfected with Myc-tagged Lrfn2; Myc-tagged Lrfn2ΔC, which cannot bind PSD-95 due to the deletion of two C-terminal amino acids in the PDZ-binding domain; and Flag-tagged PSD-95 (ref. 22).
We first assessed plasma membrane expression of GluA1 and GluA2 in the transfected neurons with a surface biotinylation assay (Fig. 8a). Immunoblots for the biotinylated surface fractions showed that overexpression of Lrfn2 increased the surface expression of not only of endogenous GluN1 but also of GluA1 and GluA2, compared to that of the empty vector. In contrast, the transfected Lrfn2ΔC, which was excluded from the surface, did not elicit any changes in the receptors' surface expression. These results suggest that the PDZ-binding domain is required for the surface expression of both Lrfn2 and glutamate receptors. Overexpression of PSD-95 increases surface expression of AMPAR, leading to neuronal excitability in cultured hippocampal neurons46. Concordant with this, we observed increased surface expression of receptor subunits in our PSD-95–overexpressing neurons. Furthermore, co-expression of Lrfn2 with PSD-95 resulted in more membrane targeting of Lrfn2 and the glutamate receptors than did expression of Lrfn2 or PSD-95 alone. Co-expression of Lrfn2ΔC with PSD-95 increased surface expression of Lrfn2ΔC, but it selectively reduced the surface expression of GluA1 and GluA2 (but not GluN1) to the level of the control vector, indicating that the PDZ-binding domain mediates the membrane targeting and stability of AMPAR through PSD-95. In addition, Lrfn2ΔC seemed to preferentially interact with GluN1, excluding endogenous Lrfn2; this is consistent with the idea that Lrfn2 helps NMDAR trafficking due to the direct physical interaction between Lrfn2 and GluN1 via its extracellular or transmembrane domain23.
We also recorded miniature excitatory postsynaptic currents (mEPSCs) from the transfected hippocampal neurons (Fig. 8b–d). Neither Lrfn2- nor Lrfn2ΔC-overexpressing neurons showed a statistical difference in the averaged mEPSC frequency and amplitude compared with the control vector. PSD-95 transfection significantly enhanced the amplitude and frequency of mEPSCs, consistent with a previous report46. Furthermore, the transfection of Lrfn2 with PSD-95 resulted in an additional increase in both frequency and amplitude of mEPSCs compared to PSD-95 alone, indicating that the additional AMPAR targeted to synapses by exogenous Lrfn2 and PSD-95 are functional. In contrast, co-transfection of Lrfn2ΔC and PSD-95 resulted in mEPSCs comparable to those elicited by the control vector, even though PSD-95 was overproduced. This result indicates that interaction with the PDZ-binding domain is a critical process for the synaptic targeting of AMPAR, and Lrfn2ΔC seems to have a dominant-negative effect on endogenous Lrfn2. Taken together, these findings suggest that Lrfn2 functions at synapses together with PSD-95 and serves to target and stabilize AMPAR at postsynaptic sites.
Functionally defective LRFN2 mutations in patients
Alterations in glutamatergic synapse structure and function represent a common underlying pathology in intellectual disability, ASD, schizophrenia2. The ASD-like behaviours of Lrfn2 KO mice led us to conduct a pilot survey of mutations in human LRFN2. LRFN2 protein-coding exons and flanking introns were sequenced in DNA samples from 165 individuals with autism and 910 patients with schizophrenia and 1,060 mentally healthy controls. Patients with schizophrenia were included because of the partial genetic commonality in the etiologies of ASD and schizophrenia46,47 and the presence in Lrfn2 KO mice of cognitive dysfunction typically found in mouse models of schizophrenia, such as the PPI deficit47. We detected several novel nonsynonymous single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs; Fig. 9). When we analysed the positions of the nonsynonymous SNPs identified in the current study and the known SNPs, we found two SNP clusters, one in the leucine-rich repeat C-terminal domain and the other in the FnIII domain (Fig. 9a). Eight out of the 12 nonsynonymous SNPs were located in positions highly conserved among vertebrates (Supplementary Fig. 6). The minor allele frequencies varied from 0.00047 to 0.0090, and the most common SNP was D770N, located near the C-terminus.
The allele frequencies of most of the nonsynonymous SNPs were not significantly different from those of healthy control subjects. However, R274H was found only in the patients with autism (allelic P=0.018 by Fisher's exact test; Fig. 9b). Although R274H was identified in nonsymptomatic consanguineous family members (Supplementary Fig. 7a), it may be worth considering its involvement in ASD pathogenesis in light of the strong evolutionary conservation of R274 (Supplementary Fig. 6). In addition, several other SNPs across the gene displayed nominally significant transmission distortion in a Chinese cohort (Supplementary Table 2). As a rare variant, we found E462D only in individuals with schizophrenia (Fig. 9b), which also occurs in an evolutionarily conserved position (Supplementary Fig. 6). The amount of LRFN2 transcript in the postmortem brains of individuals with schizophrenia was comparable to that of control subjects (Supplementary Fig. 7b).
When we quantified PSD-95 co-localized on LRFN2 R274H, A337T, E462D or WT-transfected hippocampal neurons by immunofluorescence staining, the signals on the R274H and E462D transfectants were significantly lower than that of WT, and the differences were recapitulated in mouse Lrfn2 R274H and E462D equivalent mutants (Fig. 9c and Supplementary Fig. 8d,e). In immunoprecipitation assay, both Lrfn2 R274H and Lrfn2 E462D co-precipitated less PSD-95 than Lrfn2 WT (Fig. 9d). We also noticed that the dense signals on cell body were more frequent in E462D transfectants (Supplementary Fig. 8a,b), which may be caused by trafficking abnormality (Supplementary Fig. 8c). These results indicated the presence of R274H and E462D impaired the molecular function of LRFN2.
Discussion
LTP was enhanced in CA1 pyramidal neurons in Lrfn2 KO hippocampal slices. LTP induction involves the passage of calcium through NMDAR following activation of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinases II and the resultant recruitment and insertion of AMPAR into the postsynaptic membrane48. Overexpression of Lrfn2 and PSD-95, an Lrfn2-binding protein, resulted in augmentation of AMPAR-EPSCs and increased surface expression of AMPAR compared to the excitability generated by PSD-95 alone (Fig. 8). Because overexpression of Lrfn2 alone did not cause any apparent changes, regulation of synaptic efficacy by Lrfn2 may depend on the expression level of PSD-95. On the other hand, the combination of PSD-95-binding–deficient Lrfn2ΔC and PSD-95 selectively inhibited AMPAR current/surface expression. Therefore, the Lrfn2–PSD-95 association is essential for PSD-95 to exert its role as a regulator of AMPAR dynamics. The role of Lrfn2 in this regard seems to fit with the concept of a ‘slot protein'49,50, which serves as the receptor binding site and determines the number of surface AMPAR.
The analysis of Lrfn2 KO mice supported the presumptive role of Lrfn2 in the recruitment and subsequent stabilization of AMPAR at synaptic sites. First, we found a reduction of the AMPAR/NMDAR ratio (Fig. 6c), increment of silent synapse (Fig. 6e,f) and less GluA1 protein in the Triton X-100-insoluble fraction (PSD-enriched fraction) in Lrfn2 KO hippocampus (Fig. 7). Because synapses in Lrfn2 KO mice possess less AMPAR, there is more room for AMPAR to be inserted at postsynaptic sites, and synaptic transmission can be potentiated more48. Second, immunoblot and immunostaining analyses showed a significant reduction of PSD-95 protein levels and the clustering of PSD-95 protein in the radial layer of CA1 pyramidal cells. These two results suggest that the absence of Lrfn2 results in decreased synaptic PSD-95, followed by destabilized retention of AMPAR at the synapse.
