Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Eur Psychiatry. 2017 Feb 9;43:9–13. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.01.327

Coping as a mediator of stress and psychotic-like experiences

Arielle Ered 1, Lauren E Gibson 1, Seth D Maxwell 1, Shanna Cooper 1, Lauren M Ellman 1
PMCID: PMC5474127  NIHMSID: NIHMS848632  PMID: 28365470

Abstract

Background

There is evidence that individuals along the whole psychosis continuum have increased responsiveness to stress; however, coping responses to stressors have not been extensively explored in subthreshold psychotic symptoms.

Methods

In 454 undergraduates, psychotic-like experiences (PLEs) were evaluated using the positive items of the Prodromal Questionnaire. Perceived stress and traumatic life events were assessed using the Life Events Checklist and Perceived Stress Scale, and coping was measured using the Brief COPE. We also examined whether different coping styles mediated the relationship between perceived stress and PLEs, as well as whether different coping styles mediated the relationship between traumatic life events and PLEs.

Results

Both number of traumatic life events and current level of perceived stress were significantly associated with PLEs. These relationships were both mediated by higher levels of maladaptive coping.

Conclusions

Results have the potential to inform treatment strategies, as well as inform targets for exploration in longitudinal studies of those at risk for psychosis.

Keywords: psychosis, psychosocial stress, coping, trauma, psychotic-like experiences

1.1 Introduction

Psychosocial stress has been found to be a risk factor for various mental disorders, including psychotic disorders (1). There is evidence that individuals at greater risk for developing psychosis are more likely to have experienced traumatic life events in childhood (2), as well as to perceive events to be more stressful (3). Indeed, both cross-sectional studies and prospective studies suggest that childhood traumatic life events have a dose-dependent link to psychotic symptoms (4, 5). There also is some evidence that increased perceived stress may contribute to this relationship in those at risk for psychosis (6) and those exhibiting subthreshold psychotic symptoms (3, 7). Though perceived stress and traumatic life events have long been studied in relation to psychosis risk (4, 8, 9), few studies have explored the potential contributions of coping to this association.

Coping is an action-oriented or intrapsychic effort to manage, master, tolerate, reduce, or minimize stressful events or a stressful environment (10). Categorizing coping responses is complex and varies with the measure used. Folkman and Lazarus (11) described coping strategies as either problem-focused (also called task-focused): attempting to change the individual’s circumstances, or emotion-focused: attempting to change the individual’s response to the circumstances. Many questionnaire-based assessments of coping rely on the distinction between approach and avoidance coping (12); however, categorizing avoidance as a coping style is problematic, as it can also be viewed in certain instances as a failure to cope but still indicates an acknowledgment and a type of response to a stressor (13). A more common approach in psychosis studies utilizes a distinction between adaptive and maladaptive coping styles, which incorporates many of the previous definitions of coping (1517). Drug and alcohol use, self-blame, and denial fall into the maladaptive category, which are also captured by avoidance coping, while the adaptive category includes approach coping styles such as active coping, planning, and the use of emotional and instrumental support (12, 15). Studies have found that schizophrenia outpatients employ maladaptive coping styles significantly more often than non-psychiatric counterparts (16). Specifically, individuals with schizophrenia have been found to employ more emotion-focused coping and less task-focused coping (18, 19), significantly more distraction-based coping and worrying, as well as significantly less emotional expression and comforting cognition (e.g., self-encouragement and soothing thoughts) than non-psychiatric controls (20). Cumulatively, these findings suggest that schizophrenia patients rely on coping strategies that are either maladaptive and/or have the potential of exacerbating distress.

Few studies have been conducted on coping among individuals at ultra-high-risk (UHR) for developing psychosis, but in these few studies, UHR subjects were found to cope in similar ways to patients diagnosed with schizophrenia. UHR subjects used significantly fewer task-oriented and social diversion (i.e., engaging with others) coping methods and engaged in far more emotion-oriented coping compared to a non-clinical comparison group (21). Another study found that not only did UHR subjects use active coping styles less frequently than non-psychiatric controls, but also used active coping strategies significantly less than first episode schizophrenia patients (22). Additionally, several studies found that UHR individuals tend to engage in less adaptive coping and more maladaptive coping than non-psychiatric controls (17, 22, 23).

While only the most frequent and distressing psychotic symptoms are considered diagnostically relevant (24), limiting inclusion to only those individuals with diagnosable symptoms may in fact underrepresent the contribution of subthreshold psychotic symptoms to the liability for psychotic disorders (25). PLEs have been linked to risk for developing a psychotic disorder in the general population (24). Additionally, the risk factors for subclinical and clinical psychosis overlap significantly (26). Only one study has examined coping in the context of a continuum of psychosis, using subthreshold psychotic experiences as a spectrum of psychotic risk. Lin and colleagues (27) found that emotion-focused coping was bi-directionally related to increased experience of subthreshold psychotic symptoms in a longitudinal study of a non-clinical sample of adolescents, such that more emotion-focused coping predicted increased PLEs, and increased PLEs predicted higher levels of emotion-focused coping. However, this study did not take into account perceived stress or trauma as additional variables that may affect symptoms, TLEs and perceived stress may actually be driving these relationships, as we have previously found both factors to influence PLEs (7). The aim of the present study was to determine the role of different coping strategies (adaptive/maladaptive) in mediating the relationship between TLEs and PLEs, and perceived stress and PLEs. We hypothesized that experiencing a greater number of TLEs and higher levels of perceived stress will be associated with significantly higher PLEs, as found in our previous studies (7). Additionally, we hypothesized that these relationships will be mediated by the use of maladaptive coping styles, but not adaptive coping styles. While our primary hypotheses focus on mediation, moderation will also be tested.

2.1 Methods

2.1.1 Participants and procedures

Four hundred and fifty four undergraduate students at Temple University participated and were recruited from an online subject pool as a requirement from various interdisciplinary courses. Questionnaires were completed online in the laboratory, with lab staff available to provide instructions and answer questions. The study was approved by the university’s Institutional Review Board and all participants provided informed consent.

2.1.2 Instruments

PLEs were evaluated using the positive scale (45 items) of the full length, 92-item Prodromal Questionnaire (28). Focusing on the last month, individuals are asked whether they have experienced symptoms while not under the influence of drugs, alcohol, or medications. The variable of interest was the total number of PLEs endorsed. Endorsing 8 or more PLEs has been validated against the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes (SIPS) in predicting psychosis risk syndromes with 90% sensitivity and 49% specificity (28, 29).

The Perceived Stress Scale (30) was used to evaluate perceived stress among participants. The scale measures perceived global stress, with a focus on the predictability and controllability of events in the past month (30). This scale has high concurrent and predictive validity with physical and psychiatric outcomes, moderate internal and test-retest reliability, and significant correlations with physiological measurements of stress (3133). Significant differences in PSS scores have been found in ultra-high risk for psychosis groups vs. non-psychiatric controls, and has been correlated with additional perceived stress measurements, such as experience sampling methods (34, 35). The PSS sum score was used.

The Life Events Checklist (LEC) assessed traumatic life event (TLE) exposure (36). For each life event listed, subjects respond if the TLE: 1 (happened to me), 2 (witnessed it), 3 (learned about it), 4 (not sure) 5 (does not apply). Responses of 3, 4 and 5 were excluded, consistent with previous studies and better test–retest reliability, as more proximal events are more closely associated with PTSD risk (36, 37). Responses of “1” for the first 16 TLEs were included as well as responses of “2” for scenarios where “1” was not a viable option or less likely to be related to PTSD outcome, e.g., sudden, violent death; sudden, unexpected death of someone close to you; and serious injury, harm, or death you caused to someone else (38, See Table 1). The “other” TLEs item was excluded from analyses, as additional information about the TLEs was not available, and thus has not been validated. The LEC has been shown to be adequate when evaluating consistency with the actual occurrence of events, has demonstrated good convergent validity, and has moderate temporal stability (36). Total number of TLEs was examined.

Table 1.

Life Events Checklist items

Life Events Checklist
1. Natural disaster
2. Fire or explosion
3. Transportation accident
4. Serious accident at work, home, or during recreational activity
5. Exposure to toxic substance
6. Physical assault
7. Assault with a weapon
8. Sexual assault
9. Other unwanted or uncomfortable sexual experience
10. Combat or exposure to a war-zone (in the military or as a civilian)
11. Captivity
12. Life-threatening illness or injury
13. Severe human suffering
14. Sudden violent death
15. Sudden accidental death
16. Serious injury, harm, or death you caused to someone else
17. Other very stressful event or experience

Coping was assessed using the Brief COPE, a shortened version of the COPE questionnaire, which has been validated previously in non-psychiatric samples (39). Items were separated into seven types of coping comprised of two items each, each representing different ways of coping with stressful experiences utilized overall in the past six-month period. Two subscales were coded as targets for analysis: adaptive and maladaptive styles of coping, which have been previously used in the psychosis and UHR literature (15, 17). Adaptive coping included active coping, planning, use of emotional support, use of instrumental support, positive reframing, religion, and humor items. Maladaptive coping included venting, denial, substance use, behavioral disengagement, self-distraction, and self-blame items (see Table 2). Overall coping with daily stressors was the target of the measure.

Table 2.

Brief COPE items for adaptive and maladaptive coping

Adaptive Coping Maladaptive Coping
Active Coping Venting
Planning Denial
Use of Emotional Support Substance Use
Use of Instrumental Support Behavioral Disengagement
Positive Reframing Self-distraction
Acceptance Self-blame
Religion
Humor

2.1.3 Data Analysis

First, the PLEs variable was examined for normality by examining skewness and kurtosis values and through visual inspection of the data. Second, bivariate analyses using Pearsons correlations were conducted to determine 1. Whether the main independent variables (LEC and PSS) were associated with both the potential mediators (coping variables) and dependent variable (PLEs) and 2. Whether the potential mediators (coping variables) were associated with the main dependent variable (PLEs) (40). Only variables that met conditions 1 and 2 were examined in mediation models. Age and gender were tested at potential covariates by determining if they were associated with the main independent and dependent variables. Hayes’ (41) PROCESS macro for SPSS was used for mediation analyses. The indirect effect was tested using a bootstrap estimation approach with 5000 samples. Mediation and moderation were conducted on total number of PLEs continuously. Significant mediation was determined by the 95% CI not including zero, as p values are not associated with this test (42).

3.1 Results

Demographic characteristics of the sample are presented in Table 3. No significant differences were found relating to gender (p= .10–.52), and age related only to number of TLEs (p= .05), but no other factors (p= .11–.65); therefore, these variables were not controlled for in analyses. Bivariate correlations were found to be significant between number of PLEs endorsed and perceived stress (r= .49, p< .01), total number of TLEs (r= .28, p< .01), use of adaptive coping styles (r= .11, p= .02), and use of maladaptive coping styles (r= .48, p< .01). Maladaptive coping was significantly related to total number of TLEs (r= .21, p< .01) and perceived stress (r= .57, p< .01). Adaptive coping was significantly related to TLEs (r= .11, p= .03), but not to perceived stress (r= −.05, p= .25).

Table 3.

Demographics and clinical characteristics

Overall sample (n=454)

Male, n (%) 103 (23%)

Age (years), mean (SD) [range] 20.04 (2.31) [18–34]

Ethnicity, Hispanic n (%) 38 (8%)

Race n (%)
 American Indian/Alaska Native 2 (1%)
 Asian 78 (17%)
 Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander 3 (1%)
 Black/African American 86 (19%)
 White 261 (57%)
 Biracial 24 (5%)

Total APPS score, mean (SD) [range] 9.82 (7.56) [0–44]

Total distressing APPS score, mean (SD) [range] 4.17 (5.20) [0–28]

Total PSS score, mean (SD) [range] 25.33 (8.64) [2–55]

Total number of traumas endorsed, mean (SD) [range] 1.98 (1.78) [0–10]

Adaptive Coping, mean (SD) [range] 4.71 (1.14) [2–7.75]

Maladaptive Coping, mean (SD) [range] 3.46 (0.90) [2–6.5]

As Table 4 indicates, indirect bootstrapping results indicated that maladaptive coping mediated the relationship between perceived stress and PLEs, as well as the relationship between number of TLEs and PLEs. Adaptive coping did not mediate the relationship between TLEs and PLEs, as indicated by the CI including zero (See Table 4).

Table 4.

Indirect effect of stress and attenuated positive psychotic symptoms (APPS) through coping

Number of attenuated positive psychotic symptoms (APPS) endorsed (n=454)
IV Mediator Effect SE LLCI ULCI
Number of TLEs Maladaptive Coping .3809 .1054 .1903 .6092
Perceived Stress Maladaptive Coping .1454 .0291 .0901 .2046
Number of TLEs Adaptive Coping .0363 .0274 −.0003 .1135

Maladaptive coping did not moderate the relationship between PLEs and TLEs [F(1,454)= .22, p= .64] or perceived stress [F(1,454)= .02, p= .88]. Similarly, adaptive coping also did not moderate the relationship between PLEs and TLEs [F(1,454)= .26, p= .61] or perceived stress [F(1,454)= 1.51, p= .22].

4.1 Discussion

This is the first study, to our knowledge, to determine that the relationship between stress (TLEs and perceived stress) and PLEs was mediated by increases in maladaptive coping. The significant mediation models indicate that while trauma exposure and perceived stress are related to PLEs, it is the use of maladaptive coping styles that may be driving greater risk for psychotic symptomology. Additionally, neither adaptive nor maladaptive coping moderated the relationship between stress (TLEs and perceived stress) and PLEs. These findings are consistent with previous studies that have found individuals experiencing positive psychotic symptomology to be more likely to use maladaptive coping styles (17, 22, 23, 27). However, the present study is the first to directly test whether coping mediates the previously found relationships between stress, traumatic life events in those experiencing subthreshold psychotic symptoms. These relationships should be examined in other samples with active psychotic symptoms, including UHR individuals and schizophrenia patients, to confirm findings.

Contrary to hypotheses, increases in adaptive coping were significantly associated with increases in PLEs and increases in TLEs, although these associations were weak, both only accounting for .01% of the variance. While higher levels of adaptive coping were related to both TLEs and PLEs, this form of coping did not mediate the relationship between these two variables. These findings suggest that some individuals use adaptive coping strategies in conjunction with both TLEs and PLEs; however, these strategies do not appear to contribute to the TLEs-PLEs relationship. Our findings suggest that PLEs may act as stressful events to be coped with, leading to increases in adaptive coping following these experiences. Further, perceived stress and adaptive coping were not related, indicating that adaptive coping likely does not play a role in the relation between perceived stress and PLEs.

It is possible that what has previously been seen as greater perceived stress in individuals at risk for psychosis may actually indicate a decreased ability to activate or mobilize coping strategies. This could be due to either an inability to cope, poor forms of coping, or cognitive deficits leading individuals to misidentify stressful situations (e.g., evaluate neutral situations as stressful). Failure to properly cope with stressful life events due to increases in perceived stress may contribute to or magnify the salience of stressors. Thus, the perception of a stressor becomes critical (12, 22). Events are filtered through the appraisals an individual applies to them (30) and are coped with based on that appraisal. If an objectively low-stress event appraised as high-stress, the individual may fail to cope, or employ a less effective coping strategy, perhaps due to a perceived lack of capacity to cope.

In this study, we confirm our previous findings connecting TLEs and perceived stress to subthreshold positive psychotic symptoms (7), and extend them to include coping style. Our sample is unlike most in ultra-high risk for psychosis research in that it is large and non-treatment-seeking, affording us more external validity for the general population. Though we examine an undergraduate population, which may lower generalizability, our university’s population is quite large and diverse both demographically and socioeconomically, and is likely a valid subset of their same-age peers from the population at large.

A limitation to consider is the cross-sectional nature of the study, which relied on self-report, as well as retrospection (e.g., Life Events Checklist). This limits conclusions we can draw regarding directionality and causality of the relationships presented. Additionally, while no gender differences were found, it is worth noting that there were far more women than men in this sample. Although the amount of men was large enough to detect any potential differences, future studies should aim to have an even gender distribution. Further, while we assessed the primary coping strategies used in previous psychosis studies, evidence suggest that coping strategies may be more or less effective depending on the circumstance or context in which it is used (14). Future studies are necessary to examine real-time coping strategies among in psychosis spectrum populations, to further clarify our findings. Finally, though our non-clinical sample increases generalizability to the general population, it limits generalizability to clinical samples, and may contain more false-positives than would exist in other UHR samples (43).

Future studies should assess stress appraisals of individual TLEs, as well as which coping style is used in response to each individual TLE, rather than an estimated general coping style in individuals experiencing subthreshold positive psychotic symptoms. Additionally, other potentially relevant variables such as resilience should be examined, as resilience has been found to relate closely to coping (44).

4.1.1. Conclusions

Our findings have potentially important clinical implications for treatment, as treatments for psychosis could work to increase the use of adaptive coping styles, such as planning and positive reframing, and attempt to reduce maladaptive coping styles. Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) may be beneficial in those experiencing PLEs, especially when the treatment includes a component on teaching of coping strategies.

Acknowledgments

Funding

This work was supported by CLA Research Award, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA (L.M.E.), a start up grant awarded to L.M.E., and the National Institute of Mental Health (MH096478, Principal Investigator L.M.E.).

Biographies

graphic file with name nihms848632b1.gif

Arielle Ered is a second year graduate student in the clinical psychology and neuroscience doctoral programs at Temple University. Arielle received her BA from UCLA and worked in UCLA’s department of psychiatry prior to starting at Temple, investigating recent-onset schizophrenia. Arielle’s research interests include stress and emotion-cognition interactions in early and subthreshold psychosis.

graphic file with name nihms848632b2.gif

Lauren E. Gibson, Ed.M., M.A. is a sixth year graduate student in the clinical program at Temple University completing her internship at the Maryland VA Health Care System. Before starting at Temple, she worked in the psychiatry department of Boston’s Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and Harvard Medical School investigating schizophrenia spectrum disorders, particularly the early risk phase. She received her bachelor’s degree from Ursinus College in psychology and Spanish, and master’s degree from Harvard University in human development and psychology. Lauren is interested in the influence trauma has on major psychopathology, as well as risk factors for early psychosis.

graphic file with name nihms848632b3.gif

Seth D. Maxwell is the Research Coordinator for Dr. Lauren Ellman’s lab in the Department of Psychology at Temple University, where he contributes to studies of risk factors for major psychopathology. He is pursuing a Master’s in Public Health with a concentration in Applied Biostatistics at Temple’s College of Public Health.

graphic file with name nihms848632b4.gif

Shanna Cooper, M.A. is currently a fifth year doctoral candidate in the clinical psychology and neuroscience programs at Temple University. Prior to her graduate work, she worked on a number of projects investigating schizophrenia spectrum disorders at the San Francisco VA and the University of California, San Francisco. She received her bachelor’s degree from the University of Minnesota and her first master’s degree from San Francisco State University. Shanna incorporates behavioral, cognitive, and neuroimaging methods to explore the relations between cognition and emotion in those at risk for a psychotic disorder.

graphic file with name nihms848632b5.gif

Lauren M. Ellman, Ph.D. is an Associate Professor in the Clinical area of the Psychology department at Temple University. Generally, Dr. Ellman’s research focuses on two vulnerable periods of development in the prediction of schizophrenia and related disorders, the prenatal period and adolescence/young adulthood. This research is aimed at determining risk factors for schizophrenia in order to ultimately identify those who are vulnerable to developing the disorder and intervene at early stages to prevent the onset of serious psychiatric symptoms.

Footnotes

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Nuechterlein KH, Dawson ME. A heuristic vulnerability/stress model of schizophrenic episodes. Schizophrenia bulletin. 1984;10(2):300. doi: 10.1093/schbul/10.2.300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Addington J, Heinssen R. Prediction and prevention of psychosis in youth at clinical high risk. Annual review of clinical psychology. 2012;8:269–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Myin-Germeys I, van Os J. Stress-reactivity in psychosis: evidence for an affective pathway to psychosis. Clinical psychology review. 2007;27(4):409–24. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Read J, Os Jv, Morrison A, Ross CA. Childhood trauma, psychosis and schizophrenia: a literature review with theoretical and clinical implications. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2005;112(5):330–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00634.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Varese F, Smeets F, Drukker M, Lieverse R, Lataster T, Viechtbauer W, et al. Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: a meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective-and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophrenia bulletin. 2012;38(4):661–71. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lardinois M, Lataster T, Mengelers R, Van Os J, Myin-Germeys I. Childhood trauma and increased stress sensitivity in psychosis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2011;123(1):28–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01594.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Gibson LE, Anglin DM, Klugman JT, Reeves LE, Fineberg AM, Maxwell SD, et al. Stress sensitivity mediates the relationship between traumatic life events and attenuated positive psychotic symptoms differentially by gender in a college population sample. Journal of psychiatric research. 2014;53:111–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Van Winkel R, Stefanis NC, Myin-Germeys I. Psychosocial stress and psychosis. A review of the neurobiological mechanisms and the evidence for gene-stress interaction. Schizophrenia bulletin. 2008;34(6):1095–105. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Van Os J. Behavioural sensitization to daily life stress in psychosis. Psychological medicine. 2005;35(05):733–41. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704004179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Taylor SE, Stanton AL. Coping resources, coping processes, and mental health. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2007;3:377–401. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Folkman S, Lazarus RS. If it changes it must be a process: study of emotion and coping during three stages of a college examination. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1985;48(1):150. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.48.1.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ventura J, Nuechterlein KH, Subotnik KL, Green MF, Gitlin MJ. Self-efficacy and neurocognition may be related to coping responses in recent-onset schizophrenia. Schizophrenia research. 2004;69(2):343–52. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Holahan CJ, Moos RH. Personal and contextual determinants of coping strategies. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1987;52(5):946. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.52.5.946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Moos RH. 2001 INVITED ADDRESS: The Mystery of Human Context and Coping: An Unraveling of Clues. American journal of community psychology. 2002;30(1):67–88. doi: 10.1023/a:1014372101550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Meyer B. Coping with severe mental illness: Relations of the Brief COPE with symptoms, functioning, and well-being. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2001;23(4):265–77. doi: 10.1023/A:1010929402770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Horan WP, Blanchard JJ. Emotional responses to psychosocial stress in schizophrenia: the role of individual differences in affective traits and coping. Schizophrenia research. 2003;60(2):271–83. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(02)00227-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Jalbrzikowski M, Sugar CA, Zinberg J, Bachman P, Cannon TD, Bearden CE. Coping styles of individuals at clinical high risk for developing psychosis. Early intervention in psychiatry. 2014;8(1):68–76. doi: 10.1111/eip.12005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Ritsner MS, Gibel A, Ponizovsky AM, Shinkarenko E, Ratner Y, Kurs R. Coping patterns as a valid presentation of the diversity of coping responses in schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Research. 2006;144(2):139–52. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Lysaker PH, Bryson GJ, Lancaster RS, Evans JD, Bell MD. Insight in schizophrenia: associations with executive function and coping style. Schizophrenia research. 2003;59(1):41–7. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00383-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.van den Bosch RJ, Rombouts R. Coping and cognition in schizophrenia and depression. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 1997;38(6):341–4. doi: 10.1016/s0010-440x(97)90930-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Phillips LJ, Edwards J, McMurray N, Francey S. Comparison of experiences of stress and coping between young people at risk of psychosis and a non-clinical cohort. Behavioural and cognitive psychotherapy. 2012;40(01):69–88. doi: 10.1017/S1352465811000397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Pruessner M, Iyer SN, Faridi K, Joober R, Malla AK. Stress and protective factors in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis, first episode psychosis and healthy controls. Schizophrenia research. 2011;129(1):29–35. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kim KR, Song YY, Park JY, Lee EH, Lee M, Lee SY, et al. The relationship between psychosocial functioning and resilience and negative symptoms in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2013;47(8):762–71. doi: 10.1177/0004867413488218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness–persistence–impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychological medicine. 2009;39(02):179–95. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Reeves LE, Anglin DM, Heimberg RG, Gibson LE, Fineberg AM, Maxwell SD, et al. Anxiety mediates the association between cannabis use and attenuated positive psychotic symptoms. Psychiatry Research. 2014;218(1):180–6. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.03.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.van Os J, Linscott RJ. Introduction: the extended psychosis phenotype—relationship with schizophrenia and with ultrahigh risk status for psychosis. Schizophrenia bulletin. 2012;38(2):227–30. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lin A, Wigman J, Nelson B, Vollebergh W, Van Os J, Baksheev G, et al. The relationship between coping and subclinical psychotic experiences in adolescents from the general population–a longitudinal study. Psychological medicine. 2011;41(12):2535–46. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711000560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Loewy RL, Johnson JK, Cannon TD. Self-report of attenuated psychotic experiences in a college population. Schizophrenia research. 2007;93(1):144–51. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Loewy RL, Bearden CE, Johnson JK, Raine A, Cannon TD. The prodromal questionnaire (PQ): preliminary validation of a self-report screening measure for prodromal and psychotic syndromes. Schizophrenia research. 2005;79(1):117–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of health and social behavior. 1983:385–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Cohen S. Psychosocial models of the role of social support in the etiology of physical disease. Health psychology. 1988;7(3):269. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.7.3.269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hewitt PL, Flett GL, Mosher SW. The Perceived Stress Scale: Factor structure and relation to depression symptoms in a psychiatric sample. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 1992;14(3):247–57. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Cohen S, Tyrrell DA, Smith AP. Negative life events, perceived stress, negative affect, and susceptibility to the common cold. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1993;64(1):131. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.64.1.131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Palmier-Claus J, Dunn G, Lewis S. Emotional and symptomatic reactivity to stress in individuals at ultra-high risk of developing psychosis. Psychological medicine. 2012;42(5):1003. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711001929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Tso IF, Grove TB, Taylor SF. Self-assessment of psychological stress in schizophrenia: preliminary evidence of reliability and validity. Psychiatry Research. 2012;195(1):39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Gray MJ, Litz BT, Hsu JL, Lombardo TW. Psychometric properties of the life events checklist. Assessment. 2004;11(4):330–41. doi: 10.1177/1073191104269954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Elhai JD, Gray MJ, Docherty AR, Kashdan TB, Kose S. Structural validity of the Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist among college students with a trauma history. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2007;22(11):1471–8. doi: 10.1177/0886260507305569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Association AP. APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Carver CS. You want to measure coping but your protocol’ too long: Consider the brief cope. International journal of behavioral medicine. 1997;4(1):92–100. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm0401_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1986;51(6):1173. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hayes AF. PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling. 2012 [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior research methods. 2008;40(3):879–91. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.3.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Cannon TD, Cadenhead K, Cornblatt B, Woods SW, Addington J, Walker E, et al. Prediction of psychosis in youth at high clinical risk: a multisite longitudinal study in North America. Archives of general psychiatry. 2008;65(1):28–37. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Tait L, Birchwood M, Trower P. Adapting to the challenge of psychosis: personal resilience and the use of sealing-over (avoidant) coping strategies. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;185(5):410–5. doi: 10.1192/bjp.185.5.410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES