Abstract
Background
Neighborhood food cues have been inconsistently related to residents’ health, possibly due to variations in residents’ sensitivity to such cues. This study sought to investigate the degree to which children’s predisposition to eat upon exposure to food environment and food cues (external eating), could explain differences in strength of associations between their food consumption and the type of food outlets and marketing strategies present in their neighborhood.
Methods
Data were obtained from 616 6–12 y.o. children recruited into a population-based cross-sectional study in which food consumption was measured through a 24-h food recall and responsiveness to food cues measured using the external eating scale. The proportion of food retailers within 3 km of residence considered as “healthful” was calculated using a Geographical Information System. Neighborhood exposure to food marketing strategies (displays, discount frequency, variety, and price) for vegetables and soft drinks were derived from a geocoded digital marketing database. Adjusted mixed models with spatial covariance tested interaction effects of food environment indicators and external eating on food consumption.
Results
In children with higher external eating scores, healthful food consumption was more positively related to vegetable displays, and more negatively to the display and variety of soft drinks. No interactions were observed for unhealthful food consumption and no main effects of food environment indicators were found on food consumption.
Conclusions
Children differ in their responsiveness to marketing-related visual food cues on the basis of their external eating phenotype. Strategies aiming to increase the promotion of healthful relative to unhealthful food products in stores may be particularly beneficial for children identified as being more responsive to food cues.
Electronic supplementary material
The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/s12966-017-0540-9) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Keywords: Food environment, Food marketing, External eating, Children, Food consumption
Background
The ubiquitous presence of calorie-dense food and food cues in urban environments, be it in the absolute or in relation to healthier options, has been repeatedly blamed for the rising trends in overconsumption and associated obesity over the last few decades [1, 2]. For instances, within- and cross-country analyses have linked increases in the share of the population diet taken up by modern processed food typically rich in calories, fats and sugars with increases in obesity prevalence [3–5]. Beyond food per se, observational studies have linked changes in marketing practices, such as food advertising [6–8] and prices [9], to an obesogenic shift in consumption patterns. Other practices such as product packaging and portion sizes have similarly been associated with unhealthful diets [10–12].
Further evidence for the relationship between caloric excess/obesity and environmental exposure conditions comes from studies assessing the absolute or relative availability of food access points typically considered as unhealthful (e.g., fast food restaurants, convenience store) or healthful (e.g., supermarket, fresh food outlets) [13]. These studies tend to use geo-referenced store locations to determine the availability of food stores, most commonly within one’s neighborhood, be it defined using administrative units like census tracts, or unique geographical areas around the individual’s residence (or other reference points) using a pre-defined distance (e.g. 1-km or 1-mile radius). These studies have provided some evidence for associations between availability of fast-food stores or supermarkets and dietary and weight outcomes; however, null and unexpected findings are also common, especially in children (for reviews, see [14–18]). Most of these studies rely on the assumption that the presence of certain types of food outlets (e.g. supermarkets or fast food restaurants) is a reasonable proxy for availability of predominantly healthful or unhealthful food. It is however important to acknowledge that variation may exist in the type of food available within similar types of food outlets. For instance, some supermarkets may carry as much, if not more unhealthful food products, compared to healthful alternatives. A few studies have captured attributes of food products sold within outlets that are expected to influence purchase behaviors, such as shelf space, displays, price or promotions, and their associations with dietary and weight outcomes [19–21], which may provide a more accurate and comprehensive picture of neighborhood food cues than access-only models. These studies have mostly relied on store audits, with few capitalizing on the rich information available from digital marketing databases, which can provide a thorough, dynamic and comprehensive assessment of local food environment cues [22]. Only a minority of these studies have been conducted in children, with some evidence that children’s food consumption is related to fast-food and fruit and vegetable prices [23, 24]. Food marketing research has been mostly focused on television advertising with calls for more research on the impact of non-television food marketing [25], including sales promotions of energy-dense nutrition poor foods [26].
It is possible that the inconsistent findings on the relationship between neighborhood food environment and obesity at the population level are tied to the lack of consideration to putative individual differences that exist in one’s responsiveness to external exposure to food and food cues. Some evidence for individual differences in food responsiveness can be found in the neuroscience literature, especially from studies related to the dopamine pathways, which are thought to be key in guiding eating and other motivated behaviors in response to prevailing internal states and external cues, such as food pictures [27] and food advertisement [28, 29]. For instance, when comparing obese to lean adults or children, research demonstrated heightened response to food cues in “hedonic” regions receiving strong dopaminergic projections, such as the striatum, insula, amygdala, hippocampus and the orbitofrontal cortex, as well as altered response (often heightened) in prefrontal and cingulate cortices [30, 31], and altered feeding-related modulation of hedonic and executive region responses [32]. Lean youth at risk for future obesity by virtue of parental obesity have shown hyper-responsivity of reward regions to palatable food receipts [33]. Weight gains have been associated with hyper-responsivity to food intake [34], food images [35], and food commercials [36]. Laboratory studies examining eating behavior in children in response to controlled food environments (snack test) have also found similar results using either genetic markers or neurocognitive tasks related to differential dopamine signaling [37, 38]. Finally, a population-based observational study reported that a positive association between fast food consumption and neighborhood exposure to fast-food restaurants was only observed in adult participants with a high behavioral approach response [39], as measured by the behavioral approach system scale (BAS; [40]), a psychological scale reflecting one’s predisposition to incentives and appetitive stimuli, which has been linked to neural responses to food cues [41].
In this paper, we attempt to better characterize, in a population-based sample, children who may be more responsive to both unhealthful and healthful environmental food cues. A number of self-reported food-related trait measures have been developed over the years (for a review see [42]), many of which reflect the concept of uncontrolled eating [43]. In the present study, we examine external eating [44] as a food-specific index for individual difference in neurobehavioral environment responsiveness. External eating is a psychological measure that reflects one’s predisposition to eat upon exposure to environment food and food cues. This food-specific index of environment responsiveness has been associated with greater attentional bias and more positive evaluation of food cues [45], food cravings [46], and greater food consumption in response to food (vs neutral) commercials [47].
The aim of the study was to determine the degree to which external eating modifies the associations between measures of the food environment and food behavior in children, examining its effect on the consumption of both unhealthful food (i.e., nutrient poor, high fat/sugar/cal food) and healthful alternatives. It is hypothesized that healthful and unhealthful environmental cues related to the type of food outlets available within neighborhoods and to marketing strategies used within local stores (promotional activities and variety) would be more strongly associated with consumption in children with a higher sensitivity to external food cues.