Collectively, Lrfn2 may regulate the surface AMPAR fraction through its interaction with PSD-95. We propose that this property of Lrfn2 largely accounts for the enhanced LTP in the KO mice. In agreement with this idea, PSD-95–deficient mice consistently show enhanced LTP14,15,51 and exhibit more synapses that lack AMPAR (increased silent synapses)15.
While the interaction between Lrfn2 and PSD-95 can explain the reduced surface AMPAR and the enhanced synaptic plasticity in Lrfn2 KO mice, increment of GluN2A amount and reduced PSD-95 overlapping GluN2A puncta in Lrfn2 KO hippocampus remains to be clarified. However, at this point, we speculate that following two mechanisms could be involved in the GluN2A abnormalities. First, the reduced PSD-95 in Lrfn2 KO may have altered the GluN2A protein dynamics because GluN2A interacts directly with PSD-95 and PSD-95 increases GluN2A surface expression by regulating insertion and internalization of NMDAR (reviewed in ref. 52). Second, the absence of GluN1-binding activity of Lrfn2 (ref. 25) may have influenced the GluN2A protein dynamics because GluN1 plays crucial role in the release of GluN2A from endoplasmic reticulum53. In either explanation, regulatory mechanism underlying GluN2A re-distribution and metabolism may be needed for better understanding.
Besides the enhanced synapse plasticity in Lrfn2 KO mice, our results shed new light on the roles of Lrfn2 in controlling neuronal morphology. First, the altered spine shape in the Lrfn2 KO mice was similar to that in PSD-95 KO mice15 in that spine length was significantly increased. Furthermore, the role of PSD-95 in spine enlargement and stabilization has also been shown in overexpression46 and RNAi knockdown12 experiments. Therefore, the morphological changes in Lrfn2 KO synapses may partly reflect the reduced PSD-95 protein level. There is a close correlation between spine head size and PSD size54 and between PSD size and AMPAR/NMDAR ratio55. These facts are very consistent with the reduction of spine width, PSD length and AMPAR/NMDAR ratio in Lrfn2 KO.
Second, we saw oddly shaped thin protrusions from spines that seemed different from the filopodia that emerge from the surface of the dendritic shaft. Rather, the protrusions from the oddly shaped spines in Lrfn2 KO mice are similar to so-called synaptic spinules or actin-based protrusions from spine head/SHP (spine head protrusion). On EM, these can be seen to project from the perforation of the PSD or at the edge of the PSD on the spine head and to invaginate into the presynaptic terminal39. In support of this similarity, the number of perforated synapses with split PSD increased in Lrfn2 KO hippocampus (Fig. 5g). Postsynaptic perforations and spinules on spines increase after the high-frequency stimulation that induces LTP56,57, and their presence implies dynamic synaptic remodelling or coordination that depends on synaptic activity. Lrfn2 may maintain the intact spine shape by decreasing the spontaneous motility of spines.
Third, the widening of the synaptic cleft implies that the absence of Lrfn2 results in the weakening of synaptic adhesion in vivo. This defect may reflect the loss of the adhesive function that Lrfn2 provides through its interaction with presumptive presynaptic molecules, as seen in the absence of other synaptic adhesion molecules6,18,19, or it may occur indirectly through the cis-interaction of Lrfn2 with other Lrfn proteins58.
Last, the cultured Lrfn2 KO hippocampal neurons showed increased dendrite length and dendritic arborization. We emphasize again the functional relevance to PSD-95, because PSD-95 knockdown also results in increased primary and secondary dendrite number59. Taken together, we speculate that Lrfn2 regulates neuronal structure at both spines and dendrites in vivo.
Above abnormalities in synaptic function and morphology may underlie the behavioural abnormalities in Lrfn2 KO mice. In our behavioural test battery, Lrfn2 KO mice exhibited social withdrawal, a tendency to perseverate and impaired communication skills, which are all included among the core symptoms of autism, together with sensory dysfunction and a tendency to depression, which are common comorbid symptoms of autism60. These phenotypes are fairly common among known autism model mice30. We therefore consider that the behavioural abnormalities in Lrfn2 KO mice may partly mimic the pathophysiological status of autism patients. Furthermore it is tempting to speculate the relationship between the social withdrawal in Lrfn2 KO and the antisocial personality disorder which is associated with a locus near LRFN2 (ref. 26). While we clarified the defective synapse morphology and function in Lrfn2 KO hippocampus, the neural circuit basis of the defective social behaviour in Lrfn2 KO is still an open question.
On the other hand, the enhanced spatial learning and fear memory seem to be behavioural phenotypes unique to Lrfn2 KO mice. Interestingly, an LRFN2-happloinsufficiency patient showed an enhanced long-term memory with speech delay25, suggesting the common role of the gene in humans. Memory function is also enhanced in a narrowly restricted domain in a subset of ASD patients (‘savant syndrome')61. The enhanced memory function of Lrfn2 KO could reflect the altered cognitive function of ASD patients, although it is premature to conclude this definitively.
Because Lrfn2 protein levels in the hippocampus and cerebral cortex increase during postnatal development, Lrfn2 may be a key component of excitatory synapse maturation in terms of increasing the AMPAR/NMDAR ratio, spine head size and PSD size. The role of Lrfn2/SALM1 in synapse maturation is well contrasted with excitatory synapse number regulation by Lrfn4/SALM3 and Lrfn3/SALM4. Lrfn4/SALM3 KO mice show normal hippocampal CA1 synaptic morphology and plasticity, and normal memory functions whereas the excitatory synapse density is decreased62. SALM4 negatively regulates excitatory synapses via cis inhibition of SALM3 (ref. 63). Thus the Lrfn family proteins cooperatively regulate synapse formation and synapse maturation. Together with the profound cognitive function deficits, we believe that Lrfn2 KO is a model for neurodevelopmental disorders that conforms to the synapse maturation defect as a cause of ASDs. Furthermore, the behavioural abnormalities of the Lrfn2 KO and the functionally impaired LRFN2 missense mutation warrant more extensive genetic analyses of LRFN2 using larger samples and different populations.
Methods
Animals
All animal experiments were approved by the institutional ethical committees and carried out in accordance with the guidelines for animal experimentation in the Nagasaki University Graduate School of Biomedical Sciences, Gunma University Graduate School of Medicine, and RIKEN.
