Abstract
Reduced global motion sensitivity, relative to global static form sensitivity, has been found in children with many neurodevelopmental disorders, leading to the “dorsal stream vulnerability” hypothesis (Braddick et al, Neuropsychologia, 2003). Individual differences in typically developing children’s global motion thresholds have been shown to be associated with variations in specific parietal cortical areas (Braddick et al, J Cog Neuro, 2016). Here, in 125 children aged 5–12 years, we relate individual differences in global motion and form coherence thresholds to fractional anisotropy (FA) in the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), a major fiber tract communicating between parietal lobe and anterior cortical areas. We find a positive correlation between FA of the right SLF and individual children’s sensitivity to global motion coherence, while FA of the left SLF shows a negative correlation. Further analysis of parietal cortical area data shows that this is also asymmetrical, showing a stronger association with global motion sensitivity in the left hemisphere. None of these associations hold for an analogous measure of global form sensitivity. We conclude that a complex pattern of structural asymmetry, including the parietal lobe and the superior longitudinal fasciculus, is specifically linked to the development of sensitivity to global visual motion. This pattern suggests that individual differences in motion sensitivity are primarily linked to parietal brain areas interacting with frontal systems in making decisions on integrated motion signals, rather than in the extra-striate visual areas that perform the initial integration. The basis of motion processing deficits in neurodevelopmental disorders may depend on these same structures.
Keywords: individual differences, visual brain development, global motion sensitivity, global form sensitivity, fractional anisotropy, superior longitudinal fasciculus
1. INTRODUCTION
Recent studies of individual differences in measures of visual performance have proved informative for exploring the structure of visual processes (e.g. Goodbourn et al, 2012; Peterzell, 2016). Further insights may be gained by relating these differences in performance to variations in brain structure. The brains of individual children, while following clear common developmental pathways in terms of regional cortical area, thickness, and white matter development, also show marked individual differences in all these measures (e.g. Brown, 2016). Such variations have been correlated with individual differences in behavioral and cognitive function, for example in executive function (Fjell et al, 2012), anxiety (Newman et al, 2015) or intelligence (Fjell et al, 2015). Using this approach with visual perceptuo-cognitive functions, we can consider what brain structures constrain aspects of visual performance in development.
Here we examine structural correlates of children’s performance in the detection of global visual motion, a function dependent on extra-striate processing in the dorsal cortical stream. Global motion sensitivity develops through middle childhood, more slowly than the analogous measure for global static form, with considerable variation between individual children and a dependence on spatio-temporal parameters (for example, Gunn et al, 2002; Atkinson & Braddick, 2005; Hadad, Maurer & Lewis, 2014; Meier & Giaschi, 2014). We have developed child-friendly tests providing comparable measures of global motion and global form, first testing sensitivity for translational motion (‘The Road in the Snowstorm’; Atkinson et al, 1997; Gunn et al, 2002) and in a subsequent version with rotary motion matching form and motion stimuli more closely (‘Find the Ball in the Grass’; Atkinson & Braddick, 2005). Figure 1 illustrates the form and motion components of the ‘Find the Ball in the Grass’ test).
Global motion sensitivity is specifically impaired, compared to global form, in a range of genetic and acquired developmental disorders, summarized in table 1, leading us to propose the hypothesis of ‘dorsal stream vulnerability’ (Atkinson et al, 1997; Braddick Atkinson & Wattam-Bell, 2003, Braddick & Atkinson, 2011). These disorders show other deficits of visuo-motor function, spatial cognition, and attention, associated with the functions of the dorsal stream (Atkinson & Braddick, 2011; Kravitz et al, 2011). Understanding the brain correlates of global motion sensitivity, therefore, should help us understand the developmental constraints on these functions and suggest structures that may be associated with its vulnerability in developmental disorders.
Table 1.
Disorder group | Origin | Reference |
---|---|---|
young Williams Syndrome children | genetic | Atkinson et al (1997, 2003) |
adult Williams Syndrome | genetic | Atkinson et al (2006) |
autism | possibly genetic | Spencer et at (2002); Simmons et al (2009) |
hemiplegic children | acquired | Gunn et al (2002) |
developmental dyslexia | ? | Hansen et al (2001) |
fragile X | genetic | Kogan et al (2004) |
very preterm born’ | acquired | Atkinson et al, 2007; Taylor et al (2009) |
congenital cataract | acquired? | compare Ellemberg et al, (2002) with Lewis et al (2002) |
strabismic amblyopia | acquired? | Simmers et al (2005) ; Ho et al (2005) |
The opportunity to address these questions comes from an extensive study of a group of children, aged from 5–12 years, who underwent multimodal brain imaging and extensive behavioral assessment, including measurement of global form and motion thresholds, in the PLING study (Pediatric Longitudinal Imaging, Neurocognition & Genetics) (Brown et al, 2012; Fjell et al, 2012; Akshoomoff et al, 2014; Jernigan et al 2016; see http://www.chd.ucsd.edu/research/pling.html) at the University of California, San Diego. We have already reported results from initial analyses on regional measurements of cortical area in this group, which show that good individual performance on a test of global motion sensitivity is associated with relative enlargement of the parietal lobe, especially in the region of the intraparietal sulcus, and relatively smaller area of the occipital lobe (Atkinson et al, 2014; Braddick et al, 2016). Here we extend this analysis to individual differences in fractional anisotropy (FA) in white matter fibre tracts and extend our analysis of regional measurements of cortical areas.
Specifically, we test the hypothesis that individual differences in children’s global motion performance are associated with individual differences in FA in the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), a major fibre bundle with extensive connections in the parietal lobe and providing two-way communication with frontal and prefrontal areas, including supplementary motor and premotor areas (Kamali et al, 2014). Given the relationship of global motion sensitivity to parietal area, we expect that this tract may play an important role in transmitting motion information to anterior cortical areas, and perhaps in top-down modulation of motion processing in parietal and extra-striate areas. It is possible that such modulation, a form of attentional control, may play a significant role in individual abilities to detect global motion. There is long-standing evidence that spatial attention has a lateralized brain basis in the right hemisphere (e.g. Mesulam, 1981) and more specifically, parieto-frontal attentional networks, including the superior longitudinal fasciculus, are known to be lateralized (e.g. Szczepanski et al, 2010; Thiebaut de Schotten et al, 2011). We therefore examined relations with the right and left superior longitudinal fasciculiincluded separately in our statistical models.
2. METHODS
2.1 Participants
The participants were drawn from a sample of children making their first visits for data collection in the PLING study at the University of California, San Diego (UCSD). In addition to measurements of global form and motion sensitivity, participants completed an extensive battery of cognitive tests, as well as a structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) session in which diffusion weighted images were acquired. The work was carried out in accordance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) and the human research protections program and institutional review board at UCSD approved all experimental and consenting procedures. Written parental informed consent was obtained for all participants and child assent from those 7 years and older. Participants were screened for history of major developmental, psychiatric, or neurological disorders, brain injury, or other medical conditions that affect development. Only participants aged between 5:0 and 12:11 years, who completed two measurements of both global motion and global form sensitivity, with neuroimaging data including fractional anisotropy images that passed a quality check, were included.
Data are presented from 125 children whose diffusion weighted imaging data met the quality control criteria, a subset of the 154 whose cortical area measurements were analyzed in Braddick et al (2016). They included 66 males and 59 females, mean age 7.53 yr (s.d.= 1.76 yr); numbers in each 1-year band were: 5 yr+ N=31; 6 yr+ N=23; 7 yr+ N=18; 8 yr+ N=27; 9 yr+ N=13; 10 yr+ N=13.
2.2 Global form and motion testing: the ‘Find the ball in the grass’ test
Global motion and form thresholds were determined as the minimum percentage of coherently organized elements, embedded among random noise elements, for which children could detect global structure. The test was similar to that of Gunn et al (2002) but with concentric stimulus displays (Atkinson & Braddick, 2005) on a laptop computer screen, designed to make the form and motion tasks as comparable as possible. Participants had to report whether a circular region, ‘the Ball’, containing concentrically organized short arcs or trajectories of moving dots, appeared on the left or right of center in a background of randomly oriented elements.
The display subtended 25 × 18 deg arc at the viewing distance of 50 cm. The global motion display contained 3000 dots each 11 min arc diameter, moving at 4.1 deg/sec. Each dot had a lifetime of 8 frames (133 msec) after which it disappeared from the screen. Within the circular target region (the “Ball”), diameter 9.5 deg arc centered 6.3 deg left or right of screen center, coherent dots moved in concentric circular paths. The percentage of dots sharing this coherent motion varied from trial to trial as described below. The remaining dots within this region, and all the dots elsewhere on the screen, moved in randomly oriented arcs whose curvature followed the same distribution as the population of coherently moving dot trajectories.
The global form display contained 3000 stationary arc segments, each 8 min arc wide × 42 min arc long. Within the same circular “Ball” regions as in the global motion display, the coherent arcs were oriented to be concentric. The percentage of arcs sharing this coherent alignment varied according to the same adaptive procedure as for motion. The remaining arcs within this region, and all the arcs elsewhere on the screen, were randomly oriented arcs with the same distribution of curvature as the coherently oriented arcs. Both displays are illustrated in Figure 1.
Participants completed four test runs, alternating runs with the global form and the global motion display, with the starting test randomized across participants. On each trial, the structured “Ball” region was presented randomly on the left or right of centre, and the child was asked to point to the side that contained the “Ball” (circular pattern), or, for older children, to press the corresponding arrow key on the keyboard. The display terminated if the child had not responded within 10 seconds, although most responses were made in the first few seconds. The child’s response terminated the trial. Each run began with coherence fixed at 100%, and these trials were continued until the tester was satisfied that the child understood the task. In the following test phase, the coherence level of the target region was varied according to the Psi adaptive procedure (Kontsevich & Tyler, 1999) which uses a Bayesian approach to place each trial at the point where it will give the most information about the 2-dimensional posterior probability distribution of the threshold and slope of the psychometric function. The estimated threshold was the mean threshold from this distribution, after the completion of 30 trials. Since the adaptive procedure leads to difficult decisions as stimuli are delivered increasingly close to the individual’s threshold, the children’s motivation was maintained by delivering an easily visible stimulus (100% coherence) on every sixth trial. In over 95% of occasions, the responses to these ‘catch’ trials was correct; when it was not correct the child was reminded to indicate the side containing the circular pattern. No children were eliminated for repeated errors on these ‘catch’ trials.
The thresholds used in analysis were the mean of two values from a child’s two test runs with the specific stimulus (global motion or global form)
2.3 Neuroimaging and analysis: MRI scanning protocol
The neuroimaging protocol was as described in Jernigan et al (2016a). A standardized multiple-modality high-resolution structural MRI protocol was implemented, involving 3D T1-weighted volumes and a set of diffusion-weighted scans, on a GE 3T Signa HD× scanner and a 3T Discovery 750× scanner (GE Healthcare) using eight-channel phased array head coils. The protocol included a conventional three-plane localizer, a sagittal 3D inversion recovery spoiled gradient echo T1-weighted volume optimized for maximum gray/white matter contrast (echo time = 3.5 ms, repetition time = 8.1 ms, inversion time = 640 ms, flip angle = 8°, receiver bandwidth = ±31.25 kHz, FOV = 24 cm, frequency =256, phase = 192, slice thickness = 1.2 mm), and two axial 2D diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) pepolar scans (30-directions b-value= 1,000, TE = 83 ms, TR = 13,600 ms, frequency = 96,phase = 96, slice thickness = 2.5 mm, FOV =24 cm).
2.4 Analysis of fractional anisotropy of white matter
Our methods for defining fractional anisotropy (FA) of the Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus followed closely those of Vestergaard et al (2011). All fibre tracts were spatially normalized and aligned across all subjects using Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (Smith et al, 2006), a module in FSL 5.0.2 (Smith et al, 2004). The FA images of all subjects were aligned into a common space by implementing the non-linear registration tool FNIRT (Andersson et al, 2007a,b). Next, a subset group of 50 participants were selected to be representative of the whole group matched in distribution of age and sex. For these 50 participants, all FA images were non-linearly registered to the FA images of every other subject within this group, in order to find the group’s most representative FA image. Using affine registration, the most representative FA image was then aligned to MNI space and subsequently the subset of 50 participants were aligned to this image and transformed into 1 mm3 MNI space. The mean FA image for the subset group of 50 participants was created, and formed the study-specific target to which the entire dataset was aligned, and transformed into 1 mm3 MNI space. Next, a cross-subject mean FA image from the entire dataset was created and thinned to generate the mean FA skeleton for the entire dataset, representing the centers of all tracts common to the group. The mean FA skeleton for the entire dataset was thresholded at FA > 0.2 and contained approximately 119,017 1 mm3 interpolated voxels. All participants’ aligned FA data were then projected onto this skeleton, by locating the voxels with the highest local FA value in the direction perpendicular to the tracts in the skeleton and assigning this value to the subject’s skeleton at the given standardized location. Such projection yields a mapping of each voxel location in the skeleton to a specific voxel in the individual participants’ FA maps. This process accounts for residual misalignments between individuals after the initial registration, and minimizes any systematic between-participants differences in tract location. An eigenvalue color-coded map corresponding to the target FA map was created by applying the affine transformation generated when aligning the most representative FA image to MNI space to corresponding primary eigenvector (v1) image using the FSL “vecreg” command.
The Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus region-of-interest (ROI) was delimited for both the left and right hemisphere on the mean FA skeleton overlaid on the target FA map. It was defined using the color-coded target FA map in accordance with the MRI atlas from Mori et al. (2005). The SLF ROI contained the “stalk” or core of the tract, which extended from superior parietal cortex to dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and limited to voxels with anterior-posterior oriented primary eigenvectors. Of the distinct branches of the SLF (Makris et al, 2005), it corresponds most closely to SLF II, although it tends to exclude angled or bending fibres because of the restriction to anterior-posterior eigenvectors. Figure 2 illustrates the region of interest so defined. The left SLF contained 577 voxels and the right SLF contained 522 voxels. The mean FA within each region of interest was extracted for both the left and the right SLF as well as whole skeleton FA, an estimate of global white matter FA, for all participants. The mean SLF in the bilateral SLF was found to be normally distributed across participants (W = 0.99, p-value = 0.84 on the Shapiro-Wilk normality test)
2.4 Area measures of individual cortical lobes
Results reported here for relation to cortical area were obtained from the procedures described in Braddick et al (2016). In particular the areas of the occipital and parietal lobes were defined from a genetically informed, cortical parcellation scheme, which used a fuzzy clustering method to analyze the matrix of genetic correlations among vertex-wise estimates of cortical surface expansion in a sample of monozygotic and dizygotic twins (Chen et al, 2012).
2.5 Statistical analysis
The relationships between the visual performance measures of global form and motion, and the anatomical measures of fractional anisotropy and of lobe area, were analyzed by multiple regression models fitted by a least-squares criterion using the software package JMP Pro 12 (SAS Institute, Cary NC). Children’s global motion thresholds or global form thresholds were used as the dependent variable, with age, age2 gender, and MRI scanner as predictors, as well as the anatomical measures of interest. These analyses yielded tables of parameter estimates with t-values and significance levels for each effect: examples of these are presented in tables 2–5.
Table 2.
Motion coherence threshold | Form coherence threshold | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Term | t ratio | p > |t| | t ratio | p > |t| |
Intercept | 4.98 | <0.0001**** | 2.07 | 0.040* |
Age | −7.75 | <0.0001**** | −4.34 | <0.0001**** |
Age* age | 3.57 | <0.0005*** | 1.99 | 0.049* |
Gender | 0.34 | 0.733 | 0.30 | 0.765 |
Left SLF FA | 2.43 | 0.017* | 0.45 | 0.652 |
Right SLF FA | − 2.99 | 0.003** | 0.10 | 0.917 |
= p ≤ 0.0001
= p ≤ 0.001
= p ≤ 0.01
= p ≤ 0.05
Table 5.
Term | t ratio | Prob > |t| |
---|---|---|
Intercept | 5.7 | <0.0001**** |
Age | −7.78 | <0.0001**** |
Age*age | 4.00 | 0.0001**** |
Gender | −1.14 | 0.258 |
Bilateral parietal area | −2.57 | 0.011* |
Right SLF FA | −2.76 | 0.0068** |
Left SLF FA | 2.49 | 0.014* |
To explore the spatial distribution of relationships beyond the hypothesised link between SLF and global motion, separate effect size maps were also constructed, to showing the t-value of the correlations between global motion thresholds and FA, and global form thresholds and FA, adjusted for age, age2, and gender. The t-maps were generated in FSL using whole brain linear regression across the whole skeleton with the global motion measure, age, age2, and gender as predictors and FA as the dependent variable. Voxel level t-value ranges for both positive and negative correlations (df = 120), uncorrected for multiple comparisons, are indicated by a color code described in the legend for Figure 6.
RESULTS
3.1 Motion and form coherence thresholds as a function of age
As expected, there was a marked improvement over the age range of this sample in sensitivity both to global form and to global motion. Data from the present sample are illustrated in Figure 3. In line with earlier findings (Gunn et al 2002, Atkinson & Braddick 2005, Braddick et al, 2016), these measurements showed a steeper improvement with age for global motion than for form, especially in the younger part of the range, and a greater individual variability for motion than for form. The approach to an asymptotic value around 8–9 years is consistent with these and other results (e.g. Braunitzer et al, 2012; see also review by Hadad et al, 2015). However, despite the different age functions, global form and motion thresholds had a substantial shared variance: when the effect of age was removed by analysing the studentized residuals for each threshold in a regression against age and gender, individual form and motion thresholds showed a correlation of r = 0.597.
3.2 Relation of coherence thresholds to fractional anisotropy in the superior longitudinal fasciculus
Table 2 summarises statistical models for the prediction of motion and form coherence thresholds from the fractional anisotropy measures of the superior longitudinal fasciculus. The right and left SLF were opposite predictors of global motion sensitivity: higher fractional anisotropy in the right-hemisphere’s superior longitudinal fasciculus is associated with lower motion thresholds (high sensitivity), while higher fractional anisotropy in the left hemisphere’s superior longitudinal fasciculus was associated with higher motion thresholds (low sensitivity). Figure 4 shows leverage plots, (i.e. the unique effects of left and right fractional anisotropy respectively on global motion thresholds). In contrast to these results for global motion, no such relationship was found with global form sensitivity. Associations with global motion sensitivity but not global form were similarly found in the structural measures of cortical area reported by Braddick et al (2016).
Similar models were tested for prediction by the whole skeleton fractional anisotropy. These yielded no significant associations with either global motion or global form sensitivity, either when entered alone into the model or alongside fractional anisotropy of right and left superior longitudinal fasciculus (Table 3). The latter result indicates that the associations with the left and right superior longitudinal fasciculus were anatomically specific and not a reflection of overall white matter maturation.
Table 3.
Motion coherence threshold | ||
---|---|---|
Term | t ratio | p > |t| |
Intercept | 3.14 | <0.0021** |
Age | −6.72 | <0.0001*** |
Age* age | 3.56 | <0.0005*** |
Gender | 0.15 | 0.881 |
Left SLF FA | 2.54 | 0.012* |
Right SLF FA | −2.59 | 0.011** |
Total skeleton FA | −0.83 | 0.406 |
= p ≤ 0.001
= p ≤ 0.01
= p ≤ 0.05
Since global motion performance was strongly associated with age in the range studied, one might ask whether the relationship with fractional anisotropy in the left and right superior longitudinal fasciculus reflects differential maturation between individual children, that is, was the asymmetry between the tracts a feature of the more mature brain? This appears unlikely. The average fractional anisotropy of the left and right increased with age (t= 4.14, p < 0.0001 in a model including age, age2, and gender: see Figure 5a). However an analogous model for the ratio (left SLF FA):(right SLF FA) showed no age effect and a very flat relation (Figure 5b). Over the group as a whole, there was no significant difference between fractional anisotropy values for the left and right superior longitudinal fasciculi (means 0.497 and 0.499 respectively; matched-pairs t=1.206, p = 0.23)
3.3 Associations with coherence thresholds in whole-brain fractional anisotropy data
The primary goal of this study was to test the specific anatomical hypotheses about the relationship between microstructure of white matter in the superior longitudinal fasciculus, and variations in children’s sensitivity to global motion. Therefore neither a whole-brain, voxel-wise analysis of the effects (appropriately adjusted for multiple comparisons), nor a restricted voxel-wise analysis with small volume correction was considered appropriate for testing this a priori hypothesis. However, since the analyses we used produced estimates of the effect size at each voxel, we have provided visualization of the effect-size maps showing positive and negative associations between FA and motion coherence threshold, adjusted for age, age2, and gender (Figure 6A–D). A similar effect size map for the positive and negative associations between FA and form coherence threshold is presented in Figure 6E–G.
As well as the right superior longitudinal fasciculus, which was tested in our original hypothesis, clusters of voxels with negative association with motion coherence threshold were found in the corona radiata (Figures 6C–D). The region in the right anterior corona radiata also overlaps strongly with voxels showing a negative association with form coherence thresholds (Figure 6E, F). Finally, positive association with form coherence thresholds (i.e high fractional anisotropy linked to low global form sensitivity) occurred in a midline region of the cerebellum (Figure 6G). However, in no case did a voxel meet the criterion for significance at p<0.05 when corrected for multiple comparisons. For the motion threshold, 1,403 voxels had a t-value above 1.980 and 5,183 voxels had a t-value below −1.980 (uncorrected p<0.05), comprising 5.53% of the voxels in the skeleton. For the form threshold, 1,811 voxels had a t-value above 1.980 and 3,266 voxels had a t-value below −1.980 (uncorrected p<0.05), comprising 4.27% of the voxels in the skeleton.
3.4 Superior longitudinal fasciculus and cortical area
The asymmetry in the relation between motion thresholds and left and right superior longitudinal fasciculus prompted us to re-examine the association of these thresholds with the area of cortical lobes. The analysis in Braddick et al (2016) examined relationships with the total area of each lobe, combining left and right hemispheres. We have now tested regression models of these data in which left and right parietal and occipital lobes are examined separately. The results (Tables 4a and 4b) showed that the effects of area (lower motion thresholds with high parietal area, higher motion thresholds with high occipital area) were in the same direction for left and right hemispheres. However, the effects of parietal and occipital area previously reported in the bilateral data were driven most strongly by the areas of these lobes in the left hemisphere. Thus the overall results reflect a complex pattern of structural asymmetry, in which cortical area effects were dominated by the left hemisphere, where SLF fractional anisotropy shows a negative relation with motion performance, and the positive association of the right superior longitudinal fasciculus was accompanied by a weaker relationship of lobe areas to motion performance in that hemisphere.
Table 4a.
Term | t ratio | p > |t| | t ratio | p > |t| | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 7.04 | <0.0001**** | 7.46 | <0.0001**** | |
Age | −7.30 | <0.0001**** | −8.06 | <0.0001**** | |
Age* age | 3.81 | 0.0002*** | 3.95 | 0.0001**** | |
Gender | −0.92 | 0.359 | −0.87 | 0.384 | |
L occipital area | 2.45 | 0.016* | L parietal area | −2.14 | 0.0348* |
L total cortical area | −3.18 | 0.0019** | L total cortical area | 1.24 | 0.219 |
= p ≤ 0.0001
= p ≤ 0.001
= p ≤ 0.01
= p ≤ 0.05
Table 4b.
term | t ratio | p > |t| | t ratio | p > |t| | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 7.12 | <0.0001**** | 7.38 | <0.0001**** | |
Age | −7.61 | <0.0001**** | −7.67 | <0.0001**** | |
Age* age | 3.37 | 0.001*** | 3.87 | 0.002** | |
Gender | −0.86 | 0.390 | −1.01 | 0.315 | |
R occipital area | 2.15 | 0.034* | R parietal area | −1.07 | 0.286 |
R total cortical area | −2.85 | 0.005** | R total cortical area | 0.38 | 0 .707 |
= p ≤ 0.0001
= p ≤ 0.001
= p ≤ 0.01
= p ≤ 0.05
Do these effects, nonetheless, reflect a common phenotype of asymmetrical brain development? This can be tested by a regression model which includes bilateral parietal area, together with fractional anisotropy in the right and the left superior longitudinal fasciculus (Table 5). Each of these structural measures made a significant, independent contribution to the prediction of global motion thresholds. These relationships, therefore, cannot depend primarily of shared variance between the structural measures. In fact, although fractional anisotropy of the left and right superior longitudinal fasciculi showed substantial shared variance, as did and left and right areas of parietal and occipital lobes, the correlations between fractional anisotropy of the superior longitudinal fasciculus and cortical area measures were very weak when age, overall area and whole skeleton fractional anisotropy are taken into account (Table 6).
Table 6.
R parietal area | L occipital area | R occipital area | L SLF FA | R SLF FA | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
L parietal area | 0.356 P < 0.001 |
−0.408 p < 0.001 |
−0.400 p < 0.001 |
−0.111 | −0.031 |
R parietal area | −0.240 p<0.01 |
−0.268 p < 0.01 |
−0.173 p = 0.532 |
−0.045 | |
L occipital area | 0.783 p < 0.001 |
0.173 p = 0.539 |
0.097 | ||
R occipital area | 0.090 | −0.034 | |||
L SLF FA | 0.698 p < 0.001 |
3. DISCUSSION
Our earlier analysis (Braddick et al, 2016) showed that children’s global motion performance was related to variations in cortical structure in the parietal lobe, particularly in the region of the intraparietal sulcus (IPS). On this basis, we hypothesised that the superior longitudinal fasciculus, through which this area interacts with anterior cerebral structures, might show a similar relation to visual motion processing. Our results confirmed that such a link exists, but that it is complex, with opposite relationships between the structure of the superior longitudinal fasciculus and motion sensitivity in the two hemispheres. Furthermore, on closer examination, the relation with parietal and occipital cortical areas also was found to be asymmetrical. The sign was the same in both hemispheres, but was markedly stronger for the left hemisphere. Thus the brain of the child who has high sensitivity to global motion shows a complex pattern of asymmetry relative to the brain of a child with lower performance, although the different components of this asymmetry contribute independently, at least to some degree.
Csete et al (2014) have also reported a relationship between motion coherence threshold and fractional anisotropy for some voxels in a number of white matter regions, in a smaller group of adult participants. They did not test specific tracts, although they state that some of the locations they identify lie within the medial branch of the left superior longitudinal fasciculus. For all the voxels they reported, the correlation of fractional anisotropy with motion thresholds is positive – that is, higher fractional anisotropy goes with reduced sensitivity, in line with our own finding for the left SLF.
We tested a specific hypothesis of association between global visual performance and the superior longitudinal fasciculus. The whole-skeleton analysis of effect size allows us to examine how this association may be distributed in white matter. Additional locations for association were found in the corona radiata. These included a region of the right anterior corona radiata which was associated with both global motion and global form sensitivity, and so may play a different role to the apparently motion-specific structure in the superior longitudinal fasciculus. While the corona radiata is most often thought of as the fan of fibres which converge into the internal capsule and project to subcortical and spinal targets, it does include cortico-cortical tracts also (Wakana et al, 2004). Fractional anisotropy in the corona radiata has been associated with individual variations in a number of cognitive functions, including children’s numerical abilities (Matejko & Ansari, 2015), cognitive control (Seghete et al, 2013; Chaddock-Heyman et al (2013); literacy (Myers et al, 2014), and IQ scores (Chiang et al, 2009). This structure may therefore be associated rather broadly with the development of information processing abilities, and its specific role in brain networks subserving these abilities, and in global visual processes specifically, is unclear. In any case, given that this and other relationships shown in the effect size maps have only been shown with uncorrected significance levels, they must be seen as pointers for possible further investigation rather than established findings.
4.1 Asymmetries in motion processing
A number of reports find that individual participants show hemispheric asymmetries in cerebral responses to motion, for example, Patzwahl et al, (1994), Hollants-Gilhuijs et al (2000), and Nakamura et al, (2003), which they ascribe to area V5/MT. However, there does not appear to be any consistent difference across individuals between left and right hemispheres in these responses, which might be associated with the asymmetric anatomical relationships we find with motion coherence thresholds. An exception is an overall right-hemisphere advantage reported by Patzwahl et al (1994), which is ascribed by them to “[the special role of the right hemisphere] attentional system amplifying the activation of the right parietal cortex”. In contrast, Hollants-Gilhuijs et al (1998) found a behavioral advantage for right-field motion detection (implying left-hemisphere advantage) in 6- to 16-year-old children (but not in adults), which they ascribed to differential maturation of extra-striate areas in the two hemispheres. A number of other visuo-spatial tasks have been found to show hemispheric asymmetries, not necessarily in this direction (e.g. Atkinson & Egeth, 1973). Since our task involved intermixed left- and right-field stimuli, and the children could freely fixate while making their decision, our data provide no evidence on half-field differences and do not allow us to examine whether such a functional asymmetry is linked to the structural correlates we find. This is a possible question for future studies using a task which requires central fixation and brief stimulus presentations in either left or right field, which would not however be practical with children across the age range of the present study.
4.2 Attentional and cognitive role of the superior longitudinal fasciculus
Our findings present a specific link of structure of the superior longitudinal fasciculus to the processing of motion information. However, neuroimaging and lesion studies in adults have identified a number of broader aspects of cognitive function that are associated with this tract, especially within the domain of spatial cognition and attention. Bennett et al (2012), Mayer and Vuong (2014) and Chechlacz et al (2015) report correlations of fractional anisotropy in the superior longitudinal fasciculus with various measures of visuo-spatial attention; Rodriguez-Herreros et al (2015) find that fractional anisotropy in SLF-2 correlates with the individual resistance to TMS disruption in visually guided reaching. Damage to the superior longitudinal fasciculus is also a key cause of the spatial neglect syndrome (e.g. Shinoura et al, 2009; Ptak & Schnider, 2010; Chechlacz et al, 2012; Lunven & Bartolomeo, 2016).
Other associations have been found between fractional anisotropy of the superior longitudinal fasciculus, and working memory (Vestergaard et al, 2011; Rizio & Diaz, 2016), executive function and sustained attention in children (Klaborg et al, 2013; Urger et al, 2015), and children’s reading development (Travis et al, 2016; Wang et al, 2016).
Fractional anisotropy in the superior longitudinal fasciculus is also reduced compared to controls in several neurodevelopmental disorders, including cerebral palsy, where it is correlated with IQ (Ballester-Plané et al, 2016) and amblyopia, where it is correlated with visual acuity (Li et al, 2015). It is also correlated with response time variability in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (Wolfers et al, 2015). However in Williams Syndrome, fractional anisotropy of the superior longitudinal fasciculus is increased relative to controls, and high fractional anisotropy is associated with poor visuospatial performance (Hoeft et al, 2007).
More broadly, early disorders of cerebral white matter are a major feature of neonatal brain injury associated with deficits of early attention and dorsal stream function in preterm born children (Atkinson et al, 2008) and of early brain development in Williams syndrome (Mercuri et al, 1997).
The networks associated with spatial attention are not symmetrical: Thiebaut de Schotten et al (2011) describe a “lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention” in which the volume of right SLF II is associated with left bias in line bisection and with faster left field detection. These results, and the wider findings of attention-related functions suggest that our finding of fractional anisotropy of the right superior longitudinal fasciculus associated with good motion performance may be linked to deployment of attention in the motion processing task. Studies of functional connectivity (Friston & Büchel, 2000) have shown that activity in the parietal lobe is associated with attentional modulation of the transmission of motion information from V1/V2 to V5/MT. However, it should be noted that the global form task, which shares many of the attention demands of the global motion task, shows none of the same relations with brain structure.
The relationship between fractional anisotropy and function is complex, since a number of different tissue properties will contribute to the measured FA (Beaulieu, 2002). It is often assumed that fractional anisotropy reflects the integrity of tract organization, in terms of the parallelism and packing of axons which restricts radial diffusion and hence increases fractional anisotropy; myelination is another functionally important aspect of brain development which similarly increases fractional anisotropy. However, some factors which reduce fractional anisotropy may be associated with enhanced function: for example increased axonal diameter which may be responsible for the association seen between faster choice reaction time and reduced fractional anisotropy in the optic radiation (Tuch et al, 2005). Hoeft et al (2007), discussing the increased fractional anisotropy and its association with poor visuospatial test performance in Williams Syndrome, suggest that this higher fractional anisotropy may be associated with reduced branching in the tract, a proposed characteristic of this neurodevelopmental disorder (Eckert et al, 2006). Thus we cannot necessarily assume that a positive correlation with high fractional anisotropy means that a particular fibre tract contributes positively to motion processing in development (or vice versa for a negative correlation). Furthermore, the direction of causality between behavioral performance and structural differences remains uncertain.
4. CONCLUSIONS
The finding that the structure of the superior longitudinal fasciculus is related to individual variations in children’s global motion processing adds support to two conclusions from our earlier findings on their relation to regional cortical area (Braddick et al, 2016).
First, these individual differences do not necessarily reflect the stage, in extrastriate areas such as MT/V5 or V3a, where local motion signals are initially integrated to provide information about globally coherent motion (Mikami, Newsome & Wurtz, 1986; Newsome & Paré, 1988; Braddick et al, 2001), although we cannot exclude localized variations in these areas. Instead, the correlations with individual differences in global motion sensitivity are provided by variations in the area of the parietal lobe (which receives input from extrastriate motion areas), and in the superior longitudinal fasciculus through which parietal cortex communicates with anterior cerebral areas. These results suggest that the critical point for determining these individual differences may be the levels where sensory evidence for global motion is accumulated for perceptual decision-making, and at which these decisions are communicated to response systems (Shadlen & Newsome, 2001; Hanks et al, 2006). The role of attention systems in contributing to this performance has been discussed above; top-down modulation of parietal function may play an important role in individual differences.
Second, we identify structural correlates of global motion sensitivity but have not yet confirmed any for sensitivity to static global form. Impaired motion rather than form sensitivity provides the widespread signature of neurodevelopmental disorders (“dorsal stream vulnerability” – Braddick Atkinson & Wattam-Bell, 2003). It appears that even within the range of typical development, it is again motion processing which provides the more sensitive index of variations in brain development.
The developmental course of these structure-function relationships is still unknown. Future work using longitudinal data sets from this and other groups of children may help to show whether early structural differences predict later functional development. Studies of genetic associations with motion thresholds and with brain structure (e.g. Schork, 2012) may also help to understand the causal pathways that determine these patterns of individual differences.
Highlights.
Children’s individual motion sensitivity correlates with fractional anisotropy of the Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus (SLF)
This correlation is opposite in left and right hemispheres
Individual differences in motion processing depend on levels beyond V5/MT
Differences in SLF and parietal structures may contribute to “dorsal stream vulnerability”
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health and by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development of the National Institutes of Health ((grant numbers RC2DA029475, R01HD061414, R24HD075489). JA received support from an Emeritus Fellowship from the Leverhulme Foundation. We thank the members of the PLING Data Camp team for assistance with data collection, and the participating children and their families.
Abbreviations
- TBSS
tract-based spatial statistics
- FA
fractional anisotropy
- SLF
superior longitudinal fasciculus
- FNIRT
FMRIB nonlinear image registration tool
- MNI
Montreal Neurological Institute
- IPS
intraparietal sulcus
- TMS
transcranial magnetic stimulation
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Akshoomoff N, Newman E, Thompson WK, McCabe C, Bloss CS, Chang L, Amaral DG, Casey BJ, Ernst TM, Frazier JA, Gruen JR, Kaufmann WE, Kenet T, Kennedy DN, Libiger O, Mostofsky S, Murray SS, Sowell ER, Schork N, Dale AM, Jernigan TL. The NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery: results from a large normative developmental sample (PING) Neuropsychology. 2014;28:1–10. doi: 10.1037/neu0000001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson JLR, Jenkinson M, Smith S. Non-linear optimisation. FMRIB Technical Report TR07JA1. 2007a from www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/analysis/techrep.
- Andersson JLR, Jenkinson M, Smith S. Non-linear registration, aka Spatial normalisation. FMRIB technical report TR07JA2. 2007b from www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/analysis/techrep.
- Atkinson J, Braddick O. Dorsal stream vulnerability and autistic disorders: The importance of comparative studies of form and motion coherence in typically developing children and children with developmental disorders. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive (Current Psychology of Cognition) 2005;23(1–2):49–58. [Google Scholar]
- Atkinson J, Braddick OJ. Visual and visuocognitive development in children born very prematurely. Progress in Brain Research. 2007;164:123–149. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)64007-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atkinson J, Braddick O. From genes to brain development to phenotypic behavior: “dorsal-stream vulnerability” in relation to spatial cognition, attention, and planning of actions in Williams syndrome (WS) and other developmental disorders. Progress in Brain Research. 2011;189:261–83. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53884-0.00029-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atkinson J, Braddick O, Anker S, Curran W, Andrew R. Neurobiological models of visuospatial cognition in children with Williams Syndrome: Measures of dorsal-stream and frontal function. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2003;23:141–174. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2003.9651890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atkinson J, Braddick O, Anker S, Nardini M, Birtles D, Rutherford M, Mercuri E, Dyet L, Edwards D, Cowan F. Cortical vision, MRI and developmental outcome in preterm infants. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 2008;93:F292–F297. doi: 10.1136/adc.2007.116988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atkinson J, Braddick O, Rose FE, Searcy YM, Wattam-Bell J, Bellugi U. Dorsal-stream motion processing deficits persist into adulthood in Williams Syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:828–833. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atkinson J, Braddick O, Wattam-Bell J, Akshoomoff N, Newman E, Girard H, Dale A, Jernigan T. Global motion, mathematics and movement: dorsal stream sensitivity relates to children’s individual differences in cognitive abilities and regional brain development. Journal of Vision. 2014;14(10):1324–1324. Abstract. [Google Scholar]
- Atkinson J, Egeth H. Right hemisphere superiority in visual orientation matching. Canadian Journal of Psychology. 1973;27:152–158. doi: 10.1037/h0082464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atkinson J, King J, Braddick O, Nokes L, Anker S, Braddick F. A specific deficit of dorsal stream function in Williams’ syndrome. NeuroReport. 1997;8:1919–1922. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199705260-00025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballester-Plané J, Laporta-Hoyos O, Macaya A, Póo P, Meléndez-Plumed M, Vázquez É, Delgado I, Zubiaurre-Elorza L, Narberhaus A, Toro-Tamargo E, Russi ME, Tenorio V, Segarra D, Pueyo R. Measuring intellectual ability in cerebral palsy: The comparison of three tests and their neuroimaging correlates. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2016;56:83–98. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2016.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beaulieu C. The basis of anisotropic water diffusion in the nervous system—a technical review. NMR in Biomedicine. 2002;15:435–455. doi: 10.1002/nbm.782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett IJ, Motes MA, Rao NK, Rypma B. White matter tract integrity predicts visual search performance in young and older adults. Neurobiology of Aging. 2012;33:433.e21–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braddick O, Atkinson J. Development of human visual function. Vision Research. 2011;51:1588–1609. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braddick O, Atkinson J, Newman E, Akshoomoff N, Kuperman JM, Bartsch H, Chen CH, Dale AM, Jernigan TL. Global visual motion sensitivity: associations with parietal area and children’s mathematical cognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2016 Jul;26:1–12. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01018. 2016. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braddick O, Atkinson J, Wattam-Bell J. Normal and anomalous development of visual motion processing: motion coherence and ‘dorsal stream vulnerability’. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:1769–1784. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00178-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braddick OJ, O’Brien JMD, Wattam-Bell J, Atkinson J, Hartley T, Turner R. Brain areas sensitive to coherent visual motion. Perception. 2001;30:61–72. doi: 10.1068/p3048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown TT, Kuperman JM, Chung Y, Erhart M, McCabe C, Hagler DJ, Venkatraman VK, Akshoomoff N, Amaral DG, Bloss CS, Casey BJ, Chang L, Ernst TM, Frazier JA, Gruen JR, Kaufmann WE, Kenet T, Kennedy DN, Murray SS, Sowell ER, Jernigan TL, Dale AM. Neuroanatomical assessment of biological maturity. Current Biology. 2012;22:1693–1698. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown TT. Individual differences in human brain development. WIREs Cognitive Science. 2016 doi: 10.1002/wcs.1389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaddock-Heyman L, Erickson KI, Voss MW, Powers JP, Knecht AM, Pontifex MB, Drollette ES, Moore RD, Raine LB, Scudder MR, Hillman CH, Kramer AF. White matter microstructure is associated with cognitive control in children. Biological Psychology. 2013;94:109–15. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chechlacz M, Rotshtein P, Humphreys GW. Neuroanatomical dissections of unilateral visual neglect symptoms: ALE meta-analysis of lesion-symptom mapping. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012 Aug 10; doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00230. 2012 | http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Chechlacz M, Gillebert CR, Vangkilde SA, Petersen A, Humphreys GW. Structural Variability within Frontoparietal Networks and Individual Differences in Attentional Functions: An Approach Using the Theory of Visual Attention. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:10647–58. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0210-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen C-H, Gutierrez ED, Thompson W, Panizzon MS, Jernigan TL, Eyler LT, Fennema-Notestine C, Jak AJ, Neale MC, Franz CE, Lyons MJ, Grant MD, Fischl B, Seidman LJ, Tsuang MT, Kremen WS, Dale AM. Hierarchical genetic organization of human cortical surface area. Science. 2012;335:1634–1636. doi: 10.1126/science.1215330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiang MC, Barysheva M, Shattuck DW, Lee AD, Madsen SK, Avedissian C, Klunder AD, Toga AW, McMahon KL, de Zubicaray GI, Wright MJ, Srivastava A, Balov N, Thompson PM. Genetics of brain fiber architecture and intellectual performance. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:2212–24. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4184-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Csete G, Szabo N, Rokszin A, Toth E, Braunitzer G, Benedek G, Vecseia L, Tamas Kincses Z. An investigation of the white matter microstructure in motion detection using diffusion MRI. Brain Research. 2014;1570:35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eckert MA, Galaburda AM, Mills DL, Bellugi U, Korenberg JR, Reiss AL. The neurobiology of Williams syndrome: cascading influences of visual system impairment? Cellular & Molecular Life Sciences. 2006a;63:1867–1875. doi: 10.1007/s00018-005-5553-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellemberg D, Lewis TL, Maurer D, Brar S, Brent HP. Better perception of global motion after monocular than after binocular deprivation. Vision Research. 2002;42:169–179. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00278-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Brown TT, Kuperman JM, Chung Y, Hagler DJ, Jr, Venkatraman V, Roddey C, Erhart M, McCabe C, Akshoomoff N, Amaral DG, Bloss CS, Libiger O, Darst BF, Schork NJ, Casey BJ, Chang L, Ernst TM, Gruen JR, Kaufmann WE, Kenet T, Frazier J, Murray SS, Sowell ER, van Zijl P, Mostofsky S, Jernigan TL, Dale AM, for the Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition, and Genetics Study Multimodal imaging of the self-regulating developing brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA. 2012;109:19620–19625. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1208243109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fjell AM, Westlye LT, Amlien I, Tamnes CK, Grydeland H, Engvig A, Espeseth T, Reinvang I, Lundervold AJ, Lundervold A, Walhovd KB. High-expanding cortical regions in human development and evolution are related to higher intellectual abilities. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;25:26–34. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ, Büchel C. Attentional modulation of effective connectivity from V2 to V5/MT in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U S A. 2000;97:7591–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.13.7591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodbourn PT, Bosten JM, Hogg RE, Bargary G, Lawrance-Owen AJ, Mollon JD. Do different ‘magnocellular tasks’ probe the same neural substrate? Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 2012;279:4263–71. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.1430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunn A, Cory E, Atkinson J, Braddick O, Wattam-Bell J, Guzzetta A, Cioni G. Dorsal and ventral stream sensitivity in normal development and hemiplegia. NeuroReport. 2002;13:843–847. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200205070-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hadad BS, Daphne Maurer D, Lewis TL. Long trajectory for the development of sensitivity to global and biological motion. Developmental Science. 2011;14:1330–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01078.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanks TD, Ditterich J, Shadlen MN. Microstimulation of macaque area LIP affects decision-making in a motion discrimination task. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:682–9. doi: 10.1038/nn1683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hansen PC, Stein JF, Orde SR, Winter JL, Talcott JB. Are dyslexics’ visual deficits limited to measures of dorsal stream function? NeuroReport. 2001;12:1527–30. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200105250-00045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ho CS, Giaschi DE, Boden C, Dougherty R, Cline R, Lyons C. Deficient motion perception in the fellow eye of amblyopic children. Vision Research. 2005;45:1615–1627. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoeft F, Barnea-Goraly N, Haas BW, Golarai G, Ng D, Mills D, Korenberg J, Bellugi U, Galaburda A, Reiss AL. More is not always better: increased fractional anisotropy of superior longitudinal fasciculus associated with poor visuospatial abilities in Williams syndrome. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:11960–5. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3591-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollants-Gilhuijs MAM, De Munck JC, Kubova Z, van Royen E, Spekreijse H. The development of hemispheric asymmetry in human motion VEPs. Vision Research. 2000;40:1–11. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00173-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollants-Gilhuijs MAM, Ruijter JM, Spekreijse H. Visual half-field development in children: Detection of motion-defined forms. Vision Research. 1998;38:651–657. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00202-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jernigan TL, Brown TT, Hagler DJ, Jr, Akshoomoff N, Bartsch H, Newman E, Thompson WK, Bloss CS, Murray SS, Schork N, Kennedy DN, Kuperman JM, McCabe C, Chung Y, Libiger O, Maddox M, Casey BJ, Chang L, Ernst TM, Frazier JA, Gruen JR, Sowell ER, Kenet T, Kaufmann WE, Mostofsky S, Amaral DG, Dale AM, Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition and Genetics Study The Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition, and Genetics (PING) Data Repository. Neuroimage. 2016;124(Pt B):1149–1154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamali A, Flanders AE, Brody J, Hunter JV, Hasan KM. Tracing superior longitudinal fasciculus connectivity in the human brain using high resolution diffusion tensor tractography. Brain Structure & Function. 2014;219:269–81. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0498-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klarborg B, Skak Madsen K, Vestergaard M, Skimminge A, Jernigan TL, Baaré WF. Sustained attention is associated with right superior longitudinal fasciculus and superior parietal white matter microstructure in children. Human Brain Mapping. 2013;34:3216–32. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kogan CS, Boutet I, Cornish K, Zangenehpour S, Mullen KT, Holden JJA, Kaloustian VMD, Andermann E, Chaudhuri A. Differential impact of the FMR1 gene on visual processing in fragile X syndrome. Brain. 2004;127:591–601. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kontsevich LL, Tyler CW. Bayesian adaptive estimation of psychometric slope and threshold. Vision Research. 1999;39:2729–2737. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(98)00285-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kravitz DJ, Saleem KS, Baker CI, Mishkin M. A new neural framework for visuospatial processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2011;12:217–230. doi: 10.1038/nrn3008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis TL, Ellemberg D, Maurer D, Wilkinson F, Wilson HR, Dirks M, Brent HP. Sensitivity to global form in Glass patterns after early visual deprivation in humans. Vision Research. 2002;42:939–948. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00041-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Q, Zhai L, Jiang Q, Qin W, Li Q, Yin X, Guo M. Tract-based spatial statistics analysis of white matter changes in children with anisometropic amblyopia. Neurosci Lett. 2015 Jun 15;597:7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2015.04.027. 2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lunven M, Bartolomeo P. Attention and spatial cognition: Neural and anatomical substrates of visual neglect. Annals of Physical & Rehabilitation Medicine. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2016.01.004. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makris N, Kennedy DN, McInerney S, Sorensen AG, Wang R, Caviness VS, Jr, Pandya DN. Segmentation of subcomponents within the superior longitudinal fascicle in humans: a quantitative, in vivo, DT-MRI study. Cerebral Cortex. 2005;15:854–69. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matejko AA, Ansari D. Drawing connections between white matter and numerical and mathematical cognition: A literature review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2015;48(2015):35–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer KM, Vuong QC. TBSS and probabilistic tractography reveal white matter connections for attention to object features. Brain Structure & Function. 2014;219:2159–2171. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0631-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meier K, Giaschi D. The maturation of global motion perception depends on the spatial and temporal offsets of the stimulus. Vision Research. 2014;95:61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mercuri E, Atkinson J, Braddick O, Rutherford M, Cowan F, Counsell S, Dubowitz L, Bydder G. Chiari I malformation and white matter changes in asymptomatic young children with Williams syndrome: clinical and MRI study. European Journal of Pediatric Neurology. 1997;5/6:177–181. doi: 10.1016/s1090-3798(97)80055-4. 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mesulam MM. A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Annals of Neurology. 1981;10:309–25. doi: 10.1002/ana.410100402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikami A, Newsome WT, Wurtz RH. Motion selectivity in macaque visual cortex. I. Mechanisms of direction and speed selectivity in extrastriate area MT. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1986;55:1308–1327. doi: 10.1152/jn.1986.55.6.1308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mori S, Wakana S, Nagae-Poetscher LM, van Zijl PC. MRI Atlas of Human White Matter. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers CA, Vandermosten M, Farris EA, Hancock R, Gimenez P, Black JM, Casto B, Drahos M, Tumber M, Hendren RL, Hulme C, Hoeft F. White matter morphometric changes uniquely predict children’s reading acquisition. Psychological Science. 2014;25:1870–83. doi: 10.1177/0956797614544511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakamura H, Kashii S, Nagamine T, Matsui Y, Hashimoto T, Honda Y, Shibasaki H. Human V5 demonstrated by magnetoencephalography using random dot kinematograms of different coherence levels. Neuroscience Research. 2003;46:423–33. doi: 10.1016/s0168-0102(03)00119-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman E, Thompson WK, Bartsch H, Hagler DJ, Jr, Chen CH, Brown TT, Kuperman JM, McCabe C, Chung Y, Libiger O, Akshoomoff N, Bloss CS, Casey BJ, Chang L, Ernst TM, Frazier JA, Gruen JR, Kennedy DN, Murray SS, Sowell ER, Schork N, Kenet T, Kaufmann WE, Mostofsky S, Amaral DG, Dale AM, Jernigan TL. Anxiety is related to indices of cortical maturation in typically developing children and adolescents. Brain Structure & function. 2015 Jul 17; doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-1085-9. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newsome WT, Paré EB. A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) Journal of Neuroscience. 1988;8:2201–2211. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-06-02201.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patzwahl DR, Zanker JM, Altenmuller EO. Cortical potentials reflecting motion processing in humans. Visual Neuroscience. 1994;11:1135–1147. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800006945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterzell DH. Discovering sensory processes using individual differences: A review and factor analytic manifesto. Proceedings of the 2016 IS&T Symposium on Electronic Imaging: Conference on Human Vision and Electronic Imaging 2016 [Google Scholar]
- Ptak R, Schnider A. The dorsal attention network mediates orienting toward behaviorally relevant stimuli in spatial neglect. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:12557–65. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2722-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizio AA, Diaz MT. Language, aging, and cognition: frontal aslant tract and superior longitudinal fasciculus contribute toward working memory performance in older adults. NeuroReport. 2016;27:689–93. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodríguez-Herreros B, Amengual JL, Gurtubay-Antolín A, Richter L, Jauer P, Erdmann C, Schweikard A, López-Moliner J, Rodríguez-Fornells A, Münte TF. Microstructure of the superior longitudinal fasciculus predicts stimulation-induced interference with on-line motor control. Neuroimage. 2015;120:254–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schork AJ, Madsen KS, Baare WFC, Akshoomoff N, McCabe C, Erik Newman E, Atkinson J, Braddick O, Bloss CS, Sarah S, Murray SS, Nicholas J, Schork 9, Terry L, Jernigan 1,3,4,5. Common repeat polymorphism in the dopamine transporter gene SLC6A3 is associated with stop signal reaction time (SSRT) and motion coherence threshold (MCT). Presentation 393.13 at the 2012 Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience; New Orleans, LA. 2012. http://www.abstractsonline.com/Plan/ViewAbstract.aspx?sKey=0bb2fe49-2960-4a09-994f-0dfaa250eecc&cKey=8afe990a-c63c-467f-b881-3c63b4d843a0&mKey=%7b70007181-01C9-4DE9-A0A2-EEBFA14CD9F1%7d. [Google Scholar]
- Seghete KL, Herting MM, Nagel BJ. White matter microstructure correlates of inhibition and task-switching in adolescents. Brain Research. 2013;1527:15–28. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP.) of the rhesus monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2001;86:1916–1936. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.4.1916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shinoura N, Suzuki Y, Yamada R, Tabei Y, Saito K, Yagi K. Damage to the right superior longitudinal fasciculus in the inferior parietal lobe plays a role in spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2600–3. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simmers A, Ledgeway T, Hess R. The influences of visibility and anomalous integration processes on the perception of global spatial form versus motion in human amblyopia. Vision Research. 2005;45:449–460. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.08.026. 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simmons DR, Robertson AE, McKay LS, Toal E, McAleer P, Pollick FE. Vision in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research. 2009;49:2705–2739. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE, Niazy R, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Matthews PM. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage. 2004;23(S1):208–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H, Rueckert D, Nichols TE, Mackay CE, Watkins KE, Ciccarelli O, Cader MZ, Matthews PM, Behrens TE. Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage. 2006;31:1487–505. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spencer J, O’Brien J, Riggs K, Braddick O, Atkinson J, Wattam-Bell J. Motion processing in autism: evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency. NeuroReport. 2000;11:2765–2767. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200008210-00031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szczepanski SM, Konen CS, Kastner S. Mechanisms of spatial attention control in frontal and parietal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:148–60. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3862-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor NM, Jakobson LS, Maurer D, Lewis TL. Differential vulnerability of global motion, global form, and biological motion processing in full-term and preterm children. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2766–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Forkel SJ, Simmons A, Vergani F, Murphy DG, Catani M. A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:1245–1246. doi: 10.1038/nn.2905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Travis KE, Ben-Shachar M, Myall NJ, Feldman HM. Variations in the neurobiology of reading in children and adolescents born full term and preterm. Neuroimage: Clinical. 2016;11:555–65. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tuch DS, Salat DH, Wisco JJ, Zaleta AK, Hevelone ND, Rosas HD. Choice reaction time performance correlates with diffusion anisotropy in white matter pathways supporting visuospatial attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U S A. 2005;102:12212–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407259102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urger SE, De Bellis MDSR, Donald P, Woolley PD, Chen SD, Provenzale J. The superior longitudinal fasciculus in typically developing children and adolescents: Diffusion Tensor Imaging and neuropsychological correlates. Journal of Child Neurology. 2015;30:9–20. doi: 10.1177/0883073813520503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vestergaard M, Madsen KS, Baaré WF, Skimminge A, Ejersbo LR, Ramsøy TZ, Gerlach C, Akeson P, Paulson OB, Jernigan TL. White matter microstructure in superior longitudinal fasciculus associated with spatial working memory performance in children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;23:2135–46. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wakana S, Jiang H, Nagae-Poetscher LM, van Zijl PCM, Mori S. Fiber tract–based atlas of human white matter anatomy. Radiology. 2004;230:77–87. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2301021640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y, Mauer MV, Raney T, Peysakhovich B, Becker BL, Sliva DD, Gaab N. Development of tract-specific white matter pathways during early reading development in at-risk children and typical controls. Cerebral Cortex. 2016 Apr 25; doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw095. 2016. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfers T, Onnink AMH, Zwiers MP, Arias-Vasquez A, Hoogman M, Mostert JC, Kan CC, Slaats-Willemse D, Buitelaar JK, Franke B. Lower white matter microstructure in the superior longitudinal fasciculus is associated with increased response time variability in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience. 2015;40:344–351. doi: 10.1503/jpn.140154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]