Abstract
Language exerts a powerful influence on our concepts. We review evidence documenting the developmental origins of a precocious link between language and object categories in very young infants. This collection of studies documents a cascading process in which early links between language and cognition provide the foundation for later, more precise ones. We propose that, early in life, language promotes categorization at least in part through its status as a social, communicative signal. But over the first year, infants home in on the referential power of language and, by their second year, begin teasing apart distinct kinds of names (e.g., nouns, adjectives) and their relation to distinct kinds of concepts (e.g., object categories, properties). To complement this proposal, we also relate this evidence to several alternative accounts of language’s effect on categorization, appealing to similarity (labels-as-features), familiarity (auditory overshadowing), and communicative biases (natural pedagogy).
The power of human language comes from its links to our conceptual systems. In acquiring language, we acquire a means of encoding perceptual input as objects of thought (Fausey & Boroditsky, 2008; Frank, Everett, Fedorenko, & Gibson, 2008; Gleitman & Papafragou, 2005; Winawer et al., 2007), and a means of combining elemental concepts to form more complex ones (Chomsky, 2011; Condry & Spelke, 2008; Murphy, 1990). Language is also the bedrock of cultural transmission, providing an exceptionally powerful and efficient channel for sharing our thoughts and beliefs with others (Anggoro, Waxman, & Medin, 2008; Pinker & Jackendoff, 2005; Tomasello, 2000; Vygotsky, 1962; Waxman, 1999b). Although research in philosophy and psychology makes it quite clear that there are distinctions between language and thought, they are often so deeply intertwined in our experience of the world that they seem inseparable (Gleitman & Papafragou, 2005; Pinker, 2007). It is unsurprising, then, that some of the most compelling and enduring questions in the developmental and cognitive sciences have focused on identifying the links between language and thought, and how these are shaped over development (Gentner & Goldin-Meadow, 2003; Waxman, 1999b, 2002).
Over a half century of research has unearthed at least one striking link between language and one fundamental conceptual process, object categorization. Studies of this link reveal that ways in which objects are named guides learners’ organization of these objects into mental categories. When the same noun is applied consistently to a set of distinct objects, both infants and adults alike are more likely to represent them as members of the same object category (Gelman & Heyman, 1999; Keates & Graham, 2008; Lupyan, 2008; Lupyan, Rakison, & McClelland, 2007; Waxman, 1999b; Waxman & Booth, 2001; 2003; Waxman & Hall, 1993; Waxman & Markow, 1995). Conversely, hearing different nouns applied to a set of distinct objects draws learners’ attention to distinctions among objects, facilitating their representations as distinct individuals or distinct categories (Dewar & Xu, 2007; 2009; Feigenson & Halberda, 2008; Ferguson, Havy, & Waxman 2015; Keates & Graham, 2008; Landau & Shipley, 2001; Scott & Monesson, 2009; Waxman & Braun, 2005; Xu, 2002; Xu, Cote, & Baker, 2005; Zosh & Feigenson, 2009).
Categorization is a fundamental building block of cognition (Mandler & McDonough, 1993; Mareschal & Quinn, 2001; Mervis & Rosch, 1981; Murphy, 2004; Sloman, Malt, & Fridman, 2001; Smith & Medin, 1981), and thus this evidence documenting the power of naming on categorization has garnered considerable attention (e.g., Diesendruck, 2003; Gershkoff-Stowe, Thal, Smith, & Namy, 1997; Lupyan et al., 2007; Plunkett, 2008; Sloutsky & Fisher, 2004; Waxman & Gelman, 2009). When we identify two objects as members of the same category, we establish their equivalence, permitting us to identify new members of the category and to make inferences about non-obvious properties from one member of the category to another (Bhatt, Wasserman, & Reynolds, 1988; Murphy, 2004; Smith & Heise, 1992). This seemingly simple feat has tremendous consequences on subsequent learning; for example, by establishing the category DOG, we can learn from just one negative encounter to avoid all angry dogs (even ones we have not yet seen) instead of painfully and repeatedly learning from encounters in which each individual dog bares its teeth.
Categorization is also fundamental to word learning. To successfully learn the meaning of a novel word, infants and young children must map a phonological representation to the identifiable category, or referent, to which it refers. In other words, they must understand that the referent of a novel noun like fridge applies not only to the appliance in their own kitchen but also in others’. Recent research suggests that infants have established such mappings; that is, they extend even their earliest words beyond named exemplars to other members of the same object category (Bergelson & Swingley, 2012; Tincoff & Jusczyk, 2012). Most of infants’ early words are nouns, and most of these extend beyond distinct individuals (e.g., “Magic”) to categories (doggie). Moreover, infants’ ability to map nouns to object categories serves as a stepping stone for the acquisition of other kinds of words, including verbs and adjectives, because the meanings of these predicates are informed by the nouns that they take as arguments (Fisher, Gertner, Scott, & Yuan, 2010; Gleitman, 1990; Klibanoff & Waxman, 2000; Waxman & Lidz, 2006). From this perspective, then, infants’ and young children’s early links between language and object categories serve as an engine that catalyzes subsequent language and conceptual development.
Our goal in this paper is to summarize the evidence documenting the emergence of a link between naming and object categorization and how it is shaped in the first few years of life. We begin by describing a foundational study, one that demonstrates the power of naming on object categorization at 12 months of age. We then look ahead in development, pointing to evidence documenting that toddlers increasingly refine this link over the second year of life, as they cull distinct kinds of words in the input (e.g., nouns, adjectives, verbs) and link each to a distinct kind of referent (e.g., categories of objects, properties of objects, categories of events or relations). Next, we set our sights in a different direction, looking back in development to identify the origin of infants’ links between language and categorization in the first year of life.
This review – looking forward and backward in developmental time – reveals a cascading process in which infants’ earliest language-cognition links provide the foundation for later ones. To foreshadow, we propose that the power of language on cognition is initially grounded in its status as a social, communicative signal. Within the first year, infants home in on its referential status and, in the second year, they begin to tease apart the distinct kinds of words (e.g., nouns, verbs, adjectives) and link them to distinct kinds of reference.
We also discuss several alternative theoretical proposals. Some have attributed the link between language and cognition entirely to lower-level perceptual processes. On one view, labels-as-features, words promote object categorization simply because infants associate the words that co-occur with objects as perceptual ‘features’ of the objects themselves (Deng & Sloutsky, 2012; Sloutsky, 2010; Sloutsky & Fisher, 2012). On this view, because objects from the same category tend to co-occur with the same labels, naming (like any shared perceptual feature) increases the similarity among named objects and in this way promotes object categorization. However, as will become clear as our review unfolds, this view cannot account for the evidence. First, there is strong evidence that when names are paired systematically with objects, they consistently promote categorization, but that when other engaging sounds (e.g., tone sequences, backward speech) are paired systematically with objects, they engender no such boost to infants’ categorization. Second, this view cannot accommodate the fact that within the second year of life, different kinds of words highlight different kinds of commonalities among objects. The labels-as-features view has no account for why, at this juncture, nouns highlight category-based commonalities but adjectives highlight property-based commonalities, including color and texture.
Another low-level account focused on processing, auditory overshadowing, argues that the gap between language and other non-linguistic sounds can be reduced to an effect of auditory familiarity. Here the claim is that because infants are more familiar with the sounds of speech than to other non-linguistic sounds (Robinson & Sloutsky, 2007a; 2007b; Robinson, Best, Deng, & Sloutsky, 2012; Sloutsky & Robinson, 2008), and because it is less costly to process familiar than novel stimuli, non-linguistic sounds can ‘overshadow’ infants’ ability to process materials simultaneously presented in the visual modality (see also Lewkowicz, 1988a; 1988b). Therefore, although language appears to promote object categorization, it may in fact be merely less disruptive than the other less familiar sounds. While this account can capture some differences between linguistic and non-linguistic sounds, like the labels-as-features above, it is stretched to explain why different kinds of language (e.g., nouns, adjectives) which differ in meaning – but, critically, not in acoustic familiarity – have different conceptual consequences, or why, as we will discuss, a select group of unfamiliar signals also promotes categorization early in infancy.
