Abstract
Early stages of sensorimotor system development in mammals are characterized by the occurrence of spontaneous movements. Whether and how these movements support correlated activity in developing sensorimotor spinal cord circuits remains unknown. Here we show highly correlated activity in sensory and motor zones in the spinal cord of neonatal rats in vivo. Both during twitches and complex movements, movement-generating bursts in motor zones are followed by bursts in sensory zones. Deafferentation does not affect activity in motor zones and movements, but profoundly suppresses activity bursts in sensory laminae and results in sensorimotor uncoupling, implying a primary role of sensory feedback in sensorimotor synchronization. This is further supported by largely dissociated activity in sensory and motor zones observed in the isolated spinal cord in vitro. Thus, sensory feedback resulting from spontaneous movements is instrumental for coordination of activity in developing sensorimotor spinal cord circuits.
Spontaneous movements are important for mammalian development but how network activity underlies the generation of these actions remains unclear. Here the authors show that both spontaneous twitches and complex movements enable correlated activity in motor and sensory networks of the rat spinal cord in vivo, and that sensory feedback is instrumental in this synchronization.
Spontaneous movements are a hallmark of early stages of mammalian development1,2,3. These largely uncoordinated early movement patterns are generated by motor commands from the spinal cord4, with a developmental control from the brainstem5, notably red nucleus6, and persist after decerebration5 or deafferentation2,7. Growing evidence indicates that spontaneous movements are implicated in a number of activity-dependent developmental processes including formation of connections between motoneurons and muscles8, development of sensorimotor spinal cord circuits and reflexes9,10, and formation of thalamocortical body maps11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18. In rodent embryos, generation of early motor activities in the spinal cord was shown to involve transient circuits, electrical synapses and excitatory actions of GABA19,20. Similar early movement patterns implicated in a variety of developmental processes also have been described in non-mammalian species, notably chick embryos19,21 and crustaceans (for review see Marder and Rehm22). Yet, the network activity patterns that underlie the generation of motor commands and support sensorimotor coordination in spinal cord circuits during spontaneous motor behaviours in rodents during the postnatal period, which corresponds to the second half of gestation in humans23, remain largely unknown.
Current knowledge on the function of the neonatal rodent spinal cord in relation to early movements is mainly based on behavioural and network modelling approaches9,24,25,26. These studies suggest important roles of sensory feedback resulting from spontaneous movements in tuning sensorimotor connectivity. Through a series of elegant studies, Schouenborg and colleagues9,26,27 have developed a model of motor-directed somatosensory imprinting to explain the functional adaption of the nociceptive withdrawal reflex, which occurs through the postnatal period in rats. The model applies an anti-Hebbian rule to self-organize sensory-motor connections within nociceptive withdrawal reflex modules in the spinal cord (with postsynaptic activity in movement-generating neurons preceding sensory feedback). Consistently, aberrant sensory input artificially introduced during spontaneous twitches increased the nociceptive withdrawal reflex error rate9. These behavioural observations and model predictions of spinal cord circuit function also are supported by results obtained for higher sensorimotor stations (relay thalamus and sensorimotor cortex), where sensory feedback from spontaneous twitches and reflexive responses was shown to trigger internally generated thalamocortical spindle-burst and early gamma oscillations (EGOs)11,12,13,14,17,18,28 (see also refs 15, 16). Spontaneous movements also were shown to be efficient triggers of hippocampal activity in neonatal animals29. Taken together, the existing evidence, based on behavioural observations and model studies at the level of spinal cord and recordings of neuronal activity at higher brain centres, suggests that sensory feedback is central to the roles fulfilled by spontaneous twitches in coordinating sensorimotor spinal cord networks. However, spinal cord activity in the context of spontaneous movements has never been previously addressed directly. Moreover, several observations question the ubiquity of the sensory feedback hypothesis in relation to spontaneous movements. First, recordings of neuronal activity in cortex of neonatal rats revealed that different types of spontaneous movements evoke cardinally different cortical responses: while twitches and startles occurring during sleep were followed by excitation of neurons in primary motor cortex and hippocampus (in agreement with the sensory feedback hypothesis), complex, long-lasting movements occurring during awake states were not associated with cortical activation30. It has been suggested that these two types of motor events are differently processed in cortex, the twitches being processed as unexpected events, whereas expected sensory input during self-generated complex movements is gated via corollary discharges31. Gating of sensory feedback could occur at the level of spinal cord, for example via local GABAergic interneuron mediated inhibitory corollary discharge on dorsal sensory neurons and primary afferents32, but this remains hypothetical. Even more complexity is added by observations made in the in vitro spinal cord preparation, which displays spontaneous motor bursts33, and in which other forms of communication between motor and sensory neurons have been suggested, including excitatory efferent copy, mediated either by interneurons activating primary afferents via depolarizing GABA actions or through ephaptic interactions34,35. However, it remains unknown whether sensorimotor network activity and the local interactions described in the in vitro spinal cord preparation correspond to the spinal cord network dynamics in behaving animals.
In this study, we addressed the spatiotemporal dynamics of spinal cord circuitry in relation to neonatal spontaneous movements, in vivo, through simultaneous recordings of translaminar spinal cord network activity and spontaneous movements in spine-restrained behaving neonatal rats. We also compared, using a similar approach, the in vivo activity with the activity patterns expressed in the isolated spinal cord in vitro. Our main finding is that both spontaneous twitches and complex movements enable correlated activity in motor and sensory networks of the spinal cord in vivo, and that sensory feedback is instrumental in this synchronization. We also find that although the isolated spinal cord in vitro displays spontaneous bursting activities in sensory and motor zones, their spatiotemporal dynamics poorly matches that of the spinal cord in vivo, even after deafferentation. These findings provide, for the first time, a description of sensorimotor spinal cord dynamics in relation to neonatal twitches and complex movements, indicate supraspinal mechanisms of inhibition of cortical activation during complex movements, and also show that sensory feedback from twitches indeed creates conditions for a reversed-type of Hebbian learning, with the activity in movement-generating networks preceding the afferent input, which supports the Schouenborg’s model of motor-directed somatosensory imprinting.
Results
Functional probing of the neonatal rat spinal cord in vivo
To explore the dynamics of spinal cord network activity in relation to early motor behaviour, we developed a method enabling recording simultaneously translaminar, spinal cord network events and movements, in vivo. Subjects were neonatal rats, 5–7 days old (P5–P7, P0=day of birth). Extracellular, local field potential (LFP) and multiunit activity (MUA) were recorded through a linear silicone-based electrode array (16 recording sites with a centre-to-centre separation of 100 μm). Motor behaviour (left hindlimb movements) was recorded using a piezoelectric transducer. Our experimental setup is schematically illustrated in Fig. 1a. The electrode array was inserted at the lumbar enlargement (hindlimb representation), at the L3-L5 left hemisegment level. Anatomical location of each recording site was estimated based on the electrode array insertion coordinates and on the corresponding or age-matched histological assessment (as demonstrated in Fig. 1b and Supplementary Fig. 1). Throughout the text, depth refers to the subdural spinal depth along the electrode array axis.
Systematic mechanical stimulation of different points along the hindlimb allowed establishing topographic relationships, identifying the location of intraspinal sensory neurons and examining the spatiotemporal properties of mechanical stimulus-evoked activity in the ipsilateral L3-5 spinal hemisegment. Mechanical stimulation consisted of a brief (5–10 ms) touch, provided at a rate of 0.2 s−1. The two recording sites (or equivalent intraspinal depths) showing the shortest response latencies were considered as sensory zone. Figure 1c shows a case example of response (sensory zone, average within-burst MUA frequency) by stimulation point, reminiscent of the large receptive fields previously described for this age group36,37. Stimulations of the back, tail, contralateral hindlimb and forelimbs did not produce alterations in spinal activity in L3-5 hemisegments. The stimulation point for which we obtained the shortest latency and highest multiunit frequency response was considered topographic, and this response was analysed further. While mapping was done essentially under light anesthesia (0.5% isoflurane), recordings of topographic, mechanical stimulus-evoked activity were done under no or light anesthesia (0.5% isoflurane). In both conditions, the stimulus reliably evoked a primary, sharp LFP deflection and sink within dorsal horn laminae, with field potentials reversing at relatively deeper dorsal horn laminae (Fig. 1d, non-anesthetized animal, and Supplementary Fig. 2, anesthetized animal—same as in Fig. 1d). These field events were associated with an increase in spiking activity (Fig. 1e), from 11.9±4.6 s−1 (baseline) to 193.2±141.4 s−1 (average within-burst), (mean±s.d., n=10 non-anesthetized animals, P≈0.002, Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Evoked bursts onset and offset were estimated at 12±2 ms and 77±43 ms post-stimulus, respectively (mean±s.d., n=10 non-anesthetized animals, Fig. 1e). For the vast majority of cases (12 of 14 animals), a secondary activity burst also was observed. These secondary bursts were restricted to the upper most regions activated by mechanical stimulation (generally at the subdural depths of 100–200 μm, consistent with the location of laminae I-II), and occurred with a delay ranging from approximately 70 to 100 ms in relation to stimulus onset. Primary bursts likely result from incoming A-fibre inputs, and secondary bursts might reflect responses to C-fibre activation, as shown previously36. While we obtained qualitatively and quantitatively similar sensory responses under no or light anesthesia, one main difference was found: in the no anesthesia condition, sensory bursts correlated with activity in motor zones (Fig. 1e and Supplementary Fig. 2). This activity profile is reminiscent of the activation of reflex pathways. In addition, the mean baseline MUA in sensory zones was approximately twofold higher in the absence of isoflurane: 4.9±2.4 s−1 (with isoflurane, n=9 animals) and 11.9±4.6 s−1 (without isoflurane, n=10 animals) (mean±s.d., P≈6.5 × 10−4, Wilcoxon rank-sum test). The mean MUA in motor zones also was higher in the absence of isoflurane: 9.6±6.3 s−1 (with isoflurane, n=9 animals) and 18.2±11.4 s−1 (without isoflurane, n=10 animals) (mean±s.d., P≈0.045, Wilcoxon rank-sum test). It should be noted that topographic, mechanical stimulus-evoked burst onsets in sensory zones tended to decrease from P5 to P7 (n=15 animals; pooled data with and without isoflurane, P=0.0531, Kruskal–Wallis test), about 2 ms per day (Supplementary Fig. 2), which is largely in agreement with previous reports on the development of sensory processing in the spinal cord37,38 and, in particular, with fibre myelination39,40.
To probe the anatomical location of premotor/motor networks, we provided brief electrical pulses (50 μs) at different spinal cord depths, with electrical stimulation sites matching recording sites (intraspinal microstimulation, ISMS). For that purpose, a bipolar electrode was lowered to the maximum depth used for recordings (expected location of motoneurons): the minimum current required to elicit detectable movement was determined and used at all depths (100–200 μm steps). Electrical stimulation was performed in deeply anesthetized (1.5% isoflurane) animals. Muscle activity was reliably evoked for electrical stimulations in intermediate to ventral horn laminae (Fig. 1f). Movement onset and offset (as detected on the piezoelectric transducer signal) were located at 11±2 ms and 146±15 ms post-stimulus, accordingly (mean±s.d., n=9 animals, Fig. 1f). The two depths of electrical stimulation associated with the highest amplitude movements were considered as motor zone.
Closer examination of the depth distribution of topographic mechanical stimulus-evoked MUA across animals revealed increases in spiking between a minimum depth of 100 μm and a maximum depth of 600 μm (n=9 animals, Fig. 1g), with the highest within-burst spiking rates being located at 100–300 μm, estimated to correspond roughly to laminae I-IV (n=23–37 responses of 15 animals per depth range, P=0.0046, Wilcoxon rank-sum test, Fig. 1h). Consistently, short-latency MUA responses also were found predominantly at the depths of 100–300 μm (n=23–37 responses of 15 animals per depth range, P=0.0030, Wilcoxon rank-sum test, Fig. 1h). Indeed, while in adult rats Aβ-fibre inputs (innocuous tactile stimulation) are restricted to laminae III-IV, in neonatal rats A-fibre inputs reach superficial and deeper dorsal horn laminae9,38. Muscle activity, on the other hand, was evoked for stimulations at a minimum depth of 600 μm (deep dorsal horn) and reached the highest magnitude when stimulations were performed at a depth of 1,000–1,200 μm (ventral-most regions, n=14–16 responses of 9 animals per depth range, P=0.046, Wilcoxon rank-sum test), without any depth-dependence in movement onset (Fig. 1h).
Network dynamics during twitches and complex movements
We recorded spontaneous spinal cord activity and concomitant motor behaviour in non-anaesthetized animals, under buprenorphine analgesia. Spontaneous spinal cord activity was characterized by intermittent LFP deflections and correlated MUA bursts (Fig. 2a,b), which, within sensory and motor zones, occurred at the frequencies of 4.3±1.0 min−1 and 3.6±0.9 min−1, respectively (mean±s.d., n=15 animals, 4249 sensory zone bursts and 3362 motor zone bursts). The occurrence of activity bursts did not differ significantly between the two zones (Wilcoxon rank-sum test). Mean multiunit spiking activity also did not differ significantly between motor (55±20 s−1) and sensory (41±18 s−1) zones (mean±s.d., n=15 animals, Wilcoxon rank-sum test). Temporal analysis of spinal cord activity and ipsilateral hindlimb movements (4.5±2.1 min−1, mean±s.d., n=15 animals, 4378 events) revealed that ≈75% of the recorded MUA bursts in motor zones were followed (within 100 ms) by overt hindlimb movement. Importantly, the majority (≈60%) of MUA bursts in sensory zones were preceded (within 100 ms) by movement.
Motor behaviour was characterized, qualitatively, by relatively abundant twitching of the extremities, which occurs typically during bouts of active sleep, and attempted crawling and pivoting, which at these ages (P5-7) dominate awake behaviour in rats41. Indeed, of analysed independent behavioural events (n=4378), 53±15% (mean±s.d.) fell in the twitches category, which was defined essentially as all events lasting less than 600 ms (252±122 ms, mean±s.d.) and occurring in a background of atonia, i.e. in the absence of preceding or following, continued motor activity (Fig. 2c). Complex movements (events lasting more than 900 ms, 1.7±1.4 s, mean±s.d.) were 29% of analysed events (Fig. 2c; for methodological details, see Supplementary Fig. 3). Parallel simultaneous recordings of limb movements and nuchal muscle activity (electromyogram (EMG)) in an additional group of animals (n=3) revealed that limb movements occurring during bouts of sleep (twitches/startles) on average last 376 ms (n=1,237), and that limb movements occurring during awake periods are in average of 1.3 s duration (n=461), and thus complex movement sequences likely represent awake behaviour. Overall, these results are in line the predominance of active sleep and of the twitching behaviour in neonatal rats, over the first postnatal week42.
Analysis of translaminar network dynamics during twitching epochs revealed that twitches were reliably preceded by relatively short-lasting bursting activity in intermediate-ventral zones, whereas activity within sensory zones, given by LFP deflections, current sinks and increased firing rates, was consistently observed only following twitch onset (MUA peak at 106±69 ms after twitch onset) (Fig. 2d, single twitch; Fig. 2e,f, single animal; Fig. 2g, n=15 animals, 2,217 twitches). The observed general pattern of peri-twitching network activity includes two additional main features. First, in all animals, we found two main current sinks initiated prior to twitching, within the ventral and intermediate spinal cord. Second, increases in spiking frequency within sensory and motor zones overlapped, to some extent, after twitch onset. Cross-correlating motor and sensory spikes occurring within a −0.3 to 1 s time period in relation to twitch onset revealed peak correlations at a lag of 54±21 ms (sensory zone spike times referenced to motor spike times, mean±s.d., 15 animals, Fig. 2h). Cross-correlating spike times in motor and intermediate zones further confirmed a predominantly synchronous activation of these two zones (Fig. 2h).
When analysing spinal cord network dynamics during complex movements, we observed a phenomenon similar to that described for twitching epochs, with activity in intermediate-motor zones preceding complex movement onset, in turn followed by elevated firing of dorsal units, which was maximal after movement onset and declined through the time course of the complex movements (Fig. 2i–l). We did not find fundamental differences in the spatiotemporal organization of network patterns (LFP and MUA) during twitching or complex movement epochs, at the single segment level, except that complex movement-related events lasted longer than those associated with twitches.
Deafferentation suppresses activity bursts in dorsal horn
Next, we examined, causally, whether activation of sensory neurons by a feedback mechanism is at the origin of the twitches/complex movement-related dorsal horn activation. For that purpose, in a subset of animals (n=3 of 15), we recorded spinal cord activity before and after deafferentation. Specifically, we transected dorsal rootlets innervating the electrode array-implanted region, as illustrated in Fig. 3a (see also Supplementary Fig. 4). Transection was performed in deeply anesthetized (1.5% isoflurane) animals. Local deafferentation was confirmed by lack of topographic, mechanical stimulus-evoked LFP deflections and correlated MUA bursts (Fig. 3b).
Transection of dorsal rootlets caused, in all animals, a striking reduction (of approximately 66±30%, mean±s.d.) in the occurrence of bursts within sensory zones (Fig. 3c). Consistently, the spiking capacity of neuronal populations within the dorsal also was decreased following deafferentation (the mean firing rate in sensory zones after deafferentation—through entire recording sections, per animal—was 39±28% of the control condition, Fig. 3c). Activation of dorsal horn neurons after twitching or complex movements, as observed in the control condition, was strikingly diminished or completely abrogated by deafferentation (Fig. 3d,e, single animal, and Fig. 3f, group data). Consistently, the previously described correlation between spike times in motor and sensory zones during twitching or complex movement epochs was no longer present after deafferentation (Fig. 3g).
The partial overlap between activity in sensory and motor zones raises the possibility that sensory feedback contributes to motor activity. Following afferent transection, activity within motor zones was at least to a large extent preserved (Fig. 3c, group data—general effect, Fig. 3d,e, single animal, and Fig. 3f, group data—different epochs), as we did not detect alterations in bursting frequency (Fig. 3c, group data—general effect), or in the peri-twitching bursting behaviour, including multiunit frequency peak value and time, as well as return to baseline (Fig. 3h). Moreover, the occurrence of twitches, as well individual twitch maximal amplitude, power (that is, signal waveform power normalized to duration) or duration did not change following afferent transection (Fig. 3i). Similarly, global metrics of more complex motor behaviours were not affected by the deafferentiation procedure (Fig. 3i). Following deafferentation, movements were consistently evoked by electrical stimulation at the expected depths (Supplementary Fig. 4), as demonstrated before (Fig. 1).
Together, our in vivo data suggest that the predominant function of developing spinal cord networks consists in generating spontaneous bursts in intermediate to ventral motor zones, which drive twitches and complex movements, that both types of movements cause, through sensory feedback, activation of dorsal sensory zones, and that sensory feedback is instrumental for the synchronization of activity in sensory and motor zones of the spinal cord.
Dissociation of sensory-motor network function in vitro
Spontaneous activity also has been described in vitro, using the neonatal rat spinal cord preparation or neonatal rat spinal cord slices, in which correlated sensorimotor activity may be supported by intrinsic connections and emphatic interactions33,34,35,43. Therefore, we further investigated interactions between sensory and motor zones in the in vitro spinal cord (from P5-7 rats) preparation, through silicone probe recordings, as performed in vivo (Supplementary Fig. 5). Spontaneous activity in isolated spinal cords was characterized by regular bursts within the dorsal horn, occurring at 15±4.9 min−1 (mean±s.d., n=5 animals, 6860 bursts) (Fig. 4a,c). These were associated with a dorsal sink (Fig. 4f) and MUA bursts (Fig. 5a), lasting approximately 119±28 ms (mean±s.d., n=5 animals, 6,860 bursts). A main feature of activity in motor zones was its organization in megabursts−3.0±0.3 s long periods of oscillatory activity at 6.0±0.1 s Hz, occurring at 1.7±0.3 min−1 (mean±s.d., n=5 animals, ≈760 bursts; Fig. 4d,g). These megabursts could be also evoked by rhythmic dorsal root stimulation (Supplementary Fig. 5). Isolated, short lasting bursts (with a duration of about 62±14 ms), reminiscent of twitch-related events in vivo, also occurred in motor zones, at a frequency of approximately 2 min−1 (5 animals, 760 bursts; Fig. 4e,h).
Sensory or motor bursts-triggered histograms of MUA, and cross-correlation analysis of bursts in both zones revealed weakly (or failed to reveal any) correlated firing (Fig. 5a–e). Indeed, only about 3% of bursts within sensory zones were followed (within a maximum lag of 100 ms) by bursts within motor zones (average, n=5 animals, for which in total 6860 events were detected). Moreover, in average, only 5% of mega- and 16% of short-lasting motor bursts were preceded by sensory bursts. Further cross-correlation analysis of units in sensory and motor zones referenced to entire recording sessions showed only a weak correlation, with sensory neurons firing 2–46 ms ahead of motor units (peak value of 5.4 spikes s−1). This is in striking contrast to the activity recorded in vivo, where motor units led sensory activity both during twitching and complex movement episodes, with a lag of 58±22 ms (peak value of 24±17 spikes ms−1, mean±s.d., n=15 animals). It also differs from the overall activity pattern through entire recording sessions after transecting dorsal rootlets in vivo, where the correlation between the two zones was dramatically decreased or completely lost (Fig. 5e).
Pharmacological analysis of spontaneous activity in the isolated spinal cord revealed that combined application of the ionotropic glutamate receptor antagonists CNQX and APV completely suppressed network-driven events both in sensory and motor zones (Supplementary Fig. 6), and that blockade of GABA(A) receptors with gabazine eliminated regular bursts in sensory zones and significantly increased LFP power and MUA during motor megabursts, with loss of the prominent within-burst 6 Hz oscillation; during these epileptiform discharges in motor zones evoked by disinhibition44, activation of neurons in sensory zones was not observed (Supplementary Fig. 7).
Thus, in contrast to the in vivo situation, activity in the isolated spinal cord was characterized by much weaker interactions between sensory and motor zones, and by a temporal leading of sensory neurons in the isolated sensory-motor network, inverse to the in vivo situation, where motor zones headed population activity. Taken together, data obtained in the isolated spinal cord provide strong support to the hypothesis that sensorimotor integration in spinal cord under physiological conditions is primarily driven by sensory feedback resulting from motor unit-driven movements.
Discussion
We explored the spatiotemporal dynamics in spinal cord sensorimotor networks in relation to spontaneous movements of neonatal rat pups in vivo. We observed highly correlated activity in motor and sensory zones of the spinal cord during spontaneous movements, both twitches and complex movements, with the following activation sequence: motoneurons—movements—sensory neurons. Deafferentation through local transection of dorsal roots/rootlets profoundly suppressed activity bursts in sensory zones and desynchronized sensory and motoneurons without affecting global metrics of activity in motor zones and movements. Dissociation of the activity in motor and sensory zones also was observed in the isolated spinal cord preparation in vitro. Taken together, our results strongly support the hypothesis that sensory feedback from spontaneous movements is instrumental for the temporal binding of motor and sensory neurons in the developing spinal cord.
The hypothesis that sensory feedback, or reafferentation, synchronizes the activity of motor and sensory neurons during neonatal spontaneous movements is based on the assumption that movements are associated with the activation of proprioceptors, tactile and eventually pain receptors. Provided that primary afferents and sensory neurons are mature enough, activation of primary sensory neurons during movements should result in activation of secondary sensory neurons in the spinal cord. This hypothesis is supported by a number of observations. First, sensory inputs drive activity in the spinal cord dorsal horn from early developmental stages38. Second, conditioning through artificial introduction of aberrant sensory input during twitching leads to an increased nociceptive reflex error rate, thus underlying sensory-to-motor connectivity might be guided by sensory feedback7,9. Finally, spontaneous twitches reliably precede activation of thalamo-cortical networks11,13,45. The detailed characterization of the translaminar spinal network dynamics in behaving rat pups provided by this study further supports the sensory feedback hypothesis. We observed that activation of sensory neurons follows twitch/complex movement onset, and that the delay of MUA bursts in sensory zones from movement onset is close to the delay of sensory-evoked responses. In the case of an underlying excitatory efferent copy mechanism, as suggested on the basis of results obtained in the isolated spinal cord in vitro, the delay in sensory neuron activation should have been much shorter, or sensory neuron activation could even have preceded movement onset34. Moreover, we found that local deafferentation almost completely eliminated activation of sensory neurons during spontaneous movements. This directly indicates that sensory feedback is a necessary condition for correlated activation of motor and sensory neurons. Finally, we also failed to observe any prominent correlated activity between sensory and motor neurons in the isolated spinal cord preparation in vitro, despite the rich repertoire of spontaneous activity patterns occurring essentially independently in motor and sensory zones. Previous studies revealed that different types of neonatal spontaneous movements, twitches and complex movements, evoke cardinally different cortical responses29,30. Twitches and startles, occurring typically during active sleep, are followed by excitation of neurons in primary motor cortex and hippocampus, which is largely in agreement with the results that have been obtained in the present study. In contrast, complex, long-lasting movements that occur during awake periods failed to activate cortical regions. It has been suggested that these two types of motor events are processed differently, the twitches being processed as unexpected events, whereas complex movements are associated with inhibition of cortical activation via corollary discharges. Gating of sensory inputs during complex movements could occur at the level of spinal cord, for example, via local GABAergic interneuron-mediated inhibitory corollary discharge on dorsal sensory neurons and primary afferents32. In the present study, we found that complex movements are associated with the activation of neurons in sensory zones (Fig. 3), pointing to supraspinal mechanisms of inhibition of cortical-hippocampal activation during complex movements.
Interestingly, activity in motor spinal cord regions and corresponding twitches and complex movements were not grossly affected by deafferentation, which is in keeping with previous behavioural results, where no effect of deafferentation on the expression and structure of twitches was shown7. This is in apparent contradiction with our findings that sensory stimuli activate not only sensory, but also intermediate and motor units (Fig. 1), suggesting that sensory feedback has the potential to modulate motor bursts through the excitatory feedback loop involving sensory feedback from movements. However, neither firing of motor units nor duration of motor bursts associated with twitching or complex movements (Fig. 3) were reduced by deafferentation, indicating the autonomy and robustness of the motor generating circuits. It should be noted that, both in the present and in previous studies7, spinal motor activity and associated spontaneous movements were assessed only during a relatively short period of time after deafferentation. However, changes in the normal expression and development of motor activity and behaviour are likely to occur at a longer time scale25, in line with the model of plasticity induced by sensory feedback in developing sensorimotor networks9.
Our study revealed the temporal organization and intralaminar dynamics of sensory-evoked responses in the neonatal rat spinal cord in vivo, at the network level. We found that there are important age-specific differences in the properties of sensory-evoked responses in neonatal rats compared to adult animals. First, the response delays were longer, which likely reflects the myelination process39,40,46,47 and matches the delays reported previously36,37. In agreement with previous results, we also observed a much shorter delay between the responses conveyed by tactile and putative nociceptive afferents. Putative C-fibre inputs triggered spiking of sensory units located exclusively in superficial laminae and, in the no-anesthesia condition, also triggered spiking of motor units, likely reflecting the activation of the nociceptive withdrawal reflex networks9,38. Second, while in adult animals sensory stimulation leads to short-lasting responses, we found that in P5-7 rats, sensory stimulation evokes relatively long-lasting bursts of activity (≈70 ms) in sensory zones. This suggests that the sensory spinal cord network in P5-7 rats operates in an immature bursting mode, as do so many other neuronal networks in that period19,48,49. These relatively long-lasting sensory responses are further conveyed bottom-up and likely contribute to bursting in thalamus-cortex. However, the duration of thalamocortical oscillations evoked by sensory stimulation is significantly longer, up to 1 s13,14. In addition, sensory-evoked bursts in the spinal cord were not organized in any prominent oscillatory pattern in the frequency range characteristic of EGOs11,14,28 or spindle-bursts11,13,50. This suggests that sensory output from the spinal cord triggers, but does not drive thalamocortical oscillations. This is also supported by the findings that spindle-bursts in S1 hindlimb area persist, although at lower frequency, after spinalization at mid-thoracic level, that completely eliminates input from the secondary sensory neurons13.
Our main goal of using the isolated spinal cord preparation was to explore whether and how local intraspinal circuits directly support interactions between sensory and motor neurons, independently of sensory feedback, during bursting network behaviours. In keeping with the results of previous studies, the in vitro neonatal rat spinal cord preparation displayed spontaneous bursting activity33,34,35,44. Interestingly, the activity in motor zones was organized in two main patterns: short-duration bursts, which are reminiscent of the short twitch-producing bursts in vivo (see also Bos et al.34), and long duration megabursts, which are similar to the in vivo complex motor events. The in vitro megabursts were characterized by prominent 6 Hz oscillations, which were never observed during complex bursts in vivo, however. Such spontaneous complex movement-like events, which typically occur during awake, explorative states, have not been reported previously with respect to the isolated spinal cord. This finding indicates that not only twitches, but also complex motor events can be generated in the neonatal spinal cord independently of top-down commands. In the dorsal horn, the activity was organized in regular short bursts, similar in duration to the twitch-triggered bursts, yet occurring at much higher frequency than in vivo, particularly in the deafferentated state. Thus, despite of some difference in the frequency of events and oscillatory feature of the megabursts, the global organization of activity in the isolated spinal cord in vitro was remarkably similar to the activity patterns expressed by the spinal cord in vivo, considering the activity in dorsal and ventral zones separately. However, a striking difference was found in the temporal correlations between the activity in motor and sensory zones, which essentially behaved independently, as in the deafferentated spinal cord in vivo, and the mere correlations, when expressed, were inverted from a motor to sensory lead in vivo to a sensory to motor lead in vitro (see also Bos et al.34). These results confirm our conclusion on the instrumental role of sensory feedback in synchronizing sensory and motor neurons, as revealed by our in vivo experiments, while pointing to limited roles of local communication between motor and sensory neurons in the generation of coordinated sensorimotor activities.
Pharmacological analysis indicated a primary contribution of glutamatergic mechanisms in the generation of spontaneous bursts in motor and sensory zones, as all organized activity was suppressed by glutamate receptor antagonists, in agreement with previous studies34,51,52. However, GABAergic mechanisms differently contributed to the function of dorsal and ventral networks: while the recurrent bursts in dorsal horn were completely suppressed by the GABA(A) receptor antagonist, activity in motor zones was boosted and converted to epileptiform discharges (see also refs 34, 44, 53). Importantly, even these hypersynchronous events in the disinhibited motor zones failed to activate dorsal horn neurons, indicating that glutamatergic networks implicated in generation of motor activity do not project to dorsal neurons. This is also in keeping with a perinatal switch in GABA actions from excitatory to inhibitory on motor and sensory spinal cord neurons54,55,56,57,58. On the other hand, spontaneous bursts in dorsal roots and following them excitatory postsynaptic potentials in motoneurons and ventral root discharges in the spinal cord in vitro are suppressed by the NKCC1 blocker bumetanide and enhanced by the positive allosteric GABA(A) modulator diazepam, indicating an involvement of NKCC1-dependent excitatory actions of GABA on primary afferents34. Yet, in the intact neonatal animals in vivo bumetanide was shown to have little effect, whereas diazepam was shown to exert mainly inhibitory actions on spontaneous and sensory-evoked activity in the somatosensory and visual cortex suggesting limited contribution of the NKCC1-dependent excitatory networks actions of GABA in vivo59,60,61. Such a difference in the GABAergic contributions to the spinal cord functions could explain the inversed temporal correlations between the activity in sensory and motor zones and higher frequency of the dorsal bursts in vitro than in vivo, but this remains to be experimentally demonstrated.
The main goal of our study was to characterize the dynamics of motor and sensory networks in the spinal cord of neonatal rats in the context of their motor behaviour. Our main conclusion is that the activity in sensory zones follows the activity in motor zones, and that sensory feedback resulting both from short twitches and more complex movement sequences is instrumental for sensorimotor synchronization. While the idea of the importance of sensory feedback from neonatal movements in driving the activity in sensory networks is not novel, the present study provides strong evidence to this hypothesis through an exploration of the dynamics of sensorimotor networks in the developing spinal cord.
Methods
All experiments were performed in accordance with protocols approved by the Ethical Committee for Animal Research of Marseille (reference 00708.01).
Subjects
Subjects, in total 25, were 5–7 days old male and female Wistar rats, weighing 12.0–22.6 g, from multiple litters. Animals were obtained from the in-house breeding colonies. All animals were housed in enriched laboratory cages, in climate-controlled rooms (22 °C, 60% humidity), under a 12 h light/12 h dark cycle and with ad libitum access to water and food. Prior to the onset of experiments, we verified that the pups had been recently fed (with ingested milk being visible through the abdominal skin). Animals were randomly selected from multiple litters; age selection was based on colony availability. Experimental cases that did not meet the pre-established criteria—first, the positioning of the electrode, which was verified post hoc, and, second, lack of discernable tissue damage—were excluded from the study (n=2 animals—1 in vivo; 1 in vitro).
Electrophysiological recordings in vivo
We developed a method for simultaneously recording electrical activity across spinal cord laminae and motor behaviour in neonatal rats, based in part on previous preparations for chronic imaging of the dorsal spinal cord and spinal single-wire or patch-clamp electrophysiological recordings in anesthetized, adult rodents62,63,64,65. Specifically, prior to and during surgery, animals (n=15) were anesthetized using isoflurane (Aerrane) and given the analgesic buprenorphine (0.03 mg Kg−1 of body weight, Buprecare). Local anesthesia and vasoconstriction were additionally provided by subcutaneous injection of lidocaine-adrenaline (Xylocaïne-Adrenaline, AstraZeneca) prior to skin incision. Throughout the experiments, body temperature was kept at 37 °C using a heating pad (TC-344B, Warner Instruments). Animals were, at all times, surrounded by a cotton nest, mimicking the presence of the mother and littermates. Dorsal laminectomy was performed typically at the level of vertebrae Th13 and L1. The spinal cord was protected by silicone oil up to the insertion of the electrode array, and a ring-shaped metal adaptor was fixed onto additionally exposed spinal vertebrae, using a thin layer of cyanoacrylate and dental adhesive. The adaptor was attached to a customized stereotactic frame, enabling spinal cord stabilization. The position of the heating path was carefully adjusted, on the z axis, to not constrain natural respiration-related displacements, which could otherwise translate into an upward pressure and spinal displacement. To avoid relatively large movements, animals were partially head-restrained through a thin cylinder-shaped stereotaxic frame adaptor gently fixed onto a small portion of the head. To further avoid potential spinal displacements during tail movements, the tail was very mildly restrained using a small piece of adhesive tape. Buprenorphine was re-administered every 6 h (if applicable).
A linear silicone-based electrode array (15 μm thick, width of 33 μm at the penetrating electrode and of 125 μm at the surface-most electrode; 16 recording sites, with a centre-to-centre separation of 100 μm, each with an area of 703 μm2; A1 × 16-5 mm-100-703-A16, Neuronexus) was inserted in the spinal cord. The tip of the electrode array was positioned at 0.3–0.5 mm lateral to the central vein, and the array was lowered, following dura puncture, with a medial to lateral angle of approximately 17°. Labelling of the electrode array with the fluorescent dye 1,1′-Dioctadecyl-3,3,3′,3′-tetramethylindocarbocyanine perchlorate (DiI, Sigma-Aldrich) prior to insertion facilitated the later anatomical location of the array track (Fig. 1 and Supplementary Fig. 1). Spinal pulsations were minimized by topical application of low-melt agarose. Two chlorided silver wires placed at the surface of the spinal cord and vertebral column served as reference and ground electrodes, respectively.
Recordings were initiated after a minimum waiting period of 30 min following implantation of the electrode array and performed as described previously13. Briefly, wideband neurophysiological signals were amplified × 1,000 through a custom-built amplifier and acquired at 10 kHz (Digidata, Axon Instruments). Tactile stimuli were provided through a 0.6 mm-diameter metal bar driven by a piezoelectric bending actuator (PAB-4010, Nihon Ceratec), which was controlled by a square pulse (5–10 ms) delivered at 0.2 s−1 using a stimulator (Master 8, AMPI) and stimulus isolator (ISO-Flex, AMPI). We performed approximately 100 trials per stimulation point. Spontaneous activity was recorded 30–60 min after discontinuing anesthesia.
Following recordings, animals were given an overdose of urethane (3 g Kg−1, Sigma-Aldrich) and perfused intracardially with saline for 1 min and, subsequently, with 4% formaldehyde for 2 min (at a rate of 5 ml min−1).
Motor behaviour
Ipsilateral hindlimb movements were simultaneously recorded through a piezoelectric transducer (KPEG165, Kingstate, Fig. 1a), which enabled detection of the smallest visible movements, including respiration-related ones. The signal was digitized at 10 kHz, as described before (Digidata interface).
Intra-spinal microstimulation (ISMS)
A bipolar 50 μm nichrome-wire (A-M Systems) electrode was inserted in the spinal cord, under deep anesthesia (1.5% isoflurane). The electrode was lowered to the maximum depth used for recordings (near motoneuron pools); the current required to evoke detectable movements was determined (threshold) and used for all subsequent stimulations. Stimulation sites matched recording sites. Electrical pulses (50 μs, delivered at a frequency of 0.2 s−1) at near-threshold current were generated by an AMPI stimulator and stimulus isolator, as above described. We performed approximately 50 trials per stimulation depth.
Transection of dorsal rootlets
In a subset of animals (n=3 of 15), deeply anesthetized using 1.5% isoflurane, the electrode array was retracted from the spinal cord, and durotomy was performed to expose the dorsal rootlets innervating the array-implanted and adjacent regions (Fig. 5a and Supplementary Fig. 4). Rootlets were gently set aside with the aid of an ultrafine forceps and transected using ultrafine micro-scissors. The completeness of local afferent transection was verified by lack of tactile stimulus-induced spinal responses.
Defining sleep-awake states and related movements
In a separate group of animals (n=3 sham, head-restrained animals), we detected limb movements through piezoelectric transducers, as above described, and simultaneously recorded nuchal EMG, essentially as described by others66. Signals were amplified and digitized at 32 kHz using a Digital Lynx SX, Neuralynx data acquisition system. Defining sleep-awake states in neonatal rats, at stages in which the expression of characteristic sleep-wake neurophysiological patterns is not yet present is commonly achieved through nuchal EMG recordings1.
Extracellular recordings in vitro
Spinal cords were isolated essentially as described previously by others34. Animals (n=6) were anesthetized using isoflurane, decapitated and rapidly eviscerated. The preparation was immersed quickly in ice-cold, carbogenated (95% O2–5% CO2) artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) and pinned down. We then performed a dorsal laminectomy, from sacral to cervical segments. The dura matter was carefully excised, and the spinal cord, together with dorsal and ventral roots, was quickly transferred to and maintained in a holding chamber (containing carbogenated aCSF) at room temperature (RT) for at least 1 h. The spinal cord was then placed, dorsal side up, in a modified interface recording chamber67, where it was submerged in and perfused with carbogenated (95% O2–5% CO2) aCSF (3–4 ml min−1) of the following composition, in mM: 130 NaCL, 4 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 1.3 MgSO4, 0.58 NaHPO4, 25 NaHCO3 and 10 glucose at RT. The electrode array and implantation procedure were as described for in vivo recordings (Supplementary Fig. 5a).
Signals were amplified using a xCellAmp64 amplifier and digitized at 10 kHz (DIPSI). Dorsal and ventral roots were stimulated using a bipolar 50 μm nichrome-wire electrode; electrical pulses (50 μs) were generated by a Grass Products stimulator and stimulus isolator and delivered at 0.2–2 Hz. In a subset of experiments, recordings were performed under control conditions and in the presence of 2R)-amino-5-phosphonopentanoate (APV, 50 μM) and 6-cyano-7-nitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (CNQX, 20 μM), or 4-[6-imino-3-(4-methoxyphenyl)pyridazin-1-yl] butanoic acid hydrobromide (gabazine, 5 μM), all from Sigma-Aldrich. Upon completion of the experiment, one preparation was gently turned ventral side up, and the silicone probe inserted in order to match the previous dorsal side up configuration; the activity patterns obtained in the dorsal side up preparation were verified in the inversed set-up.
Histology
Isolated spinal cords were kept in 4% formaldehyde for at least 1 week, at 4 °C. First, to confirm the spinal cord segment in which the electrode array was inserted, we studied the position of the dorsal DiI signal in relation to lumbar ventral roots. In parallel, the (dorsal) lumbar enlargement was imaged: epifluorescence (DiI) and dark-field micrographs were acquired under standardized conditions using an Olympus SZX16 microscope and the CellSens software (Olympus). In acquired images, the area corresponding to the lumbar enlargement (dorsal surface) was divided in six equal segments, considered as L1-L6 (≈1 mm). Next, the lumbar spinal cord was sliced transversally. Slices (200 μm, thick) were imaged as above described prior to immunohistochemistry. We performed choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) and neuronal nuclei (NeuN) immunohistochemistry. Slices (free-floating) were rinsed three times with PBS and kept in a 5% blocking solution—5% normal serum (Jackson ImmunoResearch) and 0.25% Triton X-100 (Sigma-Aldrich) in PBS—for 1 h at RT. Thereafter, slices were incubated with a goat anti-ChAT antibody (1:100, Chemicon, Merck Millipore) and a mouse anti-NeuN antibody (1:200, Chemicon) in 2% blocking solution overnight at 4 °C. After rinsing, slices were incubated with an anti-goat Alexa633-conjugate antibody and an anti-mouse Alexa488-conjugate antibody (both from Molecular Probes, Invitrogen) in 2% blocking solution for 2 h at RT. Fluorescent dyes (DiI, Alexa488 and Alexa633) were imaged using a Leica TCS SP5 X confocal miscroscope (Leica). Note that the DiI signal was strikingly diminished by the immunohistochemistry protocol. To our knowledge, there is no available systematic study on the anatomical organization of the neonatal rat spinal cord. Hence, laminae borders were extrapolated from analysis of dark-field images (slice morphology), ChAT and NeuN immunoreactivity patterns, and knowledge of the adult rat spinal cord. Note that images were linearly altered for better visualization of slice morphology. The anatomical location of each recording site of the electrode array was estimated using the insertion coordinates and the corresponding or age-matched histological assessment (as demonstrated in Fig. 1b and Supplementary Fig. 1). Throughout the manuscript, depth refers to the subdural depth considering the electrode array (and not the dorsal-ventral) axis, for simplification.
Data analysis
Data were analysed offline using the Axon Instruments software, as well as conventional and custom-made MATLAB routines. Raw data were preprocessed using a custom-developed set of programs in the MATLAB analysis environment. For spike detection, the original wide-band signal was band-pass filtered (300–4000 Hz), and negative events exceeding 3.2 (in vitro) to 4 (in vivo) × minimum s.d. (using 10 ms windows) were considered spikes. Simultaneous events, that is, spikes detected in more than half of the recording sites with zero jitter, were considered artifacts and eliminated (rare). With respect to in vitro recordings, electrical stimulation artefacts were eliminated by truncating the original wide-band signal at −1 to 1 ms considering the stimulus first peak. Positive polarity is depicted up in all figures. Tactile stimulus times or behavioural event onsets (in vivo), as well as electrical stimulation of dorsal and ventral roots or spontaneous sensory and motor bursts onsets (in vitro) were used as triggers to compute mean LFP and current source density (CSD, second spatial derivative of LFP) maps, as well as individual trigger spectrograms of field. CSD analysis was used to eliminate volume conduction and localize synaptic currents; CSD was computed for each recording site and smoothed with a triangular kernel of length 3. Spectral analysis was carried out using the Chronux toolbox. Spectral power was given by direct multi-taper estimators (generally, 5 Hz bandwidth, 3 tapers, 200 ms spectral window); to remove the slow frequency envelope, the LFP was filtered (5–100 Hz). For presentation, translaminar peri-stim time-histograms (PSTHs) and peri-event time-histograms (PETHs) of MUA, firing rates for each recording site (bin size=1 ms) were smoothed by a 10-point moving average, and a between channels interpolation of 7th order was used.
For each animal and tactile stimulation point (in vivo), PSTHs of MUA were computed. Detection of channels showing stimulus-evoked activity was achieved by the Otsu’s method. In all cases, the identified channels were located within the dorsal horn, as estimated histologically. It should be noted, however, that when recordings were done in the absence of isoflurane, an elevation of MUA at the level of the ventral horn also could be found (see Fig. 2 and Supplementary Fig. 2). For each identified channel, the precise average response onset and offset were determined using a trapezoidal sum-based method (adapted from Mitrukhina et al.68). Then, we selected the two channels showing the earliest onsets; these were always contiguous and the respective depths referred to as ‘sensory zone’. The corresponding within-response MUA frequencies were averaged, and normalized to the maximum value obtained across stimulation points (for each animal). Normalized MUA frequency values were represented as colour-coded dots overlaid on respective stimulation points in aged-matched hindlimb photographs, allowing the creation of a ‘response map’. The stimulation point associated with the shortest onset and maximal MUA frequency (‘topographic’) was considered for further single animal and group data analyses.
An equivalent analysis was carried out with respect to the activity evoked by electrical stimulation of different dorsal roots (in vitro).
For each intraspinal depth of electrical stimulation (in vivo), the stimulus onset was used as trigger for averaging the corresponding movement signal (recorded through a piezoelectric transducer). Movement onset and offset were determined as described above68. Results are presented as the mean signal per depth overlaid on a colour-coded amplitude map, which was normalized to the maximum value obtained across depths of stimulation. The two depths associated with the highest evoked-movement amplitudes were considered as motor zone and used for subsequent analysis. With respect to the intermediate zone, we considered the two recording sites located 300 and 200 μm dorsal to the dorsal-most recording site corresponding to the motor zone; thus, intermediate zones corresponded to the average depths of 680–780 μm.
With respect to activity evoked by electrical stimulation of ventral roots (in vitro), the stimulus onset was used as a trigger for averaging LFP and computing a CSD map. Response-associated recording sites, generally 2, were identified by analysis of PSTHs of MUA and confirmed by visual inspection of single responses (population spikes).
To quantify the relationship between behaviour and spinal cord activity (in vivo), we first defined behavioural events. We determined the minimum standard deviation of the original movement signal found in sliding windows of 1–106 ms (log scale), with 50% overlap. For all records, a striking increase in the minimum s.d. was obtained for windows bigger than 104 ms, and thus, a multiplier (generally 4–6) of the minimum s.d. obtained when using 10 ms windows used was as (±) threshold to discriminate noise from real events, on demeaned traces. Then, local minimum and maximum signal values were calculated, and peaks less than 300 ms apart were considered part of a single event, taking into account signal resolution and previous studies13,69. The first derivative of the demeaned trace was then calculated, and the precise twitch onset given by the first zero of the differential preceding the event’s first intersection with the threshold. Offset detection followed an equivalent rule (Supplementary Fig. 3). Thereafter, events shorter than 600 ms were considered as putative twitches and manually classified using a customized data viewer, showing single cases in a time window of 4 s. Specifically, events were classified as twitches (likely including indiscernible bouts of twitches at different joints) if occurring on a background of atonia (that is, there were no detected events in the preceding and following ≈1 s). All remaining events were classified as short-lasting movements (typically ending complex movement sequences, 26.6% of all detected events) or noise (14.8% of all detected events), and were not considered for further analysis. Events lasting 600–900 ms were considered intermediate behavioural events (and were included in the total number of detected independent behavioural events; note that in Fig. 1a,c, these events also are represented in grey, together with short-lasting movements, for simplification). Events lasting more than 900 ms were considered as complex movements. For the parallel analysis of simultaneous recordings of limb movements (through piezoelectric transducers) and nuchal EMG, piezoelectric transducer-derived and EMG signals were treated independently. Limb movements were detected as above described. With respect to nuchal EMG, the original wide-band signal was filtered (300–700 Hz), demeaned, and negative events exceeding 3.2–4 × minimum s.d. (using 10 ms windows) were detected and transferred to the frequency domain (through 25 ms sliding windows, with a 1 ms overlap). Then, events larger than 1 × s.d. of the frequency trace were detected and joined when: (1) occurring less than 800 ms apart; (2) the mean inter-event activity (original trace, absolute values) was higher than threshold. Finally, events lasting more than 1 s were considered as awake bouts. All remaining periods of the recording session were considered as sleep.
For each animal, translaminar PETHs of MUA, with detected twitch onsets as reference point, were computed essentially as described before. For further analysis of spontaneous translaminar cord MUA, we used data from previously identified sensory and motor zones, as well as from intermediate zones. For each twitch, the mean baseline (entire session) was used to detect MUA frequency peaks in motor, intermediate and sensory zones; results are presented as average MUA frequency peak times and baseline return times. A similar analysis was carried out for complex movements. However, due to the large repertoire of complex movement durations and lack of stereotypy, each complex movement period (onset-offset) was divided in 100 bins, and thus firing dynamics during complex movement epochs are presented as time-normalized PETHs. With respect to pre-onset and post-offset periods, we considered −0.5 and +0.5 s periods, respectively; binning was done considering the mean complex movement duration (1.7 s). For each animal, the mean of pre-onset period MUA activity was considered as baseline mean. Cross-correlation analyses between motor and sensory zone spikes, and between motor and intermediate zone spikes were performed taking into account twitching epochs (300 ms prior to and to 1 s after twitch or 300 ms prior to 1.5 s), complex movement epochs (onset to offset), or entire recording sessions (per animal). Each cross-correlogram (bin size=1 ms) was smoothed by a 3-point moving average.
To further understand temporal correlations between behaviour and spinal cord network activity, we detected activity bursts within sensory and motor zones, based on a previously described method70. For that purpose, individual channel’s instantaneous firing rates were calculated using sliding windows of 50 ms (10% overlap). Bursts were generally considered as events exceeding 5 s.d. of the instantaneous firing rate, and events (MUA frequency peaks) less than 500 ms apart were considered as one. Then, event onsets and offsets were estimated taking into account the nearest intersections with the baseline mean (respectively).
To detect in vitro bursts of activity within identified sensory and motor zones, we used an approach similar to that described for the in vivo data, with a few modifications. Bursts were generally considered as events exceeding 3 s.d. of the instantaneous firing rate. Bursts within sensory zones were rather stereotypical. In motor zones, however, we observed two main event types: short- and long-lasting (or mega-) bursts organized in ≈6 Hz LFP and MUA coherent oscillations. Here, a first threshold (3 s.d.) was used to more sensibly establish mega-bursts, while a second threshold (4 s.d.) was used to eliminate between-bursts potentially spurious increases in firing rates (that is, isolated short-lasting events −56% of detected events were excluded). Here, local minimum LFP values were used to align events. Sensory and motor events were then used as triggers to compute mean LFP, CSD maps and PETHs of MUA. Onsets and offsets of sensory and short-lasting motor events were estimated using the trapezoidal sum-based method on triggered, spike histograms. Mega-bursts onsets and offsets were simply estimated by the first and last local minimum LFP value.
Cross-correlation analysis of spike times, and bursts times (onset phase) in the two different zones was carried out essentially as described previously.
Statistics
We did not use statistical methods to calculate the required sample size, a priori, but the number of animals used in this study is consistent with that used in previous reports13. Data could not be acquired by an experimenter blinded to the study design. Nonetheless, data were analysed offline, in serial runs, and was independently verified by different analysts. Unless otherwise indicated, the statistical tests used do not assume a normal distribution of the data, and to compare two groups, we used the Wilcoxon rank-sum test (unpaired data) or the Wilcoxon signed-rank test (paired-data), as indicated in the ‘Results’ section and figure captions. In some cases (twitches-related within-bursts MUA peak frequency and respective time in relation to twitch onset, as well as bursts offset, that is, peak frequency to baseline return, twitch/complex movements, maximal amplitudes, power and durations, before and after deafferentation), the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was used instead. In additional particular cases, namely comparisons relative to firing, bursting or movement rates before and after deafferentation, to test for significance, we used a shuffling/bootstrap method. For each animal, we randomly selected 30 recorded minutes. The spikes/bursts train in a selected sensory or motor channel was circularized, and the beginning of a trial was randomly chosen. Then, surrogate trials were recomputed 100 times to generate a global band of confidence (the 5% highest and lowest values of the 100 trials × 3 animals). Results were considered significant when P<0.05; in the figures, P<0.05, P<0.01 and P<0.001 were represented by one, two or three asterisks, respectively; specific P were included in the ‘Results’ section.
Data availability
The authors declare that all data and MATLAB codes used to generate the figures are available upon request.
Additional information
How to cite this article: Inácio, A. R. et al. Sensory feedback synchronizes motor and sensory neuronal networks in the neonatal rat spinal cord. Nat. Commun. 7, 13060 doi: 10.1038/ncomms13060 (2016).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Drs G. Buzsaki and R. Cossart for their careful and critical reading of the manuscript. We also thank Drs L. Khalki, E. Pearlestein for important input concerning isolation of the neonatal rat spinal cord and V. Tsintsadze for assistance with in vitro recordings, A. Minlebaeva and E. Ogievetsky for assistance with histology, Drs C. Hammond and C. Rivera for kindly providing antibodies, and Robert Martinez for technical support. Finally, we thank Dr M. Minlebaev for fruitful discussions. This work was supported by INSERM (LIA to R.K.), a Marie Curie Intra-European Fellowship (to A.R.I.), Ministry of Education and Research of the Russian Federation (R.K., A.N., J.L.), Government of the Russian Federation (grant 11.G34.31.0075 to R.K.), and the Program of Competitive growth of the Kazan Federal University.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Author contributions A.R.I., J.L. and R.K. performed the experiments. A.R.I., A.N. and R.K. analysed the data. A.R.I. and R.K. designed the experiments and wrote the manuscript. All authors edited the manuscript.
References
- Blumberg M. S., Freeman J. H. & Robinson S. R. Oxford Handbook of Developmental Behavioral Neuroscience Oxford University Press (2010). [Google Scholar]
- Hamburger V. in The Mammalian Fetus: Comparative Biology and Methodology ed. Hafez E. S. 69–81Charles C. Thomas (1975). [Google Scholar]
- De Vries J. I., Visser G. H. & Prechtl H. F. The emergence of fetal behaviour. I. Qualitative aspects. Early Hum. Dev. 7, 301–322 (1982). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson S. R., Blumberg M. S., Lane M. S. & Kreber L. A. Spontaneous motor activity in fetal and infant rats is organized into discrete multilimb bouts. Behav. Neurosci. 114, 328–336 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreider J. C. & Blumberg M. S. Mesopontine contribution to the expression of active ‘twitch’ sleep in decerebrate week-old rats. Brain Res. 872, 149–159 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Rio-Bermudez C., Sokoloff G. & Blumberg M. S. Sensorimotor processing in the newborn rat red nucleus during active sleep. J. Neurosci. 35, 8322–8332 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waldenstrom A., Christensson M. & Schouenborg J. Spontaneous movements: Effect of denervation and relation to the adaptation of nociceptive withdrawal reflexes in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 98, 532–536 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanes J. R. & Lichtman J. W. Development of the vertebrate neuromuscular junction. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 389–442 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersson P., Waldenstrom A., Fahraeus C. & Schouenborg J. Spontaneous muscle twitches during sleep guide spinal self-organization. Nature 424, 72–75 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendelsohn A. I., Simon C. M., Abbott L. F., Mentis G. Z. & Jessell T. M. Activity regulates the incidence of heteronymous sensory-motor connections. Neuron. 87, 111–123 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang J. W., Hanganu-Opatz I. L., Sun J. J. & Luhmann H. J. Three patterns of oscillatory activity differentially synchronize developing neocortical networks in vivo. J. Neurosci. 29, 9011–9025 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcano-Reik A. J. & Blumberg M. S. The corpus callosum modulates spindle-burst activity within homotopic regions of somatosensory cortex in newborn rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 28, 1457–1466 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khazipov R. et al. Early motor activity drives spindle bursts in the developing somatosensory cortex. Nature 432, 758–761 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang J. W. et al. Thalamic network oscillations synchronize ontogenetic columns in the newborn rat barrel cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 23, 1299–1316 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milh M. et al. Rapid cortical oscillations and early motor activity in premature human neonate. Cereb. Cortex. 17, 1582–1594 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanhatalo S. & Kaila K. Development of neonatal EEG activity: from phenomenology to physiology. Semin. Fetal Neonatal Med. 11, 471–478 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- An S. M., Kilb W. & Luhmann H. J. Sensory-evoked and spontaneous gamma and spindle bursts in neonatal rat motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 34, 10870–10883 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McVea D. A., Mohajerani M. H. & Murphy T. H. Voltage-sensitive dye imaging reveals dynamic spatiotemporal properties of cortical activity after spontaneous muscle twitches in the newborn rat. J. Neurosci. 32, 10982–10994 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blankenship A. G. & Feller M. B. Mechanisms underlying spontaneous patterned activity in developing neural circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 18–29 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moody W. J. & Bosma M. M. Ion channel development, spontaneous activity, and activity-dependent development in nerve and muscle cells. Physiol. Rev. 85, 883–941 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Donovan M. J., Chub N. & Wenner P. Mechanisms of spontaneous activity in developing spinal networks. J. Neurobiol. 37, 131–145 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marder E. & Rehm K. J. Development of central pattern generating circuits. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 86–93 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clancy B., Darlington R. B. & Finlay B. L. Translating developmental time across mammalian species. Neuroscience 105, 7–17 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marques H. G., Bharadwaj A. & Iida F. From spontaneous motor activity to coordinated behaviour: a developmental model. PLoS Comput. Biol. 10, e1003653 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumberg M. S. et al. Development of twitching in sleeping infant mice depends on sensory experience. Curr. Biol. 25, 656–662 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waldenstrom A., Thelin J., Thimansson E., Levinsson A. & Schouenborg J. Developmental learning in a pain-related system: evidence for a cross-modality mechanism. J. Neurosci. 23, 7719–7725 (2003). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schouenborg J. & Weng H. R. Sensorimotor transformation in a spinal motor system. Exp. Brain. Res. 100, 170–174 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minlebaev M., Colonnese M., Tsintsadze T., Sirota A. & Khazipov R. Early gamma oscillations synchronize developing thalamus and cortex. Science 334, 226–229 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mohns E. J. & Blumberg M. S. Synchronous bursts of neuronal activity in the developing hippocampus: modulation by active sleep and association with emerging gamma and theta rhythms. J. Neurosci. 28, 10134–10144 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiriac A., Del Rio-Bermudez C. & Blumberg M. S. Self-generated movements with “unexpected” sensory consequences. Curr. Biol. 24, 2136–2141 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crapse T. B. & Sommer M. A. Corollary discharge across the animal kingdom. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 587–600 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudomin P. & Schmidt R. F. Presynaptic inhibition in the vertebrate spinal cord revisited. Exp. Brain. Res. 129, 1–37 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fellippa-Marques S., Vinay L. & Clarac F. Spontaneous and locomotor-related GABAergic input onto primary afferents in the neonatal rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 12, 155–164 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bos R., Brocard F. & Vinay L. Primary afferent terminals acting as excitatory interneurons contribute to spontaneous motor activities in the immature spinal cord. J. Neurosci. 31, 10184–10188 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kremer E. & Lev-Tov A. GABA-receptor-independent dorsal root afferents depolarization in the neonatal rat spinal cord. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 2581–2592 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgerald M. The post-natal development of cutaneous afferent fibre input and receptive field organization in the rat dorsal horn. J. Physiol. 364, 1–18 (1985). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgerald M. & Jennings E. The postnatal development of spinal sensory processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96, 7719–7722 (1999). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgerald M. The development of nociceptive circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 507–520 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friede R. L. & Samorajski T. Myelin formation in the sciatic nerve of the rat. A quantitative electron microscopic, histochemical and radioautographic study. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 27, 546–570 (1968). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Webster H. D. The geometry of peripheral myelin sheaths during their formation and growth in rat sciatic nerves. J. Cell Biol. 48, 348–367 (1971). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills T. J., Muessig L. & Cacucci F. The development of spatial behaviour and the hippocampal neural representation of space. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 369, 20130409 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumberg M. S. Beyond dreams: do sleep-related movements contribute to brain development? Front Neurol. 1, 140 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demir R., Gao B. X., Jackson M. B. & Ziskind-Conhaim L. Interactions between multiple rhythm generators produce complex patterns of oscillation in the developing rat spinal cord. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 1094–1105 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bracci E., Ballerini L. & Nistri A. Localization of rhythmogenic networks responsible for spontaneous bursts induced by strychnine and bicuculline in the rat isolated spinal cord. J. Neurosci. 16, 7063–7076 (1996). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiriac A., Uitermarkt B. D., Fanning A. S., Sokoloff G. & Blumberg M. S. Rapid whisker movements in sleeping newborn rats. Curr. Biol. 22, 2075–2080 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fulton B. P. Postnatal changes in conduction velocity and soma action potential parameters of rat dorsal root ganglion neurones. Neurosci. Lett. 73, 125–130 (1987). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sima A. Studies on fibre size in developing sciatic nerve and spinal roots in normal, undernourished, and rehabilitated rats. Acta Physiol. Scand Suppl. 406, 1–55 (1974). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colonnese M. T. et al. A conserved switch in sensory processing prepares developing neocortex for vision. Neuron. 67, 480–498 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Ari Y. Developing networks play similar melody. Trends. Neurosci. 24, 353–360 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minlebaev M., Ben Ari Y. & Khazipov R. NMDA receptors pattern early activity in the developing barrel cortex in vivo. Cereb. Cortex. 19, 688–696 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ren J. & Greer J. J. Ontogeny of rhythmic motor patterns generated in the embryonic rat spinal cord. J. Neurophysiol. 89, 1187–1195 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers C. P. et al. Cholinergic input is required during embryonic development to mediate proper assembly of spinal locomotor circuits. Neuron. 46, 37–49 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duchen M. R. Excitation of mouse motoneurones by GABA-mediated primary afferent depolarization. Brain Res. 379, 182–187 (1986). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao B. X. & Ziskind-Conhaim L. Development of glycine-and GABA-gated currents in rat spinal motoneurons. J. Neurophysiol. 74, 113–121 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu W. L., Ziskind-Conhaim L. & Sweet M. A. Early development of glycine- and GABA-mediated synapses in rat spinal cord. J. Neurosci. 12, 3935–3945 (1992). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jean-Xavier C., Pflieger J. F., Liabeuf S. & Vinay L. Inhibitory postsynaptic potentials in lumbar motoneurons remain depolarizing after neonatal spinal cord transection in the rat. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 2274–2281 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baccei M. L. & Fitzgerald M. Development of GABAergic and glycinergic transmission in the neonatal rat dorsal horn. J. Neurosci. 24, 4749–4757 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stil A. et al. Developmental up-regulation of the potassium-chloride cotransporter type 2 in the rat lumbar spinal cord. Neuroscience. 164, 809–821 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minlebaev M., Ben-Ari Y. & Khazipov R. Network mechanisms of spindle-burst oscillations in the neonatal rat barrel cortex in vivo. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 692–700 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valeeva G., Tressard T., Mukhtarov M., Baude A. & Khazipov R. An optogenetic approach for investigation of excitatory and inhibitory network GABA actions in mice expressing channelrhodopsin-2 in GABAergic neurons. J. Neurosci. 36, 5961–5973 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirmse K. et al. GABA depolarizes immature neurons and inhibits network activity in the neonatal neocortex in vivo. Nat. Commun. 6, 7750 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham M. G., Donalds R. A., Scouten C. W. & Tresch M. C. A versatile, low-cost adaptor for stereotaxic and electrophysiologic spinal preparations in juvenile and adult rodents. Brain Res. Bull. 68, 157–162 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farrar M. J. et al. Chronic in vivo imaging in the mouse spinal cord using an implanted chamber. Nat. Methods. 9, 297–302 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenrich K. K. et al. Long-term in vivo imaging of normal and pathological mouse spinal cord with subcellular resolution using implanted glass windows. J. Physiol. 590, 3665–3675 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graham B. A., Brichta A. M. & Callister R. J. An in vivo mouse spinal cord preparation for patch-clamp analysis of nociceptive processing. J. Neurosci. Methods. 136, 221–228 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karlsson K. A. & Blumberg M. S. The union of the state: myoclonic twitching is coupled with nuchal muscle atonia in infant rats. Behav. Neurosci. 116, 912–917 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsintsadze V., Minlebaev M., Suchkov D., Cunningham M. O. & Khazipov R. Ontogeny of kainate-induced gamma oscillations in the rat CA3 hippocampus in vitro. Front Cell Neurosci. 9, 195 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitrukhina O., Suchkov D., Khazipov R. & Minlebaev M. Imprecise whisker map in the neonatal rat barrel cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 25, 3458–3467 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumberg M. S., Coleman C. M., Gerth A. I. & McMurray B. Spatiotemporal structure of REM sleep twitching reveals developmental origins of motor synergies. Curr. Biol. 23, 2100–2109 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaneoke Y. & Vitek J. L. Burst and oscillation as disparate neuronal properties. J. Neurosci Methods 68, 211–223 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The authors declare that all data and MATLAB codes used to generate the figures are available upon request.