Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Psychiatr Res. 2017 Feb 24;91:83–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.02.023

5-HTTLPR moderates the association between attention away from angry faces and prospective depression among youth

Jessica L Jenness 1, Jami F Young 2, Benjamin L Hankin 3
PMCID: PMC5473433  NIHMSID: NIHMS860707  PMID: 28325682

Abstract

Attention bias to emotion has been studied as a risk factor associated with depression. No study has examined whether attention bias within the context of measured genetic risk leads to increased risk for clinical depressive episodes over time. The current study investigated whether genetic risk, as indexed by the serotonin-transporter-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR), moderated the relationship between attention bias to emotional faces and clinical depression onset prospectively across 18-months in a community sample of youth (n=428; mean age=11.97, SD =2.28; 59% girls). Youth who attended away from angry emotional faces and were homozygous for the S allele of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism were at greater risk for prospective depressive episode onset. The current study’s findings highlight the importance of examining risk for depression across multiple levels of analysis and demonstrate attention away from threat as a possible point of intervention related to attention bias modification and depression treatment among youth.

Keywords: depression, attention bias, 5-HTTLPR, youth

Introduction

Rates of depression increase markedly across adolescence (Hankin et al., 1998) and depression onset in adolescence predicts emotional and behavioral difficulties into adulthood (Rutter et al., 2006). Cognitive theories of depression posit that information processing of affective information (i.e., attention bias to emotion) may contribute to the development, maintenance, and recurrence of depression (Beck, 2008). There is a growing body of literature suggesting certain biologically plausible candidate genes may contribute to the association between attention bias and depression (Gibb et al., 2013). However, previous research has been primarily cross-sectional, so it is unknown whether attention bias to emotion functions as a cause, correlate, or consequence of clinical depression. The current study addressed this gap by examining attention bias to emotional faces and molecular genetic risk as prospective predictors of depression onset among youth.

Previous research demonstrates that currently depressed adults exhibit cognitive biases for negative material (Mathews and MacLeod, 2005), and there is evidence supporting similar cognitive theories of depression among youth (Gibb et al., 2013). Many of the theorized underlying cognitive mechanisms (e.g., encoding, attention, memory) are thought to function outside of one’s awareness and are best measured by information processing tasks that are less susceptible to reporting biases when compared to self-report measures (Gotlib and Neubauer, 2000). Several cross-sectional studies in adults have utilized computerized attention to emotion tasks, such as the dot-probe task (MacLeod et al., 1986), and shown those diagnosed with current or past major depression demonstrate attentional biases toward negatively valenced stimuli (Gotlib et al., 2004; Joormann and Gotlib, 2007). Specifically, meta-analytic findings among depressed adults show a medium effect size of attention bias to negative stimuli (i.e., sad faces, negatively valenced photos) and a trend toward greater attentional bias toward socially threatening stimuli among depressed adults (Peckham et al., 2010). However, far less is known about the association between depression and attention bias among youth.

There have been mixed findings within the youth literature with some studies showing attention away from negative emotion and others finding attention toward negative emotion relating to depression. Cross-sectional studies utilizing similar dot-probe tasks and stimuli presentation times (1,000ms, Hankin et al., 2010; 1,500ms, Joormann et al., 2007) found at-risk (e.g., youth whose mothers have a history of depression) and currently depressed youth demonstrated biased attention toward negative emotion, particularly sad faces, as compared to control participants. However, two recent studies examining attention bias to emotion with a more precise eye-tracking methodology found attention away from negative emotion related most strongly with depression. Specifically, a cross-sectional study (Harrison and Gibb, 2014) found currently depressed youth attended away from sadness when passively viewing emotional faces for 20s, while a longitudinal examination of attention bias using a standard dot-probe task (2,000ms presentation time) found attention away from fearful faces predicted greater depression symptom severity across 2 years (Price et al., 2015).

It is possible the discrepancies within the youth literature stem from variation across tasks (i.e., passive viewing versus dot-probe) and method of assessing bias (i.e., behavioral versus eye-tracking). Alternatively, developmental theory suggests that young children attend away from negative stimuli as a means of emotion regulation (Gross and Thompson, 2007). While adaptive in young childhood, this method of regulation may become maladaptive throughout development leading to greater risk for depression onset. Indeed, research has shown avoidance or disengagement based coping strategies are associated with internalizing psychopathology (Connor-Smith et al., 2000). Although aspects of emotion regulation may play a role in the association between attention bias and depression, there is a need for prospective designs that incorporate theoretically relevant moderators in order to draw temporal inferences regarding the role of attention bias in the development of clinical depression.

The ability to effectively process and regulate emotion may be an important activator in the pathway from attention bias to depression. The serotonin-transporter-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) in the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) has been of interest when studying attention bias and stress-related psychopathology (Lesch et al., 1996; Sen et al., 2004), including depression (Gibb et al., 2013), given its role in influencing neurotransmitters involved in the processing and regulation of emotion. The serotonin transporter (5-HTT) is a protein critical to the regulation of serotonin function in the brain because it terminates the action of serotonin in the synapse via reuptake. This is a well-studied protein that has a functional number of tandem repeats (VNTR) polymorphism in the promoter region referred to as 5-HTTLPR. Variants of 5-HTTLPR are the long allele (L), consisting of 16 copies of an approximately 22 base pair (bp) repeat unit, and a short allele (S), comprised of 14 copies (Hariri and Holmes, 2006). The S allele is associated with decreased transcriptional efficiency resulting in approximately 50% less serotonin being recaptured in the pre-synaptic neuron when compared to the L allele (Lesch et al., 1996). There is a large body of research showing S carriers of 5-HTTLPR demonstrate disruptions in emotion processing and reactivity including increased amygdala activation and reduced connectivity between regions of the prefrontal cortex and amygdala when viewing negative stimuli (Heinz et al., 2004) and greater cortisol reactivity to a laboratory stressor (Hankin et al., 2015a).

Specific to attention bias, SS carriers have shown attention bias for negatively valenced material (Jenness et al., 2015; Pergamin-Hight et al., 2012). Given that 5-HTTLPR, particularly SS carriers, has been implicated in emotion processing and reactivity, the current research posits this polymorphism may act as an important moderator to the relationship between attention bias to emotion and depression onset. Building upon the 5-HTTLPR and emotion processing literature, Gibb and colleagues (2009) found a relationship between increases in child depressive symptoms over time and mothers’ symptoms of depression among children at high genetic risk who avoided negative emotion. Among youth, attention away from negative emotion may confer risk for depression given the prospective maladaptive psychological outcomes associated with behavioral avoidance and disengagement (Seiffge-Krenke and Klessinger, 2000); however, once in a current negative mood state, there may be a shift in biases toward self-referential attentional preference toward negative emotion (Mogg and Bradley, 2005). Further prospective data is necessary to better understand whether attentional preference or avoidance is associated with risk for depression onset.

The current study aimed to investigate whether genetic risk (i.e., 5-HTTLPR) moderates the association between attention bias to emotional faces and clinical depression onset prospectively across 18-months in a large community sample of youth. Of note, findings have generally shown attention toward negative emotion during a current negative mood state, but attention away from negative emotion when prospectively predicting increases in depressive symptoms (Gibb et al., 2009; Price et al., 2015). Therefore, we hypothesized that youth at highest genetic risk (i.e., SS carriers of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism; Pergamin-Hight et al., 2012) who avoided negative emotion, such as sadness or anger, would be at increased risk for clinical depression over time. This study was designed to fill several notable gaps within the attention bias and depression literature including, 1) examining clinical depression onset via diagnostic interview rather than self-reported depression symptomatology, 2) utilizing a prospective design in order to establish a temporal precedence between attention bias and the development of depression, and 3) studying the relationship among attention bias, genetic risk, and depression in a sample of youth, which provides a developmentally significant time frame to identify risk factors for depression onset (Rutter et al., 2006).

Method and Materials

Participants

Participants included a subsample of 428 children and adolescents drawn from a larger project utilizing a community sample of youth recruited from schools within urban and suburban Colorado and New Jersey (Hankin et al., 2015b) who completed the attention bias task. The larger study’s primary goal was to understand genetic, cognitive, and affective factors contributing to youth depression. Parents of youth who were in 3rd (n=115; 27%), 6th (n= 175; 41%), or 9th (n= 138; 32%) grade completed a brief screening to determine their child’s eligibility. Youth were excluded if they had a severe learning or psychiatric problem (e.g., autism, psychosis) that was likely to interfere with completion of an extensive laboratory protocol. The sample was approximately evenly divided by sex, was of mixed ethnic origin representative of their geographic region, and ranged in age from 7 to 16 years old (Table 1). Parents of youth were primarily mothers (91%). Median annual parental income was $90,000 and 16% of the youth received free/reduced lunch at school.

Table 1.