Another relevant theory, natural pedagogy, is closer in spirit to our own position, but still differs considerably, especially with regard to the developmental processes underlying the link between language and categorization in the first two years of life. Natural pedagogy asserts that the power of language comes, at least in part, from its social, communicative status, and we agree. But natural pedagogy also claims that other communicative signals (e.g., eye gaze, pointing) are on par with language vis-à-vis their effects on cognition (Csibra & Gergely, 2006; 2009; Csibra & Shamsudheen, 2015; Futó, Téglás, Csibra, & Gergely, 2010; Hernik & Csibra, 2015; Marno, Davelaar, & Csibra, 2014; 2016; Yoon, Johnson, & Csibra, 2008), that human infants are born with an expectation that information conveyed by a pedagogical partner (e.g., a parent) via ostensive, communicative signals is “kind-relevant”, and that as a result communicative signals (including, but not limited to, language) bias infants toward establishing categories of object kinds. We agree that infant cognition is guided by the social, communicative status of language in the first year. Where we differ is in our view of the function of language as primarily kind-relevant and in our view of developmental processes underlying language over the course of this first year. In our view, language “parts company ” from the other communicative signals in the first year, as infants pinpoint with increasing precision the range of meaning that can be conveyed with language.
We discuss these alternative accounts at various junctures in this review, as evidence relevant to each account is introduced.
Linking language and categorization – A foundational study
Waxman and Markow (1995) offered the first evidence of a link between language and object categorization in infants who were on the verge of producing their first words. They recruited 12-month-old infants to participate in a classic categorization task, one that included a familiarization phase and a test phase (see Figure 1). During familiarization, infants were shown several members of a category (e.g., ANIMAL), each accompanied by a phrase. What varied was the particular phrase infants heard. Infants in a Word condition heard a novel noun applied to each object (e.g., “Look! This is a blick! … Do you see the blick?”); those in a No Word control condition heard phrases that drew their attention to the objects but included no novel words (e.g., “Look what’s here … Do you like it?”). At test, infants viewed two novel objects simultaneously. One was a novel member of the now-familiar category (e.g., a new animal), and the other a member of a new object category to which infants had not yet been exposed (e.g., a piece of fruit).
This design took advantage of decades of research in infant cognition (Colombo & Bundy, 1983; Eimas & Quinn, 1994; Fantz, 1963; Spelke & Kestenbaum, 1986) documenting that if infants notice the commonality among the objects presented during familiarization, then they show a preference for the novel over the familiar test object and that conversely, infants who fail to detect this category during familiarization show no preference at test.
Building on this logic, Waxman and Markow (1995) manipulated the design to consider the contribution of naming. They reasoned that, if novel nouns support object categorization in infants as young as 12 months, then infants in the Word condition should more successfully form categories than those not hearing novel words (No Word condition). Their results supported this prediction, documenting that by 12 months of age, infants have begun to establish a link between object naming and object categorization (for further evidence at 12 months, see Ferguson, Havy, & Waxman, 2015; Fulkerson & Haaf, 2006; Waxman & Braun, 2005).
Balaban and Waxman (1997) provided additional evidence for the power of language in slightly younger infants. They compared the effect of novel words versus tone sequences on 9-month-olds’ categorization. Once again, infants in a Word condition heard a naming phrase accompanying each familiarization object. But infants in a Tones condition heard a sequence of sine-wave tones accompanying each object. These tone sequences were carefully-matched to match the Word condition in mean frequency, duration and pause length. They reasoned that, if any consistently applied sound promotes 9-month-olds’ object categorization, then infants in both of these conditions should succeed in forming the category; however, if language exerts a unique effect on categorization as early as 9 months, then infants in the Word condition but not the Tones condition should succeed. The results were clear: infants in the Word condition successfully formed categories, but those in the Tones condition performed at chance level. This documented an advantage for novel words over carefully-matched non-linguistic control stimuli in infants as young as 9 months of age.
Together, these studies provided evidence that the link between language and categories is established early, and that it is not built up from associations between words in infants’ existing vocabulary (Smith, 2000). After all, infants at 9 and 12 months of age produce only a few, if any, words on their own. Instead, the data reveal that a link between language and object categories is not the result of lexical development, but instead is in place early enough to support infants’ vocabulary development from the start.
Notice that neither the labels-as-features nor the auditory overshadowing accounts can account for both of these results on their own. The labels-as-features account best explains Waxman and Markow’s (1995) finding that infants who count noun consistently applied to a set of objects more reliably categorize them than do infants in a No Word condition. On the labels-as-features account, for infants in the Word (but not the No Word) condition, the shared novel noun increases the similarity naming the familiarization objects, thereby supports categorization. Infants in the No Word condition did not benefit from this increased similarity and therefore failed to form the categories. But this account cannot accommodate Balaban and Waxman’s (1997) finding that novel tone sequences – which were also applied consistently to all familiarization objects – failed to exert this advantageous effect. If any consistently-paired auditory ‘feature’ account can increase the similarity of the objects with which it is paired, then both words and tones should exert the same influence.
On the other hand, auditory overshadowing can explain Balaban and Waxman’s (1997) finding, but not Waxman and Markow’s (1995). In the case of Balaban and Waxman (1997), auditory overshadowing would suggest that infants hearing language (but not tones) formed object categories because the tone sequences were less familiar than language. But the auditory overshadowing account cannot explain why certain kinds of language (e.g., “Look at the toma”) facilitate categorization while other kinds of language (e.g., “Look at this”) fail to do so. In short, each of these alternative proposals can accommodate one set of findings, but neither can explain both.
These results also bear on the proposal concerning natural pedagogy (Csibra & Gergely, 2009). In Waxman and Markow (1995), all infants were introduced to the familiarization objects in conjunction with human speech – a pedagogical cue. Although infants in the Word condition (“Look at the toma”) successfully formed object categories, those in the No Word condition (“Look at this!”) did not. This reveals that by 12 months, infants have precise expectations about the functions on language: Novel nouns, but not any referring phrase, refer to object categories. Thus, infants do not interpret all communicative signals as kind-relevant (cf. Csibra & Gergely, 2009); rather, by their first birthdays, when infants begin to build their own productive lexicons, they have distinguished naming from other functions of language and link object naming alone to object categorization.
This evidence from 9- to 12-month-old infants, although impressive, also raised new developmental questions. When do infants establish more precise links, mapping certain kinds of words (e.g., nouns) to object categories, but other kinds of words (e.g., adjectives, verbs) to different kinds of meaning (e.g., object properties, event categories)?
Specifying the link: A look forward in development
The links between language and categorization expressed in 12-month-olds do not remain constant across development. On the contrary, infants’ expectations about naming become increasingly precise during their second year. During this time, infants tease apart the nouns from the other grammatical forms (e.g., adjectives, verbs) and map them specifically to object categories (rather than surface properties (like color) or actions in which they are involved (like running). Consider, for example, a scene in which a group of horses jumps over the fence. Infants in the second year of life focus on different aspects of this scene, depending upon how it is described. So do older children and adults. For example, nouns (e.g., ‘Look! They’re horses!’) focus our attention on the object category. But verbs (e.g., ‘Look! They’re running!’) direct our attention to the action, and adjectives (e.g., ‘Look! They’re white!’) refer neither to the objects or event, but to a property of the objects. We know that even infants can use the position of a word within a sentence to distinguish among grammatical categories (Hall, Veltkamp, & Turkel, 2004; Höhle, Weissenborn, Kiefer, & Schulz, 2004; Mintz, 2003; Shi, 2014; Waxman & Lidz, 2006; Weisleder & Waxman, 2010) and, by 18 to 24 months, they forge increasingly precise links between distinct grammatical forms and their distinct kinds of meaning. They link nouns to object categories, verbs to actions and relations among objects, and adjectives to object properties.