Methods
Overview
Data for this cross-sectional study were obtained from a larger study (the “Brain-to-Society” study) which is examining multi-level (neighborhood, household, individual) drivers of childhood obesity. Parallel data collections targeting families with children age 6–12 were undertaken in Canada and India. This study focuses on the Canadian sample, which consisted of a convenience sample of households from the Montreal Metropolitan area. Recruitment and data collection was undertaken by an independent research marketing firm on behalf of the research team. Participants were randomly selected from a pre-established database of households identified as likely to have children in the target age group, and who had indicated their willingness to participate in academic research. This frame was provided by the research marketing firm.
A total of 4947 potential households were initially contacted by telephone and asked about their willingness to complete a survey about children’s eating and lifestyle habits. Of those, 813 were non-residential records and 633 were not eligible. A further 2352 had unknown eligibility. Of those eligible, 69 only partially completed the interview, 355 refused, 99 could not be recontacted, 10 were excluded due to gender quota and 616 completed the interviews for an estimated response rate of 23%, which accounts for a proportion of the unknown eligibility group that may be eligible. Additional information on sample and recruitment can be found in Electronic Additional file 1.
After confirming whether any child aged between 6 and 12 years old lived in the household, interviewers asked to speak with a parent/guardian with the best knowledge of the child’s daily habits, who was then asked to answer a series of questions regarding their child. When more than one eligible child lived in the household, the child with the next birthday was selected. Interviews were conducted between March and August 2013 and had a duration of 50 min. Participants received a CAD$10 incentive for their participation. Verbal consent was obtained from all participants included in the study, and ethical approval was obtained from McGill University’s Institutional Research Board.
Measures
Food consumption was measured using a selective 24-h food recall in which parents/guardians were asked to identify the number of times their child consumed selected food items the day prior to the interview, from the time the child woke up to bed time. A selective 24-h recall was chosen to provide a time-efficient way to obtain an initial assessment of dietary intake during the phone interview, during which a range of measures was administered. A more complete 24-h recall was used in a follow up data collection on a reduced sample. The food items consisted of 15 food items and 12 drink options. Items were selected for their representativeness of children’s healthful and unhealthful dietary intake [48–50] in addition to cover food categories commonly targeted by health promotion and marketing activities and are provided in Electronic Additional file 2.
Two intake variables were created to capture the child’s consumption of healthful foods and unhealthful foods based on items expected to be sold in food stores. Healthful food consumption was based on the sum of the reported frequency of consumption of (1) whole grain foods, (2) fruits, (3) green vegetables, (4) other vegetables, and (5) water. Unhealthful food consumption was based on the sum of the reported frequency of consumption of (1) soft drinks, (2) salty snacks (e.g. chips, nachos, buttered popcorn), (3) candy package or chocolate bar, (4) cake, pie, cookies, doughnut, brownie, or other baked sweets, and (5) ice cream, ice cream bar, frozen yogurt, or popsicle. The two intake variables were compared to nutrient intake values computed from the more extensive 24-h dietary recall collected from a subset of the sample (n = 276). The healthful food consumption variable was correlated with fiber (r = 0.15, p = 0.01) and water (r = 0.12, p = 0.04) intake and the unhealthful food consumption variable was correlated with sucrose (r = 0.17, p = 0.006), carbohydrate (r = 0.12, p = 0.04), and fatty acid (r = 0.15, p = 0.01).
External eating was assessed using items from the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire [44] as reported by parents. The ten items were re-worded for parents to report on their child’s behavior and adapted to fit the cultural context and improve suitability for children. For instance, the term delicious was replaced for yummy and food sources like cafes replaced for corner stores (‘dépanneur’). The number of response options were also changed from five to three consistent with previous adaptations of the scale for children [51]. Internal consistency of this scale for the sample, assessed following the approach recommended by Gadermann and colleagues [52] for estimating reliability coefficients for ordinal data, was found to be within the acceptable range, although not excellent (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.77).
Food environment
In order to provide a more comprehensive assessment of neighborhood food cues and consistent with calls for multi-dimensional measures of the neighborhood food environment that not only include access but also in-store food measures [53], two types of food environment measures were used in this study: the type of food retailers locally available and the marketing strategies used within local outlets. The types of food outlets locally available were measured using the modified Retail Food Environment Index (mRFEI), an index representing the proportion of food retailers classified as healthful developed by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) and used to standardize reports on community food environment [54]. The choice of a retail food environment index over availability of specific types of food stores is supported by evidence that combined indices are more robustly related to weight-related outcomes than individual food store availability measures [15]. Food outlets in the Montreal Census Metropolitan Area were identified using the Expanded Points of Interest (DMTI) database (2013). Field validation of this database has shown acceptable “representativity” of food outlets in the same city as the present study [55]. As per the CDC definition, supermarkets, grocery stores, fruit and vegetable stores, and supercenters were classified as “healthful”, and convenience stores and fast-food restaurants as “less healthful”. More information on the identification of food retailers can be found in Electronic Additional file 3. Food outlets and their locations were integrated into a Geographic Information System to identify all food outlets located within 3 km of each participant’s residence. The number of outlets was expressed using kernel density estimates (KDE; a distance-decay function weighing businesses closer to participants’ residence more heavily than those farther away) based on a 3-km circular buffer. A 3-km radius was selected based on results from a pilot analysis showing that over 99% of cohort home locations had one or more food outlets within this distance. The mRFEI was then calculated by dividing KDE value for “healthful” food outlets by the sum of the KDE values for “healthful” and “less healthful” food outlets. This proportion was then multiplied by 100 to obtain a percentage of outlet density categorized as healthful.
Food marketing strategies
It is well known from the consumer behavior literature (for a review [56]) that business practices grouped under the umbrella of the four “P’s” of marketing (Product/Place/Price/Promotion) impact behavior in general, including food consumption. While the above mRFEI measure taps into the type and location of products at the community level, it fails to address price and promotion strategies as well as the range of products available. We therefore investigated two types of promotional activity (frequency of price-promotion and food displays), regular price, and variety of food products available within selected healthful and unhealthful food categories. Promotional activities and variety were expected to act as visual food-related environmental cues. Regular price was included as a strategy that would not necessarily be expected to act as visual food cue and was included as control strategy.