Demographics and Descriptive Statistics by Depression Group Status

No Depressive Episode
(n=358)
Depressive Episode
(n= 70)
Total Sample
(n=428)
n % n % n %
Past Depressive Episode*
 No 289 81.4 39 55.7 328 77.2
 Yes 66 18.6 31 44.3 97 22.8
Gende
 Girls 206 57.5 48 68.6 254 59.3
 Boys 152 42.5 22 31.4 174 40.7
Ethnicity
 Hispanic 27 8.3 3 4.4 30 7.7
 Non-Hispanic 297 91.7 65 95.6 93
Race
 Caucasian 221 68.2 49 72.0 270 68.9
 African American 39 12.0 7 10.3 46 11.7
 Asian/Pacific Islander 43 13.3 4 5.9 47 12.0
 Other/Mixed Race 21 6.5 8 11.8 29 7.4
5-HTTLPR Genotype
 LL/SL 251 78.4 44 75.9 295 78.0
 SS 69 21.6 14 24.1 83 22.0
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Age* 11.77 2.28 12.96 2.07 11.97 2.28
Attention Bias
 Angry Bias −8.56 54.37 −9.23 44.46 −8.67 52.83
 Happy Bias −11.18 53.79 −11.53 45.43 −11.24 52.46
 Sad Bias −3.11 53.57 −2.44 45.30 −3.301 52.27
MASC 41.50 16.36 43.72 13.71 41.86 15.97

Note. No Depressive Episode= No onset of clinical depression between baseline and 18-month follow-up assessment; Depressive Episode= Onset of clinical depression between baseline and 18-month follow-up. Past Depressive Episode= yes or no whether youth experienced an episode of clinical depression at or before the baseline visit. MASC = Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children.

*

p<.001 and

p< .06

Procedures

Each eligible parent and youth participated in four points of data collection including the baseline assessment and 6-month, 12-month, and 18-month follow-up assessments. Parents provided informed written consent for their own and their child’s participation, and youth provided written assent at the baseline laboratory visit. The baseline assessment consisted of youth completing the dot-probe task, collecting youth DNA via saliva collection, and diagnostic interviewing with youth and parents about their child, in that order. The 6- and 12-month follow up assessments consisted of diagnostic interviews with youth and parents about their child by phone. Families returned to the laboratory for the 18-month follow-up assessment where updated diagnostic interviewing took place. The retention rate from baseline to 18-month follow-up was 89%. Attrition did not differ across primary variables of interest including attention bias scores (ps>.25), genotype group (p=. 40), or history of depression diagnosis (p=. 62). All procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board at both sites. Youth and parents were reimbursed for their participation at all assessments.

Measures

Attention bias

Youth’s attentional biases for facial displays of emotion were assessed using a modified dot-probe task (MacLeod et al., 1986) administered using E-Prime. Stimuli for the dot-probe task consisted of pairs of facial expressions that contained one affective (angry, sad, or happy), and one neutral photograph from the same actor taken from a standardized stimulus set (Tottenham et al., 2009). Photographs from each actor (16 men and 16 women) were used to create sad-neutral, happy-neutral, and angry-neutral stimulus pairs (96 pairs).

Each stimulus pair was presented in random order across two blocks, with a rest in between blocks, for a total of 192 trials. Each trial began with a white fixation cross in the middle of the screen for 1,000 milliseconds (ms). Then, a pair of pictures was presented for 1,000ms, followed by a dot presented for 1000ms that replaced either the affective or neutral picture. Youth were instructed to indicate as quickly as possible the location of the dot (left versus right side of the screen) using the computer keyboard (“z” labeled “left”; “/” labeled “right”). The computer recorded the accuracy and response time for each response. Consistent with prior research (Gotlib et al., 2004), trials with response errors were excluded as were trials with response times less than 150ms or greater than 1,500ms. Error rates were low (less than 1.5%), and a small portion (1.8%) were excluded for being out of response time range. Of the 428 children who completed the dot-probe task, 378 had complete genetic data. Those with complete genetic data did not differ from the total sample on age, gender, or ethnicity/race (ps >.13).

Mean attention bias scores (Mogg et al., 1995) were calculated separately for each affective stimulus type (angry, sad, or happy face) by subtracting the mean response time for trials when the probe replaced the affective face from mean response times for trials when the probe replaced the neutral face.

Genotyping

Saliva samples were obtained from participants with Oragene™ (DNA Genotek, Ontario, Canada) collection kits, and DNA was extracted using standard salting-out and solvent precipitation methods. The method for 5-HTTLPR and SNP rs25531 (n=378) is detailed elsewhere (Whisman et al., 2011). The rs25531 SNP genotypes (LA vs. LG) were obtained by incubating the PCR products with MspI (Wendland et al., 2006). Based on meta-analytic findings showing those homozygous for the S allele demonstrate biased attention for negative material as compared to L allele carriers (Pergamin-Hight et al., 2012), two groups of participants were formed based on their 5-HTTLPR genotyping: youth homozygous for the lower expressing S or LG alleles (i.e., SS) and those heterozygous or homozygous for the higher expressing LA allele (i.e., SL/LL). The successful call-rate for the overall project was 97.5% for 5-HTTLPR. 5-HTTLPR was in Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium.