These more specific links between distinct kinds of words and distinct kinds of meaning unfold in a cascading fashion (see Waxman and Lidz, 2006 for a comprehensive review). First, by 13 months, infants tease apart the nouns from other grammatical categories and link them specifically to object categories. Next, with this noun-object category link in place, they go on to forge the more precise links for predicates, including adjectives and verbs, whose meaning depends in part upon the nouns they take as arguments.
Until roughly 12 months of age, infants appear to be “generalists” when it comes to linking words and concept. Novel words, be they presented as nouns or adjectives, highlight any kind of commonality among objects (e.g., category-based or property-based commonalities) (Waxman, 1999; Waxman & Booth, 2003; Waxman & Markow, 1995). A clear demonstration of this can be found in a study by Waxman and Booth (2003) in which they presented 11-month-old infants with a set of four objects (e.g., 4 different purple horses) that shared both a category-based (horse) and property-based (purple) commonality (see Figure 2). At issue was whether infants focused on categories or properties, and whether their focus was shaped by the language they heard as they viewed these objects (Waxman & Booth, 2003). To assess this, infants participated in either a property extension test (e.g., pitting a new purple horse against a new green horse) or a category extension test (e.g., pitting a new purple horse against a new purple chair). They reasoned as follows: if infants expect that different kinds of words refer to different kinds of meaning, then their performance in the Noun and Adjective conditions should differ. More specifically, if they map nouns to object categories and adjectives to object properties, then (1) infants for whom the familiarization objects were introduced with a novel noun should successfully extend the noun to another horse but not to other objects sharing only color, but not category membership, and (2) infants who were introduced to novel adjectives should successfully extend them to the object property, but not the category. Demonstrating their status as generalists at this age, Waxman and Booth (2003) found that 11-month-olds who heard either kind of novel word (either nouns or adjectives) focused on either kind of commonality (category- or property-based); they extended the novel word either by property or by category, depending on their test condition. In contrast, 11-month-olds in a No Word condition (Can you give me that one?) performed at chance.
But infants do not remain generalists for long. By 13 months, they have teased apart the nouns in the input and have begun to link them specifically to object categories, but not object properties. In other words, in the categorization task described above, 13-month-olds extend novel nouns on the basis of category-based, but not property-based commonalities (Waxman, 1999a). Nevertheless, 13-month-olds have not yet acquired a comparably precise expectation for adjectives. Instead, for most of their second year, infants continue to link novel adjectives to either category-based (e.g., horse) or property-based (e.g., color, texture) commonalities (Booth & Waxman, 2009; Imai & Gentner, 1997; Waxman, 1999a; Waxman & Booth, 2003), only later do they begin mapping novel adjectives specifically to property-based, and not category-based, commonalities (Waxman & Markow, 1998). Moreover, infants’ expectations for novel verbs appears to follow an even more protracted developmental course: only by 24 months do infants reliably map novel verbs to event categories rather than object categories (Arunachalam & Waxman, 2010; Arunachalam, Escovar, Hansen, & Waxman, 2013; Syrett, Arunachalam, & Waxman, 2013; Tomasello & Kruger, 2009; Waxman, Lidz, Braun, & Lavin, 2009; Yuan & Fisher, 2009).
By tracing infants’ expectations for novel words through the second year of life, a developmental cascade becomes evident, one in which infants discover that there are distinct kinds of words and that each refers to a distinct kind of meaning. This cascade, in which precise expectations for nouns paves the way for expectations for predicate forms, poses challenges for accounts that appeal to perception alone.
The labels-as-features perspective asserts that words are nothing more than perceptual features of the objects to which they are applied. If this were correct, then it is puzzling that novel nouns highlight category-based (but not property-based) commonalities among objects at 13 months (Waxman, 1999a; Waxman & Booth, 2003). This outcome reveals that labels do more than simply increase the perceived similarity among objects, otherwise novel nouns should highlight both category and property-based commonalities equally.
Arguments for auditory overshadowing fare no better in accounting for this developmental cascade. After all, infants in the Noun, Adjective and Verb conditions in these various experiments were all listening to speech. In fact, they heard the very same novel wordforms paired with the very same sets of objects; thus infants’ familiarity with the wordforms and the objects are held constant across conditions and experiments. The only thing that varied was the grammatical context in which a novel word – the same novel word – appeared. Infants’ distinct responses to different kinds of words in these experiments reveal the insufficiency of an auditory overshadowing account. Infants’ performance is mediated by more than the ‘familiarity’ of speech; they are also sensitive to distinctions among distinct kinds of words and the concepts to which they refer.
Finally, these findings also reveal shortcomings in the predictions of natural pedagogy, highlighting that this proposal requires greater precision. Communicative signals of all kinds – including language, eye gaze and pointing – can highlight either objects and events (Liszkowski & Carpenter, 2007; Namy & Waxman, 2005; Peirce, 1932). But only language can single out which of the myriad possible commonalities, present within a particular set of entities, a speaker is referring to. For infants as young as 13 months of age, language does more than highlight object categories or kinds. By this point, infants use the grammatical form a novel word to shift their perspective on the scene at hand.
The origins of the link: Looking back into infants’ first year of life
In more recent work in our lab, we have shifted our focus to looking back in developmental time. Our goal is to uncover the origin of infants’ earliest links between language and cognition, and to trace how this link unfolds in infants’ first year.
As a first step in this direction, Fulkerson and Waxman (2007) adapted Balaban and Waxman’s (1997) categorization task to examine the effect of language on categorization in 6-month-old infants (see Figure 3). In the familiarization phase, infants viewed eight images from a single category (e.g., dinosaurs) one at a time, in random order on a screen. What varied was the auditory input accompanying each image. Infants either heard a novel word (e.g., Look at the modi! Do you see the modi!) or the sequence of sine-wave tones. At test, infants viewed two new images, presented in silence – a new member of the familiar category (e.g., another dinosaur) and an object from a novel category (e.g., a fish). Infants who listened to language during familiarization formed object categories, as witnessed by their reliable preference for the novel object at test. In contrast infants who listened to tone sequences performed at chance levels. Thus, at 6 months, when infants are just beginning to comprehend their first words (Bergelson & Swingley, 2012; 2013, Bergelson & Swingley, 2014; Tincoff & Jusczyk, 1999; 2012), they have already begun to link language and object categories.
Armed with this evidence, Ferry, Hespos, and Waxman (2010) considered still younger infants, extending this task to 3- and 4-month-old infants. The results were surprising, and revealed an advantage for language over tones vis-à-vis categorization even in these very young infants: although 3- and 4-month-olds listening to language successfully formed object categories, those listening to sine-wave tone sequences performed at chance levels, just like at 6 and 12 months (Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007).
These results reveal strong developmental continuity in infants’ response to language versus tones in the first year of life. They also illuminate a surprisingly precocious link between language and categorization, one that is in place early enough to support infants’ very first forays in language and cognitive development. But why does listening to human language ‘boost’ infant cognition so early in development? It is unlikely that 3-month-old infants understand the meanings of any words (Fenson et al., 1993; Frank, Braginsky, & Yurovsky, 2016). Indeed, there is little evidence that they can even parse individuals words from the ongoing stream of language (Aslin, 1993; Bortfeld, Morgan, Golinkoff, & Rathbun, 2005; Jusczyk & Aslin, 1995; Seidl, Tincoff, Baker, & Cristia, 2014). What is it, then, that underlies the cognitive advantage conferred by language at 3 and 4 months? It must be different than at 12 months, because Waxman and Markow’s (1995) study clearly demonstrated that, by 12 months, identifying a novel word in the speech stream is critical (recall that infants formed object categories when they heard a novel noun consistently applied to the familiarization objects, but not when they heard the same kinds of phrases with no novel word (e.g., ‘Look at this!’). If 3- and 4-month-olds do not yet parse distinct words from the continuous stream of speech, then what is the mechanism by which language confers its advantage?
Ferry et al. (2010) proposed that for 3- and 4-month-olds, simply listening to language might promote object categorization. Previous studies have shown that infants prefer listening to human speech over other, non-speech sounds (Shultz & Vouloumanos, 2010; Vouloumanos, Hauser, Werker, & Martin, 2010). Of course, a preference for speech cannot explain why infants link speech to their construal of the world (that is, the objects they view in our tasks). Perhaps listening to speech not only engages infants’ attention but also promotes their learning. One intriguing aspect of the studies on infants’ preferences for language is that, early on, infants prefer both human speech and nonhuman primate vocalizations over other sounds, suggesting that they tune their preferences to human speech over the first months of life (Shultz, Vouloumanos, Bennett, & Pelphrey, 2014; Vouloumanos & Werker, 2004, 2007). Might nonhuman primate vocalizations also promote 3- and 4-month-olds object categorization?
To address this possibility, Ferry, Hespos, and Waxman (2013) examined the effect of listening to two new sounds – nonhuman primate vocalizations and backward speech – on infants’ object categorization at 3-, 4-, and 6-months. The design was identical to the studies by Fulkerson and Waxman (2007) and Ferry et al. (2010); what varied were the sounds infants listened to during the familiarization period. For half of the infants, the familiarization images were accompanied by a vocalization from a blue-eyed Madagascar lemur (Eulemur macaco flavifrons); for the others, the images were accompanied by a segment of backward speech (the language stimuli from prior experiments, played in reverse). If the initial link between language and cognition, like infants’ initial preferences, encompasses human speech and nonhuman primate vocalizations, then 3- and 4-month-olds listening to lemur vocalizations should successfully form object categories. Alternatively, if any complex sound promotes object categorization at this young age, then infants listening to either lemur vocalizations or backward speech should successfully form categories.
These results of this study, testing the breadth of sounds that promote 3- and 4-month-old’s categorization, were clear. Infants listening to backward speech failed to form categories at any age, echoing the results with sine-wave tone sequences at the same ages in Ferry et al. (2010) and Fulkerson et al. (2007) with a more complex auditory signal. In contrast, the lemur vocalizations conferred the same cognitive advantage as listening to human language: 3- and 4-month-olds in the lemur condition successfully formed object categories, performing identically at test as infants in Fulkerson and Waxman’s study with human speech. Yet this effect was short-lived; by 6 months, infants had tuned the link specifically to language. At 6 months, lemur vocalizations no longer conferred infants any benefit in categorization (Ferry et al., 2013).
This work offers two insights into the origins of infants’ earliest links between language and cognition. First, at 3 and 4 months, the link is sufficiently broad to encompass vocalizations of both humans and nonhuman primates. Second, by 6 months, infants tune this initially-broad link to the signal that will ultimately carry meaning, human speech.
These results also posed new challenges to alternative accounts for the link between language and cognition in infancy. First, the auditory overshadowing account cannot accommodate the facilitative effect of lemur vocalizations on 3- and 4-month-olds’ object categorization. Lemur calls are certainly unfamiliar to 3- and 4-month-olds, yet they facilitated (rather than hindered) infants’ object categorization. Auditory overshadowing also fails to account for the finding that infants tune out the effect of lemur vocalizations by 6 months. After all, the assumption underlying the overshadowing account rests on the processing load imposed by an unfamiliar versus familiar signal. Yet infants’ exposure to lemur vocalizations likely remains sparse – and therefore constant – between 3 and 6 months.
These results also expose limitations in the theory of natural pedagogy, a theory that has not engaged key developmental questions, including which signals very young infants identify as communicative and how the pedagogical force of these signals changes over the first years. Ferry et al.’s (2010; 2013) results provide clear evidence that what counts as a communicative signal changes with development.
In subsequent work, we have gone further to consider the processes that mediate infants’ interpretation signals like lemur calls and tone sequences over the first year, pinpointing the role of passive and communicative experience.
A closer look: How do infants ‘tune’ the link between language and object categorization
Ferry et al.’s (2013) results documented the first evidence that the link between language and categorization may be ‘tuned’ early in development. Tuning processes are ubiquitous in infant perceptual development (e.g., face perception, speech perception; Krentz & Corina, 2008; Lewkowicz & Ghazanfar, 2009; Maurer & Werker, 2013; Palmer, Fais, Golinkoff, & Werker, 2012; Pascalis et al., 2014; Quinn, Lee, Pascalis, & Tanaka, 2015; Scott & Monesson, 2009; Werker & Tees, 1984). But the results reported by Ferry et al. (2013) document more than just perceptual tuning. Instead, their results were the first to document that infants tune the link between language and categorization in the first 6 months of life.
With this effect as a foundation, we have gone on to examine the relative contributions of maturation and experience as infants tune this link (Perszyk & Waxman, 2016; Perszyk, Ferguson & Waxman, in revision) (see Figure 4).
How far can experience take us? Documenting the effect of “mere exposure” to non-language sounds
In one recent line of research, we asked whether and how infants’ experience contributed to tuning this link between language and categorization. Perhaps infants’ frequent exposure to human speech in their everyday environments permits them to maintain the link between speech and object categorization while ‘tuning out’ the influence of nonhuman primate vocalizations, which are likely absent in their environments.
One way to assess the role of experience is to manipulate it experimentally. A signature of experience-based tuning processes is the powerful role of later exposure: Once infants have tuned out an earlier sensitivity, this sensitivity may be re-instated if infants are re-exposed to the signal anew, during what is known as a ‘sensitive period’ (Johnson & Newport, 1989; Kuhl, Tsao, & Liu, 2003; Werker & Hensch, 2015). Might this signature of experience-based tuning be evident in the link between a signal and categorization? If infants’ experience is essential, then exposing infants to lemur vocalizations might permit them to ‘re-open’ the link to categorization.
Perszyk and Waxman (2016) addressed this question by systematically manipulating 7-month-old infants’ exposure to lemur vocalizations. When infants entered the lab’s waiting room, they listened to a 10-minute audio track comprised of instrumental music (e.g., a Bach quartet), interspersed at irregular intervals with several distinct lemur vocalizations. This provided infants with a total of 2 minutes of passive exposure to lemur vocalizations. Importantly, these vocalizations were not connected to any communicative function. Next, infants entered the testing room to participate in the same categorization task while listening to lemur vocalizations (as in Ferry et al., 2013). If experience is instrumental in tuning the link, then even this brief exposure with lemur vocalizations should be enough for 7-month-olds to re-instate the earlier link between lemur vocalizations and object categorization.
This prediction was borne out. In contrast to their peers provided with no such exposure (Ferry, Hespos, & Waxman, 2013), 7-month-olds who had been exposed to lemur vocalizations in the lab successfully formed object categories while listening to lemur vocalizations (Perszyk & Waxman, 2016). This identifies infants’ flexibility and a critical role for experience in tuning the link to cognition: even 2 minutes of exposure permitted 7-month-olds to link lemur vocalizations to categorization. Without this exposure, the link had been severed.
But perhaps exposure to any sound – not only those that initially promote categorization – would have been sufficient to promote infants’ categorization. This is the prediction of the auditory overshadowing account. Perszyk and Waxman (2016) provided clear evidence against this possibility by exposing another group of infants to the same classical music audio track but, this time, replacing the lemur vocalizations with segments of backward speech, a signal that fails to promote object categorization at any age (Ferry et al., 2013). Although infants’ exposure to backward speech or lemur vocalizations were identical in the two conditions, the results were quite different: infants exposed to backward speech failed to form object categories in our task. This striking contrast suggests that exposure may be instrumental in maintaining a link between an auditory signal and categorization only if that signal is part of the initially-privileged set of sounds that infants previously linked to categorization. A goal of our ongoing work is to specify the range of signals that are initially privileged in this way.
Can infants interpret otherwise arbitrary sounds as communicative? The power of embedding signals in a social-communicative exchange
In a complementary line of work, we have asked about the developmental fate of signals that fall outside the initially-privileged set – like sine-wave tone sequences and backward speech – signals that infants consistently fail to link to object categorization throughout their first year (Ferry, Hespos, & Waxman, 2010; Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007). As adults, we can flexibly link many signals to meaning, even unnatural signals like the beeps of Morse code. But what about infants? Might there be some path by which even infants will privilege these otherwise inert sounds to communicative status and link them to categorization? Or does this capacity come only later, after they have established a foundational communicative system, such as language?
