Abstract
Background: Previous studies have shown associations of physical fitness and cognition in children and in younger and older adults. However, knowledge about associations in high-school adolescents and young adults is sparse. Thus, the aim of this study was to evaluate the association of physical fitness, measured as maximal oxygen uptake (), muscle mass, weekly training, and cognitive function in the executive domains of selective attention and inhibitory control, in healthy male high-school students.
Methods: Fifty-four males (17.9 ± 0.9 years, 72 ± 11 kg and 182 ± 7 cm) completed a test, a body composition test and a visual cognitive task based on the Posner cue paradigm with three types of stimuli with different attentional demands (i.e., stimuli presentation following no cue, valid cue or invalid cue presentations). The task consisted of 336 target stimuli, where 56 (17%) of the target stimuli appeared without a cue (no cue), 224 (67%) appeared in the same rectangle as the cue (valid cue) and 56 (17%) appeared in the rectangle opposite to the cue (invalid cue). Mean reaction time (RT) and corresponding errors was calculated for each stimuli type. Total task duration was 9 min and 20 s In addition, relevant background information was obtained in a questionnaire.
Results: Linear mixed model analyses showed that higher was associated with faster RT for stimuli following invalid cue (Estimate = −2.69, SE = 1.03, p = 0.011), and for stimuli following valid cue (Estimate = −2.08, SE = 1.03, p = 0.048). There was no association of muscle mass and stimuli (F = 1.01, p = 0.397) or of weekly training and stimuli (F = 0.99, p = 0.405).
Conclusion: The results suggest that cardiorespiratory fitness is associated with cognitive performance in healthy male high-school students in the executive domains of selective attention.
Keywords: aerobic fitness, VO2max, cognitive function, cue-target paradigm, attention
Introduction
In recent years, researchers have become increasingly interested in investigating possible impacts of physical activity and physical fitness on cognitive functions, and several studies have suggested that higher physical fitness levels are associated with cognitive benefits (Guiney and Machado, 2013; Dupuy et al., 2015; Cox et al., 2016).
The vast majority of research has been conducted with older adults (above 50 years), and shows positive associations of physical activity and cognitive function (Colcombe and Kramer, 2003), as well as a reduced risk of age-related cognitive decline (Paillard et al., 2015). Recently, a growing number of studies have shown positive associations between physical fitness and cognitive function in developing children and young adolescents (Hillman et al., 2009; Chaddock et al., 2011; Fedewa and Ahn, 2011; Esteban-Cornejo et al., 2015). Positive associations are also reported in young adults (Themanson and Hillman, 2006; Kamijo and Takeda, 2009), however with inconsistent findings (Scisco et al., 2008; Hayes et al., 2014). Knowledge about the associations in high-school adolescents and young adults are still sparse (Cox et al., 2016).
Given that executive functions develop throughout the childhood and decline with age (Friedman et al., 2009), the effects of physical fitness may be elusive in high-school students and in young adults where cognition is developmentally peaking (Hayes et al., 2014). As a result, specific and sensitive methods of measurement are required. In general, a large number of different methods of measurement have been employed in research in this field, as well as different definitions of cognitive function (Biddle and Asare, 2011). Dupuy et al. (2015) have stressed that a major weakness in many studies is the fact that fitness is often measured by subjective self-reports or through submaximal tests. An objective measure of cardiorespiratory fitness, , was therefore chosen in the present study, in addition to self-reported weekly physical activity and objectively measured muscle mass.
In general, previous research indicates that the association of physical fitness and cognitive function is stronger for tasks requiring higher level of executive functioning (Colcombe and Kramer, 2003; Hillman et al., 2009; Pontifex et al., 2011; Guiney and Machado, 2013), particularly those tasks related to different aspects of attentional ability such as inhibition and task switching (Themanson et al., 2008; Guiney and Machado, 2013; Buckley et al., 2014). A cognitive task with different attentional load was therefore chosen in the present study to investigate the association of physical fitness and cognitive load.