Generation of Lrfn2 KO mice
A BAC clone containing Lrfn2 was purchased from BACPAC resources of the Children's Hospital Oakland Research Institute. An Lrfn2 targeting vector was constructed to replace exon 2, which contains the ATG translation initiation codon, with a neomycin resistance gene cassette flanked by a loxP sequence (Neo). Homologous genomic DNA with the Neo cassette was joined with a diphtheria toxin—a cassette for negative selection. Linearized targeting vectors were electroporated into E14 ES cells, and homologous recombinants were isolated by G418 selection. The ES clones were screened by Southern blot analysis. Correctly targeted ES clones were injected into blastocysts of C57BL/6J mice, which were then used to produce chimeric mice. After confirmation of germline transmission, the Neo cassette was removed by crossing mice that had germline transmission with transgenic mice expressing Cre recombinase in germ cells64. The Cre recombination was confirmed by PCR and Southern blot analyses. Lrfn2-heterozygous (Lrfn2+/−) mice were backcrossed into the C57BL/6J inbred background for at least six generations before the behavioural experiments were started. Mutant animals were genotyped by PCR using DNA and the following primers: forward primer F1 for WT and KO allele (5′-CACATGGTGCGTGCAATTTAGG-3′); Reverse primers R1 for WT allele (5′-GGCAATTGCCCTTATCAAGAGC-3′) and R2 for KO allele (5′-TTGAGTAAGAGCAAGAACCCAGC-3′). Alternatively, the mutant animals were genotypes by Southern blot analysis after BglII and EcoRI digestion of genomic DNA with 5′ and 3′ probes.
Antibodies
The following antibodies were used: GluA1 (for immunoblot, 1:400, PC246, Calbiochem; for immunostaining, 1:200, SC-13152, Santa Cruz), GluA2 (1:500 for immunoblot, 1:200 for immunostaining, MAB397, Chemicon), GluN1 (1:2,000 for immunoblot, 1:200 for immunostaining, 114011, Synaptic Systems), GluN2A (for immunoblot, 1:1,000, 1500-NR2A, PhosphoSolutions; for immunostaining, 1:300, AB_2571605, Frontier Institute), GluN2B (1:200, 610416, BD Biosciences), PSD-95 (for immunoblot, 1:1,000, 05-494, Upstate or 1:2,000, 75-028, NeuroMab; for immunostaining, 1:400, 75-028, NeuroMab), Synaptophysin (1:4,000 for immunoblot, 1:200 for immunostaining, S-5768, Sigma), Synapsin I (1:500, AB1543P, Chemicon), Vglut1 (1:300, 135302, Synaptic Systems), Vgat (1:500, 131003, Synaptic Systems), TGN38 (1:500, 2F7.1, 804095, Alexis), tubulin (1:1,000, G7121, Promega), actin (1:1,500, A2066, Sigma), GFP (1:1,000, 598, MBL), Myc (1:1,000, 9E10, SC-40, Santa Cruz), Flag (1:2,000, M2, F3168, Sigma), HA (1:500, 12CA5, 11583816001, Roche; 1:500, 3F10, 11867423001, Roche). Rabbit polyclonal antibodies to Lrfn family proteins were raised against peptides as follows: Lrfn2N, APPLSFSFGGNPLHC; Lrfn2C (-ESTV), CARGTFGSSEWVM; Lrfn2C, CEESDLVGARGTFGSSEWVMESTV; Lrfn1, CGDLGLGSARARLAFTSTEWML; Lrfn3, CEPWGPSHEPAGP; Lrfn4, CRGVGGSAERLE; Lrfn5, CTNVDQNVQETQRLESI. Antisera were affinity purified on SulfoLink columns (Pierce) that were coupled with antigen peptides following the instructions of the manufacturer.
DNA constructs
Mouse Lrfn proteins and PSD-95 expression vectors were previously described24. The wild-type LRFN2 expression vector was generated by inserting human LRFN2 protein-coding regions PCR amplified from the human brain cDNA (Clontech) and cloned into the pGEMT easy plasmid vector (Promega). PCR-based site-directed mutagenesis was carried out using appropriate primers for R274H, A337T and E462D. The cloned fragments were sequenced and subsequently PCR modified to have a BglII site just downstream of the predicted signal peptide cleavage and a NotI site just downstream of the termination codon. The generated fragments were inserted into N-terminally epitope-tagged pSecTag2 vectors24.
Biochemical analysis
For the protein preparation from mouse brains (Fig. 1), whole or parts of brains were homogenized in RIPA buffer (50 mM Tris-HCl [pH 7.5], 150 mM NaCl, 1% NP-40, 0.5% SOD, 0.1% SDS, 10 mM EDTA) supplemented with complete protease inhibitor cocktail (Roche), and cell debris was removed by centrifugation at 12,000g at 4 °C for 30 min. Proteins for the antibody checking, subcellular fractionation analysis and immunoprecipitation analysis (Supplementary Fig. 1) were prepared as described65. For quantification of hippocampal proteins (Fig. 4a), hippocampi from 8-week-old WT mice (n=8) and Lrfn2 KO mice (n=9) were individually collected, followed by homogenization and centrifugation (1,000g, 10 min) to obtain the S1 fraction. SDS was added to a portion of the S1 fraction to a final concentration of 1%, and the solution was then agitated for 1 h at 4 °C and sonicated. For preparation of the Triton X-100-insoluble fraction (Fig. 7), P2 crude synaptosomal fractions were pelleted by centrifugation (10,000g, 30 min) of the remaining S1, which was then dissolved in an equal volume of TET buffer (1% Triton X-100, 2 mM EDTA, 20 mM Tris-HCl, pH 7.5) supplemented with the protease inhibitor cocktail and agitated for 1 h at 4 °C. After centrifugation at 100,000g for 1 h, the pellet was resuspended in 0.1 vol (of the original S1) of 1% SDS, 2 mM EDTA, and 20 mM Tris-HCl, pH 7.5; 0.9 volume of TET buffer was added, and the solution was agitated for 1 h at 4 °C.
For immunoblot analysis, equal amounts of protein extracts were separated by SDS-PAGE and transferred onto PVDF Immobilon-P membranes (Millipore). The membranes were blocked with 5% skim milk in TBST (20 mM Tris-HCl [pH 7.5], 150 mM NaCl, 0.1% Tween-20) and probed with specific antibodies, followed by peroxidase-conjugated secondary antibodies. Signals were developed using the ECL Plus reagents (GE Healthcare) and exposed to Hyperfilm ECL (GE Healthcare). Densitometric analysis of bands was performed using ImageJ software (http://rsbweb.nih.gov/ij/). Total net signal (total signal—background) for each animal was normalized against the corresponding signal for actin or tubulin, and the average signal was calculated for each genotype.
For immunoprecipitation, the synaptosomal fraction (250 μg) from a mouse brain without the cerebellum was incubated in RIPA buffer for 1 h at 4 °C. The lysates were obtained by centrifugation for 20 min at 12,000g and precleaned with protein A-agarose, then incubated with anti-Lrfn2 antibody or normal rabbit IgG of the same quantity overnight at 4 °C. After incubation with protein A-agarose, the agarose beads were precipitated and washed with RIPA buffer five times, and then subjected to the immunoblot analysis.
For mRNA level analysis, RNA was isolated from Lrfn2 WT and KO hippocampus with Trizol Reagent (Thermo Fisher). cDNA was synthesized with SuperScript II Reverse Transcriptase (Thermo Fisher). Realtime RT-PCR analysis was carried out using Power SYBR Green PCR Master Mix (Thermo Fisher), ABI PRISM 7900HT (Thermo Fisher), and following primers (GluN2A, 5′-ATTCAACCAGAGGGGCGTA-3′ and 5′-TTCAAGACAGCTGCGTCATAG-3′; GluN2B, 5′-CCCTAGGCATGTGACTTGAAA-3′ and 5′-CACACACACACGCACACG-3′; PSD-95, 5′-CACCCTATCGCCATCTTCA-3′ and 5′-GCTCCTCTGTGATCCGCTTA-3′; Lrfn2, 5′-GGGTCGCTACTTTTGGCATA-3′ and 5′-GCAGCTTGTGTGTGTGCTG-3′).