These measures were derived from a digital marketing database and have been described elsewhere [22]. In short, data for every single Universal Product Code (UPC) within target categories sold within stores located in the province of Québec, Canada were obtained from data on weekly purchases and marketing activities related to consumer packaged goods in stores (ScanTrack) purchased from the Nielsen Corporation for the period of 2008–2013. Retailers consisted of grocery stores, mass merchandisers and convenience stores. Their locations were represented using forward sortation areas (FSA), which are geographic regions defined by the first three digits of Canadian postal codes, and linked to participants based on their residential postal code. For each UPC, the following information was available: weekly price and in-store promotion, item description, brand name, package size, and the number of individual items within the pack. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) data on average serving size and unit package size were used to derive number of servings per unit.
Regular price, frequency of price promotions, variety and food display indicators were selected for the following product categories: regular soft drinks and vegetables. Both categories were selected due to their established links with weight, obesity and cardio-metabolic diseases [57–61] and the considerable focus of public health policies and interventions on these food categories. Regular price per serving was derived from the weighted average of the highest price of each UPC within the category over a three-month moving window [62]. Frequency of price promotion (discounts) category was obtained from the weighted average number of weeks in which the price of a given UPC was at least two standard deviations below its average price [63]. In both cases, overall market shares of each UPC in the entire period (2008–2013) within their category were used as weights. Non-price promotion (food displays) was operationalized as the proportion of food products (UPCs) within a target food category that was on display (e.g. end of aisles displays) in a given week in a given store. Variety was operationalized as the average number of different Stock Keeping Unit (SKU; a number that uniquely identifies distinct products (and package sizes)) within product categories weighted by stores monthly sales. Marketing indicators data collected during the 12 months preceding participants’ recruitment were averaged and used as a measure of marketing exposures in analyses.
Analyses
Generalized linear mixed models were used to model, separately, children’s healthful or unhealthful food consumption as a function of food environment measures, external eating and their interactions. Models were estimated with exponential spatial covariance structure, where the covariance between two observations depended on the distance between the two observations. Linear models were used for healthful food consumption, whose distribution was approximately normal and Poisson models were used for unhealthful food, whose distribution was positively skewed. Food environment measures were modelled separately to maximize sample size for each food environment measure and to avoid multiple interaction terms in the same model. External eating and environmental variables were standardized prior to analyses. All analyses accounted for socio-demographic characteristics such as children’s age, gender, and household income (0-45 K,45-65 K, and >65 K CAD), and language in which the interview was done (French/English, proxy for cultural background). Area-level median household income and employment rates were initially considered as covariates but not included due to lack of associations with outcome variables. Statistical significance was set at alpha = 0.05, but interactions terms with level of significance up to alpha = 0.10 are also discussed considering the recognized lower statistical power of interaction testing [64], especially in non-experimental research [65].
Different sample sizes were available for the different exposure variables, with marketing indicators being available for fewer participants compared to the relative availability of food outlet indicator. In order to capitalize on all information available, each analysis was conducted using the maximum sample size available. In order to minimize the chance of bias due to missing marketing data, the odds of participants having a missing marketing indicator was modelled in relation to behavioral outcomes and all environment-unrelated predictors, population density (2011 Census Data) and socio-economic characteristics of the Census Tract of residence (% high school completion, median household income, prevalence of low income, % immigrants, employment rate (2006 Census Data (not available from 2011 Census)). Statistically significant predictors of missing marketing data (employment rate, median household income, and participant’s gender for soft drink indicators, and % high school completion and population density for vegetable marketing indicators) were used to obtain probability of missing marketing data and the inverse of this probability was used as weight in their respective analyses (inverse probability weighting).
Results
Descriptive statistics
Of the 616 children recruited in the study, two had missing food consumption information and 30 had missing household income information. Eight participants had a missing mRFEI, which resulted in a sample size of 576 children. Marketing data on regular soft drinks were available for 465 participants whereas vegetable data were available for 257 participants. The lower numbers of observations for vegetables were due to absence of sales from this food category in stores sampled one year prior to recruitment. Socio-demographic profiles of the analytic samples for the mRFEI, soft drink marketing, and vegetable marketing indicators are provided in Table 1 along with descriptive statistics on outcome and exposure variables. Comparisons of soft-drink marketing variables across the two marketing indicator analytic samples suggested that the vegetable marketing sample had lower soft drink prices, greater number of displays and variety, and more frequent discounts suggesting a possible greater representation of larger stores (e.g. supermarkets) compared to smaller stores. No differences in socio-demographic or behavioral profile were found across the three analytical samples.
Table 1.
mRFEI analysis | Marketing strategy analyses | ||
---|---|---|---|
(n = 576) | Soft drinks (n = 465) | Vegetables (n = 257) | |
Age (mean(SD)) | 9.1 (1.7) | 9.1 (1.7) | 9.0 (1.6) |
Gender (n(%) boys) | 284 (49.3%) | 217 (46.7%) | 109 (42.2%) |
Household Income (n(%)) | |||
< 45 K | 111 (19.3%) | 96 (20.6%) | 48 (18.6%) |
45–65 K | 114 (19.8%) | 94 (20.2%) | 45 (17.4%) |
> 65 K | 351 (60.9%) | 275 (59.1%) | 165 (64.0%) |
Language survey conducted (n (%) French) | 320 (55.6%) | 256 (55.1%) | 149 (57.7%) |
Healthful eating score (mean (SD)) | 8.6 (3.6) | 8.7 (3.6) | 8.7 (3.6) |
Unhealthful food score (mean (SD)) | 1.6 (1.2) | 1.6 (1.2) | 1.6 (1.3) |
External eating score (Range: 10–30; mean (SD)) | 21 (4) | 21 (4) | 21 (4) |
mRFEI (mean (SD)) | 23.3 (11.7) | 22.4 (10.5) | 22.4 (10.7) |
Soft drinks discount frequency (mean (SD)) | 0.41 (0.19)a | 0.50 (0.15) a | |
Soft drinks display (mean (SD)) | 0.26 (0.22) a | 0.38 (0.17) a | |
Soft drinks regular price per serving (mean (SD)) | 0.37 (0.12) a | 0.30 (0.04) a | |
Soft drinks variety (mean (SD)) | 128.2 (60.3) a | 171.8 (44.7) a | |
Vegetable discount frequency (mean (SD)) | 0.47 (0.13) | ||
Vegetable display (mean (SD)) | 0.05 (0.04) | ||
Vegetable regular price per serving (mean (SD)) | 0.31 (0.08) | ||
Vegetable variety (mean (SD)) | 410.5 (116.7) |
mRFEI modified Retail Food Environment Index (proportion of food retailers classified as healthful), SD Standard Deviation
a mean difference statistically significant (p < 0.05)
Relative availability of healthful food outlets
Results related to the availability of healthful food outlets are reported in Table 2. No main or interactive effects of the food environment was observed, but a statistically significant positive association was found between external eating and unhealthful food consumption.
Table 2.
Predictors | Healthful food consumption | Unhealthful food consumption | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Estimate | 95% CI | P | RR | 95% CI | P | |
External eating score (1 SD) | −0.18 | (−0.49, 0.12) | 0.23 | 1.09 | (1.03, 1.16) | 0.004 |
mRFEI (1 SD) | −0.05 | (−0.35, 0.25) | 0.75 | 0.98 | (0.92, 1.04) | 0.49 |
mRFEI x external eating score | 0.24 | (−0.06, 0.54) | 0.11 | 1.00 | (0.94, 1.06) | 0.87 |
Results of regression models of healthful and unhealthful food consumption predicted by external eating status, mRFEI and their interactions, adjusted for child age, gender, language household income, and population density (n = 576), RR relative risk, CI Confidence Interval, mRFEI modified Retail Food Environment Index (proportion of food retailers classified as healthful), SD Standard Deviation
Marketing food strategies
Results of analyses on the discount frequency, display, regular price, and variety of regular soft drinks are presented in Table 3. Similar to results related to the relative availability of healthful food outlets, no main effects of food environment indicators were found, but external eating was found to be positively related to unhealthful food consumption. No interaction effects between food environment indicators and external eating reached the 0.05 statistical significance level. Marginally significant interactions were, however, observed for soft drink display and variety in relation to healthful food consumption, with both interaction terms being negative suggesting that associations between these soft drink marketing indicators and healthful food consumption may be more negative for participants with higher external eating scores.
Table 3.
Healthful food consumption | Unhealthful food consumption | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Estimate | 95%CI | P | RR | 95%CI | P | |
External eating (1 SD) | −0.25 | (−0.58, 0.07) | 0.14 | 1.10 | (1.03, 1.18) | 0.005 |
Soft drink Discount Frequency (1 SD) | 0.13 | (−0.21, 0.47) | 0.45 | 1.01 | (0.94, 1.08) | 0.87 |
External eating x Discount frequency | −0.22 | (−0.54, 0.11) | 0.20 | 0.98 | (0.92, 1.05) | 0.57 |
External eating (1 SD) | −0.26 | (−0.59, 0.06) | 0.11 | 1.10 | (1.03, 1.18) | 0.007 |
Soft drink Display (1 SD) | 0.12 | (−0.23, 0.48) | 0.49 | 1.00 | (0.92, 1.07) | 0.91 |
External Eating x Display | −0.33 | (−0.67, 0.01) | 0.057 | 0.97 | (0.90, 1.04) | 0.39 |
External eating (1 SD) | −0.24 | (−0.57, 0.09) | 0.15 | 1.10 | (1.03, 1.18) | 0.005 |
Soft drink Regular price (1 SD) | 0.04 | (−0.32, 0.39) | 0.84 | 0.97 | (0.90, 1.05) | 0.49 |
External Eating x Regular Price | 0.07 | (−0.24, 0.40) | 0.65 | 1.01 | (0.94, 1.08) | 0.85 |
External eating (1 SD) | −0.25 | (−0.58, 0.07) | 0.09 | 1.10 | (1.03, 1.18) | 0.007 |
Variety (1 SD) | −0.01 | (−0.36, 0.37) | 0.97 | 0.98 | (0.91, 1.06) | 0.62 |
External eating x Variety | −0.31 | (−0.65, 0.03) | 0.076 | 0.95 | (0.88, 1.02) | 0.17 |
Results of regression models analysis testing the interactive effect of external eating and marketing indicators for soft drinks on healthful and unhealthful food consumption (n = 465) adjusted for child age, gender, language, and household income. RR relative risk, CI Confidence Interval, SD Standard Deviation
Results of analyses on the display, regular price, variety and discount frequency of vegetables are presented in Table 4. As for the analyses for the relative availability of healthful food outlets and soft drink marketing indicators, no main effects of marketing indicators were observed. Evidence of interactions between external eating and vegetable displays was found for healthful food consumption. The interaction term was positive, suggesting that a positive association between vegetable display and healthful food consumption might be more positive for children with higher external eating scores. The association between external eating and unhealthful food consumption observed previously did not reach statistical significance in this reduced sample.
Table 4.
Healthful food consumption | Unhealthful food consumption | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Estimate | 95%CI | P | RR | 95%CI | P | |
External eating (1 SD) | −0.42 | (−0.96, −0.01) | 0.07 | 1.08 | (0.97, 1.20) | 0.17 |
Vegetables Discount Frequency (1 SD) | 0.13 | (−0.18, 0.72) | 0.54 | 0.99 | (0.90, 1.10) | 0.89 |
External eating x Discount frequency | −0.30 | (−1.15, −0.09) | 0.26 | 0.95 | (0.85, 1.07) | 0.42 |
External eating (1 SD) | −0.39 | (−0.84, 0.06) | 0.09 | 1.08 | (0.97, 1.20) | 0.15 |
Vegetables Display (1 SD) | −0.11 | (−0.57, 0.35) | 0.64 | 0.95 | (0.85, 1.06) | 0.35 |
External Eating x Display | 0.55 | (0.06, 1.04) | 0.027 | 1.02 | (0.91, 1.15) | 0.71 |
External eating (1 SD) | −0.42 | (−0.88, 0.04) | 0.07 | 1.08 | (0.97, 1.20) | 0.16 |
Vegetables Regular price (1 SD) | −0.01 | (−0.44, 0.42) | 0.95 | 1.00 | (0.91, 1.11) | 0.92 |
External Eating x Regular Price | −0.22 | (−0.83, 0.40) | 0.49 | 0.94 | (0.82, 1.08) | 0.39 |
External eating (1 SD) | −0.03 | (−0.94, 0.89) | 0.95 | 1.19 | (0.97. 1.46) | 0.09 |
Vegetable Variety (1 SD) | 0.21 | (−0.67, 1.09) | 0.64 | 0.97 | (0.80, 1.19) | 0.79 |
External Eating x Variety | −0.47 | (−1.41, 0.48) | 0.33 | 0.88 | (0.71, 1.09) | 0.25 |
Results of regression models separately testing the main and interactive effects of external eating and marketing indicators for vegetables (n = 257) on health and unhealthful food consumption adjusted for child age, gender, language, and household income. RR Relative risk, CI Confidence Interval, SD Standard Deviation
Visualization of interaction effects
Consumption frequency predicted from models in which statistically significant, or marginally significant interactions were found were plotted for different levels (mean ± one standard deviation) of the food environment indicators and external eating scores (see Figure 1). High external eaters seemed to consume less healthful food compared to low or intermediate external eaters in environments that can be considered less healthful, with a difference in daily healthful food consumption frequency of 1.9 in environments with low vegetable displays, and 1.2 and 1.1 in environments with high soft drink displays and variety, respectively. No differences in consumption were apparent for values of the food environment considered more healthful (higher vegetable display and lower soft drink variety and display).
Discussion
This study investigated variations in the strength of association between various aspects of the neighborhood food environment and food consumption in children as a function of individual differences in responsiveness to food cues, as measured using the external eating scale. No evidence was found for a moderating influence of external eating on the relationship between the relative availability of healthful, compared to unhealthful food retailers in proximity to a child’s residence, and children’s diet. However, some evidence for a moderating effect of external eating was found when looking at marketing practices related to visual food cues such as store displays used within food outlets to promote healthful (vegetables) and unhealthful foods (soft drinks), and variety of soft drink products. Specifically, the direction of the effect of store displays and soft drink variety for children with greater external eating was consistent with the dietary quality of each category (more healthful eating for vegetables and less healthful food consumption for soft drinks). These results are consistent with a laboratory-based study conducted with adults showing that the impact of viewing a food commercial (compared to a neutral commercial) while watching television was only observed in high, but not low, external eaters [47].
Our results, if replicated, would suggest that display strategies may be important environmental cues for children with external eating tendencies. Our findings would suggest that external eaters in this sample may not only respond to high-fat and high-sugar foods, but also to other types of food when they are more present in their immediate environment. The fact that external eating is associated with better diets in children when exposed to healthful food environment is consistent with the proposition that external eating might be adaptive in children [66, 67] and potentially explain negative or null correlation found between external eating and body mass index [66, 68].
No evidence of main or interactive effects of regular price or discount frequency was found. Regular pricing was not expected to lead to selected attention to particular food products, especially in the targeted age group and was included as a control strategy. Although discount frequency might be associated with greater product visibility, it remains mainly a financial purchasing incentive, which may not be a strong incentive for children. The null finding for pricing, and to some degree discount frequency, therefore supports the specificity of the results for strategies that are associated with specific environmental visual cues.
A main effect of external eating, but no interactions with food environment measures, were found for unhealthful food consumption. It is possible that unhealthful foods, with their high rewarding values and their omnipresence in urban environments, may have reached a “visibility threshold” so that additional variance in their availability is insufficient to translate into behavioral differences, even for high external eaters. The relative overrepresentation of unhealthful foods is supported with our data where only 23% of food outlets were considered as a source of healthful food (as suggested by the mRFEI) and where in-store displays of soft drinks were also much more frequent than displays of vegetables.
The lack of main effect of marketing indicators on either food consumption measures contrasts with earlier work analyzing store sales data [69] showing that the same reinforcing marketing practices that increase sales of unhealthful food (namely soft drinks), when applied to healthful food (namely vegetables), were also successful in increasing their sales. Discount frequency results also contrast with findings from a recent study showing that the increase in sales associated with frequency of price discounts was largest for less healthy compared to healthier food categories [70]. However, when testing whether responsiveness to price discounts of the healthy and less healthy food categories varied by SES status, evidence for a SES gradient was only found for healthy compared to less healthy categories, a similar pattern to what was found in this study. It must be noted that the above results are based on sales data, which generally provide large sample sizes, but limited information on individuals beyond basic demographics as well as an incomplete representation of individual diets.
Our findings suggest high responsiveness to food cues, while tied to potential adverse consequences when exposed to unhealthful environments, may translate into a beneficial impact in more healthful environments. This interpretation is also consistent with recent evidence that children carrying the 7-repeat allele on DRD4 (reflecting genetic predisposition to high environmental responsiveness) from a disadvantaged socio-economic background consumed more fat than the average, while the same carriers who were, in contrast, coming from a more advantaged socio-economic background consumed less fat than the average [71, 72]. That study concluded that the genetic marker identified individuals with “differential susceptibility” to socio-economic disadvantage, which has also been linked to the nutritional quality of the food environment.
Limitations
In this study, it was assumed that neighborhood food environment would influence children dietary behavior through children’s influence on parents’ purchases. It could be argued that children’s responsiveness to these food cues is unlikely to influence food behavior in this age group, who may not play an active role in household food purchasing decisions. However, evidence exists that children play an important role in supermarket product purchases [73, 74], and that younger children may be more likely to ask for advertised food products compared to adolescents [75]. Food marketing is seen as a predictor of food preference and attitude in children [25, 76], even in children as young as 5–6 years old [77]. This was reflected in this sample, for which a subsample (n = 379) of parents were asked questions specifically related to the role of children in purchasing decisions, with 85% stating that they would never, or rarely, not allow their children to ask for products and only 17% that would never or rarely tell their children they would consider their preferences when making purchases.
It is also possible that children’s external eating tendencies are confounded by their parents’ own external eating behaviors or other type of parental influence. This is supported by evidence that maternal external eating is related to external eating of daughters and sons [78] and external eating in children is positively associated with perceived parental pressure to eat in boys and negatively associated with perceived parental restriction to eat in boys and girls [79]. Future research should aim to account for parental factors in these relationships. In addition, parents may not have accurately reported their child’s intake or external eating tendencies, especially for older children, and their reports may have been influenced by a social desirability bias. Moreover, the food consumption instrument focused only on a selection of food items and did not provide a comprehensive assessment of the child’s diet. The cross-sectional nature of the study precludes any causal inference and future studies should explore how these associations evolve over time. The relatively small sample size available for some analyses, especially those involving strategies to promote vegetable purchases, as well as the relatively low reliability of the external eating scale, resulted in limited statistical power for detecting interaction effects. In addition, food marketing variables were only available at a relatively large area level, which may not be a very representative picture of children’s most immediate food environment. However, a recent review comparing food environment associations across different expressions of neighborhood suggested that more immediate food environment may not be as important as food environment defined for larger surroundings [15]. In order to assess the separate influence of healthful and unhealthful food marketing and compare ‘visual’ strategies like displays with more economic strategies like pricing and discounts, a relatively high number of interactions were tested. This, combined with the use of a statistical significance level of 0.10, may have increased the risk of chance findings. However, consistency in the type of behaviors and strategies for which statistical significance was found, as well as the absence of associations for the ‘control’ strategy do not suggest that results are due to chance alone. Finally, the convenience sample used limits the generalizability of our results. Future studies should replicate findings in other populations.
Conclusion
Findings from this study suggest that marketing strategies used to promote healthful and unhealthful food products within local stores may be more strongly associated with food consumption in children who were identified as being more responsive to food cues. These children’s intake appeared to be related to healthful and to some degree unhealthful food cues. These results, if replicated, would suggest that the development of interventions that simultaneously aim to reduce unhealthful local food cues while increasing healthful food cues may have a double effect in this subpopulation. Findings indicated that, in this sample, not all types of food cues interacted with external eating tendencies, with “visual cues” like in-store displays being more consistently related to healthful food consumption than “economic” cues like price or discounts in high-external eating children. If replicated, the results of this study may inform the development of targeted strategies promoting healthier food choices that are likely to have a greatest impact in specific sub-populations. Future studies should explore other measures of sensitivity to food cues measured through a variety of markers (genetic, neurocognitive, psychological) and their potential to identify responsiveness to a similar range of environmental healthful and unhealthful food cues, as well as clinical outcomes like weight status.
Additional files
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Melissa Iskandar, Vinita Akula, Dr. Aida Faber, Dr. Uku Vainik, Mr. Hiroshi Mamiya, and Imen Farhat for their assistance in the preparation of this manuscript.
Funding
This study was funded by a Collaborative Team Grant, Canada – India Initiative on Childhood funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research and the Indian Council of Medical Research (#85512; http://www.cihr-irsc.gc.ca/; http://www.icmr.nic.in/). Catherine Paquet was funded by a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC; https://www.nhmrc.gov.au/) Program Grant (#0631947). Funding bodies played no role in the design or analysis of this study.
Availability of data and materials
The datasets during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Abbreviations
- CAD
Canadian dollars
- FDA
Food and Drug Administration
- FSA
Forward sortation areas
- mRFEI
modified retail food environment index
- SIC
Standard Industry classification
- SKU
Stock keeping unit
- UPC
Universal product code
Authors’ contributions
CP, LD designed the study. LDM, AL, YM, DB, and NA contributed to the data collection. CP analysed and interpreted the data, and drafted the manuscript with the contribution of LD. All authors have revised the manuscript critically and approved the final version.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Verbal consent was obtained from all participants included in the study, and ethical approval was obtained from McGill University’s Institutional Research Board. All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/s12966-017-0540-9) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Contributor Information
Catherine Paquet, Email: Catherine.paquet@unisa.edu.au.
Luc de Montigny, Email: luc@demontigny.consulting.
Alice Labban, Email: alice.labban@pepperdine.edu.
David Buckeridge, Email: david.buckeridge@mcgill.ca.
Yu Ma, Email: yu.ma@mcgill.ca.
Narendra Arora, Email: nkarora@inclentrust.org.
Laurette Dubé, Email: laurette.dube@mcgill.ca.
References
- 1.Swinburn BA, Sacks G, Hall KD, McPherson K, Finegood DT, Moodie ML, Gortmaker SL. The global obesity pandemic: shaped by global drivers and local environments. Lancet. 2011;378:804–814. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60813-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Paeratakul S, Ferdinand DP, Champagne CM, Ryan DH, Bray GA. Fast-food consumption among US adults and children: dietary and nutrient intake profile. J Am Diet Assoc. 2003;103:1332–1338. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(03)01086-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Popkin BM. Global nutrition dynamics: the world is shifting rapidly toward a diet linked with noncommunicable diseases. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;84:289–298. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/84.1.289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Monteiro CA, Levy RB, Claro RM, de Castro IRR, Cannon G. Increasing consumption of ultra-processed foods and likely impact on human health: evidence from Brazil. Public Health Nutr. 2011;14:5–13. doi: 10.1017/S1368980010003241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Moubarac J-C, Martins APB, Claro RM, Levy RB, Cannon G, Monteiro CA. Consumption of ultra-processed foods and likely impact on human health. Evidence from Canada. Public Health Nutr. 2013;16:2240–2248. doi: 10.1017/S1368980012005009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Dhar T, Baylis K. Fast-food consumption and the ban on advertising targeting children: the Quebec experience. J Mark Res. 2011;48:799–813. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.48.5.799. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Goldberg ME. A quasi-experiment assessing the effectiveness of TV advertising directed to children. J Mark Res. 1990;27:445–454. doi: 10.2307/3172629. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Buijzen M, Schuurman J, Bomhof E. Associations between children's television advertising exposure and their food consumption patterns: a household diary–survey study. Appetite. 2008;50:231–239. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Finkelstein EA, Ruhm CJ, Kosa KM. Economic causes and consequences of obesity. Annu Rev Public Health. 2005;26:239–257. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Scott ML, Nowlis SM, Mandel N, Morales AC. The effects of reduced food size and package size on the consumption behavior of restrained and unrestrained eaters. J Consum Res. 2008;35:391–405. doi: 10.1086/591103. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Young LR, Nestle M. The contribution of expanding portion sizes to the US obesity epidemic. Am J Public Health. 2002;92:246–249. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.92.2.246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hollands GJ, Shemilt I, Marteau TM, Jebb SA, Lewis HB, Wei Y, Higgins J, Ogilvie D. Portion, package or tableware size for changing selection and consumption of food, alcohol and tobacco. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2015(9):CD011045. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD011045.pub2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 13.Glanz K, Sallis JF, Saelens BE, Frank LD. Healthy nutrition environments: concepts and measures. Am J Health Promot. 2005;19:330–333. doi: 10.4278/0890-1171-19.5.330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Black C, Moon G, Baird J. Dietary inequalities: what is the evidence for the effect of the neighbourhood food environment? Health Place. 2014;27:229–242. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2013.09.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Cobb LK, Appel LJ, Franco M, Jones-Smith JC, Nur A, Anderson CA. The relationship of the local food environment with obesity: a systematic review of methods, study quality, and results. Obesity. 2015;23:1331–1344. doi: 10.1002/oby.21118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Black JL, Macinko J. Neighborhoods and obesity. Nutr Rev. 2008;66:2–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2007.00001.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Caspi CE, Sorensen G, Subramanian SV, Kawachi I. The local food environment and diet: a systematic review. Health Place. 2012;18:1172–1187. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2012.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Fleischhacker SE, Evenson KR, Rodriguez DA, Ammerman AS. A systematic review of fast food access studies. Obes Rev. 2011;12:e460–ee71. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2010.00715.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Drewnowski A, Aggarwal A, Hurvitz PM, Monsivais P, Moudon AV. Obesity and supermarket access: proximity or price? Am J Public Health. 2012;102:e74–e80. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.300660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Gustafson A, Hankins S, Jilcott S. Measures of the consumer food store environment: a systematic review of the evidence 2000–2011. J Community Health. 2011;37:897–911. doi: 10.1007/s10900-011-9524-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Nakamura R, Pechey R, Suhrcke M, Jebb SA, Marteau TM. Sales impact of displaying alcoholic and non-alcoholic beverages in end-of-aisle locations: an observational study. Soc Sci Med. 2014;108:68–73. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.02.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ma Y, Labban A, Cherian M, Shaban-Nejad A, Buckeridge DL, Dubé L. System of indicators for the nutritional quality of marketing and food environment: product quality, availability, affordability, and promotion. In: Preedy RV, Hunter L-A, Patel BV, editors. Diet quality: an evidence-based approach, volume 2. New York, NY: Springer New York; 2013:383–396.
- 23.Engler-Stringer R, Le H, Gerrard A, Muhajarine N. The community and consumer food environment and children’s diet: a systematic review. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:1–15. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Le H, Engler-Stringer R, Muhajarine N. Walkable home neighbourhood food environment and children’s overweight and obesity: proximity, density or price? Can J Public Health. 2016;107:42–47. doi: 10.17269/cjph.107.5347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Harris JL, Pomeranz JL, Lobstein T, Brownell KD. A crisis in the marketplace: how food marketing contributes to childhood obesity and what can be done. Annu Rev Public Health. 2009;30:211–225. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.031308.100304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hawkes C. Sales promotions and food consumption. Nutr Rev. 2009;67:333–342. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2009.00206.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Van der Laan L, De Ridder D, Viergever MA, Smeets PA. The first taste is always with the eyes: a meta-analysis on the neural correlates of processing visual food cues. NeuroImage. 2011;55:296–303. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Burger KS, Stice E. Neural responsivity during soft drink intake, anticipation, and advertisement exposure in habitually consuming youth. Obesity. 2014;22:441–450. doi: 10.1002/oby.20563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Gearhardt AN, Yokum S, Stice E, Harris JL, Brownell KD. Relation of obesity to neural activation in response to food commercials. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014;9:932–938. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Carnell S, Gibson C, Benson L, Ochner C, Geliebter A. Neuroimaging and obesity: current knowledge and future directions. Obes Rev. 2012;13:43–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00927.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bruce AS, Holsen LM, Chambers RJ, Martin LE, Brooks WM, Zarcone JR, Butler MG, Savage CR. Obese children show hyperactivation to food pictures in brain networks linked to motivation, reward and cognitive control. Int J Obes. 2010;34:1494–1500. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2010.84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Martin LE, Holsen LM, Chambers RJ, Bruce AS, Brooks WM, Zarcone JR, Butler MG, Savage CR. Neural mechanisms associated with food motivation in obese and healthy weight adults. Obesity. 2010;18:254–260. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Stice E, Yokum S, Burger K, Epstein L, Small D. Youth at risk for obesity show greater activation of striatal and somatosensory regions to food. J Neurosci. 2011;31:4360–4366. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6604-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Geha PY, Aschenbrenner K, Felsted J, O'Malley SS, Small DM. Altered hypothalamic response to food in smokers. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;97:15–22. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.043307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Demos KE, Heatherton TF, Kelley WM. Individual differences in nucleus accumbens activity to food and sexual images predict weight gain and sexual behavior. J Neurosci. 2012;32:5549–5552. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5958-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Yokum S, Gearhardt AN, Harris JL, Brownell KD, Stice E. Individual differences in striatum activity to food commercials predict weight gain in adolescents. Obesity. 2014;22:2544–2551. doi: 10.1002/oby.20882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Levitan RD, Rivera J, Silveira PP, Steiner M, Gaudreau H, Hamilton J, Kennedy JL, Davis C, Dube L, Fellows L, et al. Gender differences in the association between stop-signal reaction times, body mass indices and/or spontaneous food intake in pre-school children: an early model of compromised inhibitory control and obesity. Int J Obes. 2015;39:614–619. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2014.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Silveira P, Kennedy J, Sokolowski M, Levitan R, Meaney M. Genetic and Developmental Origins of Food Preferences and Obesity Risk: The Role of Dopamine. In: Seckl JR, Christen Y (editors). Hormones, Intrauterine Health and Programming, Volume 12 (Research and Perspectives in Endocrine Interactions). Switzerland: Springer International Publishing; 2014. p. 157–74.
- 39.Paquet C, Daniel M, Knäuper B, Gauvin L, Kestens Y, Dubé L. Interactive effects of reward sensitivity and residential fast-food restaurant exposure on fast-food consumption. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;91:771–776. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2009.28648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67:319. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Beaver JD, Lawrence AD, van Ditzhuijzen J, Davis MH, Woods A, Calder AJ. Individual differences in reward drive predict neural responses to images of food. J Neurosci. 2006;26:5160–5166. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0350-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Vainik U, Dagher A, Dubé L, Fellows LK. Neurobehavioural correlates of body mass index and eating behaviours in adults: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37:279–299. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Vainik U, Neseliler S, Konstabel K, Fellows LK, Dagher A. Eating traits questionnaires as a continuum of a single concept. Uncontrolled eating. Appetite. 2015;90:229–239. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Van Strien T, Frijters JE, Bergers G, Defares PB. The Dutch eating behavior questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. Int J Eat Disord. 1986;5:295–315. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(198602)5:2<295::AID-EAT2260050209>3.0.CO;2-T. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Brignell C, Griffiths T, Bradley BP, Mogg K. Attentional and approach biases for pictorial food cues. Influence of external eating. Appetite. 2009;52:299–306. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Burton P, Smit HJ, Lightowler HJ. The influence of restrained and external eating patterns on overeating. Appetite. 2007;49:191–197. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.van Strien T, Peter Herman C, Anschutz D. The predictive validity of the DEBQ-external eating scale for eating in response to food commercials while watching television. Int J Eat Disord. 2012;45:257–262. doi: 10.1002/eat.20940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Camirand H. Les défis de l’alimentation des enfants et adolescents québécois (9-18 ans); Enquête sur la santé dans les collectivités canadiennes, cycle 2.2, Nutrition (2004). Québec: Institut de la statistique du Québec; 2011.
- 49.Garriguet D. Plus de fruits, moins de gras: Les habitudes alimentaires des canadiens. Transition. 2007;37:7–10. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Johnson-Down L, Ritter H, Starkey LJ, Gray-Donald K. Primary food sources of nutrients in the diet of Canadian adults. Can J Diet Pract Res. 2006;67:7–13. doi: 10.3148/67.1.2006.7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.van Strien T, Oosterveld P. The children's DEBQ for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating in 7-to 12-year-old children. Int J Eat Disord. 2008;41:72–81. doi: 10.1002/eat.20424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Gadermann AM, Guhn M, Zumbo BD. Estimating ordinal reliability for Likert-type and ordinal item response data: a conceptual, empirical, and practical guide. Pract Assess Res Eval. 2012;17:1–13. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Rose D, Bodor JN, Hutchinson PL, Swalm CM. The importance of a multi-dimensional approach for studying the links between food access and consumption. J Nutr. 2010;140:1170–1174. doi: 10.3945/jn.109.113159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Children’s food environment state indicator report 2011. Atlanta (GA): CDC; 2011.
- 55.Clary CM, Kestens Y. Field validation of secondary data sources: a novel measure of representativity applied to a Canadian food outlet database. Int J Beh Nutr Phys Act. 2013;10:77. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-10-77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Chandon P, Wansink B. Is food marketing making us fat? A multi-disciplinary review. Foundations and Trends in Marketing. 2011;5:113–196. doi: 10.1561/1700000016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Malik VS, Schulze MB, Hu FB. Intake of sugar-sweetened beverages and weight gain: a systematic review. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;84:274–288. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/84.1.274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Vartanian LR, Schwartz MB, Brownell KD. Effects of soft drink consumption on nutrition and health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Public Health. 2007;97:667–675. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.083782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Hu FB, Malik VS. Sugar-sweetened beverages and risk of obesity and type 2 diabetes: epidemiologic evidence. Physiol Behav. 2010;100:47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.01.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Dauchet L, Amouyel P, Hercberg S, Dallongeville J. Fruit and vegetable consumption and risk of coronary heart disease: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. J Nutr. 2006;136:2588–2593. doi: 10.1093/jn/136.10.2588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Carter P, Gray LJ, Troughton J, Khunti K, Davies MJ. Fruit and vegetable intake and incidence of type 2 diabetes mellitus: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ. 2010;341 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 62.Raju JS. The effect of price promotions on variability in product category sales. Mark Sci. 1992;11:207–220. doi: 10.1287/mksc.11.3.207. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Nijs VR, Dekimpe MG, Steenkamps J-BE, Hanssens DM. The category-demand effects of price promotions. Mark Sci. 2001;20:1–22. doi: 10.1287/mksc.20.1.1.10197. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Whisman MA, McClelland GH. Designing, testing, and interpreting interactions and moderator effects in family research. J Fam Psychol. 2005;19:111–120. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.1.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.McClelland GH, Judd CM. Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychol Bull. 1993;114:376. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.2.376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.van Strien T, Herman CP, Verheijden MW. Eating style, overeating, and overweight in a representative Dutch sample. Does external eating play a role? Appetite. 2009;52:380–387. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Rodin J. Current status of the internal–external hypothesis for obesity: what went wrong? Am Psychol. 1981;36:361. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.36.4.361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Silva JR, Capurro G, Saumann MP, Slachevsky A. Problematic eating behaviors and nutritional status in 7 to 12 year-old Chilean children. Int J Clin Health Psychol. 2013;13:32–39. doi: 10.1016/S1697-2600(13)70005-X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Dubé L, Labban A, Moubarac JC, Heslop G, Ma Y, Paquet C. A nutrition/health mindset on commercial big data and drivers of food demand in modern and traditional systems. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2014;1331:278–295. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Nakamura R, Suhrcke M, Jebb SA, Pechey R, Almiron-Roig E, Marteau TM. Price promotions on healthier compared with less healthy foods: a hierarchical regression analysis of the impact on sales and social patterning of responses to promotions in great Britain. Am J Clin Nutr. 2015;101:808–816. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.114.094227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Silveira PP, Gaudreau H, Atkinson L, Fleming AS, Sokolowski MB, Steiner M, Kennedy JL, Meaney MJ, Levitan RD, Dubé L. Genetic differential susceptibility to socio-economic status and childhood obesogenic behavior: why targeted prevention may be the best societal investment. JAMA Pediatr. 2016; 170:359-64 [DOI] [PubMed]
- 72.Belsky J. The differential susceptibility hypothesis: sensitivity to the environment for better and for worse. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170:321–322. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.4263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Wilson G, Wood K. The influence of children on parental purchases during supermarket shopping. Int J Consum Stud. 2004;28:329–336. doi: 10.1111/j.1470-6431.2004.00393.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Campbell S, James EL, Stacey FG, Bowman J, Chapman K, Kelly B. A mixed-method examination of food marketing directed towards children in Australian supermarkets. Health Promot Int. 2014;29:267–277. doi: 10.1093/heapro/das060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Kelly B, Chapman K, Hardy LL, King L, Farrell L. Parental awareness and attitudes of food marketing to children: a community attitudes survey of parents in new South Wales. Australia J Paediatr Child Health. 2009;45:493–497. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2009.01548.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Cairns G, Angus K, Hastings G. The extent, nature and effects of food promotion to children: a review of the evidence to December 2008. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 77.Campbell KJ, Crawford DA, Hesketh KD. Australian parents' views on their 5–6-year-old children's food choices. Health Promot Int. 2007;22:11–18. doi: 10.1093/heapro/dal035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Jahnke DL, Warschburger PA. Familial transmission of eating behaviors in preschool-aged children. Obesity. 2008;16:1821–1825. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.van Strien T, Bazelier FG. Perceived parental control of food intake is related to external, restrained and emotional eating in 7–12-year-old boys and girls. Appetite. 2007;49:618–625. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.03.227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.