In terms of age, the high-students in our study came close to what could be categorized as young adulthood, thus the study adds to previous work done on subjects in late adolescence or young adulthood. Our study further contributes to previous work exploring associations between physical fitness and selective attention performed on this age group.
Thus, the aim of this study was to investigate if physical fitness was associated with cognitive function in healthy male high-school students. To do this, participants completed a test, answered questions regarding weekly physical activity, underwent a body composition test and performed a pc-based visuospatial attention task to determine performance in the executive domains of selective attention and inhibitory control. The Posner cue target paradigm (Posner, 1980) was chosen to study the association of cardiorespiratory fitness and attention capacity to stimuli with different attentional load. Responding to stimuli presented after an invalid cue is particularly demanding, compared to stimuli following valid cue and no cue presentations, as it involves an interaction between goal-directed actions and inhibition of reflexive bottom-up processing of attention initiated by the distracting cue. To have a homogeneous population, only males were included. The main hypothesis was that higher physical fitness is associated with faster reaction time (RT) for all stimuli categories, due to better selective attention and inhibitory control. Moreover, we expected to find the fastest RT for stimuli following valid cue, followed by invalid cue and no cue.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Out of the 85 young male students attending two different high schools in Norway, 66 (77.7%) were recruited, with 54 (63.5%) participants (17.9 ± 0.9 years, 72 ± 11 kg and 182 ± 7 cm) completing both the physical test and cognitive task.
Procedures
Information regarding the study was sent via e-mail to the principals of two different high schools in Norway, and they replied quickly, expressing their willingness to participate in the study. An information meeting was then arranged at which the students who were interested in participating in the study, signed informed consent forms. When recruiting participants, it was made clear that both trained/active and untrained/sedentary participants were needed.
Data was collected in the course of 6 weeks, from September–November 2015. The tests were conducted in two independent test-runs for all participants, one for the physical tests (Day A) and one for the cognitive task (Day B; Figure 1). The cognitive task was performed between 10:15 and 15:22. On average, there was 12 ± 10 days between tests. All physical tests were conducted in the physiological test lab at Bergen University College, and the cognitive task was mainly completed at the students’ respective schools. Five to ten minutes before the participants started with the cognitive performance task, they completed a web-based electronic questionnaire.
All participants were told to dress appropriately for the physical tests, wearing shorts or pants, a t-shirt and running shoes. Participants were asked if they had felt ill during the last week before the test, especially with symptoms of fever and/or impaired general condition. Participants were excluded if they had been diagnosed with heart disease, asthma or any type of disease that could potentially affect their cardiorespiratory system in a strenuous exercise test. Before testing, all participants were informed orally of the test procedures and were given the opportunity to ask questions regarding the tests. In addition, they were asked not to engage in any strenuous physical exercise the day before the physical test as this could affect the results.
Body Composition
A direct segmental multi-frequency bioelectrical impedance analysis (DSM-BIA) for determining body composition (body weight (kg), muscle mass (%)) was performed using the In-Body720 (Biospace Co. Ltd, Seoul, Korea) body composition analyzer (Lim et al., 2009; Tompuri et al., 2015). Standard procedures were followed for all participants.
Cardiorespiratory Fitness Test
The test started with a 10-min warm-up with a gradient of 1.7% on a motorized treadmill (Woodway PPS 55, USA) where the velocity was gradually increased until the participant maintained a heart rate (HR) in the area of 73%–82% of age-predicted maximum HR (HRmax). Following a short rest period (<3 min), an incremental test protocol was followed with a treadmill-gradient of 5.3% and a start velocity of 8 km · t−1. The manual test protocol was controlled by the test leader, whom increased the speed by 1 km · t−1 each minute until volitional exhaustion. Notifications of the amount of time left to complete each speed and the amount of time left until the apparatus would attain a new sample registration were given throughout. When each participant was nearing possible exhaustion (respiratory exchange ratio >1), the test leader asked if a speed increase would be tolerable; the participant then had the opportunity, through the use of different hand signals, to agree to a velocity increase of 1 km · t−1 or 0.5 km · t−1, or to maintain the same speed.