Behavioural testing
Adult Lrfn2 KO and WT mice (2- to 6-month-old male littermates from mated heterozygotes) were used for behavioural tests unless otherwise noted. All mice were male, 2–3 months of age at the start of behavioural testing, and were tested beginning with less stressful behavioural tasks. Mice were housed on a 12:12 h light:dark cycle, with the dark cycle occurring from 20:00 to 8:00; behavioural experiments were carried out between 10:00 and 17:00.
Classical fear conditioning
A fear conditioning test was conducted for 3 consecutive days. This test consisted of three parts, a conditioning trial (Day 1), a context test trial (Day 2), and a cued test trial (Day 3). For fear conditioning on Day 1, mice were placed in a novel rectangular chamber with a grid floor (34 × 26 × 30 cm). After a 2-min baseline period, 2 tone-shock pairings were presented. Each pairing consisted of a 20-s, 85-dB, 2,800-Hz tone ending simultaneously with a 2-s, 0.75-mA shock. There was a 1-min interval between each pairing. The mice remained in the chamber for 1 min after the second shock before being returned to the home cage. The total duration of training was 500 s. A context test was performed in the same conditioning chamber for 3 min in the absence of the white noise 24 h after the conditioning trial. Further, a cued test was performed in an alternative context with distinct cues; the test chamber was different from the conditioning chamber in terms of colour, floor structure (no grid), and shape (triangular). The cued test was conducted 24 h after the contextual test was finished and consisted of a 2-min exploration period to evaluate nonspecific contextual fear followed by a 2-min conditioned stimulus (tone without foot shock) to evaluate the acquired cued fear. The percentage of the freezing response (immobility excluding respiration and heartbeat) duration in each time bin was measured as an index of fear memory. Data were collected and analysed using Image J FZ2 (O'Hara, Tokyo, Japan).
Morris water maze
The water maze apparatus was composed of a circular pool (1.0-m diameter) filled with opaque water (25 °C) and an escape platform (10-cm diameter), which was submerged 1 cm below the water surface. The training protocol consisted of 2 days of four blocks (two training trials per block). The swim patterns were monitored by computer-linked overhead video camera, and the escape latency to the platform was recorded. We carried out two probe tests. The first test was done at the end of the third session and the second at the end of the last session. During the probe test, the platform was removed and the mice were allowed to swim in the pool for 60 s. The time spent in each quadrant was recorded. The movements of mice in the maze was recorded and analysed with Image J WM (O'Hara).
Open field locomotion
Spontaneous activity in the open field was tested in an open field apparatus (50 × 50 × 40 cm; O'Hara). Each mouse was placed in the centre of the open field and was allowed explore for 15 min. The behaviour was recorded by a computer-linked overhead video camera. Data were collected and analysed using Image J OF4 (O'Hara).
Home cage activity measurement
Spontaneous activities of mice in the home cage were measured by a 24-channel ABsystem 4.0 (Neuroscience, Tokyo, Japan) for 6 days. Cages were individually set into compartments made of stainless steel in a negative pressure rack (JCL, Tokyo, Japan). A piezoelectric sensor on the ceiling of each compartment detected movements of the mice after habituation to the cages for 1 day.
Light/dark transition test
The apparatus used for the light/dark transition test consisted of a cage (20 × 40 × 20 cm) divided into two sections of equal size by a partition containing a door (O'Hara). One chamber was brightly illuminated (250 Lux), while the other chamber was dark (0 Lux). Mice were placed in the light box and allowed to move freely between the two chambers with the door open for 10 min. The total number of transitions, per cent time spent in the light side, and per cent distance travelled in the light side were recorded and calculated by ImageJ LD4 (O'Hara).
Wheel-running activity
Mice were housed individually in cages (24 × 11 × 14 cm) equipped with a steel running wheel (O'Hara). Wheel-running activity (3 counts per rotation) was detected by a wheel-metre and recorded every 10 min by an online PC computer system (WW-3302, O'Hara). The mouse was able to move freely in the wheel-cage and was given free access to solid food and water during the test. The data from the initial 10 days were omitted from those of total 17 days recording.
Reciprocal social interaction test
Ten male WT and 10 Lrfn2 KO mice (residents) were housed individually for 4 weeks, and 20 male DBA2 mice (intruders) were housed in groups of five mice. The home cages of the residents were not changed for at least 3 days before testing. Reciprocal social interaction tests were performed on 3 consecutive days. On each day, the test was started by placing an unfamiliar intruder in the home cage of a singly housed resident, and their behaviours were recorded on digital video for 5 min. Social responses observed in the resident were scored by the time spent in active social investigation (approaching, sniffing), digging, non-social moving, resting and social avoidance (escaping and hiding under bedding material). The scores for each behaviour were averaged for each genotype.
Three-chamber social approach test
Three-chamber sociability test apparatus was a rectangular, three-chambered box [both side chambers 20 × 15 × 25 (height) cm; centre chamber 20 × 10 × 25 (height) cm], made from a white acrylic box with removable floor. The dividing walls have a 10 cm opening in the centre and the test mice were allowed to access from the centre chamber into the both sides of chambers. The apparatus was washed and cleaned with 70% ethanol between trials. Before the test session, the test mice were placed in the centre chamber and allowed to habituate all chambers for 10 min. For test session, an unfamiliar DBA2 male (stranger) was placed in a small, transparent cylinder (8 cm diameter × 15.5 cm height) with multiple holes (1 cm diameter each) spaced over the entire surface 1 cm∼ apart, that allowed olfactory, visual, auditory and tactile contacts but prevented deep contact such as direct interaction. The cylinder with unfamiliar mouse was placed in one of the side chambers and the empty identical cylinder was placed in the opposite chamber. The location of stranger was altered between trials. The test was started by placing one of the test mice in the centre chamber. The test mice were allowed to explore the three chambers freely for 5 min. The time spent in each chamber was measured using open field monitoring system (O'Hara) linked overhead video camera and the number of nose contact to each cylinder was counted manually.
Ultrasonic vocalization analysis
USV were recorded in a soundproof box with a condenser ultrasound microphone (CM16/CMPA, Avisoft Bioacoustics, Berlin, Germany) connected to an amplifier/digitizer (Avisoft UltraSoundGate416H, Avisoft Bioacoustics). The recorded files were transferred to sound analysis software SASLab Pro Recorder Software (Avisoft Bioacoustics) for fast Fourier transform (512 FFT-length, 100% frame size, Hamming window, 50% time-window overlap). The number of calls, mean duration of each call, and peak frequency of each call were analysed. For the measurement of infant isolation calls, both male and female pups born from eight dams were tested at postnatal day 7. The home cage with a dam and her pups were moved to the experimental room at least 30 min before the recording. In the test session, the dam was removed and the pups were maintained at 35 °C in the home cage. Each pup was placed on a cotton pad in a plastic beaker (10 cm diameter, 11 cm height) and set in the soundproof box. USV were recorded for 5 min at a sampling frequency of 300 kHz. For measurement of ultrasonic courtship vocalizations, we used 15-month-old male mice that had been group-reared without exposure to females after weaning and three 1-month-old female mice that were assessed for oestrous phase on the day of experimentation. The male mice were exposed to an oestrous female mouse for 10 min once a day for 5 successive days before the recording. On the test day, the vocalizations were recorded during a 5-min exposure to an oestrous female in a home cage where the microphone was positioned above the cage. We confirmed that the male mice gave no vocalizations in the absence of a female before the tests.
Marble burying test
Transparent plastic cages (25 × 18 × 15 cm) were filled with white paper bedding material (Paperclean, Japan SLC) to a 4-cm depth. Mice were placed individually in the test cages for 20 min (habituation trial) and then returned to their home cages. Fifteen blue glass marbles were evenly set at 5-cm intervals on the surface of the bedding material in the habituated cages. Then the mice were again placed in the habituated test cage for 20 min (test trial). During both trials, the test cage with the mouse was set in the infrared beam apparatus to measure activity. After 20 min, marbles that were more than two-thirds covered with paperclean were counted as buried marbles.
Acoustic startle response and prepulse inhibition
Mice were habituated in their home cages for 1 h to 65-dB white noise. They were then placed into standard startle chambers. Each session was initiated with a 5-min acclimation period of white noise at 65 dB followed by 10 successive 120-dB tones to elicit the startle response (40 ms). Nine different trial types were then presented: 70, 75, 80, 85, 90, 95, 100, 110 or 120 dB (40 ms) with background noise at 65 dB. Each trial was presented five times, and the average response to each trial calculated. Immediately after the startle response trials, the PPI session was begun. During each PPI session, a mouse was exposed to the following types of trials: the omission of stimuli (no-stimulus trial); startle-alone trial (120 dB); three prepulse combinations (prepulse-pulse trials) using three prepulse intensities: 70, 75 and 80 dB. Each PPI session consisted of 10 presentations of each type of trial. PPI was assessed for each animal as a percentage (%PPI): 100 × (mean startle to pulse alone—mean startle to prepulse-pulse)/mean startle to pulse alone. The apparatuses and software used for data analysis were commercially available (Mouse Startle; O'Hara).
Rotarod test
The rotarod (O'Hara) is a rotating rod that was accelerated from 4 to 40 r.p.m. over the course of 5 min. Individual mice was placed on the rod, and once they were balanced, the rod accelerated. The time at which the mouse fell from the rod was recorded. Each mouse went through four consecutive trials.
Elevated plus-maze test
Each mouse was placed on the central square, facing an open arm of the plus-maze (with a central 5 × 5 cm platform, two open arms of 25 × 5 cm, two enclosed arms of 25 × 5 × 15 cm, O'Hara). Behaviour was recorded by an overhead video camera during the 5-min test period. Data analysis was performed automatically by ImageJ EPM software.
Hidden-cookie test
Mice were first habituated to butter cookies in the home cage overnight. The next day, the mice were deprived of food for 24 h. The test was conducted in a new cage. A piece of butter cookie (0.7 mg per piece) was placed in a randomly chosen area on the cage floor, and then the entire cage floor was covered with corncob bedding to a depth of 4 cm. The subject was then placed into the cage, and latency to find the cookie was recorded up to a maximum time limit of 15 min. We defined finding the cookie as when the mouse held it in both paws.
Histology and morphological analysis
For immunofluorescence staining of cryostat sections, the brains were cryoprotected with 30% sucrose after perfusion-fixation with 4% PFA. Parasagittal sections (12 μm thick) were stained with 0.1% cresyl violet/0.5% acetic acid. Images of whole sections were scanned using an NDP slide scanner (Hamamatsu Photonics). Quantitative analysis of the immunofluorescence signal was also performed on the 12-μm parasagittal or 10-μm coronal cryostat sections. Sections were postfixed with methanol for 10 min at −30 °C for GluA1 and synaptophysin staining; with the methanol/acetone treatment for PSD-95, GluN1 and GluN2A staining; or without pretreatment for GluA2 staining. After the sections were blocked with 2% normal goat serum, they were reacted with the primary antibodies at 4 °C overnight, followed by appropriate fluorescence-labelled secondary antibodies at room temperature for 1 h. The sections were transferred onto Superfrost slides (Matsunami Glass) and mounted under glass coverslips with Vectashield with 4′,6-diamidino-2-phenylindole (DAPI; Vector Laboratories). Fluorescence images of whole sections were scanned using a fluorescence microscope (BZ9000, Keyence) and hippocampus regions including CA1, CA3, and the dentate gyrus were scanned using a confocal microscope (Olympus FV1000) with a × 40 objective. For the puncta signal analysis for Vglut1, Vgat and GluN2A (Supplementary Fig. 4e–h), the fluorescence images were scanned using LSM800 (Zeiss) with × 63 objective. For comparisons between Lrfn2 KO and WT, all stained images were taken with the same laser settings, and the fluorescence intensity was quantified using ImageJ software with the same parameters. DAPI counterstaining was used as a fluorescence control.
For Golgi staining, Brains of 8-week-old littermates were stained using modified Golgi-Cox impregnation of neurons performed with an FD Rapid GolgiStain kit (FD NeuroTechnologies). We prepared coronal sections (100 μm thickness) and examined the area of the CA1 stratum radiatum of the dorsal hippocampus. At least 11 pyramidal neurons per animal with clearly visible staining from the soma to the distal dendrites were randomly selected, and >2 dendritic segments per neuron that were located on secondary or tertiary distal dendrites were scanned (86 segments in 33 neurons from three KO mice, 62 segments in 22 neurons from two WT mice) using a brightfield microscope (Axioskop 2 plus, Zeiss) with a × 100 objective. Counting of the spines (protrusions) and morphometrical analysis of spines were performed as described4. Individual spines were manually traced using NeuroLucida software (MBF Bioscience), followed by measuring the maximum length and head width of each spine. The means of these parameters were calculated for each neuron and compared between genotypes.
For electron microscopic analysis, three anaesthetized littermate mice (8-week old) were perfused with 2.5% PFA/2% glutaraldehyde in 0.1 M sodium phosphate buffer (pH 7.4). Brains were sectioned on a Vibratome (VT1000 S, Leica) at 200 μm, osmicated with 1% OsO4 in phosphate buffer, dehydrated through a gradient series of ethanol, and then embedded in epoxy resin (EPSON812, TAAB). Ultra-thin sections of the hippocampus (70 nm) were cut with an ultramicrotome (EM UC6, Leica), collected on 200-mesh uncoated copper grids (G200HH, GILDER), and counterstained with uranyl acetate and lead citrate. The grids of the CA1 stratum radiatum (about 100 μm beneath the CA1 pyramidal cell body layer) were examined with a transmission electron microscope (JEM-1200EX, JEOL). Photographs were sequentially collected from proximal areas to distal areas using an FDL5000 imaging system (FUJIFILM). Asymmetric synapses that had clear synaptic structure consisting of a visible presynaptic membrane and visible postsynaptic membrane with single or multiple (perforated) PSDs were counted at × 10,000 original magnitude (13 grids from three Lrfn2 KO and 11 grids from three WT). Mean synaptic densities were calculated for each grid and analysed for each genotype. For structural analysis, we obtained highly magnified photos (× 30,000) from the same grids. Every clear asymmetric synapse with a single PSD was manually analysed for the PSD length, PSD thickness and synaptic cleft width using ImageGauge software (FUJIFILM); in total, 301 synapses from 3 Lrfn2 KO mice and 306 synapses from three WT mice were analysed. Statistical significance of the morphological data was determined using Student's t-test or a Kolmogorov–Smirnov test with a level of significance of P<0.05.