We reasoned that if we embedded these sounds in communicative episodes, then infants might interpret them as communicative. At issue, though, was whether by raising them to communicative status, these signals might then (like language) promote infants’ categorization. Our hypothesis was motivated by three other lines of research. First, myriad studies have demonstrated that even from birth, infants are drawn not only to speech, but also to other communicative stimuli. For example, infants prefer to look at face-like stimuli over non-faces (Farroni et al., 2005; Valenza, Simion, & Cassia, 1996) and to look at communicative gestures over non-communicative pantomime (Krentz & Corina, 2008). Second, beginning around 6 months, infants appear to represent the communicative function of some signals in social interactions (Grossmann, Parise, & Friederici, 2010; Krehm, Onishi, & Vouloumanos, 2014; Lloyd-Fox, Széplaki-Köllőd, Yin, & Csibra, 2015; Parise & Csibra, 2013; Vouloumanos, Martin, & Onishi, 2014; Vouloumanos, Onishi, & Pogue, 2012). Finally, as discussed with respect to natural pedagogy, a range of communicative signals beyond speech (e.g., pointing and eye-gaze) appear to shape infants’ learning, at least in some contexts. Of particular interest to us, given that we have been investigating object categorization, is the claim that infants encode category-relevant properties of novel objects more effectively in communicative contexts than in non-communicative contexts (Csibra & Gergely, 2009; Futó et al., 2010; Hernik & Csibra, 2015; Wu, Gopnik, Richardson, & Kirkham, 2011; Yoon et al., 2008). Together, these lines of research raise an intriguing possibility: if infants are first introduced to the otherwise inert sound as if it, like language, is a communicative signal, this sound may be elevated to communicative status and might subsequently promote infants’ object categorization.
To address this possibility, we turned our focus to sine-wave tone sequences, asking whether they might, in fact, promote 6-month-olds’ object categorization if, just prior to the categorization task, we introduced infants to the tones as if they were a communicative signal. We created a brief (2-minute) vignette depicting a dialogue between two actors. One of the actors spoke in English and the other responded using sine-wave tone sequences. This vignette clearly demonstrated that the tones served a communicative function. After viewing this vignette, infants participated in the categorization task while listening to tone sequences (Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007). The vignette had a remarkable impact: after observing the tone sequences embedded in a social, communicative exchange, 6-month-olds successfully categorized while listening to tones, something we had not yet seen in any prior study at any age (Ferguson & Waxman, 2016). This suggests that when an otherwise inert signal in introduced in the context of a social, communicative exchange, 6-month-old infants elevate this signal to communicative status and forge an entirely new link between this signal and categorization.
Moreover, this effect is related specifically to communicative information; simply familiarizing infants to the tones – absent any communicative exchange – does not promote their use in categorization. To demonstrate this, we familiarized another group of infants to precisely the same tone sequences, but uncoupled them from the communicative episode, offering no evidence that tones served a communicative function. In this condition, we modified the vignette so that the ‘conversation’ (i.e., the speech and tone sounds) play in the background– as if the sounds were playing on the radio – while the two actors engaged in a separate, cooperative task. Although infants in this condition heard the precisely the same tones for precisely the same amount of time, they failed to form the categories in the subsequent categorization, performing instead at chance levels. This contrast between infants’ success in the communicative condition and failure in the non-communicative control condition reveals the power of communicative exposure alone in linking the tones to object categorization at 6 months of age.
This outcome provides the strongest evidence to date against auditory overshadowing (Robinson et al., 2012; Robinson & Sloutsky, 2007b). Ferguson and Waxman (2016) held the familiarity of the tones constant across both conditions: infants in the two conditions had the exact same amount of exposure to the tones before the categorization task. Familiarity alone, therefore, cannot explain why only those 6-month-olds exposed to tones as a communicative signal later succeeded in categorizing objects while listening to tones.
Our interpretation of the power of communicative experience in linking an otherwise inert sound (e.g., tones) to object categorization is consistent with the proposal for natural pedagogy (Csibra & Gergely, 2009). After learning that the tones were communicative, listening to tones seems to have engendered a communicative context that biased infants toward kind-relevant, generalizable information. Nevertheless, this finding also reveals that the theory of natural pedagogy (and any theory relying on infants’ interpretation of communicative signals) must specify how infants identify which signals in their environment are communicative in the first place and how their interpretation of these signals is shaped over development. In future research, it will be important to manipulate systematically infants’ experience with an inert sound such as tones, and to subsequently assess its impact on cognition. This will offer a more nuanced developmental view of how a signal becomes communicative and, from this view, “pedagogical.”
A developmental cascade: Infants’ expectations about “language” changes over the first 12 months
These investigations into the origins of the link – its initial, broad state and the processes by which it is tuned thereafter – sharpen our understanding of how an early link between language and object categorization evolves early in development. We propose that, at 3 and 4 months, an initially-privileged set of sounds – encompassing the human speech and non-human primate vocalizations (Ferry et al., 2010; 2013) – promotes categorization by broadly engaging infants’ attention. By 6 months, this link is tuned to communicative signals through complementary processes of passive exposure (maintaining the links of those signals to which infants are frequently exposed; Perszyk & Waxman, 2016) and social-communicative exposure (capable of privileging otherwise inert signals to communicative status; Ferguson & Waxman, 2016). Later, as infants approach their first birthday, this broad effect of communicative signals begins to be refined as infants discover which kinds of language are particularly relevant to categorization (Fennell & Waxman, 2010; Hollich, Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff, 2000; Marcus, Fernandes, & Johnson, 2012; May & Werker, 2014; Namy & Waxman, 2000; A. L. Woodward & Hoyne, 1999). This discovery prompts a shift in attention from those signals that are communicative to the ways in which labels alone are referential. As infants learn about the referential capacities of different kinds of labels, language becomes capable of more than broadly engaging infants’ attention but also of highlighting different conceptual interpretations of the very same objects (Booth & Waxman, 2003; 2009; Waxman & Booth, 2003). Only with additional evidence can we identify the mechanisms underlying these shifts.
In these ways, although language promotes categorization throughout the first two years of life, the nature of this influence evolves during this period along with the developing capacities of the infant. Proposals that appeal only to infants’ perceptual experience and processing of language (Robinson et al., 2012; Sloutsky & Fisher, 2012) cannot capture this dynamic, cascading developmental process. Likewise, although we propose that these links between language and concepts are grounded in infants’ representation of language as a communicative signal, proposals that posit an enduring, static bias in communicative contexts (Csibra & Gergely, 2009) also fail to capture this developmental trajectory. While the mechanisms posited by both of these views surely have some role to play in relating language to infants’ cognition, neither appears sufficient in explaining the evidence at hand.
Future directions
An important goal for future investigations is to identify which other cognitive capacities – in addition to object categorization – are shaped by language in the first year of life. There are reasons to suspect that language may cast a relatively wide facilitative net (Vouloumanos & Waxman, 2014); evidence has already begun to accumulate, suggesting that language promotes other fundamental learning processes, including abstract rule learning (Ferguson & Lew-Williams, 2016; Marcus, Fernandes, & Johnson, 2007) and associative learning (Reeb-Sutherland et al., 2011). Identifying the breadth of language’s influences – and the cognitive mechanisms that undergird them – will provide insights into the status of infants’ earliest links between language and cognition, how they are forged early in development, and will ultimately bring into sharper focus how language and thought become entwined.
References
- Anggoro FK, Waxman SR, Medin DL. Naming Practices and the Acquisition of Key Biological Concepts. Psychological Science. 2008;19(4):314. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02086.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arunachalam S, Waxman SR. Meaning from syntax: Evidence from 2-year-olds. Cognition. 2010;114:442–446. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.015. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arunachalam S, Escovar E, Hansen MA, Waxman SR. Out of sight, but not out of mind: 21-month-olds use syntactic information to learn verbs even in the absence of a corresponding event. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2013;28(4):417–425. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2011.641744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aslin RN. Segmentation of fluent speech into words: Learning models and the role of maternal input. In: de Boysson-Bardies B, Al E, editors. Developmental Neurocognition: Speech and Face Processing in the First Year of Life. 1st. Norwell, MA: Kluwer; 1993. pp. 305–315. [Google Scholar]
- Bergelson E, Swingley D. At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109(9):3253–3258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113380109. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1113380109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bergelson E, Swingley D. The acquisition of abstract words by young infants. Cognition. 2013;127(3):391–397. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.02.011. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.02.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bergelson E, Swingley D. Early word comprehension in infants: Replication and extension. Language Learning and Development. 2015;11(4):369–380. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2014.979387. http://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2014.979387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bhatt RS, Wasserman EA, Reynolds WF, Knauss KS. Conceptual behavior in pigeons: Categorization of both familiar and novel examples from four classes of natural and artificial stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behavior Processes. 1988;14(3):219–234. http://doi.org/10.1037//0097-7403.14.3.219. [Google Scholar]
- Booth A, Waxman SR. Mapping words to the world in infancy: Infants’ expectations for count nouns and adjectives. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2003;4(3):357–381. [Google Scholar]
- Booth A, Waxman SR. A horse of a different color: Specifying with precision infants’ mappings of novel nouns and adjectives. Child Development. 2009;80(1):15–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01242.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bortfeld H, Morgan JL, Golinkoff RM, Rathbun K. “Mommy” and Me: Familiar Names Help Launch Babies into Speech-Stream Segmentation. Psychological Science. 2005;16(4):298–304. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01531.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky N. Language and Other Cognitive Systems. What Is Special About Language? Language Learning and Development. 2011;7(4):263–278. http://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2011.584041. [Google Scholar]
- Colombo J, Bundy RS. Infant response to auditory familiarity and novelty. Infant Behavior and Development. 1983;6(2):305–311. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(83)80039-3. [Google Scholar]
- Condry KF, Spelke ES. The development of language and abstract concepts: The case of natural number. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2008;137(1):22–38. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.1.22. http://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.137.1.22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Csibra G, Gergely G. Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. In: Munakata Y, Johnson MH, editors. Processes of change in brain and cognitive development Attention and performance XXI. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2006. pp. 249–274. [Google Scholar]
- Csibra G, Gergely G. Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009;13(4):148–153. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Csibra G, Shamsudheen R. Nonverbal Generics: Human Infants Interpret Objects as Symbols of Object Kinds. Annual Review of Psychology. 2015;66(1):689–710. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015232. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawson C, Gerken L. From domain-generality to domain-sensitivity: 4-Month-olds learn an abstract repetition rule in music that 7-month-olds do not. Cognition. 2009;111(3):378–382. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.010. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deng W, Sloutsky VM. Carrot Eaters or Moving Heads: Inductive Inference Is Better Supported by Salient Features Than by Category Labels. Psychological Science. 2012;23(2):178–186. doi: 10.1177/0956797611429133. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611429133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dewar K, Xu F. Do 9-month-old infants expect distinct words to refer to kinds? Developmental Psychology. 2007;43(5):1227–1238. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1227. http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dewar K, Xu F. Do early nouns refer to kinds or distinct shapes? Evidence from 10-month-old infants. Psychological Science. 2009;20(2):252–257. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02278.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02278.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diesendruck G. Categories for names or names for categories? The interplay between domain-specific conceptual structure and language. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2003;18(5–6):759–787. http://doi.org/10.1080/01690960344000116. [Google Scholar]
- Eimas PD, Quinn PC. Studies on the formation of perceptually based basic-level categories in young infants. Child Development. 1994;65(3):903–917. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fantz RL. Pattern vision in newborn infants. Science. 1963;140(3564):296–297. doi: 10.1126/science.140.3564.296. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.140.3564.296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farroni T, Johnson MH, Menon E, Zulian L, Faraguna D, Csibra G. Newborns’ preference for face-relevant stimuli: effects of contrast polarity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2005;102(47):17245. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0502205102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fausey CM, Boroditsky L. English and Spanish speakers remember causal agents differently. Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. 2008:64–70. [Google Scholar]
- Feigenson L, Halberda J. Conceptual knowledge increases infants’ memory capacity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105(29):9926–9930. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0709884105. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0709884105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fennell CT, Waxman SR. What paradox? Referential cues allow for infant use of phonetic detail in word learning. Child Development. 2010;81(5):1376–1383. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01479.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01479.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Thal D, Bates E, Hartung J, et al. The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s guide and technical manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson B, Lew-Williams C. Communicative signals support abstract rule learning by 7-month-old infants. Scientific Reports. 2016;6 doi: 10.1038/srep25434. http://doi.org/10.1038/srep25434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson B, Waxman SR. What the [beep]? Six-month-olds link novel communicative signals to meaning. Cognition. 2016;146(C):185–189. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.020. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson B, Havy M, Waxman SR. The precision of 12-month-old infants’ link between language and categorization predicts vocabulary size at 12 and 18 months. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01319. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferry AL, Hespos SJ, Waxman SR. Categorization in 3‐and 4‐month‐old infants: an advantage of words over tones. Child Development. 2010;81(2):472–479. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01408.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01408.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferry AL, Hespos SJ, Waxman SR. Nonhuman primate vocalizations support categorization in very young human infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2013;110(38):15231–15235. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1221166110. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1221166110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher C, Gertner Y, Scott RM, Yuan S. Syntactic bootstrapping. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 2010;1:143–149. doi: 10.1002/wcs.17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frank MC, Braginsky M, Yurovsky D. Wordbank: An open repository for developmental vocabulary data. Journal of Child Language. 2016;18:1–18. doi: 10.1017/S0305000916000209. http://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000916000209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frank MC, Everett DL, Fedorenko E, Gibson E. Number as a cognitive technology: Evidence from Pirahã language and cognition. Cognition. 2008;108(3):819–824. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.04.007. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fulkerson AL, Haaf RA. Does object naming aid 12-month-olds’ formation of novel object categories? First Language. 2006;26(4):347–361. http://doi.org/10.1177/0142723706059217. [Google Scholar]
- Fulkerson AL, Waxman SR. Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization: Evidence from 6-and 12-month-olds. Cognition. 2007;105(1):218–228. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.09.005. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Futó J, Téglás E, Csibra G, Gergely G. Communicative function demonstration induces kind-based artifact representation in preverbal infants. Cognition. 2010;117(1):1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.06.003. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gelman SA, Heyman GD. Carrot-eaters and creature-believers: The effects of lexicalization on children’s inferences about social categories. Psychological Science. 1999;10(6):489–493. [Google Scholar]
- Gentner D, Goldin-Meadow S. Whither whorf. Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Cognition. 2003:3–14. [Google Scholar]
- Gershkoff-Stowe L, Thal DJ, Smith LB, Namy LL. Categorization and its developmental relation to early language. Child Development. 1997;68(5):843–859. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01966.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gleitman LR. The Structural Sources of Verb Meanings. Language Acquisition. 1990;1(1):3–55. [Google Scholar]
- Gleitman LR, Papafragou A. Language and thought. Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning. 2005:633–661. [Google Scholar]
- Grossmann T, Parise E, Friederici AD. The Detection of Communicative Signals Directed at the Self in Infant Prefrontal Cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2010;4 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00201. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2010.00201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall DG, Veltkamp B, Turkel W. Children’s and adults’ understanding of proper namable things. First Language. 2004;24(1):5. [Google Scholar]
- Hernik M, Csibra G. Infants learn enduring functions of novel tools from action demonstrations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2015;130(C):176–192. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.10.004. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollich G, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2000;65(3):1–123. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Höhle B, Weissenborn J, Kiefer D, Schulz A. Functional elements in infants’ speech processing: The role of determiners in the syntactic categorization of lexical elements. Infancy. 2004;5(3):341–353. [Google Scholar]
- Imai M, Gentner D. A cross-linguistic study of early word meaning: Universal ontology and linguistic influence. Cognition. 1997;62(2):169–200. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(96)00784-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson JS, Newport EL. Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology. 1989;21(1):60–99. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0. http://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jusczyk PW, Aslin RN. Infants′ Detection of the Sound Patterns of Words in Fluent Speech. Infant Pathways to Language:, Methods, Models and Research Directions. 1995;29(1):1–23. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1995.1010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keates J, Graham SA. Category Markers or Attributes Why Do Labels Guide Infants’ Inductive Inferences? Psychological Science. 2008;19(12):1287–1293. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02237.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klibanoff RS, Waxman SR. Basic level object categories support the acquisition of novel adjectives: Evidence from preschool-aged children. Child Development. 2000;71(3):649–659. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krehm M, Onishi KH, Vouloumanos A. I See Your Point: Infants Under 12 Months Understand That Pointing Is Communicative. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2014;15(4):527–538. http://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.736112. [Google Scholar]
- Krentz UC, Corina DP. Preference for language in early infancy: the human language bias is not speech specific. Developmental Science. 2008;11(1):1–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00652.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00652.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhl PK, Tsao FM, Liu HM. Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2003;100(15):9096–9101. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1532872100. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1532872100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landau B, Shipley E. Labelling patterns and object naming. Developmental Science. 2001;4(1):109–118. http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00155. [Google Scholar]
- Lewkowicz DJ. Sensory dominance in infants: I. Six-month-old infants’ response to auditory-visual compounds. Developmental Psychology. 1988a;24(2):155–171. http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.24.2.155. [Google Scholar]
- Lewkowicz DJ. Sensory dominance in infants: II. Ten-month-old infants’ response to auditory-visual compounds. Developmental Psychology. 1988b;24(2):172–182. http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.24.2.172. [Google Scholar]
- Lewkowicz DJ, Ghazanfar AA. The emergence of multisensory systems through perceptual narrowing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009;13(11):470–478. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liszkowski U, Carpenter M. Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12 months of age. Developmental Science. 2007;10(2):F1–F7. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00552.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lloyd-Fox S, Széplaki-Köllőd B, Yin J, Csibra G. Are you talking to me? Neural activations in 6-month-old infants in response to being addressed during natural interactions. Cortex. 2015;70:35–48. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.02.005. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lupyan G. The conceptual grouping effect: Categories matter (and named categories matter more) Cognition. 2008;108(2):566–577. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.03.009. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lupyan G, Rakison DH, McClelland JL. Language is not just for talking: Redundant labels facilitate learning of novel categories. Psychological Science. 2007;18(12):1077–1083. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02028.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mandler JM, McDonough L. Concept formation in infancy. Cognitive Development. 1993;8(3):291–318. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(93)80003-C. [Google Scholar]
- Marcus GF, Fernandes KJ, Johnson SP. Infant Rule Learning Facilitated by Speech. Psychological Science. 2007;18(5):387–391. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01910.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01910.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcus GF, Fernandes KJ, Johnson SP. The role of association in early word-learning. Frontiers in Psychology. 2012;3:283. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00283. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mareschal D, Quinn PC. Categorization in infancy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2001;5(10):443–450. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01752-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marno H, Davelaar EJ, Csibra G. Nonverbal communicative signals modulate attention to object properties. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2014;40(2):752–762. doi: 10.1037/a0035113. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0035113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marno H, Davelaar EJ, Csibra G. An object memory bias induced by communicative reference. Actpsy. 2016;163(C):88–96. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.11.005. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maurer D, Werker JF. Perceptual narrowing during infancy: A comparison of language and faces. Developmental Psychobiology. 2013;56(2):154–178. doi: 10.1002/dev.21177. http://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- May L, Werker JF. Can a Click be a Word?: Infants’ Learning of Non-Native Words. Infancy. 2014;19(3):281–300. http://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12048. [Google Scholar]
- Mervis CB, Rosch E. Categorization of natural objects. Annual Review of Psychology. 1981;32:89–115. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.32.020181.000513. [Google Scholar]
- Mintz TH. Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech. Cognition. 2003;90(1):91–117. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00140-9. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00140-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy GL. Noun phrase interpretation and conceptual combination. Journal of Memory and Language. 1990;29(3):259–288. http://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596x(9)90001-g. [Google Scholar]
- Murphy GL. The Big Book of Concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Namy LL, Waxman SR. Naming and Exclaiming: Infants’ Sensitivity to Naming Contexts. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2000;1(4):405–428. http://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD0104_03. [Google Scholar]
- Namy LL, Waxman SR. Symbols redefined. In: Namy LL, editor. Symbol use and symbolic representation. Malwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2005. pp. 269–278. [Google Scholar]
- Palmer SB, Fais L, Golinkoff RM, Werker JF. Perceptual Narrowing of Linguistic Sign Occurs in the 1st Year of Life. Child Development. 2012;83(2):543–553. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01715.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01715.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parise E, Csibra G. Neural Responses to Multimodal Ostensive Signals in 5-Month-Old Infants. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(8):7. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0072360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pascalis O, Loevenbruck H, Quinn PC, Kandel S, Tanaka JW, LEE K. On the Links Among Face Processing, Language Processing, and Narrowing During Development. Child Development Perspectives. 2014;8(2):65–70. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12064. http://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peirce CS. Collected papers. In: Jartshorne C, Weiss P, editors. Collected papers. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1932. [Google Scholar]
- Peña M, Arias D, Dehaene-Lambertz G. Gaze Following Is Accelerated in Healthy Preterm Infants. Psychological Science. 2014;25(10):1884–1892. doi: 10.1177/0956797614544307. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614544307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peña M, Werker JF, Dehaene-Lambertz G. Earlier Speech Exposure Does Not Accelerate Speech Acquisition. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32(33):11159–11163. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6516-11.2012. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6516-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinker S. The stuff of thought: Language as a window into human nature. New York, NY: Viking Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Pinker S, Jackendoff R. The faculty of language: what’s special about it? Cognition. 2005;95(2):201–236. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.08.004. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plunkett K. Labels can override perceptual categories in early infancy. Cognition. 2008;106(2):665–681. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.003. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quinn PC, LEE K, Pascalis O, Tanaka JW. Narrowing in categorical responding to other-race face classes by infants. Developmental Science. 2015;19(3):362–371. doi: 10.1111/desc.12301. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reeb-Sutherland BC, Fifer WP, Byrd DL, Hammock EAD, Levitt P, Fox NA. One-month-old human infants learn about the social world while they sleep. Developmental Science. 2011;14(5):1134–1141. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01062.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01062.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson CW, Sloutsky VM. Linguistic labels and categorization in Infancy: Do labels facilitate or hinder? Infancy. 2007a;11(3):233–253. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00225.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson CW, Sloutsky VM. Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on visual processing. Developmental Science. 2007b;10(6):734–740. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00627.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00627.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson CW, Best CA, Deng W, Sloutsky VM. The role of words in cognitive tasks: what, when, and how? Frontiers in Psychology. 2012;3(95):1–8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00095. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00095/abstract. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott LS, Monesson A. The Origin of Biases in Face Perception. Psychological Science. 2009;20(6):676–680. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02348.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02348.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott LS, Monesson A. Experience-dependent neural specialization during infancy. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(6):1857–1861. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.008. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seidl A, Tincoff R, Baker C, Cristia A. Why the body comes first: effects of experimenter touch on infants’ word finding. Developmental Science. 2015;18(1):155–164. doi: 10.1111/desc.12182. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shi R. Functional morphemes and early language acquisition. Child Development Perspectives. 2014;8(1):6–11. http://10.1111/cdep.12052. [Google Scholar]
- Shultz S, Vouloumanos A. Three-Month-Olds Prefer Speech to Other Naturally Occurring Signals. Language Learning and Development. 2010;6(4):241–257. http://doi.org/10.1080/15475440903507830. [Google Scholar]
- Shultz S, Vouloumanos A, Bennett RH, Pelphrey K. Neural specialization for speech in the first months of life. Developmental Science. 2014;17(5):766–774. doi: 10.1111/desc.12151. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sloman SA, Malt BC, Fridman A. Categorization versus similarity: The case of container names. In: Hahn U, Ramscar M, editors. Similarity and Categorization. Oxford University Press; 2001. pp. 73–86. [Google Scholar]
- Sloutsky VM. From Perceptual Categories to Concepts: What Develops? Cognitive Science. 2010;34(7):1244–1286. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01129.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sloutsky VM, Fisher AV. Induction and categorization in young children: a similarity-based model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2004;133(2):166–188. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sloutsky VM, Fisher AV. Linguistic labels: Conceptual markers or object features? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2012;111(1):65–86. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.07.007. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sloutsky VM, Robinson CW. The Role of Words and Sounds in Infants’ Visual Processing: From Overshadowing to Attentional Tuning. Cognitive Science. 2008;32(2):342–365. doi: 10.1080/03640210701863495. http://doi.org/10.1080/03640210701863495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith EE, Medin DL. Categories and concepts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Smith LB. Avoiding associations when it’s behaviorism you really hate. In: Golinkoff RM, Hirsh-Pasek K, editors. Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press; 2000. pp. 169–174. [Google Scholar]
- Smith LB, Heise D. Perceptual similarity and conceptual structure. In: Bums B, editor. Advances in psychology-Percepts, concepts, and categories: The representation and processing of information. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1992. pp. 233–272. [Google Scholar]
- Spelke ES, Kestenbaum R. Les origines du concept d’objet. Psychologie Française. 1986;31:67–72. [Google Scholar]
- Syrett K, Arunachalam S, Waxman SR. Slowly but Surely: Adverbs Support Verb Learning in 2-Year-Olds. Language Learning and Development. 2013;10(3):263–278. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2013.840493. http://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2013.840493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tincoff R, Jusczyk PW. Some beginnings of word comprehension in 6-month-olds. Psychological Science. 1999;10(2):172–175. http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00127. [Google Scholar]
- Tincoff R, Jusczyk PW. Six-Month-Olds Comprehend Words That Refer to Parts of the Body. Infancy. 2012;17(4):432–444. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00084.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00084.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello M. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello M, Kruger AC. Joint attention on actions: acquiring verbs in ostensive and non-ostensive contexts. Journal of Child Language. 2009;19(02):311. doi: 10.1017/s0305000900011430. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900011430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valenza E, Simion F, Cassia VM. Face preference at birth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1996;22(4):892–903. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.22.4.892. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vouloumanos A, Waxman SR. Listen up! Speech is for thinking during infancy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2014;18(12):642–646. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.10.001. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vouloumanos A, Werker JF. Tuned to the signal: the privileged status of speech for young infants. Developmental Science. 2004;7(3):270–276. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00345.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.14677687.2004.00345.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vouloumanos A, Werker JF. Listening to language at birth: evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. Developmental Science. 2007;10(2):159–164. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00549.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00549.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vouloumanos A, Hauser MD, Werker JF, Martin A. The tuning of human neonates’ preference for speech. Child Development. 2010;81(2):517–527. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01412.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01412.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vouloumanos A, Martin A, Onishi KH. Do 6-month-olds understand that speech can communicate? Developmental Science. 2014;17(6):872–879. doi: 10.1111/desc.12170. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vouloumanos A, Onishi KH, Pogue A. Twelve-month-old infants recognize that speech can communicate unobservable intentions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121057109. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1121057109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Vygotsky LS. Thought and language Cambridge. Massachusetts: MIT Press; 1962. http://doi.org/10.1007/BF02928399. [Google Scholar]
- Waxman SR. Specifying the scope of 13-month-olds’ expectations for novel words. Cognition. 1999a;70(3):B35–50. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00017-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waxman SR. The dubbing ceremony revisited: Object naming and categorization in infancy and early childhood. In: Medin DL, Atran S, editors. Folkbiology. Cambridge, MA: Bradford; 1999b. pp. 233–284. [Google Scholar]
- Waxman SR. Early word learning and conceptual development: Everything had a name, and each name gave birth to a new thought. In: Goswami U, editor. Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development. Oxford, England: Blackwell; 2002. pp. 102–126. [Google Scholar]
- Waxman SR, Booth A. Seeing Pink Elephants: Fourteen-Month-Olds’ Interpretations of Novel Nouns and Adjectives. Infant Pathways to Language:, Methods, Models and Research Directions. 2001;43(3):217–242. doi: 10.1006/cogp.2001.0764. http://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.2001.0764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waxman SR, Booth A. The origins and evolution of links between word learning and conceptual organization: New evidence from 11-month-olds. Developmental Science. 2003;6(2):128–135. http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00262. [Google Scholar]
- Waxman SR, Braun I. Consistent (but not variable) names as invitations to form object categories: New evidence from 12-month-old infants. Cognition. 2005;95:B59–B68. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.09.003. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waxman SR, Gelman SA. Early word-learning entails reference, not merely associations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009;13(6):258–263. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waxman SR, Hall DG. The Development of a Linkage between Count Nouns and Object Categories: Evidence from Fifteen‐to Twenty‐One‐Month‐Old Infants. Child Development. 1993;64(4):1224–1241. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waxman SR, Lidz J. Early word learning. In: Kuhn D, Siegler R, editors. Handbook of Child Psychology. 6. Vol. 2. Hoboken, NJ, USA: Wiley; 2006. pp. 299–335. http://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0207. [Google Scholar]
- Waxman SR, Markow DB. Words as invitations to form categories: Evidence from 12-to 13-month-old infants. Infant Pathways to Language:, Methods, Models and Research Directions. 1995;29(3):257–302. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1995.1016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waxman SR, Markow DB. Object Properties and Object Kind: Twenty-One-Month-Old Infants’ Extension of Novel Adjectives. Child Development. 1998;69(5):1313–1329. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06214.x. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waxman SR, Lidz J, Braun IE, Lavin T. Twenty four-month-old infants’ interpretations of novel verbs and nouns in dynamic scenes. Infant Pathways to Language:, Methods, Models and Research Directions. 2009;59(1):67–95. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.02.001. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weisleder A, Waxman SR. What’s in the input? Frequent frames in child-directed speech offer distributional cues to grammatical categories in Spanish and English. Journal of Child Language. 2010;37(5):1089–1108. doi: 10.1017/S0305000909990067. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000909990067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werker JF, Hensch TK. Critical periods in speech perception: new directions. Annual Review of Psychology. 2015;66(1):173–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015104. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werker JF, Tees R. Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development. 1984;7(1):49–63. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80022-3. [Google Scholar]
- Winawer J, Witthoft N, Frank MC, Wu L, Wade AR, Boroditsky L. Russian blues reveal effects of language on color discrimination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007;104(19):7780–7785. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701644104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woodward AL, Hoyne KL. Infants’ Learning about Words and Sounds in Relation to Objects. Child Development. 1999;70(1):65–77. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu R, Gopnik A, Richardson DC, Kirkham NZ. Infants learn about objects from statistics and people. Developmental Psychology. 2011;47(5):1220–1229. doi: 10.1037/a0024023. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0024023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu F. The role of language in acquiring object kind concepts in infancy. Cognition. 2002;85(3):223–250. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00109-9. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00109-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu F, Cote M, Baker A. Labeling guides object individuation in 12-month-old infants. Psychological Science. 2005;16(5):372–377. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01543.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01543.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoon JMD, Johnson MH, Csibra G. Communication-induced memory biases in preverbal infants. 2008;105(36):13690–13695. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0804388105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yuan S, Fisher C. Really? He blicked the cat?”: Two-year-olds learn distributional facts about verbs in the absence of a referential context. Psychological Science. 2009;20(5):619–626. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02341.x. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02341.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zosh JM, Feigenson L. Beyond ‘what’ and ‘how many’: capacity, complexity and resolution of infants’ object representations. In: Santos L, Hood B, editors. The Origins of Object Knowledge. Oxford University Press; 2009. pp. 25–51. [Google Scholar]