Oxygen uptake was measured using a computer that was connected to a metabolic system with a 4.2-Liter (L) mixing chamber, containing baffles (Oxycon Pro, Erich Jaeger GmbH, Hoechberg, Germany). A full system calibration was performed before the testing of each group of participants, and a volume calibration using a 3 L calibration syringe with an accuracy of 1/2 ± of 1% (Hans Rudolph Inc., Shawnee, KS, USA) was regularly performed between and during the testing of each group. Gas calibration was performed using a container that held 300 L of a gas mixture (Riessner-Gase GmbH, Carefusion, Germany) with a mixture ratio of carbon dioxide (5.84 vol. %), oxygen (15.00 vol. %), nitrogen (79.16 vol. %). Measurements of oxygen uptake were continuously recorded and saved at 30-s intervals. HR was monitored every fifth seconds using the RS400 (Polar, Kempele, Finland).
The average value of the two highest consecutive values (30-s intervals) for each participant, in any interval registration, was recorded as their individual . was defined when two of three criteria were satisfied (Dupuy et al., 2015): (1) attaining a -plateau; (2) attaining a HR >90% or equivalent to their age-predicted maximum (i.e., 220 minus age); and (3) attaining a respiratory exchange ratio >1.1.
Cognitive Performance Task
A visual cognitive task based on the Posner cue paradigm was used to assess response time (RT), accuracy and inhibition (Posner, 1980; Posner and Cohen, 1984; Gundersen et al., 2007; Irgens-Hansen et al., 2015; Figure 2). The task was performed on a 13.3″ laptop, and was programmed with E-prime 2.0, standard version (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.).
The participants were instructed to fixate on the crosshair in the middle of the screen (Figure 2A), and to respond as fast as they could by pressing “l” on the keyboard when the target stimulus (an asterisk) appeared in the right rectangle and “d” when the target stimulus appeared in the left rectangle. Sometimes during the task, the frame on one of the rectangles became broader (a cue) before the target stimulus appeared. Participants were told to ignore the cue stimulus and only press the key when the target stimulus appeared.
All participants were instructed orally and performed a small practice run before the actual task to make sure they had understood the procedure. The task duration was 9 min and 20 s, and it was carried out in a quiet location without distractions (usually a small classroom), wearing hearing protectors to exclude distracting noise.
Three different stimuli categories appeared in the task: “no cue”, “valid cue” and “invalid cue”. If the target stimuli appeared in one of the rectangles without a cue, this was called a no cue presentation (Figure 2B). If the target stimulus appeared within the rectangle with a broader frame, it was called a valid cue presentation (Figure 2C). The third category applied when the target stimulus appeared in the opposite rectangle to the cue location, and was called an invalid cue presentation (Figure 2D).
The task consisted of 336 target stimuli, with each stimulus being presented for 500 ms with an interstimuli-interval (time between stimulus) of between 600 ms and 1400 ms. The inter-stimuli interval was selected from a set of fixed values (600, 700, 900, 1000 and 1200 ms). The order of the inter-stimuli interval, stimuli and cues was fixed for all participants, with mean interstimuli-interval of 825 ± 253 ms for no cue stimuli, 1108 ± 112 ms for valid stimuli and 1164 ± 236 for invalid stimuli presentations. The cue appeared in 50% of the cases 200 ms before the target stimuli, and in 50% of the cases 400 ms before the target stimulus would appear. Fifty-six (17%) of the target stimuli would appear without a cue (no cue), 224 (67%) would appear in the same rectangle as the cue (valid cue), and 56 (17%) would appear in the rectangle opposite to the cue (invalid cue).
RT (i.e., processing speed) and response accuracy were recorded and stored on the laptop for each trial. Mean RT for stimuli following no cue, valid cue and invalid cue were calculated for each participant. Error rate for the different stimuli categories were presented in percentage. Registration of responses before the target stimulus appeared, and responses during the first 149 ms. after target stimuli presentation were defined as erroneous responses (Amano et al., 2006). If an erroneous response was corrected by pressing a second time before the next stimulus was presented, it was still considered an erroneous response.
Questionnaire
The questionnaire included questions about weekly physical activity (1 = less than once a week, 2 = 1–2 time a week, 3 = 3–4 times a week, 4 = 5–6 times a week, 5 = 7–8 times a week, 6 = 9–10 times a week), and variables that previously have been associated with cognitive performance in visuospatial tests, like ADHD diagnosis (yes/no; McDonald et al., 1999), dyslexia (yes/no; Franceschini et al., 2012), daily nicotine use (yes/no; Heishman et al., 2010) and daily video-game playing (minutes; Green and Bavelier, 2003). In order to calculate the potential covariate effects of alertness on the association of cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive performance, the participants were also asked to rate their present alertness level on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (not alert at all) to 5 (highly alert).
Ethics
The study was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki declaration. All participants were informed about the study and signed an informed consent form before participating. Participants could withdraw from the study at any point, and results were treated anonymously. Norwegian Social Science Data Services (NSD) approved the study (reference number: 44551).
Data Analysis
The descriptive data is shown as range, mean ± standard deviation (SD). The data was checked for outliers and the preliminary analysis ensured no violation of the assumptions of normality.
To investigate if physical fitness affected RT differently for stimuli following no cue, valid cue and invalid cue, a linear mixed model was applied. Stimuli (no cue, valid cue and invalid cue) were used as repeated measure and subject as random intercept. Main effect of RT for the different stimuli categories, and interactions between RT for the different stimuli and , muscle mass and weekly training were included in the model. Furthermore, the model was adjusted for daily video-game playing, time-point of cognitive testing and alertness. Post hoc test with Bonferroni correction was performed to evaluate differences in RT between the different stimuli. A model with a first autoregressive (AR1) covariance structure and maximum likelihood of 10,000 iterations was applied showing an acceptable model fit (AIC = 1454).
Because no participants reported being diagnosed with ADHD, only three participants reported dyslexia and seven reported daily nicotine use, those variables were not included in the regression analyses. For all statistical analyses, p-values of <0.05 were considered statistically significant. SPSS® version 23.0 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA) for Windows® was used for all the statistical analyses.
Results
A significant main effect of stimuli (F = 6.01, p = 0.003) was found. Faster RT was seen on stimuli following valid cue (Estimate = −146.89, SE = 43.42, p = 0.001), but not on stimuli following no cue (Estimate = −65.62, SE = 32.96, p = 0.051) when invalid cue was the reference. However, post hoc analyses with Bonferroni correction showed significant differences between all stimuli categories. RT to stimuli following invalid cues were slower compared to valid cue (Mean differences = 59.94, SE = 2.81, p < 0.001) and faster compared to stimuli following no cue (Mean differences = −23.16, SE = 2.13, p < 0.001). RT to valid cue was faster compared to no cue (Mean differences = −83.10, SE = 2.8, p < 0.001). For mean values, see Table 1.
Table 1.
Mean ± SD | Range | |
---|---|---|
RT (ms) | ||
Stimuli after no cues | 376 ± 31 | 313–451 |
Stimuli after valid cues | 293 ± 28 | 239–364 |
Stimuli after invalid cues | 353 ± 35 | 278–429 |
Errors (%) | ||
Stimuli after no cues | 3.3 ± 3.0 | 0–14.3 |
Stimuli after valid cues | 5.9 ± 5.0 | 0.5–20.5 |
Stimuli after invalid cues | 10.0 ± 7.6 | 0–32.1 |
max (ml/min/kg) | 54.2 ± 4.9 | 41.9–66.7 |
Muscle mass (%) | 49.8 ± 3.3 | 40.4–54.5 |
Weekly training sessions: | ||
Less than once a week | 9% | |
1–2 | 24% | |
3–4 | 33% | |
5–6 | 19% | |
7–8 | 13% | |
9–10 | 2% | |
Daily video game playing (min) | 73 ± 67 | 0–360 |
Time-point of cognitive testing | 11:50 ± 1.0 | 10:15–15:22 |
Alertness | 3.5 ± 0.8 | 1–5 |
Data is presented as mean ± standard deviation (SD) and range (min-max).
Moreover, there was a significant interaction of and stimuli (F = 3.67, p = 0.017). Higher was associated with faster RT for stimuli following invalid cue (Estimate = −2.69, SE = 1.03, p = 0.011), and for stimuli following valid cue (Estimate = −2.08, SE = 1.03, p = 0.048), but not for stimuli following no cue (Estimate = −1.16, SE = 1.03, p = 0.266). There was no interaction of muscle mass and stimuli (F = 1.01, p = 0.397) or of weekly training and stimuli (F = 0.99, p = 0.405). Time-point of cognitive testing was the only covariate that contributed significantly to the model (F = 14.14, p < 0.001) with significantly slower RT in the morning (Estimate = −12.32, SE = 3.28, p < 0.001). See Figure 3 for an overview of correlations between RTs of the different stimuli and and for the RTs of the different stimuli and the time-point of cognitive testing.
Discussion
Our results showed a significant interaction of and RTs for the different stimuli categories. The interaction was significant for stimuli following valid and invalid cues, with an association between higher and faster RT. There was also a significant association between time-point of cognitive testing and RT, with slower RT in the morning.
The findings of the present study show an association between higher and faster RT for stimuli following both valid and invalid cues. Both stimuli conditions initiate a reflexive bottom-up attraction of attention due to the appearing peripheral cue, but differ in their demand as the most often appearing valid cue (67%) does not demand inhibition. The invalid condition (17%) will therefore demand more of a volitional top-down control when having to inhibit the most probable response. Higher seems to be beneficial in conditions initiated by cues through a faster processing speed of information, indicating a relationship between cardiorespiratory fitness and selective attention and inhibitory control in conditions where facilitation occurs.
Our findings are in agreement with a previous study by Themanson et al. (2008) showing that higher fit individuals exhibited a better top-down control of attention, and a better modulation of responses to tasks measuring selective attention. In addition, our results support previous studies showing that physical fitness yield favorable effects on executive functions (Gomez-Pinilla and Hillman, 2013; Erickson et al., 2014; Dupuy et al., 2015; Guiney et al., 2015; Szuhany et al., 2015; Luque-Casado et al., 2016). No interaction between and RT on stimuli following no cue in the present study may be due to a lower attentional demand, or to the lack of priming in that stimuli category.
Our results showed fastest RT for stimuli following valid cue, followed by invalid cue and no cue. As our results show, cueing was consistently associated with faster RT, possibly reflecting a higher internal activation and increased responsiveness to upcoming target stimulus caused by the priming effect (Mangun and Hillyard, 1991; Gabay and Henik, 2008; Hayward and Ristic, 2013). Lack of priming may explain the slowest RT for stimuli following no cue. Slowest RT for no cue could also be related to the shorter interstimuli-interval. Time-point for cognitive testing were associated with RT with highest RT in the morning, supporting common knowledge of the circadian variation in alertness and performance (Van Dongen and Dinges, 2010).
was the only significant physical fitness-predictor of cognitive performance in the present study. Our finding is in accordance with previous studies showing that cardiorespiratory fitness is regarded as one of the components of physical fitness mostly associated with cognitive performance (Ruiz-Ariza et al., 2017).
It is worth noting that there are recent studies failing to demonstrate the fitness-executive function hypothesis (Verburgh et al., 2014; Ballester et al., 2015). Moreover, Belsky et al. (2015) found that children with higher IQ scores grew up to be adults who were less sedentary and less obese, and in turn, had better cardiorespiratory fitness. They suggest that socioeconomic status, household and neuroselection must be considered when investigating the fitness-executive function hypothesis. Thus, more studies including other explanatory factors is needed.
Participants in the present study demonstrated an above average cardiorespiratory fitness ( score of 54.2 ± 4.9 mL · kg−1 · min−1) compared to the age-relative norm of the population (46–50 mL · kg−1 · min−1; Shvartz and Reibold, 1990). Thus, we cannot exclude a greater association of and cognitive function in participants whose mean score is closer to the population average, given normally distributed scores.
Limitations and Strengths
Due to the general limitations of a cross-sectional design, it is impossible to establish cause and effect in this study. In addition, the above average fitness of the participants has some implications for generalizability. The fact that the study exclusively recruited males and that the sample size is relatively small contributes to this generalization problem. Moreover, the spatial visual attention task used in the present study may not be appropriate to identify whether the cueing effects are due to endogenous spatial orienting driven by probability manipulation, attentional capture (peripheral cues), stimulus-response compatibility, or a mixture of all of them. The strengths of the study are the use of direct measures of and a pc-based cognitive performance task with different attentional loads and the homogeneous group regarding age and gender.
Conclusion
The findings of this study show that cardiorespiratory fitness is associated with cognitive performance in healthy male high-school students. Our results suggest that cardiorespiratory fitness may be associated with better modulation of bottom-up and top-down processes in tasks involving selective attention. More research is needed to increase the knowledge about factors affecting cognitive function.
Implications for Future Research
This study contributes to the limited number of studies investigating the influence of physical fitness on the components of selective attention and inhibition in male high-school students. More studies are needed to further assess the association of physical fitness and attentional tasks demanding inhibitory control, and how this relates to the ability to maintain attention.
Author Contributions
All authors participated in planning the experiment. EW, MK and HG participated in data collection. All authors have participated in conception of this manuscript, and have assisted in revising the manuscript for important content. EW and HG wrote the first draft of the manuscript, and all authors have provided final approval of the enclosed manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
Thanks to all the participants who volunteered to take part in this study; their participation is greatly appreciated. Thanks also to the principals and teachers at the schools for their cooperation, and to Esben Martinsen, Cecilie Hannevig and Henrik Hysing-Dahl for their assistance in parts of the physiological testing.
Footnotes
Funding. This research did not receive a grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for profit sectors.
References
- Amano K., Goda N., Nishida S., Ejima Y., Takeda T., Ohtani Y. (2006). Estimation of the timing of human visual perception from magnetoencephalography. J. Neurosci. 26, 3981–3991. 10.1523/jneurosci.4343-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballester R., Huertas F., Yuste F. J., Llorens F., Sanabria D. (2015). The relationship between regular sports participation and vigilance in male and female adolescents. PLoS One 10:e0123898. 10.1371/journal.pone.0123898 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belsky D. W., Caspi A., Israel S., Blumenthal J. A., Poulton R., Moffitt T. E. (2015). Cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive function in midlife: neuroprotection or neuroselection? Ann. Neurol. 77, 607–617. 10.1002/ana.24356 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biddle S. J., Asare M. (2011). Physical activity and mental health in children and adolescents: a review of reviews. Br. J. Sports Med. 45, 886–895. 10.1136/bjsports-2011-090185 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckley J., Cohen J., Kramer A. F., McAuley E., Mullen S. P. (2014). Cognitive control in the self-regulation of physical activity and sedentary behavior. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:747. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00747 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaddock L., Pontifex M. B., Hillman C. H., Kramer A. F. (2011). A review of the relation of aerobic fitness and physical activity to brain structure and function in children. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 975–985. 10.1017/s1355617711000567 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colcombe S., Kramer A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Sci. 14, 125–130. 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox E. P., O’Dwyer N., Cook R., Vetter M., Cheng H. L., Rooney K., et al. (2016). Relationship between physical activity and cognitive function in apparently healthy young to middle-aged adults: a systematic review. J. Sci. Med. Sport 19, 616–628. 10.1016/j.jsams.2015.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dupuy O., Gauthier C. J., Fraser S. A., Desjardins-Crèpeau L., Desjardins M., Mekary S., et al. (2015). Higher levels of cardiovascular fitness are associated with better executive function and prefrontal oxygenation in younger and older women. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:66. 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00066 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erickson K. I., Leckie R. L., Weinstein A. M. (2014). Physical activity, fitness, and gray matter volume. Neurobiol. Aging 35, S20–S28. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.03.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esteban-Cornejo I., Tejero-Gonzalez C. M., Sallis J. F., Veiga O. L. (2015). Physical activity and cognition in adolescents: a systematic review. J. Sci. Med. Sport 18, 534–539. 10.1016/j.jsams.2014.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fedewa A. L., Ahn S. (2011). The effects of physical activity and physical fitness on children’s achievement and cognitive outcomes: a meta-analysis. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 82, 521–535. 10.1080/02701367.2011.10599785 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franceschini S., Gori S., Ruffino M., Pedrolli K., Facoetti A. (2012). A causal link between visual spatial attention and reading acquisition. Curr. Biol. 22, 814–819. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman D., Nessler D., Cycowicz Y. M., Horton C. (2009). Development of and change in cognitive control: a comparison of children, young adults, and older adults. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 91–102. 10.3758/cabn.9.1.91 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabay S., Henik A. (2008). The effects of expectancy on inhibition of return. Cognition 106, 1478–1486. 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gomez-Pinilla F., Hillman C. (2013). The influence of exercise on cognitive abilities. Compr. Physiol. 3, 403–428. 10.1002/cphy.c110063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green C., Bavelier D. (2003). Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature 423, 534–537. 10.1038/nature01647 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guiney H., Lucas S. J., Cotter J. D., Machado L. (2015). Evidence cerebral blood-flow regulation mediates exercise-cognition links in healthy young adults. Neuropsychology 29, 1–9. 10.1037/neu0000124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guiney H., Machado L. (2013). Benefits of regular aerobic exercise for executive functioning in healthy populations. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 73–86. 10.3758/s13423-012-0345-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gundersen H., Helland C. A., Raeder M. B., Hugdahl K., Wester K. (2007). Visual attention in patients with intracranial arachnoid cysts. J. Neurol. 254, 60–66. 10.1007/s00415-006-0280-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes S. M., Forman D. E., Verfaellie M. (2014). Cardiorespiratory fitness is associated with cognitive performance in older but not younger adults. J. Gerontol. B. Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 71, 474–482. 10.1093/geronb/gbu167 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayward D. A., Ristic J. (2013). Measuring attention using the Posner cuing paradigm: the role of across and within trial target probabilities. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:205. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00205 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heishman S. J., Kleykamp B. A., Singleton E. G. (2010). Meta-analysis of the acute effects of nicotine and smoking on human performance. Psychopharmacology 210, 453–469. 10.1007/s00213-010-1848-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillman C. H., Buck S. M., Themanson J. R., Pontifex M. B., Castelli D. M. (2009). Aerobic fitness and cognitive development: event-related brain potential and task performance indices of executive control in preadolescent children. Dev. Psychol. 45:114. 10.1037/a0014437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Irgens-Hansen K., Gundersen H., Sunde E., Baste V., Harris A., Bråtveit M., et al. (2015). Noise exposure and cognitive performance: a study on personnel on board Royal Norwegian Navy vessels. Noise Health 17, 320–327. 10.4103/1463-1741.165057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamijo K., Takeda Y. (2009). General physical activity levels influence positive and negative priming effects in young adults. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 511–519. 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.11.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim J. S., Hwang J. S., Lee J. A., Kim D. H., Park K. D., Jeong J. S., et al. (2009). Cross-calibration of multi-frequency bioelectrical impedance analysis with eight-point tactile electrodes and dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry for assessment of body composition in healthy children aged 6–18 years. Pediatr. Int. 51, 263–268. 10.1111/j.1442-200X.2008.02698.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luque-Casado A., Perakakis P., Hillman C. H., Kao S. C., Llorens F., Guerra P., et al. (2016). Differences in sustained attention capacity as a function of aerobic fitness. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 48, 887–895. 10.1249/mss.0000000000000857 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mangun G. R., Hillyard S. A. (1991). Modulations of sensory-evoked brain potentials indicate changes in perceptual processing during visual-spatial priming. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 17, 1057–1074. 10.1037/0096-1523.17.4.1057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDonald S., Bennett K. M. B., Chambers H., Castiello U. (1999). Covert orienting and focusing of attention in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychologia 37, 345–356. 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00078-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paillard T., Rolland Y., de Souto Barreto P. (2015). Protective effects of physical exercise in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease: a narrative review. J. Clin. Neurol. 11, 212–219. 10.3988/jcn.2015.11.3.212 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pontifex M. B., Raine L. B., Johnson C. R., Chaddock L., Voss M. W., Cohen N. J., et al. (2011). Cardiorespiratory fitness and the flexible modulation of cognitive control in preadolescent children. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 1332–1345. 10.1162/jocn.2010.21528 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posner M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 32, 3–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posner M. I., Cohen Y. (1984). “Components of visual orienting,” in Attention and Performance X: Control of Language Processes (Vol 32), eds Bouma H., Bouwhuis D. G. (Hillsdale: Erlbaum; ), 531–556. [Google Scholar]
- Ruiz-Ariza A., Grao-Cruces A., de Loureiro N. E. M., Martínez-López E. J. (2017). Influence of physical fitness on cognitive and academic performance in adolescents: a systematic review from 2005–2015. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 10, 108–133. 10.1080/1750984x.2016.1184699 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Scisco J. L., Leynes P. A., Kang J. (2008). Cardiovascular fitness and executive control during task-switching: an ERP study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 69, 52–60. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shvartz E., Reibold R. C. (1990). Aerobic fitness norms for males and females aged 6 to 75 years: a review. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 61, 3–11. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szuhany K. L., Bugatti M., Otto M. W. (2015). A meta-analytic review of the effects of exercise on brain-derived neurotrophic factor. J. Psychiatr. Res. 60, 56–64. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.10.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Themanson J. R., Hillman C. H. (2006). Cardiorespiratory fitness and acute aerobic exercise effects on neuroelectric and behavioral measures of action monitoring. Neuroscience 141, 757–767. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Themanson J. R., Pontifex M. B., Hillman C. H. (2008). Fitness and action monitoring: evidence for improved cognitive flexibility in young adults. Neuroscience 157, 319–328. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.09.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tompuri T. T., Lakka T. A., Hakulinen M., Lindi V., Laaksonen D. E., Kilpeläinen T. O., et al. (2015). Assessment of body composition by dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry, bioimpedance analysis and anthropometrics in children: the Physical Activity and Nutrition in Children study. Clin. Physiol. Funct. Imaging 35, 21–33. 10.1111/cpf.12118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Dongen H. P. A., Dinges D. F. (2010). “Circadian rhythms in fatigue, alertness and performance,” in Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, 3rd Edn. eds Kryger M. H., Roth T., Dement W. C. (Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier Saunders; ), 391–399. [Google Scholar]
- Verburgh L., Konigs M., Scherder E. J., Oosterlaan J. (2014). Physical exercise and executive functions in preadolescent children, adolescents and young adults: a meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 48, 973–979. 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091441 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]