Primary neuronal cultures and immunocytochemistry
Dissociated primary neuronal cultures were prepared from the hippocampi of E17–19 Sprague Dawley rats and from E18 to P0 mice. The hippocampi were dissociated with trypsin/EDTA solution. The clumps of cells were gently triturated and washed with fetal bovine serum, and the solution was replaced with Neurobasal medium supplemented with B-27 (Invitrogen) and 2 mM L-glutamine. The cells were plated onto poly-L-lysine–coated glass coverslips or culture dishes. Cultured cells were transfected with Lipofectamine 2000 (Invitrogen) in accordance with the manufacturer's protocol before miniature EPSCs were recorded, or with calcium phosphate precipitation for the biotinylation assay. For immunofluorescence staining of synaptic proteins, neurons were fixed with 4% PFA/4% sucrose in 0.1 M sodium phosphate buffer (pH 7.4) for 20 min on ice, followed by pre-chilled 100% methanol for 10 min at −20 °C. After the neurons were blocked with 5% normal goat serum, they were incubated with primary and fluorescently labelled secondary antibodies in PBS containing 1% normal goat serum. For NeuroLucida tracing, DIV10 neurons were fixed with the 4% PFA/4% sucrose solution and permeabilized with 0.2% Triton X-100 for 10 min. After incubation with an anti-MAP2 antibody in 1% normal goat serum, immunoreactive neurites were visualized using the avidin-biotin-complex method. A total of 8–12 neurons from at least three separate cultures were manually traced using NeuroLucida software; the total length of dendrites was measured, and the complexity of dendrite branching was analysed by Sholl analysis.
For the analysis human LRFN2 and its variants, hippocampal primary culture neurons were obtained from E17.5 rat embryos. Myc-Lrfn2 expression vectors were transfected at DIV7 using Lipofectamine 2000, and fixed at DIV11 (Fig. 9c right) or DIV30 (Fig. 9c left, quantitative analysis based on four independent experiments). Immunostaining was performed using anti-Myc rabbit antibody (Santa Cruz) and anti-PSD-95 antibody (NeuroMab) to detect Myc-Lrfn2 and PSD-95, respectively.
The cell surface biotinylation assay was performed with transfected rat hippocampal neurons (DIV14) as described previously6 with some modifications. The primary cultures were incubated with 0.25 μg ml–1 Sulfo-NHS-SS-biotin in PBS with 1 mM MgSO4 and 2.5 mM CaCl2 (PBS+) on ice for 20 min, then washed three times with cold PBS+ containing 50 mM glycine. Cell lysates were prepared using RIPA buffer, and cell surface biotinylated proteins were precipitated with Neutravidin-agarose and detected by immunoblot.
Electrophysiology
For recording from hippocampal slices, hippocampal slices were prepared from 8- to 14-week-old Lrfn2 KO mice and their littermates and allowed to recover for more than 2 h. For electrophysiological recordings, slices were transferred to a submersion-type recording chamber and perfused with an artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF, 28 °C) that was bubbled with 95% O2 and 5% CO2 and contained 119 mM NaCl, 2.5 mM KCl, 4 mM CaCl2, 4 mM MgSO4, 1 mM NaH2PO4, 26.2 mM NaHCO3, 11 mM glucose and 0.1 mM picrotoxin. fEPSPs were recorded from the CA1 areas with a pipette filled with the ACSF using Multiclamp 700A (Molecular Devices). Schaffer collateral/commissural inputs were stimulated at 0.05 Hz with a glass pipette placed in the stratum radiatum. LTP was induced by two trains of 100 Hz 100 pulses at 20 s interval. To obtain paired-pulse ratio, two stimuli at intervals of 10, 20, 50, 100, 200, 500 and 1,000 ms were applied to the Schaffer collaterals. Post-tetanic potentiation was induced by 100 pulses at 100 Hz in the presence of D-APV (50 μM). We also performed voltage-clamp recordings using a Multiclamp 700A (Molecular Devices). Acquisition and analyses were performed using custom Igor Pro (WaveMetrics) software routines. Whole-cell recordings were made from principal neurons in the CA1 areas through recording electrodes (3–6 Mohm) filled with a solution containing 135 mM caesium methansulfonate, 8 mM NaCl, 10 mM HEPES, 0.2 mM EGTA, 4 mM MgATP and 0.3 mM Na3GTP (pH 7.2 with CsOH, osmolality adjusted to 275–285 mOsm). Series and input resistances were monitored online throughout the experiments. Initially, AMPAR-mediated EPSCs were obtained at −70 mV, then NMDAR-mediated EPSCs were recorded at +40 mV in the presence of CNQX (10 μM), and the ratio of the mean amplitude of these EPSPCs was defined as the AMPAR/NMDAR ratio. Miniature EPSCs (mEPSCs) were recorded at 4 KHz in the presence of TTX (1 μM) and analysed with Mini Analysis software (Synaptosoft). The threshold mEPSC amplitude was set at 5 pA and each mEPSC was verified visually. In silent synapse experiments, small AMPAR-EPSCs were evoked at −70 mV at 0.1 Hz through minimal stimulation at a reduced intensity at which some failures were surely identified visually, and then NMDAR-EPSCs were recorded using the same stimuli at +40 mV (refs 66, 67). Failure rates at −70 mV (F–70) and +40 mV (F–70) were estimated by doubling that of events with an amplitude of more than and less than zero, respectively66. The percentage of silent synapses was calculated using a formula 1−Ln(F–70)/Ln (F+40)66.
For the electrophysiological analysis using cultured hippocampal neurons, recording of mEPSCs was performed 7–10 days after transfection of a series of expression vectors, each combined with an EGFP vector. EGFP-positive pyramidal cells were identified under a inverted fluorescence microscope (IX71, Olympus) and examined using whole-cell patch-clamp mode. A patch-clamp amplifier (EPC-7, List Electronics, Darmstadt, Germany) was used for recording. Data were low-pass filtered at 3 kHz and digitally sampled at 10 kHz using pClamp8.0 (Molecular Devices, Union City, CA). The bath solution consisted of 137 mM NaCl, 4 mM KCl, 2 mM CaCl2, 1 mM MgCl2, 17 mM glucose and 10 mM HEPES-NaOH (pH 7.4). Tetrodotoxin (1 μM), picrotoxin (100 μM) and DL-AP5 (50 μM) were added during recording. The intrapipette solution consisted of 110 mM K-gluconate, 30 mM KCl, 2 mM MgCl2, 1 mM EGTA, 0.25 mM CaCl2, 3 mM Na2ATP, 0.3 mM GTP and 5 mM HEPES, pH 7.2. Patch electrodes had resistances of 3–5 Mohm. Neurons were voltage clamped at −70 mV. mEPSCs were detected and measured using the MINI ANALYSIS program (Jaejin Software, Leoniua, NJ).
Subjects and mutation screening of LRFN2
We performed resequencing analysis of LRFN2 in 165 patients with autism and 910 patients with schizophrenia who were of Japanese descent. For the Japanese sample, the diagnosis of schizophrenia was made based on DSM-IV by consensus of at least 2 experienced psychiatrists. The diagnosis of autism was made on the basis of the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised criteria68. All healthy control subjects were psychiatrically screened in unstructured interviews. All controls and patients gave informed, written consent to participate in the study after being provided with, and receiving an explanation of study protocols and objectives. This study was approved by the ethics committees of RIKEN and Hamamatsu University. We resequenced all the protein-coding exons and flanking introns. The detailed information is available on request. We tested common SNPs (minor allele frequency ≥5% in Japanese population; HapMap database (release 22), http://hapmap.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov) of LRFN2 for genetic association with schizophrenia by studying two DNA samples: one includes 289 Chinese pedigrees with schizophrenia, consisting of nine trios and 279 quads, and the other includes 570 cases of schizophrenia and 570 controls from a Japanese population. The Chinese pedigree sample was made available by the NIMH Human Genetics Initiative, and detailed information about it can be obtained at http://nimhgenetics.org/.
SNP selection and association study
We obtained a list of 52 tagged SNPs, which was expected to efficiently capture information of the common variations in and around the LRFN2 (±10 kb) gene, by applying the Carlson's greedy algorithm69 to SNP genotype data of both Chinese and Japanese populations from the HapMap database (release 22). HapMap-LDSelect-Processor (http://bioapp.psych.uic.edu/HapMap-LDSelect-Processor.html) was used for this SNP tagging procedure, setting the r2 and the minor allele frequency threshold to 0.8 and 0.1, respectively. SNP genotyping was performed with the TaqMan system (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA) in accordance with the manufacturer's protocol. PCR was performed using an ABI 9700 thermocycler, and fluorescent signals were analysed using an ABI 7900 sequence detector single-point measurement and SDS v2.3 software (Applied Biosystems). The Haploview program70 was used for association testing. The transmission disequilibrium test and the allelic test were performed for the NIMH sample and the Japanese sample, respectively, to examine whether any alleles at LRFN2 are transmitted significantly more often (over-transmitted) to offspring in the pedigrees or observed significantly more often (over-represented) in cases than expected by the null hypothesis of no association. LRFN2 mRNA quantification was carried out using TaqMan probes and primers for LRFN2 and GAPDH (an internal control) (TaqMan Gene Expression Assays gene expression products, Applied Biosystems).
Statistical analysis
Data represent means±s.e.m. unless otherwise noted. Student's unpaired 2-tailed t-test or the Mann–Whitney U-test was used to determine the statistical significance of differences between two groups; single-factor ANOVA or repeated measures ANOVA was used to compare three or more groups, followed by the Tukey–Kramer post hoc test for multiple comparisons. Cumulative plots were analysed using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Statistical analyses were performed in microsoft excel and SPSS (ver. 16.0, SPSS Japan).
Data availability
All data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article and its Supplementary Information files.
Additional information
How to cite this article: Morimura, N. et al. Autism-like behaviours and enhanced memory formation and synaptic plasticity in Lrfn2/SALM1-deficient mice. Nat. Commun. 8, 15800 doi: 10.1038/ncomms15800 (2017).
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Alcino Silva, Eunjoon Kim, Alaa El-Husseini and Gail K. Seabold for valuable discussions and comments on the results, Yasuhide Iwata for helping the recruitment of autism patients, and Tsutomu Hashikawa, Takaoki Kasahara, Thomas Launey, Ryoko Nakagawa and Kazuo Okanoya for helpful advice on the experiments. We also thank patients and their families including those in Asperger Society Japan for their cooperation. This study was supported by RIKEN BSI funds, MEXT Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research (21240031, 25110736, 15K06789, 16H04666), and research grant from the Uehara Memorial Foundation.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Author contributions N.M., H.Y., K.-i.K., M.H., T.Y. and J.A. conceived and designed the study; N.M., M.H., A.K., M.M. and J.A. performed the biochemical and cellular analysis; N.M. generated anti-Lrfn antibodies; K.-i.K. generated Lrfn2 KO mice; N.M., M.O. and K. Yamada performed the behavioural experiments; N.M., M.O. and J.A. performed the immunostaining experiments; N.M., N.H.T. and M.O. performed the morphological analysis; H.Y. and K. Yamaguchi performed the electrophysiological recordings; Y.I. and T.Y. performed the resequencing and association analysis; K.N., H.M. and M.T. provided the human resources; N.M. and J.A. analysed the data; J.A. supervised the project; N.M., H.Y., T.Y. and J.A. wrote the paper.
References
- Zoghbi H. Y. & Bear M. F. Synaptic dysfunction in neurodevelopmental disorders associated with autism and intellectual disabilities. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 4, a009886 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volk L., Chiu S. L., Sharma K. & Huganir R. L. Glutamate synapses in human cognitive disorders. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 38, 127–149 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanders S. J. et al. Insights into autism spectrum disorder genomic architecture and biology from 71 risk loci. Neuron 87, 1215–1233 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jiang Y. H. & Ehlers M. D. Modeling autism by SHANK gene mutations in mice. Neuron 78, 8–27 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourgeron T. A synaptic trek to autism. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19, 231–234 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sudhof T. C. Neuroligins and neurexins link synaptic function to cognitive disease. Nature 455, 903–911 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durand C. M. et al. Mutations in the gene encoding the synaptic scaffolding protein SHANK3 are associated with autism spectrum disorders. Nat. Genet. 39, 25–27 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Funke L., Dakoji S. & Bredt D. S. Membrane-associated guanylate kinases regulate adhesion and plasticity at cell junctions. Annu. Rev. Biochem. 74, 219–245 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim E. & Sheng M. PDZ domain proteins of synapses. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 771–781 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kornau H. C., Schenker L. T., Kennedy M. B. & Seeburg P. H. Domain interaction between NMDA receptor subunits and the postsynaptic density protein PSD-95. Science 269, 1737–1740 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niethammer M., Kim E. & Sheng M. Interaction between the C terminus of NMDA receptor subunits and multiple members of the PSD-95 family of membrane-associated guanylate kinases. J. Neurosci. 16, 2157–2163 (1996). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehrlich I., Klein M., Rumpel S. & Malinow R. PSD-95 is required for activity-driven synapse stabilization. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 4176–4181 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehrlich I. & Malinow R. Postsynaptic density 95 controls AMPA receptor incorporation during long-term potentiation and experience-driven synaptic plasticity. J. Neurosci. 24, 916–927 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Migaud M. et al. Enhanced long-term potentiation and impaired learning in mice with mutant postsynaptic density-95 protein. Nature 396, 433–439 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beique J. C. et al. Synapse-specific regulation of AMPA receptor function by PSD-95. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 19535–19540 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varoqueaux F. et al. Neuroligins determine synapse maturation and function. Neuron 51, 741–754 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jamain S. et al. Reduced social interaction and ultrasonic communication in a mouse model of monogenic heritable autism. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 1710–1715 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit J. et al. LRRTM2 interacts with Neurexin1 and regulates excitatory synapse formation. Neuron 64, 799–806 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woo J. et al. Trans-synaptic adhesion between NGL-3 and LAR regulates the formation of excitatory synapses. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 428–437 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robbins E. M. et al. SynCAM 1 adhesion dynamically regulates synapse number and impacts plasticity and learning. Neuron 68, 894–906 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ko J. et al. SALM synaptic cell adhesion-like molecules regulate the differentiation of excitatory synapses. Neuron 50, 233–245 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morimura N., Inoue T., Katayama K. & Aruga J. Comparative analysis of structure, expression and PSD95-binding capacity of Lrfn, a novel family of neuronal transmembrane proteins. Gene 380, 72–83 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang C. Y. et al. A novel family of adhesion-like molecules that interacts with the NMDA receptor. J. Neurosci. 26, 2174–2183 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voineagu I. et al. Transcriptomic analysis of autistic brain reveals convergent molecular pathology. Nature 474, 380–384 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thevenon J. et al. Heterozygous deletion of the LRFN2 gene is associated with working memory deficits. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 24, 911–918 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rautiainen M. R. et al. Genome-wide association study of antisocial personality disorder. Transl. Psychiatry 6, e883 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang P. Y., Seabold G. K. & Wenthold R. J. Synaptic adhesion-like molecules (SALMs) promote neurite outgrowth. Mol. Cell Neurosci. 39, 83–94 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan G. E. et al. Deficits in sensorimotor gating and tests of social behavior in a genetic model of reduced NMDA receptor function. Behav. Brain Res. 153, 507–519 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moretti P., Bouwknecht J. A., Teague R., Paylor R. & Zoghbi H. Y. Abnormalities of social interactions and home-cage behavior in a mouse model of Rett syndrome. Hum. Mol. Genet. 14, 205–220 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silverman J. L., Yang M., Lord C. & Crawley J. N. Behavioural phenotyping assays for mouse models of autism. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 490–502 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deacon R. M. Digging and marble burying in mice: simple methods for in vivo identification of biological impacts. Nat. Protoc. 1, 122–124 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas A. et al. Marble burying reflects a repetitive and perseverative behavior more than novelty-induced anxiety. Psychopharmacology 204, 361–373 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherwin C. M. Voluntary wheel running: a review and novel interpretation. Anim. Behav. 56, 11–27 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geschwind D. H. Advances in autism. Ann. Rev. Med. 60, 367–380 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logue S. F., Paylor R. & Wehner J. M. Hippocampal lesions cause learning deficits in inbred mice in the Morris water maze and conditioned-fear task. Behav. Neurosci. 111, 104–113 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squire L. R. & Zola S. M. Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93, 13515–13522 (1996). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spacek J. & Harris K. M. Trans-endocytosis via spinules in adult rat hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 24, 4233–4241 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee Y. S. & Silva A. J. The molecular and cellular biology of enhanced cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 126–140 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silva A. J. Molecular and cellular cognitive studies of the role of synaptic plasticity in memory. J. Neurobiol. 54, 224–237 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourne J. N. & Harris K. M. Balancing structure and function at hippocampal dendritic spines. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 47–67 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kasai H., Matsuzaki M., Noguchi J., Yasumatsu N. & Nakahara H. Structure-stability-function relationships of dendritic spines. Trends Neurosci. 26, 360–368 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsuo N., Reijmers L. & Mayford M. Spine-type-specific recruitment of newly synthesized AMPA receptors with learning. Science 319, 1104–1107 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liao D., Hessler N. A. & Malinow R. Activation of postsynaptically silent synapses during pairing-induced LTP in CA1 region of hippocampal slice. Nature 375, 400–404 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isaac J. T., Nicoll R. A. & Malenka R. C. Evidence for silent synapses: implications for the expression of LTP. Neuron 15, 427–434 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grabrucker A. M., Schmeisser M. J., Schoen M. & Boeckers T. M. Postsynaptic ProSAP/Shank scaffolds in the cross-hair of synaptopathies. Trends Cell Biol. 21, 594–603 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El-Husseini A. E., Schnell E., Chetkovich D. M., Nicoll R. A. & Bredt D. S. PSD-95 involvement in maturation of excitatory synapses. Science 290, 1364–1368 (2000). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braff D. L. Prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex: a window on the brain in schizophrenia. Curr. Top Behav. Neurosci. 4, 349–371 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kerchner G. A. & Nicoll R. A. Silent synapses and the emergence of a postsynaptic mechanism for LTP. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 813–825 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lisman J. & Raghavachari S. A unified model of the presynaptic and postsynaptic changes during LTP at CA1 synapses. Sci. Signal 2006, re11 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shi S., Hayashi Y., Esteban J. A. & Malinow R. Subunit-specific rules governing AMPA receptor trafficking to synapses in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Cell 105, 331–343 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlisle H. J., Fink A. E., Grant S. G. & O'Dell T. J. Opposing effects of PSD-93 and PSD-95 on long-term potentiation and spike timing-dependent plasticity. J. Physiol. 586, 5885–5900 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun Y. et al. The regulation of GluN2A by endogenous and exogenous regulators in the central nervous system. Cell Mol. Neurobiol. 37, 389–403 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fukaya M., Kato A., Lovett C., Tonegawa S. & Watanabe M. Retention of NMDA receptor NR2 subunits in the lumen of endoplasmic reticulum in targeted NR1 knockout mice. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 4855–4860 (2003). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris K. M. & Stevens J. K. Dendritic spines of CA 1 pyramidal cells in the rat hippocampus: serial electron microscopy with reference to their biophysical characteristics. J. Neurosci. 9, 2982–2997 (1989). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takumi Y., Ramirez-Leon V., Laake P., Rinvik E. & Ottersen O. P. Different modes of expression of AMPA and NMDA receptors in hippocampal synapses. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 618–624 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nikonenko I., Jourdain P., Alberi S., Toni N. & Muller D. Activity-induced changes of spine morphology. Hippocampus 12, 585–591 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toni N., Buchs P. A., Nikonenko I., Bron C. R. & Muller D. LTP promotes formation of multiple spine synapses between a single axon terminal and a dendrite. Nature 402, 421–425 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seabold G. K. et al. The SALM family of adhesion-like molecules forms heteromeric and homomeric complexes. J. Biol. Chem. 283, 8395–8405 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charych E. I. et al. Activity-independent regulation of dendrite patterning by postsynaptic density protein PSD-95. J. Neurosci. 26, 10164–10176 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy S. E., Mandell D. S. & Schultz R. T. Autism. Lancet 374, 1627–1638 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treffert D. A. The savant syndrome: an extraordinary condition. A synopsis: past, present, future. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364, 1351–1357 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Y. et al. Splicing-dependent trans-synaptic SALM3-LAR-RPTP interactions regulate excitatory synapse development and locomotion. Cell Rep. 12, 1618–1630 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lie E. et al. SALM4 suppresses excitatory synapse development by cis-inhibiting trans-synaptic SALM3-LAR adhesion. Nat. Commun. 7, 12328 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakai K. & Miyazaki J. A transgenic mouse line that retains Cre recombinase activity in mature oocytes irrespective of the cre transgene transmission. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 237, 318–324 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlin R. K., Grab D. J., Cohen R. S. & Siekevitz P. Isolation and characterization of postsynaptic densities from various brain regions: enrichment of different types of postsynaptic densities. J. Cell Biol. 86, 831–845 (1980). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derkach V. A., Oh M. C., Guire E. S. & Soderling T. R. Regulatory mechanisms of AMPA receptors in synaptic plasticity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 101–113 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gauthier J. et al. Truncating mutations in NRXN2 and NRXN1 in autism spectrum disorders and schizophrenia. Hum. Genet. 130, 563–573 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lord C., Rutter M. & Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 659–685 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlson C. S. et al. Selecting a maximally informative set of single-nucleotide polymorphisms for association analyses using linkage disequilibrium. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 74, 106–120 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett J. C., Fry B., Maller J. & Daly M. J. Haploview: analysis and visualization of LD and haplotype maps. Bioinformatics 21, 263–265 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article and its Supplementary Information files.