Diagnostic Status

Trained interviewers administered the mood disorders and psychosis subsections of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School Age Children (K-SADS-PL) (Kaufman et al., 1997) to youth and parents about their child to assess for current and past lifetime episodes of depression and mania at baseline and current and past (i.e., last 6-months) episodes of depression and mania at each follow-up. No youth was diagnosed with a bipolar spectrum disorder or psychosis. Interviewers then utilized both youth report and parent report about youth to determine symptom count, depression severity, and diagnostic status using best estimate procedures. Interviewers were trained and supervised by Ph.D. level, licensed psychologists. Interviews (20%) were randomly selected to conduct reliability analyses (κ=.91). Furthermore, all interviews containing a subthreshold or threshold criterion symptom of depression were reviewed for accuracy by another interviewer; disagreements were resolved by consensus.

Seventy youth (16%) met DSM-IV criteria for a clinically significant depressive episode (i.e., Major Depressive Disorder or Depressive Disorder- Not Otherwise Specified) between the baseline and 18-month follow-up assessments. Rates of depressive episode onset found in the sample are similar to those found in other community samples (Avenevoli et al., 2008).

Anxiety Symptoms

The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC) (March et al., 1997) assessed youths’ self-reported anxiety symptoms at the baseline assessment. The MASC has good reliability and validity (Silverman and Ollendick, 2005). Internal consistency (α) was .90. Overall MASC scores (M=42.15, SD=15.96) were comparable to prior research with general community samples (Baldwin and Dadds, 2007). Consistent with point prevalence rates for anxiety disorders among community samples of youth (Merikangas et al., 2009), 6% of youth were above clinical cutoff (T scores >65).

Data Analytic Plan

Logistic regression analyses were used to test the Genetic (5-HTTLPR) x Attention Bias (sad, angry, or happy facial expressions) interaction as a predictor of depression group status (depression onset coded 1 and no depression onset coded 0) across 18-months using the SPSS macro PROCESS (Hayes, 2013). Attention bias scores were centered prior to analyses. History of depression (i.e., episodes of clinical depression occurring concurrent or prior to the baseline assessment) and baseline anxiety symptoms (i.e., MASC scores) were entered to control for covariance between past depression and anxiety and future depression episode onset. All main effects and interactions were entered simultaneously and unstandardized regression coefficients are reported (Hayes, 2013) for each set of analyses.

Post hoc analyses of significant interactions were conducted (Aiken and West, 1991). New product terms were computed at different levels (i.e., genotype groups) of the moderator variable. Separate regressions were conducted for each of these product terms. This enabled examination of the significance of simple slopes across genotype. Significant interactions were further explored following Hayes’(2013) guidelines for testing regions of significance according to the Johnson-Neyman (J-N) technique (Bauer and Curran, 2005). This procedure uses regression parameters to derive values of a continuous moderator (i.e., attention bias scores) at which the conditional effect of the focal predictor (i.e., 5-HTTLPR genotype) on the dependent variable (i.e., depression group status) transitions from nonsignificant to significant. Applied to the present study, the J-N technique indicated at what degree of attention bias those at higher or lower genetic risk differ significantly in their risk for depression disorder onset.

Sensitivity analyses were conducted to examine whether significant results remained once controlling for other key demographic variables known to influence depression onset, including age and biological sex. Additionally, we re-ran significant models only within the largest ethnic group (i.e., Caucasian participants) to address concerns regarding population stratification (Jorm and Easteal, 2000). Finally, we tested whether significant findings remained among participants without a history of depression at the baseline assessment. These analyses provide a more stringent test of hypotheses by examining whether the relationship between attention bias and 5-HTTLPR genotype applies specifically to first onsets of depression across the 18-months of the study.

Results

Preliminary Analyses

Demographic information and descriptive statistics for all primary variables separated by diagnostic status are presented (Table 1). Consistent with epidemiological studies of youth depression (Avenevoli et al., 2008) older youths, girls, and those with a past episode of depression were more likely to experience an onset of depression across 18-months, while attention bias scores, ethnicity/race, and genotype did not significantly differ across diagnostic groups. There were no significant differences among genotypes across age (p=. 28), race (p=. 17), gender (p=. 94), depression onset across 18-months (p=. 66), or past history of depression (p=.71). Zero-order correlations between primary variables of interest are presented in Table 2.

Table 2.

Correlations between primary variables of interest.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. Angry Bias
2. Sad Bias −.03
3. Happy Bias .05 .01
4. 5-HTTLPR −.10 −.09 −.08
5. 18-Month MDD −.01 .01 −.002 .02
6. Past MDD −.07 .10* −.07 −.04 − 23**
7. MASC −.03 .07 −.03 −.01 .05 .07

Note. 18-Month MDD refers to onset of a clinically significant depressive episode between the baseline and 18-month follow-up assessments; Past MDD refers to onset of a clinically significant depressive episode prior to or concurrent with the baseline assessment.

