Abstract
Background
Drowning is the second leading cause of unintentional injury death among U.S. children. Multiple studies describe decreased drowning risk among children possessing some swim skills. Current surveillance for this protective factor is self/proxy-reported swim skill rather than observed in-water performance; however, children’s self-report or parents’ proxy report of swim skill has not been validated.
This is the first U.S. study to evaluate whether children or parents can validly report a child’s swim skill. It also explores which swim skill survey measure(s) correlate with children’s in-water swim performance.
Methods
For this cross-sectional convenience-based sample, pilot study, child/parent dyads (N=482) were recruited at three outdoor public pools in Washington state. Agreement between measures of self- and parental-reports of children’s swim skill was assessed via paired analyses, and validated by in-water swim-test results.
Results
Participants were representative of pool’s patrons (i.e., non-Hispanic White, highly educated, high income). There was agreement in child/parent dyads’ reports of the following child swim skill measures: “ever taken swim lessons”, perceived “good swim skills”, and “comfort in water over head”. Correlation analyses suggest that reported “good swim skills” was the best survey measure to assess a child’s swim skill – best if the parent was the informant (r=0.25–0.47). History of swim lessons was not significantly correlated with passing the swim test.
Conclusion
Reported “good swim skills” was most correlated with observed swim skill. Reporting “yes” to “ever taken swim lessons” did not correlate with swim skill. While non-generalizable, findings can help inform future studies.
Keywords: Drowning, Public Health, Surveillance, Child
INTRODUCTION
Drowning is the second leading cause of unintentional injury death among U.S. children 1–17 years old.[1] In Washington state (WA), the rate of drowning among children 1–19 years old occurring in natural water settings (including while boating) is more than two times the national average (1.04 vs. 0.49 per 100,000).[2]
Drowning injury is severe; more than 50% of victims treated in the emergency department for nonfatal drowning are admitted or transferred for further care.[3] Hypoxic insult may result in long-term memory problems, learning disabilities, and permanent loss of self-help skills.[4 5]
While there is no universally accepted measure of what constitutes water competency, research has shown that drowning risk is lower among children who possess some degree of swim skill, usually obtained through swim lessons.[6–8] UNICEF and the World Health Organization, the American Academy of Pediatrics, the Open Water Drowning Prevention International Task Force, and the International Lifesaving Federation support learning how to swim and acquiring water survival skills as an important drowning prevention strategy.[9–13]
While the gold standard method is to test a child’s performance in water, data collected on swim skill have largely been based on child/self or family reports. In the United States, the validity of swim skill reports has not been assessed by correlation with a child’s swimming performance. Survey data that correlate with children’s actual swimming performance without in-water testing is needed for swim skill surveillance, as a mechanism to reduce drownings.
This study evaluated whether children (7–17 years) attending community pools in Washington State (WA) or their parents can validly report a child’s swim skill. It explored which swim skill self/proxy-reported survey measure(s) (e.g., comfort in deep water, history of swim lessons, subjective assessment of skill, self-reported swim distance) correlate best with a child’s in-water swimming performance. Specifically, this study:
Assessed children’s self-perceptions of their swim skill.
Assessed parent’s perceptions of their children’s swim skill.
Compared responses to survey swim skill measures to children’s in-water swimming performance.
METHODS
Study design
This convenience-based sample, pilot cross-sectional study was conducted at the three highest attendance, outdoor public pools associated with the Seattle and the Snohomish Departments of Parks and Recreation in WA. Participating pools were conveniently selected due to their location and schedule throughout the week. While they also offer lap swimming and learn to swim sessions, participation was restricted to those families visiting the pool to relax or have fun during the open swim sessions.
Parents with school-aged children attend the open/public swim sessions at these pools, which charged an entrance fee (≤$4.00 USD) and required children (≤17 years) to pass a life-guard administered in-water swim-test before entering the deep end. Open/public swim sessions allow patrons to freely enjoy swimming or playing in the water, different from lap swimming and learn to swim teaching sessions.
Sampling
Pool administrators estimated that 80–100 children take the swim test daily at these locations during the summertime. Power calculations estimated a minimum sample size of 436 child-parent dyads (alpha=0.05, estimated 50% in-water swim-test failure rate).
Participant inclusion and exclusion criteria
Children (7–17 years) who were accompanied by a parent or guardian, visited these pools during data collection days, did not belong to a swim team, and were fluent in English or Spanish were eligible to participate. Parental presence was required to give permission for their children’s participation, and to serve as informants on their children’s swim skills. Taking the in-water swim-test was not required; children who chose not to take the swim test were also eligible to participate. Verbal child assent, written parental permission and consent were obtained. The study protocol was approved by the authors’ Institutional Review Boards.
Data collection
Between 3–8 trained interviewers collected data at 1–2 pool locations during July 8–30, 2014, coinciding with the beginning of school-children’s summer vacation break. Interviewers approached potential participants (e.g., adults with children who appeared to be 7–17 years old) outside the pool, while waiting to buy tickets or for the open swim session to start. Adults were invited to participate, and screened for eligibility (i.e., inclusion and exclusion criteria).
Parents completed the parental consent and permission form. Subsequently, a second interviewer verbally explained the study to the child. If the child assented, parents and children were interviewed simultaneously during approximately 5 minutes, yet separately to avoid their influencing each other’s responses. They were reunited once both finished answering the questions.
Survey instruments
Interviewer-administered child/self and parental report surveys were developed in English and Spanish, based on previously used measures and expert advice. These included 4 close-ended child swim skill measures: “knowing how to swim” (1=yes, 0=no/not sure), “ever taken swim lessons” (1=yes, 0=no/not sure), “perceived good swim skill” (1=good, 0=so-so/not good/can’t swim), and “comfort in water over head” (1=comfortable/very comfortable, 0=uncomfortable/slightly uncomfortable). A unique identifying number was assigned to each child/parent dyad.
In-water swim-test
All participating pools required children to pass a basic in-water swim-test before they were allowed in the deep end of the pool during the open/public swim sessions. Children who chose not to be swim-tested were only allowed to be in the shallow end of the pool. While no specific strokes or technical skills were required, to pass the swim-test children had to propel themselves a specific distance (i.e., Pool A (83ft), Pool B (120 ft), Pool C (88 ft)), while not touching the bottom or sides of the pool, demonstrating breathing, front crawl stroke and/or arms above water. All pools required these minimum water competency requirements.
No child was required, invited or encouraged to take the in-water swim-test as part of this study. Swim-test results (pass/fail) were provided by the lifeguard administering the test, only for those participating children who were voluntarily swim-tested that day. Wearing a life-jacket during the swim-test was considered a test fail.
Statistical analyses
Child and parent survey responses, and child swim-test result data were linked and analyzed using Epi Info 7.1.1.14 and SAS v. 9.3. Cross-tabulations were used to describe the sample, and identify any differences (N, %, X2) by 3 sub-groups of child/parent dyads:
Seeking to be swim-tested that day (i.e., swim-tested)
Dyads included the children who voluntarily chose to take the in-water swim-test that day. A child’s willingness to voluntarily take the in-water swim-test could be a reflection of that child having a better self-perception of his/her ability to pass the swim-test.
Previously swim tested this season (i.e., previously swim-tested)
Dyads included children who may have a more accurate perception of his/her swim-skill, based on recent in-water swim-test experience.
Parents’ comfort in water over their head
Dyads included the parents/guardians who did or did not feel comfortable in water over their head. Parents/guardians’ comfort in water over their head may influence their decision on whether to expose their child or not to opportunities in which the child can be in the water and learn how to swim.
We paired the child/parent data, and calculated concurrence between child/parent responses to each swim-skill measure via cross-tabulations (N, %). McNemar’s (S) statistic assessed if there were statistically significant differences between child/parent reports within dyads. When differences were significant, the swim-skill measure was excluded from the remaining analyses. When differences were not statistically significant, we calculated Kappa (k) statistics to describe the level of agreement between child/parent informants for those measures as low (k<0.4), moderate (0.4≥k≥0.75), or high (k>0.75).[14] For the dyads in which the child was seeking to be swim-tested (N=305), we assessed which paired child/self and parental reports of a child’s swim skill measure correlated (Pearson’s r) best with the child passing the in-water swim-test.
RESULTS
Sample description
In total, 775 families were approached. Of the 586 eligible families, 82% agreed to participate. The final sample size was 482 child/parent dyads. The majority of the dyads were swim-tested (63%, n=305); 85% of them passed.
Most children were 7–10 years old (median=9 years), non-Hispanic White (72.4%), and represented both genders equally (Table 1). Significantly more non-Hispanic White children (79.7%) had previously been swim-tested (X2=11.40, p=0.003; Hispanic=5.7%; non-Hispanic Other=14.6%). Swim-tested children were slightly older (median=10 years) and of non-Hispanic White (87.1%) parents/guardians (non-Hispanic Other 9.6%; Hispanic 3.3%; X2=9.79, p=.0008) (data not shown).
Table 1.
Socio-demographic characteristics of all participant child/parent dyads (N=482)
Participant | Socio-demographic Characteristics | N | % |
---|---|---|---|
Children (n=482) | Age, in years | ||
Median (min, max) | 9 (7, 17) | ||
7–10 years | 322 | 66.8 | |
11–14 years | 141 | 29.3 | |
15–17 years | 19 | 3.9 | |
Missing | 0 | 0 | |
| |||
Sex | |||
Male | 240 | 49.8 | |
Female | 240 | 49.8 | |
Missing | 2 | 0.4 | |
| |||
Race and ethnicity | |||
Non-Hispanic White | 349 | 72.4 | |
Non-Hispanic Other | 83 | 17.2 | |
Hispanic | 43 | 8.9 | |
Missing | 7 | 1.5 | |
| |||
Parents or Legal Guardians (n=482) | Age, in years | ||
Median (min, max) | 44 (28, 64) | ||
25–34 years | 28 | 5.8 | |
35–44 years | 265 | 55 | |
45–54 years | 175 | 36.3 | |
55–64 years | 11 | 2.3 | |
Missing | 3 | 0.6 | |
| |||
Sex | |||
Male | 121 | 25.1 | |
Female | 361 | 74.9 | |
Missing | 0 | 0 | |
| |||
Race and ethnicity | |||
Non-Hispanic White | 401 | 83.2 | |
Non-Hispanic Other | 51 | 10.6 | |
Hispanic | 27 | 5.6 | |
Missing | 3 | 0.6 | |
| |||
Highest education completed | |||
Less than a bachelor’s degree | 126 | 26.1 | |
Bachelor’s degree or higher | 356 | 73.9 | |
Missing | 0 | 0 | |
| |||
Annual household income | |||
< $100,000 | 203 | 42.1 | |
≥ $100,000 | 231 | 47.9 | |
Declined to answer | 48 | 10 |
Parents/guardians’ were on average 44 years old, mostly female (74.9%) and non-Hispanic White (83.2%). Most parents/guardians completed a bachelor’s degree or higher (73.9%), and 47.9% of the dyads reported an annual household income of ≥$100,000 (Table 1). More swim-tested (51.5%) than non-swim-tested (41.8%) dyads reported ≥$100,000 annual household income (X2=9.17, p=.01) (data not shown).
Reports on children’s swim skills
Children’s self-reported swim skills
Most children self-reported “knowing how to swim” (92.9%), “ever taken swim lessons” (88.5%), “good swim skills” (58.7%), and feeling “comfortable in water over head” (76.6%). All swim skill measures were significantly higher among those who had previously been swim-tested. Children seeking to be swim-tested on the data collection day also reported significantly higher swim skill on all measures except “ever taken swim lessons” (Table 2).
Table 2.
Child/self and parental reports of child swim skills, overall and by children’s swim-test status
Informant | N (%) | Total (N=482) | Children’s Swim-Test Status | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||
Seeking to be Swim-Tested* | Previously Swim-Tested* | |||||||
|
|
|||||||
Yes (n=305) | No (n=177) | X2, df, p-value | Yes (n=251) | No or Unknown (n=231) | X2, df, p-value | |||
Child (Self) | Knows how to swim | |||||||
No or not sure | 34 (7.1) | 5 (1.6) | 29 (16.4) | 37.14, 1, <.0001 | 9 (3.6) | 25 (10.8) | 9.61, 1, 0.002 | |
Yes | 448 (92.9) | 300 (98.4) | 148 (83.6) | 242 (96.4) | 206 (89.2) | |||
|
|
|
||||||
Ever taken swim lessons | ||||||||
No or not sure | 55 (11.5) | 29 (9.6) | 26 (14.9) | NS | 21 (8.5) | 34 (14.9) | 4.75, 1, 0.03 | |
Yes | 422 (88.5) | 273 (90.4) | 149 (85.1) | 227 (91.5) | 195 (85.2) | |||
Missing (excluded) † | 5 | |||||||
|
|
|
||||||
Perceived good swim skill ‡ | ||||||||
No | 196 (41.3) | 92 (30.6) | 104 (59.8) | 38.80, 1, <.0001 | 83 (33.6) | 113 (49.6) | 12.46, 1, 0.0004 | |
Yes | 279 (58.7) | 209 (69.4) | 70 (40.2) | 164 (66.4) | 115 (50.4) | |||
Missing (excluded) † | 7 | |||||||
|
|
|
||||||
Comfort in water over head § | ||||||||
No | 112 (23.4) | 43 (14.1) | 69 (39.4) | 39.63, 1, <.0001 | 36 (14.3) | 76 (33.3) | 24.1, 1, <.0001 | |
Yes | 367 (76.6) | 261 (85.9) | 106 (60.6) | 215 (85.7) | 152 (66.7) | |||
Missing (excluded) † | 3 | |||||||
| ||||||||
Parent or Legal Guardian | Knows how to swim | |||||||
No or not sure | 54 (11.2) | 6 (2) | 48 (27.1) | 71.22, 1, <.0001 | 13 (5.2) | 41 (17.8) | 19.10, 1, <.0001 | |
Yes | 428 (88.8) | 299 (98) | 129 (72.9) | 238 (94.8) | 190 (82.3) | |||
|
|
|
||||||
Ever taken swim lessons | ||||||||
No or not sure | 61 (12.7) | 32 (10.5) | 29 (16.4) | NS | 27 (10.8) | 34 (14.7) | NS | |
Yes | 421 (87.3) | 273 (89.5) | 148 (83.6) | 224 (89.2) | 197 (85.3) | |||
|
|
|
||||||
Perceived good swim skill ‡ | ||||||||
No | 209 (43.4) | 83 (27.2) | 126 (71.2) | 88.18, 1, <.0001 | 79 (31.5) | 130 (56.3) | 30.13, 1, <.0001 | |
Yes | 273 (56.6) | 222 (72.8) | 51 (28.8) | 172 (68.5) | 101 (43.7) | |||
|
|
|
||||||
Comfort in water over head § | ||||||||
No | 129 (26.8) | 41 (13.4) | 88 (49.7) | 75.19, 1, <.0001 | 44 (17.5) | 85 (36.8) | 22.78, 1, <.0001 | |
Yes | 353 (73.2) | 264 (86.6) | 89 (50.3) | 207 (82.5) | 146 (63.2) |
“Seeking to be Swim-Tested” refers to dyads where the child voluntarily chose to take the swim-test on the data collection day and results were available for this study. “Previously Swim-Tested” refers to dyads where the child had been swim-tested previously during that same swim-season but prior to the study date.
Records with missing data were excluded from the analyses.
No=Can’t swim, not good, and so-so.
No=Uncomfortable or slightly uncomfortable; Yes=Comfortable or very comfortable
NS=Not statistically significant
Parental/guardian reports on children’s swim skills
Similarly, most parents/guardians said their children “know how to swim” (88.8%), had “ever taken swim lessons” (87.3%), had “good swim skills” (56.6%), and felt “comfortable in water over head” (73.2%). Parents/guardians’ reports were significantly higher among dyads where the child had previously been swim-tested, except for the “ever taken swim lessons” measure. Parents/guardians who reported feeling comfortable in water over head reported significantly more often that the child was also “comfortable in water over head” (Table 2).
Reports on parents’ swim skills
Most parents/guardians self-reported that they “know how to swim” (95%), had “ever taken swim lessons” (73.2%), and felt “comfortable in water over head” (87.5%). Females accounted for the majority of parents/guardians who did not feel comfortable in water over their heads (93.3%, X2=12.45, p=.0004) (data not shown).
Agreement between child/self and parental reports on children’s swim skills
Total paired sample (N=482) data were analyzed to evaluate agreement between children and parents/guardians’ responses within each dyad (Table 3).
Table 3.
Agreement between child/self and parental reports of child swim skill measures
Knows How to Swim* | ||||||||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||||||||||||||||
% | Total (N=482) | Seeking to be Swim-Tested | Previously Swim Tested | Parent’s Comfort in Water Over Head | ||||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||||||
Yes (N=305) | No (N=177) | Yes (N=251) | No** (N=231) | Yes (N=421) | No (N=60) | |||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
Child Report | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | ||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
Parent Report | Yes | 85.9 | 2.9 | 88.8 | 97.1 | 1 | 98.1 | 66.7 | 6.1 | 72.8 | 93.6 | 1.2 | 94.8 | 77.5 | 4.8 | 82.3 | 86.2 | 2.6 | 88.8 | 85 | 5 | 90 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
No | 7 | 4.2 | 11.2 | 1.2 | 0.7 | 1.9 | 17 | 10.2 | 27.2 | 2.8 | 2.4 | 5.2 | 11.6 | 6.1 | 17.7 | 7.6 | 3.6 | 11.2 | 3.3 | 6.7 | 10 | |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
92.9 | 7.1 | 98.3 | 1.7 | 83.7 | 16.3 | 96.4 | 3.6 | 89.1 | 10.9 | 93.8 | 6.2 | 88.3 | 11.7 | |||||||||
S, p-val. | 8.33, 0.004 | NS | 8.81, 0.003 | NS | 6.74, 0.009 | 10.26, 0.001 | NS | |||||||||||||||
k, 95%CI§ | NA | 0.35,−0.02–0.72 | NA | 0.53, 0.27–0.78 | NA | NA | 0.57, 0.23–0.91 | |||||||||||||||
| ||||||||||||||||||||||
Ever Taken Swimming Lessons* | ||||||||||||||||||||||
| ||||||||||||||||||||||
% | Total (N=477) | Seeking to be Swim-Tested | Previously Swim Tested | Parent’s Comfort in Water Over Head | ||||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||||||
Yes (N=305) | No (N=175) | Yes (N=248) | No** (N=229) | Yes (N=418) | No (N=58) | |||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
Child Report | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | ||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
Parent Report | Yes | 84.5 | 2.7 | 87.2 | 86.4 | 3 | 89.4 | 81.1 | 2.3 | 83.4 | 86.7 | 2.4 | 89.1 | 82.1 | 3.1 | 85.2 | 84.5 | 2.9 | 87.3 | 86.3 | 1.7 | 88 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
No | 4 | 8.8 | 12.8 | 4 | 6.6 | 10.6 | 4 | 12.6 | 16.6 | 4.8 | 6.1 | 10.9 | 3.1 | 11.8 | 14.9 | 4.3 | 8.4 | 12.7 | 1.7 | 10.3 | 12 | |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
88.5 | 11.5 | 90.4 | 9.6 | 85.1 | 14.9 | 91.5 | 8.5 | 85.2 | 14.9 | 88.8 | 11.3 | 88 | 12 | |||||||||
S, p-val. | NS | NS | NS | NS | NS | NS | NS | |||||||||||||||
k, 95%CI§ | 0.69, 0.58–0.79 | 0.62, 0.47–0.77 | 0.76, 0.63–0.90 | 0.59, 0.41–0.76 | 0.76, 0.64–0.88 | 0.66, 0.55–0.77 | 0.84, 0.62–1.0 | |||||||||||||||
| ||||||||||||||||||||||
Perceived Good Swim Skills† | ||||||||||||||||||||||
| ||||||||||||||||||||||
% | Total (N=475) | Seeking to be Swim-Tested | Previously Swim Tested | Parent’s Comfort in Water Over Head | ||||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||||||
Yes (N=301) | No (N=174) | Yes (N=247) | No** (N=228) | Yes (N=414) | No (N=60) | |||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||||||||
Child Report | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | ||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
Parent Report | Yes | 43.2 | 13.1 | 56.3 | 56.8 | 15.6 | 72.4 | 19.5 | 8.6 | 28.1 | 53.4 | 14.6 | 68 | 32 | 11.4 | 43.4 | 44.4 | 13.5 | 57.9 | 35 | 10 | 45 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
No | 15.6 | 28.1 | 43.7 | 12.6 | 15 | 27.6 | 20.7 | 51.2 | 71.9 | 13 | 19 | 32 | 18.4 | 38.2 | 56.6 | 15 | 27.1 | 42.1 | 20 | 35 | 55 | |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
58.8 | 41.2 | 69.4 | 30.6 | 40.2 | 59.8 | 66.4 | 33.6 | 50.4 | 49.6 | 59.4 | 40.6 | 55 | 45 | |||||||||
S, p-val. | NS | NS | 8.65, 0.003 | NS | NS | NS | NS | |||||||||||||||
k, 95%CI§ | 0.41, 0.33–0.50 | 0.32, 0.20–0.43 | NA | 0.38, 0.25–0.50 | 0.40, 0.29–0.52 | 0.41, 0.32–0.50 | 0.41, 0.18–0.63 | |||||||||||||||
| ||||||||||||||||||||||
Comfort in Water Over Head‡ | ||||||||||||||||||||||
| ||||||||||||||||||||||
% | Total (N=479) | Seeking to be Swim-Tested | Previously Swim Tested | Parent’s Comfort in Water Over Head | ||||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||||||
Yes (N=304) | No (N=175) | Yes (N=251) | No** (N=228) | Yes (N=419) | No (N=59) | |||||||||||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||||||||
Child Report | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | Yes | No | ||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
Parent Report | Yes | 62.4 | 11.1 | 73.5 | 76.6 | 9.9 | 86.5 | 37.7 | 13.1 | 50.8 | 74.9 | 7.6 | 82.5 | 48.7 | 14.9 | 63.6 | 64 | 11.2 | 75.2 | 52.5 | 10.2 | 62.7 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
No | 14.2 | 12.3 | 26.5 | 9.2 | 4.3 | 13.5 | 22.9 | 26.3 | 49.2 | 10.7 | 6.8 | 17.5 | 18 | 18.4 | 36.4 | 12.7 | 12.2 | 24.9 | 23.7 | 13.6 | 37.3 | |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||||||||||||
76.6 | 23.4 | 85.8 | 14.2 | 60.6 | 39.4 | 85.6 | 14.4 | 66.7 | 33.3 | 76.7 | 23.4 | 76.2 | 23.8 | |||||||||
S, p-val. | NS | NS | 4.59, 0.03 | NS | NS | NS | NS | |||||||||||||||
k, 95%CI§ | 0.33, 0.23–0.42 | 0.20, 0.06–0.34 | NA | 0.32, 0.17–0.50 | 0.28, 0.15–0.41 | 0.35, 0.25–0.45 | 0.22, −0.03–0.47 |
“Seeking to be Swim-Tested” refers to dyads where the child voluntarily chose to take the swim-test on the data collection day and results were available for this study. “Previously Swim-Tested” refers to dyads where the child had been swim-tested previously during that same swim-season but prior to the study date. “Parent’s Comfort in Water Over Head” was self-reported by each parent, and serves as a proxy for parent’s swim comfort. Sample sizes vary due to missing data.
NA= Not applicable, as McNemar’s test (S) found statistical significance (p<.05) in differences in child/parent reports.
NS= Not statistically significant.
No= No/Not sure.
No= So-so, Not Good, or Can’t Swim.
Yes= Comfortable/Very Comfortable. No=Uncomfortable/Very Uncomfortable.
Agreement levels: Low (k < 0.4); Moderate (0.4 >= k >= 0.75); High (k > 0.75).
No= No/Unknown.
Knows how to swim
Most parents and children within dyads (85.9%) concurred in reporting the child “knows how to swim”. However, children were statistically (S=8.33, p=0.004) more likely to report knowing how to swim (92.9%) than their parents reporting their child knows how to swim (88.8%). Therefore, the “knows how to swim” measure was excluded from the remaining analyses.
Ever taken swim lessons
Most parents and children within dyads (84.5%) concurred in reporting the child had “ever taken swim lessons”. Differences between child/self (88.5%) and parent/guardian (87.2%) reports of that child ever taking swim lessons were non-significant. Agreement was high for dyads where the child was not seeking to be (k=0.76) or had not previously been (k=0.76) swim-tested, or when the parent/guardian was not comfortable in water over their head (k=0.84).
Perceived good swim skills
While 43.2% of parents and children concurred when reporting the child had “good swim skills”, only 28.1% concurred when reporting the child did not have “good swim skills”. Agreement in child and parent/guardian responses were moderate and low (k=0.32–0.41).
Comfort in water over head
Most dyads (62.4%) agreed in reporting whether the child was comfortable in water over his/her head. Discordant child and parent responses were not statistically significant. Agreement between child and parent/guardian responses was low (k=0.20–0.35).
Correlation between child/self and parental reports, and swim-test results
Among swim-tested dyads (N=305), passing the in-water swim-test positively correlated with child/self and parental reports of the child having “good swim skills” and feeling “comfortable in water over head”. Perceived “good swim skills” most strongly correlated with passing the in-water swim-test, especially if the parent/guardian was the informant (r=0.28) and the parent/guardian also reported not being comfortable in water over head (r=0.41). The “ever taken swim lessons” measure did not significantly correlate with the child passing the in-water test, regardless of the informant. Therefore, it was excluded from the remaining analyses (Table 4).
Table 4.
Correlation of child/self- and parental reports of child swim skill measures with the child passing the in-water swim-test, among the swim-tested dyads* (N=305)
Child Swim Skill Measure | Total Swim-Tested (N=305) | Children Seeking to be Swim-Tested, and… | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||||||
Previously Swim-Tested | Parent’s Comfort in Water Over Head | ||||||||||
|
|
||||||||||
Yes (n=188) | No † (n=117) | Yes (n=268) | No (n=37) | ||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||
Informant | Child | Parent | Child | Parent | Child | Parent | Child | Parent | Child | Parent | |
Ever Taken Swim Lessons | r | ||||||||||
p-value | NS | NS | NS | NS | NS | NS | NS | NS | NS | NS | |
N | |||||||||||
| |||||||||||
Perceived Good Swim Skills | r | 0.2 | 0.28 | 0.15 | 0.25 | 0.24 | 0.27 | 0.19 | 0.26 | NS | 0.41 |
p-value | 0.001 | <.0001 | 0.04 | 0.001 | 0.01 | 0.004 | 0.002 | <.0001 | 0.01 | ||
N | 301 | 305 | 184 | 188 | 117 | 117 | 264 | 268 | 37 | ||
| |||||||||||
Comfort in Water Over Head | r | 0.17 | 0.13 | NS | 0.19 | 0.19 | NS | 0.16 | 0.15 | NS | NS |
p-value | 0.003 | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.05 | 0.009 | 0.01 | |||||
N | 304 | 305 | 188 | 11 | 267 | 268 |
Excludes the “knows how to swim” measure, given significant discordance between child and parent responses (see Table 3). “Previously Swim-Tested” refers to dyads where the child had been swim-tested previously during that same swim-season but prior to the study date. “Parent’s self-report of “Parent Comfort in Water Over Head” serves as a proxy for parent’s swim comfort.
No=No or unknown.
NS=Not statistically significant.
Paired child/self and parental reports, and swim-test results
Child/self and parental reports of a child having “good swim skills” or feeling “comfortable in water over head” were not significantly correlated among dyads where the child failed the swim-test. If the child passed the in-water swim-test, correlations were stronger for the “good swim skills” (r=0.25–0.47) measure than the “comfortable in water over head” (r=0.17–0.21) measure (Table 5).
Table 5.
Correlation of paired child/self and parental reports of child swim skill measures with swim-test results, among the swim-tested dyads* (N=305)
Child Swim Skill Measures | Total Swim-Tested (N=305) | Previously Swim-Tested | Parent’s Comfort in WaterOver Head | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|
||||||||||
Yes (n=188) | No † (n=117) | Yes (n=268) | No (n=37) | ||||||||
| |||||||||||
Swim-test result | Pass | Fail | Pass | Fail | Pass | Fail | Pass | Fail | Pass | Fail | |
| |||||||||||
Perceived Good Swim Skills | r | 0.29 | NS | 0.25 | NS | 0.35 | NS | 0.26 | NS | 0.47 | NS |
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||
p-value | <.0001 | 0.001 | 0.0007 | <.0001 | 0.008 | ||||||
|
|
|
|
|
|
||||||
N | 255 | 165 | 90 | 224 | 31 | ||||||
|
|
||||||||||
Comfort in Water Over Head | r | 0.17 | NS | 0.21 | NS | NS | NS | 0.18 | NS | NS | NS |
|
|
|
|
||||||||
p-value | 0.006 | 0.006 | 0.006 | ||||||||
|
|
|
|
||||||||
N | 258 | 169 | 227 |
Excludes the “knows how to swim” measure (significant discordant in child and parent responses for the full sample and by sub-groups were found), and the “ever taken swim lessons” measure (independent child and parent reports were not significantly correlated with the child passing the in-water swim test). “Previously Swim-Tested” refers to dyads where the child had been swim-tested previously during that same swim-season but prior to the study date. Parents’ self-report of “Parent’s Comfort in Water Over Head” serves as a proxy for parent’s swim comfort.
No=No or Unknown.
NS=Not statistically significant.
DISCUSSION
Knowing how to swim is an important strategy to reduce unintentional drowning. Children could gain this skill from community-based learn to swim programs, private lessons, at some schools or other locations. Surveillance data on swim skill is necessary to monitor this protective factor and potentially guide efforts to reduce unintentional drowning. However, testing children’s swim skills in water is not always feasible.
This study examined the validity of four survey swim skill measures that are commonly used in surveillance, yet never validated. Findings indicate agreement within participating child/parent dyad’s reports on 3 of the 4 evaluated child swim skill measures (i.e., “ever taken swim lessons”, “perceived good swim skills”, and “comfortable in water over head”).
Most studies show that swim lessons result in improved swim skills, yet developing skills takes time and repetition.[9] While showing greatest agreement within dyads, neither child/self- or parental-reports of the “ever taken swim lessons” measure correlated with passing the in-water swim-test. The high concurrence in parental- and child-self reports of “ever taken swim lessons” could be a reflection of this study’s convenience sample of children whose parents were physically present at the pool, thus showing active engagement in their children’s water recreational activities. Contrary to the assumption that children who took swim lessons have learned to swim, our findings suggest that assessing a child’s history of ever taking swim lessons (i.e., “Have you/your child ever taken swim lessons?”) without assessing the length or results of such training is not a useful measure to assess a child’s ability to pass a swim test within this population.
Correlation analyses which simultaneously considered the three data sources for this study (i.e., child/self- and parental reports of a child’s swim skill, and the child’s in-water swim-test results), suggest that reports of feeling “comfortable in water over head” and perceived “good swim skills” are the best measures to assess a child’s swim skill among participants. While correlations were overall low, it was highest when the parent is at least one of the informants for the perceived “good swim skills” measure, among dyads where the child passed the swim-test and the parent reported feeling uncomfortable in water over head. Among this population group (i.e., mostly non-Hispanic White, high income, highly educated families choosing to attend a public pool), parents feeling uncomfortable in water over head does not necessarily result in children not learning how to swim.
Study significance
Unlike previous studies, this non-generalizable, convenience sample study benefits from the collection of data on children’s swim skill via multiple methods: in-water swim-test results, child/self- and parental reports. Previous studies have found moderate or weak correlations between young adult’s self-perceived and in-water swim skills.[15 16] To our knowledge, this is the first study in the U.S. that compares children’s self/proxy-reported data with results from a basic in-water swim-test to validate swim skill surveillance measures. Valid population swim skill data could identify vulnerable populations, inform the development of drowning prevention and learn to swim policies and programs, and help organizations that promote swimming and learning to swim to evaluate their programs’ reach and effectiveness.
Swim-test criteria
Various skills have been suggested to establish water competency.[17] Based on an international survey of recreational aquatic organizations, the American Red Cross defined swim competency as having skills in entering, submersion and surfacing, propulsion, turning, floating/treading, and exiting from water.[18] Contrastingly, Dixon et al. identified 7 swim competency domains, based on young adults’ perceptions (i.e., not panicking; instinctive, basic, advanced, and rescue skills; covering a distance; ability to swim in multiple settings).[19] It is generally agreed that swimming involves propelling oneself some distance without aid, yet research has not yet defined what is the minimal distance and skills necessary to prevent drowning. Some validated programs outside of the U.S. suggest 25 meters and some specific skills to reduce the risk of drowning. [7 20]
Similarly, we observed variability in the pool’s minimum swim-test passing criteria. While all pools required children to traverse ≥25 meters (82 feet), the specific distance differed based on each pool’s dimensions. Parents and children advised that lifeguards’ enforcement of the pools’ objective swim-test passing criteria also varied, potentially affecting this study’s findings.
Establishing consistent criteria for day-to-day operations across different sites is challenging. Nonetheless, future studies could benefit from consistent swim-test criteria across water venues.
Limitations
Pool administrators confirmed that our study participants were representative of the patrons at these three specific pools (e.g., mostly non-Hispanic White, high income, highly educated). Yet, they were not representative of Washington’s diversity. Findings cannot be generalized to all of WA, all population groups (especially racial/ethnic minorities, and non-English- or Spanish-speakers), the US, or other geographical or cultural contexts. Additionally, older children were under-represented, primarily because their parents/guardians were not present; the sample is skewed towards younger children (median: 9 years). Sampling limitations impeded reliable analyses by socio-demographic characteristics.
Participants were a convenience sample of pool-attending families, whose children likely learned how to swim at community pools (63% of those who had ever taken swim lessons) and whose parents were actively engaging with them in water recreational venues. Children who voluntarily chose to be swim-tested could have had more opportunities to obtain swim skills, knowledge, and confidence to pass the swim-test than the general population. In fact, the child’s lack of swim skill or confidence, and/or fear of failing were frequently mentioned as reasons for not taking the test. This sample bias, and the fact that passing a swim-test does not confer total protection against drowning, [21] must be considered. Furthermore, passing an in-water swim test at a controlled environment (i.e., pool) does not necessarily imply the child possesses the water competency required in open water or other natural water venues (e.g., beach, rivers).
Conclusion
This convenience-based sample, pilot study serves as a first step in the validation of survey child swim skill measures commonly used in public health surveillance. Parent’s perception of his/her child having “good swim skills” was the measure that most correlated with observed swim skill among participants.
Findings are limited to the population commonly served by the participating pool venues in this study – mostly non-Hispanic White, highly educated, high income families with active parental participation in children’s swimming activities. Future studies need to evaluate the validity of swim skill measures among diverse populations, which could be reached at open water and free-admission venues. Additionally, strategic collaborations with after school programs, schools, and summer camps could increase the participation of older children and those who attend public water venues without a parent present. Findings can help pool operators develop consistency in swim-test criteria and administration, which is important for both safety and research purposes. Additionally, it is recommended that future studies incorporate verbal or pictographic descriptions of what it means to know how to swim to ensure respondents have a clear understanding on what it means to know how to swim prior to answering swim skill surveillance questions.
Acknowledgments
We thank Dr. Kathy Lofy - State Health Officer, and Kathy Williams MS - Trauma Prevention Specialist at the Washington State Department of Health; and Mr. Tony Gomez – Project/Program Manager at the King County Department of Public Health for inviting us to contribute to their children’s drowning prevention efforts through this study. We acknowledge the data collection and logistical support provided by Seattle Children’s Hospital staff Celeste Chung MPH MSW, Carolyn Vasquez, and Maryam Jafari. Additionally, we thank Tony Trofimczuk at Snohomish Department of Parks and Recreation; and Diane Jones, Kerrie Stoops, and Wendy Van De Sompele at Seattle Department of Parks & Recreation for allowing us to conduct this study at their pools and among their patrons. We thank the pool’s lifeguards and staff, and our team of volunteer data collectors (Patti Brandt; Dr. Gerri Hashisaki; Shane Hoffman; Ray Meuse; Jamal Robinson; Jake Woodward; Sue Yates) for their support.
Funding This study was partially supported by the National Swimming Pool Foundation.
Footnotes
Competing interests None.
Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
Disclaimer The findings and conclusions in this report are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. This study was conducted at the invitation of the Washington State Health Department and in collaboration with Seattle Children’s Hospital as part of an “EPI-AID Field Investigation”, which by its nature was time and resource limited.
Contributorship MCMC: Co-principal investigator. Conception, design, data collection, analysis, interpretation of findings, and manuscript writing. LQ: Co-principal investigator. Conception, background, design, data collection, interpretation of findings and manuscript review. EB/JG: Conception, design, data collection, interpretation of findings, and manuscript review. BAL: Data collection, analysis and manuscript review. CW/KW: Data collection and manuscript review. MAG: Project coordinator. Background, data collection, and manuscript review. MRS: Data analysis support and manuscript review. RL: Conception, design, analysis, interpretation of findings, and manuscript review.
References
- 1.National Vital Statistics System (NVSS). 10 Leading Causes of Unintentional Injury Deaths, United States, 2012, All Races, Both Sexes, Ages 1–17. Secondary 10 Leading Causes of Unintentional Injury Deaths, United States, 2012, All Races, Both Sexes, Ages 1–17 February 19, 2013 2014. http://www.cdc.gov/injury/wisqars/fatal.html.
- 2.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and National Center for Health Statistics (CDC NCHS) Underlying Cause of Death 1999–2010, Multiple Cause of Death Files, 1999–2010. Secondary Underlying Cause of Death 1999–2010, Multiple Cause of Death Files, 1999–2010 2014 2012. http://wonder.cdc.gov/ucd-icd10.html.
- 3.Laosee O, Gilchrist J, Rudd R. Drowning – United States, 2005–2009. MMWR. 2012;61(19):344–47. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Cummings P, Quan L. Trends in Unintentional Drowning: The Role of Alcohol and Medical Care. JAMA. 1999;281(23):2198–202. doi: 10.1001/jama.281.23.2198. [published Online First: Epub Date]|. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Spack L, Gedeit R, Splaingrard M, Havens P. Failure of aggressive therapy to alter outcomes in pediatric near-drowning. Pediatric Emergency Care. 1997;13(2):98–102. doi: 10.1097/00006565-199704000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Brenner R, Taneja G, Haynie D, et al. Association between swimming lessons and drowning in childhood: a case-control study. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 2009;163(3):203–10. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2008.563. [published Online First: Epub Date]|. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Rahman F, Bose S, Linnan M, et al. Cost-effectiveness of an injury and drowning prevention program in Bangladesh. Pediatrics. 2012;130(6):e1621–28. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-0757. [published Online First: Epub Date]|. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Yang L, Nong Q, Li C, Feng Q, Lo S. Risk factors for childhood drowning in rural regions of a developing country: a case-control study. Injury Prevention. 2007;13(3):178–82. doi: 10.1136/ip.2006.013409. [published Online First: Epub Date]|. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Peden M, Oyegbite K, Ozanne-Smith J, et al. World Report on Child Injury Prevention. Geneva, Switzerland: UNICEF, World Health Organization; 2008. pp. 59–77. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.International Lifesaving Federation (ILS) Position Statement: Swimming and Water Safety Education. Leuven, Belgium: ILS Education Committee, ILS Lifesaving Commission; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Moran K, Quan L, Franklin R, Bennett E. Where the Evidence and Expert Opinion Meet: A Review of Open-Water Recreational Safety Messages. International Journal of Aquatic Research and Education. 2011;5(3):251–70. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Weiss J The Committee on Injury VaPP. Technical Report – Prevention of Drowning. Pediatrics. 2010;126(1):e1–e10. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1265. [published Online First: Epub Date]|. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.World Health Organization. Global Report on Drowning: Preventing a Leading Killer. Geneva, Switzerland: 2014. p. 25. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Landis J, Koch G. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977;33(1):159–74. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kjendlie P-L, Stallman RK, Cabri J, editors. Real and perceived swimming competency, risk estimation, and preventing drowning among New Zealand youth. Biomechanics & Medicine in Swimming XI (Proceedings of the XIth International Symposium for Biomechanics and Medicine in Swimming); 2010 June 16–19; Oslo, Norway. Norweigan School of Sport Science; [Google Scholar]
- 16.Petrass L, Blitvich J, McElroy G, Harvey J, Moran K. Can you swim? self-report and actual swimming competence among young adults in Ballarat, Australia. International Journal of Aquatic Research & Education. 2012;6(2):136–48. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Stallman R, Junge M, Blixt T. The Teaching of Swimming Based on a Model Derived from the Causes of Drowning. International Journal of Aquatic Research and Education. 2011;2(4):372–82. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Quan L, Ramos W, Harvey C, et al. Toward Defining Water Competency: An American Red Cross Definition. International Journal of Aquatic Research & Education. 2015;9(1):12–23. doi: 10.1123/ijare.2014-0066. [published Online First: Epub Date]|. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Dixon H, Bixler R. Failure to learn to (really) swim: Inflated self-efficacy? Recreational Sports Journal. 2007;3(1):14–20. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Stallman R, Moran K, Brenner R, Rahman A. Swimming and Water Survival Competence (Chapter 30) In: Bierens JJL, editor. Drowning: Prevention, Rescue, Treatment. 2. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Olivera L, Aranha A, Resende R, Cardoso E, Pimenta N, Garrido N. Can we test swimming lessons skills in drowning prevention? British Journal of Sports Medicine. 2013;47(10):e3. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2013-092558.18. published Online First: Epub Date. [DOI] [Google Scholar]