**

p<.001,

*

p<.05

5-HTTLPR and Attention Bias

There were no significant gene-environment correlations (rGEs) between 5-HTTLPR and attention bias to any emotion (ps> .07). Logistic regression analyses revealed no significant main effects and a significant interaction between 5-HTTLPR and attention bias to anger predicting depression group status across 18-months (Table 3). This effect is shown in Fig. 1 across the range of attention bias scores seen in our sample. Post-hoc analyses showed a significant slope for those with the SS genotype (b=−1.43, SE=.66, t=−2.16, p=.03) indicating that youth homozygous for the S allele were more likely to experience an onset of clinical depression when avoiding compared to attending to angry facial expressions.1 The slope for the SL/LL genotype group (b=.82, SE=.73, t= 1.13, p=.26) was not significant. The J-N tests confirmed that the effect of 5-HTTLPR emerged only at high levels of attention away from anger (i.e., ≤ −50ms) as opposed to preferential attention to anger to predict depression onset (Fig. 1).

Table 3.

Prediction of Depressive Episode Onset Across 18-months by 5-HTTLPR and Attention Bias to Emotional, Faces (Sad, Angry, Happy)

Angry Emotion

Logistic Regression Model Summary:
−2LL= 285.61, Model LL= 28.60, McFadden= .09, CoxSnell= .07, Nagelkerke= .13

Predictor b (SE b) 95% CI z p
 Intercept −2.75(0.62) [−3.96, −1.54] −4.46 <.001
 5-HTTLPR 0.02(0.41) [−0.80, 0.83] 0.04 .97
 Angry Bias 0.01(0.004) [−0.0002, 0.01] 1.89 .06
 5-HTTLPR x Angry Bias −0.02(.008) [−0.04, −0.01] −2.91 .004
 MASC 0.01(0.01) [−0.02, 0.04] 0.75 .45
 Past MDD 1.48(0.32) [0.84, 2.11] 4.57 <.001
Sad Emotion

Logistic Regression Model Summary:
−2LL= 288.31, Model LL= 22.10, McFadden= .07, CoxSnell= .05, Nagelkerke= .10

Predictor b (SE b) 95% CI z p
 Intercept −2.59(0.61) [−3.79,−1.39] 0.20 <.001
 5-HTTLPR1 0.30(0.36) [−36.04, −0.39] −2.01 .05
 Sad Bias −0.004(0.003) [−0.01, 0.002] −1.24 .21
 5-HTTLPR x Sad Bias 0.01(0.008) [−0.005, 0.03] 1.31 .19
 MASC 0.008(0.01) [−0.02, 0.04] 0.60 .55
 Past MDD 1.37(0.31) [0.76,1.98] 4.38 <.001
Happy Emotion

Logistic Regression Model Summary:
−2LL= 294.12, Model LL= 20.42, McFadden= .06, CoxSnell= .05, Nagelkerke= .09

Predictor b (SE b) 95% CI z p
 Intercept −2.52(0.60) [−3.70, −1.34] −4.18 <.001
 5-HTTLPR 0.28(0.36) [−0.41, 0.98] 0.80 .43
 Happy Bias −0.0001(0.003) [−0.007, 0.006] −0.03 .97
 5-HTTLPR x Happy Bias 0.009(0.007) [−0.005, 0.02] 1.22 .22
 MASC 0.008(0.01) [−0.02, 0.03] 0.55 .58
 Past MDD 1.30(0.31) [0.70 1.90] 4.23 <.001

Note. MASC =Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children; Past MDD= History of clinical depression as measured at the baseline assessment by using a best estimate of parent and child K-SADS interviews.

1

Did not remain significant once removing nonsignificant higher order interaction.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Interaction between 5-HTTLPR and bias away from angry faces predicting depression onset across 18 months using observed attention bias scores within the study’s sample. Note. Negative attention bias scores indicate attention away from anger while positive scores indicate attention toward anger. Johnson-Neyman region of significance testing indicates the value of attention bias at and below (i.e., ≤ −50 ms) which the effect of 5-HTTLPR on depression onset is significant.

No significant interaction between attention bias to sad or happy faces and 5-HTTLPR predicting depression group status was found (Table 3). Non-significant interactions were removed from each regression model and no significant main effect of 5-HTTLPR or attention bias remained within sad or happy analyses (ps>.38). As covariates, previous history of clinical depression was significantly associated with depression onset across 18-months among all analyses, while baseline anxiety symptoms were not significantly associated with prospective depression onset (Table 3).

Sensitivity analyses

Analyses revealed a continued significant interaction between attention bias to anger and 5-HTTLPR (b=−.03, SE=.009, p=.002) once controlling youths’ biological sex and age. Additionally, we re-ran significant models only within Caucasian participants to further address concerns regarding population stratification (Jorm and Easteal, 2000). Again, findings showed the attention bias to anger by 5-HTTLPR interaction was comparable to the whole sample and remained significant at a trend level (b=−.02, SE=.01, p=.07). Finally, the attention bias to anger by 5-HTTLPR interaction remained significant once removing youth with a history of depression, therefore predicting first onsets of depression (b=− .03, SE=01, p=03).

Discussion

The current study investigated whether biased attention to emotion was moderated by 5-HTTLPR to predict clinical depression over time. Results supported hypotheses and demonstrated that youth who attended away from angry faces and were homozygous for the S allele of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism were more likely to experience a depressive episode onset across 18-months. Findings were robust to sensitivity analyses controlling for sex, age, ethnicity, anxiety symptoms, and examining only first onset of depression.

The current study is the first to examine prospective risk for depression conferred by attention bias to emotion and genetic risk. This is an important advancement as attention bias to emotion has been theorized to function as an intermediate trait associated with 5-HTTLPR and depression, but no previous research has tested this model within a single investigation. Our findings can be interpreted within the broader attention bias literature. Evidence suggests that individuals in a current negative mood exhibit mood congruent preferential attention to negative emotion (Hankin et al., 2010; Joormann and Gotlib, 2007) whereas the present study and other prospective investigations among youth (Gibb et al., 2009; Price et al., 2015) show attention away from negative emotion among SS carriers to be associated with prospective depression symptoms and clinical onset. This leads to the question: Why would attention away from negative emotion function as a vulnerability factor for depression among youth? Attention away from negative emotion, particularly an interpersonally threatening emotion like anger, may be associated with maladaptive responses to interpersonal stress. Research shows that interpersonal difficulties and subsequent maladaptive responses (i.e., ineffective problem solving, disengagement) are prospectively related to depression in adolescence (Flynn and Rudolph, 2010). Therefore, transactional processes between interpersonal stress and attentional away from negative emotion may contribute to depression onset, whereas mood congruent attention bias, such as attention toward sad faces, may manifest once the individual is currently experiencing a depressive episode.

Of note, while our findings showed specificity to attention away from angry faces when prospectively predicting clinical depression, findings from Gibb et al. (2009) and Price et al. (2015) were specific to sad and fearful faces, respectively. As Gibb et al. examined subclinical levels of depression among youth and Price et al. did not examine other emotional biases, it is possible the discrepancy in emotion specificity is related to methodological differences in sample characteristics and measures. Irrespective of emotion type, attention away from as opposed to towards negatively valenced stimuli among youth is an important distinction that may have clinical implications regarding attention bias modification as a treatment target for depression. In order to more clearly understand the role of attention bias specificity in the development of depression, future research must investigate attention bias pre- to post-onset of depression and possible ecologically relevant consequences of attentional bias patterns.

Furthermore, the current study’s findings highlight the importance of examining risk for depression across multiple levels, such as combined genetic and cognitive risk (Hankin, 2012). Our results suggest that attention away from negative emotion alone did not predict future onsets of depression; rather, only SS carriers who exhibited attention away from anger were at greater risk for prospective depression onset. The S allele of 5-HTTLPR has been associated with emotional reactivity (Munafò et al., 2008) and cortisol stress reactivity (Hankin et al., 2015a), so it is possible that those who are both more reactive to their environment and engage in maladaptive coping strategies, such as attending away from negative emotions, are at highest risk for experiencing depression over time. As the efficacy of attention bias modification in depressed adults is mixed (Hallion and Ruscio, 2011), the present study’s findings suggest targeting avoidance of threat among those with emotion regulation deficits observed among S allele carriers of 5-HTTLPR may improve treatment success.

Strengths of the present study include being the first to examine an integrated, prospective model examining genetic risk and attention bias to predict prospective clinical depression among either adults or youth. We employed a multi-informant, well-validated diagnostic interview to evaluate depression diagnoses using best estimate procedures over 18-months as opposed to cross-sectional or symptom level measurement of depression. A theory-based candidate gene approach versus atheoretical GWAS methodology allows for a more sensitive and powerful test of genetic effects (Caspi et al., 2010). Finally, the use of a community sample of youth provided generalizable results compared to clinically selected samples (Cohen and Cohen, 1984).

Limitations of the current study provide avenues for future research. While the current study’s measure of attention bias is more objective compared to self-report data, there are other, more precise tools available, such as eye-tracking methodology that allows for assessment of the time-course of attention bias. Finally, although 5-HTTLPR genotype status may relate to emotion dysregulation and stress reactivity, further studies should utilize self-report and objective measures (i.e., laboratory stressors) to investigate whether emotion regulation ability moderates or mediates the relationship between 5-HTTLPR, attention bias, and depression.

In sum, the current study demonstrated that youth who attended away from angry faces and were homozygous for the S allele of 5-HTTLPR were at higher risk for experiencing an episode of clinical depression over time. This finding may inform future depression intervention research involving modification of attention bias to emotion.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all of the participants who have given their time to participate in our research. We would like to thank Dr. Andrew Smolen and the Institute for Behavioral Genetics for their assistance in the collection and analysis of genetic data. We would like to thank all of our research staff at the University of Denver and Rutgers University for their time and assistance in data collection.

Role of Funding Sources

This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants 5R01MH077195 and 5R01MH077178, awarded to Benjamin L. Hankin and Jami F. Young and 1F31MH097367 awarded to Jessica L. Jenness as well as by National Institute of Child Health and Human Development T32 HD057822-06. The funding sources had no role in the study design with the exception of reviewer feedback from NIMH pre-doctora training grant 1F31MH097367. The funding sources had no role in the collection, analysis, or interpretation of the data; in writing the report; or in the decision to submit the manuscript.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

1

A similar pattern of findings emerged when predicting depression symptom count (b=−.01, SE=.006, p=.08) and severity (b=−.05, SE=.03, p=.07) among SS carriers who attend away from angry faces.

Contributors

Jessica L. Jenness developed the study concept. All authors contributed to the study design. Data collection was performed in part by Jessica L. Jenness. Jessica L. Jenness performed the data analysis and interpretation under the supervision of Benjamin L. Hankin. Jessica L. Jenness drafted the paper, and Benjamin L. Hankin and Jami F. Young provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the paper for submission.

Conflicts of Interest: None

References

  1. Aiken LS, West SG. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Sage; Newbury Park, London: 1991. [Google Scholar]
  2. Avenevoli S, Knight E, Kessler RC, Merikangas KR. Epidemiology of depression in children and adolescents. Handb Depress Child Adolesc. 2008:6–32. [Google Scholar]
  3. Baldwin JS, Dadds MR. Reliability and validity of parent and child versions of the multidimensional anxiety scale for children in community samples. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46:252–260. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000246065.93200.a1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bauer DJ, Curran PJ. Probing interactions in fixed and multilevel regression: Inferential and graphical techniques. Multivar Behav Res. 2005;40:373–400. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr4003_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Beck A. The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:969–977. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Caspi A, Hariri AR, Holmes A, Uher R, Moffitt TE. Genetic sensitivity to the environment: the case of the serotonin transporter gene and its implications for studying complex diseases and traits. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167:509–527. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09101452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cohen P, Cohen J. The clinician’s illusion. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1984;41:1178–1182. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1984.01790230064010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Connor-Smith JK, Compas BE, Wadsworth ME, Thomsen AH, Saltzman H. Responses to stress in adolescence: Measurement of coping and involuntary stress responses. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000;68:976–992. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.6.976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Flynn M, Rudolph KD. The contribution of deficits in emotional clarity to stress responses and depression. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2010;31:291–297. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2010.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gibb BE, Beevers CG, McGeary JE. Toward an integration of cognitive and genetic models of risk for depression. Cogn Emot. 2013;27:193–216. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.712950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gibb BE, Benas JS, Grassia M, McGeary J. Children’s attentional biases and 5-HTTLPR genotype: potential mechanisms linking mother and child depression. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2009;38:415–426. doi: 10.1080/15374410902851705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gotlib IH, Kasch KL, Traill S, Joormann J, Arnow BA, Johnson SL. Coherence and specificity of information-processing biases in depression and social phobia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2004;113:386. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.3.386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gotlib IH, Neubauer DL. Information-processing approaches to the study of cognitive biases in depression. Stress Coping Depress. 2000:117–143. [Google Scholar]
  14. Gross JJ, Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations 2007 [Google Scholar]
  15. Hallion LS, Ruscio AM. A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychol Bull. 2011;137:940. doi: 10.1037/a0024355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hankin BL. Future directions in vulnerability to depression among youth: Integrating risk factors and processes across multiple levels of analysis. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2012;41:695–718. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.711708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hankin BL, Abramson LY, Moffitt TE, Silva PA, McGee R, Angell KE. Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal study. J Abnorm Psychol. 1998;107:128. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.107.1.128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hankin BL, Badanes LS, Smolen A, Young JF. Cortisol reactivity to stress among youth: stability over time and genetic variants for stress sensitivity. J Abnorm Psychol. 2015a;124:54–67. doi: 10.1037/abn0000030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hankin BL, Gibb BE, Abela JR, Flory K. Selective attention to affective stimuli and clinical depression among youths: role of anxiety and specificity of emotion. J Abnorm Psychol. 2010;119:491. doi: 10.1037/a0019609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hankin BL, Young JF, Abela JR, Smolen A, Jenness JL, Gulley LD, Technow JR, Gottlieb AB, Cohen JR, Oppenheimer CW. Depression from childhood into late adolescence: Influence of gender, development, genetic susceptibility, and peer stress. J Abnorm Psychol. 2015b;124:803. doi: 10.1037/abn0000089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hariri AR, Holmes A. Genetics of emotional regulation: the role of the serotonin transporter in neural function. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10:182–191. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Harrison AJ, Gibb BE. Attentional Biases in Currently Depressed Children: An Eye-Tracking Study of Biases in Sustained Attention to Emotional Stimuli. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol Off J Soc Clin Child Adolesc Psychol Am Psychol Assoc Div. 2014;53:1–7. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.930688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach, Methodology in the social sciences. Guilford Press; New York, NY, US: 2013. [Google Scholar]
  24. Heinz A, Braus DF, Smolka MN, Wrase J, Puls I, Hermann D, Klein S, Grüsser SM, Flor H, Schumann G, et al. Amygdala-prefrontal coupling depends on a genetic variation of the serotonin transporter. Nat Neurosci. 2004;8:20–21. doi: 10.1038/nn1366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Jenness JL, Hankin BL, Young JF, Smolen A. Stressful Life Events Moderate the Relationship Between Genes and Biased Attention to Emotional Faces in Youth. Clin Psychol Sci. 2015:2167702615601000. doi: 10.1177/2167702615601000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Joormann J, Gotlib IH. Selective attention to emotional faces following recovery from depression. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007;116:80. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Joormann J, Talbot L, Gotlib IH. Biased processing of emotional information in girls at risk for depression. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007;116:135. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Jorm AF, Easteal S. Assessing candidate genes as risk factors for mental disorders: the value of population-based epidemiological studies. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2000;35:1–4. doi: 10.1007/s001270050001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao UMA, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, Ryan N. Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:980–988. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, Benjamin J, Müller CR, Hamer DH, Murphy DL. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science. 1996;274:1527–1531. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5292.1527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. MacLeod C, Mathews A, Tata P. Attentional bias in emotional disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 1986;95:15. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.95.1.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. March JS, Parker JDA, Sullivan K, Stallings P, Conners CK. The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC): factor structure, reliability, and validity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:554–565. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199704000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mathews A, MacLeod C. Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005;1:167–195. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Merikangas KR, Nakamura EF, Kessler RC. Epidemiology of mental disorders in children and adolescents. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2009;11:7–20. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2009.11.1/krmerikangas. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Mogg K, Bradley BP. Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cogn Ther Res. 2005;29:29–45. [Google Scholar]
  36. Mogg K, Bradley BP, Williams R. Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: The role of awareness. Br J Clin Psychol. 1995;34:17–36. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1995.tb01434.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Munafò MR, Brown SM, Hariri AR. Serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotype and amygdala activation: a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:852–857. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.08.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Peckham AD, McHugh RK, Otto MW. A meta-analysis of the magnitude of biased attention in depression. Depress Anxiety. 2010;27:1135–1142. doi: 10.1002/da.20755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Pergamin-Hight L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, Bar-Haim Y. Variations in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene and biased attention for emotional information: a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71:373–379. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Price RB, Rosen D, Siegle GJ, Ladouceur CD, Tang K, Allen KB, Ryan ND, Dahl RE, Forbes EE, Silk JS. From Anxious Youth to Depressed Adolescents: Prospective Prediction of 2-Year Depression Symptoms via Attentional Bias Measures. 2015 doi: 10.1037/abn0000127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rutter M, Kim-Cohen J, Maughan B. Continuities and discontinuities in psychopathology between childhood and adult life. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:276–295. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01614.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Seiffge-Krenke I, Klessinger N. Long-term effects of avoidant coping on adolescents’ depressive symptoms. J Youth Adolesc. 2000;29:617–630. [Google Scholar]
  43. Sen S, Burmeister M, Ghosh D. Meta-analysis of the association between a serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and anxiety-related personality traits. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2004;127:85–89. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Silverman WK, Ollendick TH. Evidence-Based Assessment of Anxiety and Its Disorders in Children and Adolescents. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2005;34:380–411. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3403_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, Marcus DJ, Westerlund A, Casey BJ, Nelson C. The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. 2009;168:242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Wendland JR, Martin BJ, Kruse MR, Lesch KP, Murphy DL. Simultaneous genotyping of four functional loci of human SLC6A4, with a reappraisal of 5-HTTLPR and rs25531. Mol Psychiatry. 2006 doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Whisman MA, Richardson ED, Smolen A. Behavioral inhibition and triallelic genotyping of the serotonin transporter promoter (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism. J Res Personal. 2011;45:706–709. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES