Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2017 Jun 29;6:e23532. doi: 10.7554/eLife.23532

Cortex-dependent recovery of unassisted hindlimb locomotion after complete spinal cord injury in adult rats

Anitha Manohar 1,, Guglielmo Foffani 2,3,, Patrick D Ganzer 1, John R Bethea 4, Karen A Moxon 1,5,*
Editor: Ole Kiehn6
PMCID: PMC5499944  PMID: 28661400

Abstract

After paralyzing spinal cord injury the adult nervous system has little ability to ‘heal’ spinal connections, and it is assumed to be unable to develop extra-spinal recovery strategies to bypass the lesion. We challenge this assumption, showing that completely spinalized adult rats can recover unassisted hindlimb weight support and locomotion without explicit spinal transmission of motor commands through the lesion. This is achieved with combinations of pharmacological and physical therapies that maximize cortical reorganization, inducing an expansion of trunk motor cortex and forepaw sensory cortex into the deafferented hindlimb cortex, associated with sprouting of corticospinal axons. Lesioning the reorganized cortex reverses the recovery. Adult rats can thus develop a novel cortical sensorimotor circuit that bypasses the lesion, probably through biomechanical coupling, to partly recover unassisted hindlimb locomotion after complete spinal cord injury.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23532.001

Research Organism: Rat

Introduction

Spinal cord injury leads to dramatic loss of motor function below the level of the lesion, often permanently compromising the ability to locomote (van Middendorp et al., 2011). From a translational research perspective, recovery of motor function and locomotion can in principle be achieved with at least two conceptually different approaches: (a) ‘healing’ the lesion to re-establish motor pathways (Richardson et al., 1980; David and Aguayo, 1981); (b) bypassing the lesion to reconnect motor circuits above and below (Ethier et al., 2012; Manohar et al., 2012; Bouton et al., 2016). The first approach pursues the regeneration ideal of overcoming the natural limited ability of the adult spinal cord to restore connections through the injury (Selzer, 2003; Liu et al., 2010b; Ruschel et al., 2015). The second approach represents the practical alternative of extracting motor information at higher levels and reinserting it below the lesion, either within the spinal cord, directly to the muscles or through a robotic actuator (Bensmaia and Miller, 2014; Moxon and Foffani, 2015).

The two approaches – healing vs bypassing the lesion – implicitly assume that the adult nervous system is unable to innately develop a recovery strategy to bypass the lesion. Here we will challenge this assumption. Specifically: (i) we chose a spinal cord injury model – complete thoracic transection in adult rats – that excludes any spinal transmission of motor commands through the lesion; and (ii) we delivered a combination of pharmacological treatment (5-HT agonists) and physical therapies (active treadmill training and/or passive hindlimb bike exercise) designed to maximize reorganization of sensorimotor circuits both below and above the level of the lesion (Courtine et al., 2009; Kao et al., 2009, 2011; Ganzer et al., 2013; Graziano et al., 2013; Oza and Giszter, 2014, 2015; Foffani et al., 2016; Ganzer et al., 2016).

We show that maximizing the reorganization of the hindlimb sensorimotor cortex creates a novel circuit that responds to input from the ventral forepaws and activates trunk muscles that span the lesion. This new cortical circuit bypasses the lesion, probably via biomechanical coupling, allowing animals to recover a surprising level of unassisted hindlimb weight support and locomotion after complete spinal cord injury.

Results

Combined therapeutic interventions induce hindlimb motor recovery after complete spinal cord injury

We first sought direct evidence that hindlimb motor recovery after spinal cord injury can indeed be achieved without explicit spinal transmission of motor commands through the lesion. To this end, in the first set of experiments adult rats (n = 45) received a complete spinal cord transection at thoracic level (T9/T10) and were treated with 5-HT agonists combined with either (a) physical therapy below the level of the lesion (passive hindlimb bike exercise, n = 15) or (b) physical therapy below and above the level of the lesion (passive hindlimb bike exercise + active treadmill training, n = 15), and were compared with a group of transected animals that received ‘sham’ therapy (n = 15). For simplicity, we will refer to 5-HT agonist + passive hindlimb bike exercise as ‘partial therapy’ and 5-HT agonists + passive hindlimb bike exercise + active treadmill training as ‘complete therapy’. Hindlimb motor recovery was assessed both in controlled experimental conditions, as percentage of weight supported step cycles (%WSS) during treadmill locomotion with lateral but no vertical assist (at 4, 8 and 12 weeks post transection), and in more naturalistic conditions using open field testing without any support (at 2, 4, 8 and 12 weeks post transection, normalized to group-averages at week two post transection). Behavioral measures in all groups were always performed after acute administration of drugs (the same 5-HT agonists used for therapy), in order to guarantee the functional state of the cord below the level of the lesion (Barbeau and Rossignol, 1990; Jackson and White, 1990), but without any sensory stimulation (i.e. tail pinch or perineal stimulation).

These combined therapeutic interventions markedly improved hindlimb motor performance as measured by %WSS during treadmill locomotion (2-way mixed ANOVA, time x therapy: F(4,78)=3.7, p=0.0082; Figure 1A). As expected, 4 weeks after transection the %WSS was low for all groups (1.6 ± 4.3%). Conversely, the %WSS increased by a factor of ten at 8 weeks and 12 weeks compared to 4 weeks both, for animals receiving partial (Tukey: p=0.0644, p=0.0086) and complete therapy (p=0.0300, p=0.0002), but not for transected animals receiving sham therapy (p>0.99). Moreover, at 12 weeks, the %WSS of animals receiving complete therapy (19.4 ± 17.8%) was significantly higher than that of animals that received sham therapy (1.6 ± 2.9%, p=0.0103) but animals that received partial therapy were not different from sham therapy animals (12.3 ± 15.4%, p=0.32), suggesting that complete therapy was more effective than partial therapy. Many of the WSS achieved with either therapy were consecutive (Figure 1A, inset). After 12 weeks of complete therapy, three animals were able to perform >40% of WSS (one animal went as high as 56%) during the treadmill session.

Figure 1. Locomotor recovery after a complete spinal cord injury.

(A) Percentage of weight supported step cycles (% WSS) during treadmill locomotion for complete therapy (red), partial therapy (blue) and sham therapy (black) groups at 4, 8 and 12 weeks post-injury. Pie charts indicating the fraction of these weight supported steps that were part of a consecutive bout of three or more step cycles for each group (right panel inset, black: percentage of consecutive steps, grey: nonconsecutive steps). (B) Open field score measured by the BBB scale, expressed as % change from baseline at 2 weeks post injury. (C) Pie charts showing the proportion of BBB scores that correspond to weight support in the hindlimbs during unassisted open field locomotion (≥9) at 2 weeks (top panel) and 12 weeks (bottom panel). (D) Correlation between the open field score and %WSS at 12 weeks after injury. (E) Example frames of a spinalized rat after 12 weeks of complete therapy taking plantar weight supported steps with its hindlimbs in the open field. (F) Histological verification of complete spinal transection using Nissl stain (left) of a horizontal section of the spinal cord. 5-HT stain (right) of a rostral and caudal location taken from a slice adjacent to the Nissl stained slice on the left. Boxes represent the approximate location of the higher magnification picture of 5-HT stain. # represents differences from week 2 or 4 and * represents differences within the same week. (*) p<0.1, *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23532.002

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Assisted weight support provided during training.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

At the start of each week of treadmill training the amount of the animal’s body weight that was supported by the device was adjusted to optimize stepping abilities. The animals failed to step below a certain level of body weight support and this was defined as their failure point for that week. The failure point was evaluated also in animals that received sham therapy or partial therapy. The graph tracks the failure point at 4, 8 and 12 weeks after the spinal cord transection for all three groups, expressed as % of the assisted weight support provided by the device when the animals could not bear any weight with their hindlimbs at 2 weeks after the spinal cord transection (y-axis). # represents differences from week four and * represents differences within the same week *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, Tukey post-hoc tests after significant time x therapy interaction of 2-way mixed ANOVA (F(4,72)=4.2, p=0.0009). Note that we obtained failure point data for 12 sham-therapy, 12 partial-therapy and 15 complete-therapy animals. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals. The assisted weight support at failure point decreased with partial and complete therapy as a function of time after the spinal transection, providing direct evidence of increasing levels of weight support achieved by the animals.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. A schematic representation of the body weight support device that was used during treadmill training.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

The device is a spring-mass system adapted for quadrupedal locomotion from Timoszyk et al. (2005), which introduced a similar device for bipedal locomotion. The forces acting on the device are briefly illustrated in the diagram for visualization. When the horizontal arm is parallel to the ground the moments acting around the pivot point are balanced. The device is calibrated a priori and periodically using weights to determine the amount of spring extension required for providing a fixed amount of vertical weight support corresponding to a percentage of the animal’s weight. Note that during locomotion when the forelimbs and hindlimbs are loaded and pushing on the treadmill there will be normal reaction forces acting on the animal (indicated as fp and hp on the figure) and the exact amount cannot be measured without additional sensors.

Even though %WSS were always evaluated without any assisted vertical weight support, we also evaluated the minimal amount of assisted vertical weight support that was necessary for animals to maintain a consistent quadrupedal locomotion on the treadmill, defined as the load-bearing failure point (Timoszyk et al., 2005). The assisted vertical weight support at load-bearing failure point markedly decreased at 8 weeks and 12 weeks with partial and complete therapy but not with sham therapy (Figure 1—figure supplement 1), providing additional direct evidence of increasing levels of hindlimb weight support achieved by the animals.

These important motor recoveries were confirmed by the evaluations in the open field without any support (Figure 1B). Specifically, open field scores (Basso et al., 1995) were similar across groups at 2 weeks (6.5 ± 1.3 for sham, 5.7 ± 2.0 for partial, 5.2 ± 2.4 for full; 1-way ANOVA, F(2,41)=1.7, p=0.19), and then increased at 4 weeks, 8 weeks and 12 weeks compared to 2 weeks for animals receiving complete therapy (time x therapy: F(6,123)=3.2, p=0.0064; Tukey: p<0.0001) and increased at 8 weeks and 12 weeks compared to 2 weeks for animals receiving partial therapy (p<0.0007), but not for transected animals receiving sham therapy (p>0.40). Again, at 12 weeks the open field scores of animals that received complete therapy were significantly higher than those of animals that received sham therapy (p=0.0137) but animals that received partial therapy were not different from sham (p=0.43), confirming that complete therapy was more effective than partial therapy.

To gain insight into the behavioral significance of the recovery in the open field, we focused on a critical aspect of motor performance: the ability of the animals to gain plantar weight support in the hindlimbs (BBB ≥ 9). The number of animals that achieved plantar weight support in the open field dramatically increased with partial or complete therapy from <4% at 2 weeks to 47% at 12 weeks, compared to 14% at 12 weeks in animals undergoing sham therapy (Figure 1C).

Interestingly, 12 weeks after spinal transection motor performance during weight-supported treadmill locomotion and open-field motor recovery were highly correlated (Pearson: R = 0.60, p<0.0001; n = 42; Figure 1D), suggesting that these two measures – quantitative on a treadmill with lateral support and semi-quantitative in the open field – are capturing similar aspects of functional recovery.

Taken together, these results show that even with a complete transection of the spinal cord, carefully designed combinations of therapeutic interventions can restore a significant level of hindlimb weight support and locomotion without any sensory stimulation or assisted vertical weight support after complete spinal cord injury (Figure 1E). Importantly, the completeness of the spinal lesion was histologically confirmed in all animals with both nissl and 5-HT staining (Figure 1F)

Combined therapeutic interventions induce reorganization of the deafferented motor cortex after complete spinal cord transection

A critical aspect for understanding the possible mechanisms underlying the observed motor recovery is the role of cortical reorganization. At the end of the study, 12 weeks after the spinal transection, a subset of animals (n = 36) underwent an anesthetized motor mapping procedure using intracortical microstimulation to assess the possible reorganization of the deafferented hindlimb motor cortex under the different therapy regimens (complete therapy: n = 14, partial therapy: n = 12, sham therapy: n = 10). Motor reorganization was assessed in terms of cortical area (in mm2) in which microstimulation elicited movement of the hindlimbs, trunk, forelimbs or vibrissae. An additional group of naïve intact animals (n = 5) was used to confirm the coordinates of hindpaw motor representation.

As expected, motor representations of the hindlimbs – typically observed in naïve intact rats – were absent in all lesioned animals, confirming the completeness of the spinal transection (Figure 2A). However, there was a significant therapy-dependent reorganization of the deafferented motor cortex (2-way mixed ANOVA, location x therapy: F(4,66)=3.6, p=0.0103; Figure 2A,B). Specifically, we observed an expansion of the trunk representation in animals that received complete therapy compared to animals that received partial therapy (Tukey, p=0.0484) or sham therapy (p=0.0013). This expansion was not secondary to an in increase in cortical excitability, since there were no differences between the threshold currents across groups (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). We did not observe any expansion of the cortical representations of the forelimb or of the vibrissae into the deafferented motor cortex (p>0.99).

Figure 2. Reorganization of the motor cortex induced by combined therapeutic interventions.

(A) Raw intra-cortical micro-stimulation motor maps showing the penetrations on a cortical grid (co-ordinates AP: from 0.5 mm rostral to bregma to 2 mm posterior to bregma; ML: 1 to 3.5 mm lateral to midline) color coded by the type of movement evoked for naïve-intact, sham therapy, partial therapy and complete therapy groups. (B) Average cortical area (mm2) corresponding to a specific type of movement (trunk, forelimbs or vibrissae) for all therapy groups. Correlation between cortical area corresponding to trunk movements and locomotor recovery measured by (C) % WSS and also (D) open field score. *p<0.05, **p<0.01. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23532.005

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Threshold currents during intra-cortical micro-stimulation motor maps.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

This figure shows the threshold currents corresponding to the trunk movements evoked during motor maps over the same cortical area shown in Figure 2A. The threshold currents were averaged for penetrations within an area of 0.25 mm2 and visualized using a heatmap over a scale of 0–100 µA. Hashed squares show areas where no penetrations yielded any movement. There were no differences between the threshold currents across the three groups (1-way ANOVA, F(2,85)=0.751, p=0.475). Note that penetrations, rather than animals, were considered as individual samples for this analysis to maximize power. The heat maps indicate an expansion of the trunk area, particularly for animals that received complete therapy, while using equivalent threshold currents across all groups.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Correlation measures.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

The correlations were performed both using simple linear regression measures (Pearson’s correlation, red line) and robust regression methods (Bi-square fit, green line). In the case of Figure 1C and Figure 2D, the two methods were equivalent. In the case of Figure 2C, the linear regression indicates a correlation between the measures. However the bi-square fit fails, mainly because it weights outliers with a lower value than values closer to the mean. The large variation in the data is because all the therapy groups are combined for this analysis (including animals that show no recovery as well as high perfomers). Since the high performers cannot be considered outliers for our data, we retained the linear regression values in the main figure.

Importantly, considering all animals, the cortical area of the trunk representation correlated with motor performance, measured by both %WSS (Pearson: R = 0.41, p=0.0142; n = 36; Figure 2C) and open field scores (Pearson: R = 0.51, p=0.0013; n = 36; Figure 2D; Figure 2—figure supplement 2).

Lesioning the reorganized motor cortex reverses the behavioral recovery

A correlation between cortical reorganization and motor recovery does not necessarily imply a causal relationship between the two. In order to find more convincing evidence that the reorganization of the deafferented hindlimb motor cortex directly contributed to the motor recovery, at the end of the motor map the reorganized motor cortex was electrolytically lesioned bilaterally (Figure 3A), animals received two additional weeks of therapy (partial or complete) post lesion and were then behaviorally re-evaluated at week 14 post transection (complete therapy, n = 12; partial therapy, n = 10).

Figure 3. Lesioning the motor cortex reverses behavioral recovery.

(A) Photomicrograph of exemplar electrolytic lesion in the hindlimb motor cortex (inset: cortical map showing lesion location centered at 1.0 mm caudal to bregma and 2.0 mm lateral). (B) Locomotor recovery measured by %WSS during treadmill locomotion evaluated before and after (pre-, post-) lesioning the motor cortex. (C) Proportion of BBB scores that correspond to weight support in the hindlimbs during unassisted open field locomotion (≥9) before and after (pre-, post-) cortical lesions. ***p<0.001. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23532.008

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Quality of steps assessed from the videos based on individual paw placements.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

We performed video analyses from the treadmill testing data in animals that subsequently received cortical lesion (complete therapy, n = 12; partial therapy, n = 10), during testing at week-12 (pre-cortical lesion) and at week-14 (post-cortical lesion). Each of the 100 step cycles used for the %WSS was categorized – separately for each hindpaw – as no step (i.e. dragging), dorsal step (when the dorsal surface of the paw makes contact with the treadmill during paw placement), lateral step (when the animal performs a lateral sweep like motion without plantar contact) or plantar step (when the plantar surface of the paw comes in contact with the treadmill entirely during paw placement). Animals were categorized in two groups based on an arbitrary recovery threshold of 10 %WSS at week-12: animals with good recovery (performer, %WSS > 10, average 28.4 ± 11.1%, n = 11) and animals with poor recovery (non-performer, %WSS ≤ 10, average 2.8 ± 3.8%, n = 11). Left and right hindpaw steps were separated as an independent factor. As expected, the percentage of no steps was lower in performers than in non-performers (3-way ANOVA, group: F(1,40)=10.1, p=0.0028) and increased after cortical lesion in both groups (lesion: F(1,40)=6.8, p=0.0129). The percentage of dorsal steps was similar in the two groups (group: F(1,40)=0.05, p=0.82), with a tendency to increase after cortical lesion in performers but not in non-performers (group x lesion: F(1,40)=5.3, p=0.0261). The percentage of lateral steps was again similar in the two groups (group: F(1,40)=1.2, p=0.28) and increased after cortical lesion in both groups (lesion: F(1,40)=6.8, p=0.0127). Finally, the percentage of plantar steps was markedly higher in performers (group: F(1,40)=95.7, p<0.0001) and significantly decreased after the lesion in performers (lesion x group: F(1,40)=19.9, p<0.0001; Tukey: p=0.0002) but not in non-performers (Tukey: p=0.11). These results clarify that the ability to make plantar contact with the treadmill during stepping is key to achieve good recovery and that the reorganized motor cortex plays a critical role in this recovery. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001.

Lesioning the reorganized motor cortex dramatically decreased the motor performance as measured by %WSS during treadmill locomotion (2-way ANOVA, lesion: F(1,20)=24.0, p<0.0001; lesion x therapy: F(1,20)=0.1, p=0.70; Figure 3B). The key element that differentiated animals that achieved good recovery from animals that displayed poor recovery was the ability to make plantar contact with the treadmill during stepping, which was lost after the cortical lesion (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). In agreement, the cortical lesion also decreased the number of animals that achieved plantar weight support in the open field (BBB ≥ 9) from 47% at week-12, to 36% and 27% after the cortical lesion in animals under partial or complete therapy, respectively (Figure 3C).

The %WSS for the forelimbs was 100% for all animals at week-12 and remained 100% for all animals at week-14, after the cortical lesion. Furthermore, 100% of forelimb steps were plantar steps both before and after the cortical lesion. Forelimb weight support thus remained unobstructed by the cortical lesion, supporting the specificity of the cortical lesion to hindlimb function.

As a control, the same cortical lesion performed in a group of naïve intact animals (n = 4) did not induce any change in motor performance either on the treadmill or in the open field (100%WSS and BBB = 21 both before and after the cortical lesion).

These results suggest that the reorganization of the motor cortex is at least partly responsible for the hindlimb motor recovery after complete spinal cord transection.

Corticospinal fibers from the reorganized motor cortex sprout into the thoracic spinal cord after therapy

The behavioral recovery induced by therapeutic interventions could be mediated by the sprouting of corticospinal fibers within the spinal cord. To test this possibility, in a second set of experiments, we labeled the projections from the reorganized motor cortex to the spinal cord with injections of the anterograde tracer BDA (Figure 4A), in transected animals receiving sham therapy (n = 2) and transected animals receiving complete therapy (n = 2). A group of naïve intact animals (n = 2) was used as a control. The tracer was injected in the deafferented hindlimb motor cortex (i.e. the reorganized cortex) 9 weeks after the spinal transection, animals continued therapy and were perfused 3 weeks later. For each animal, labeled axons were counted in the gray matter of the spinal hemicord at the thoracic level where alpha motor neurons innervate trunk musculature. Specifically, axons were counted at each of three different levels above the lesion (T1, T4 and T7) contralateral to the injected cortex, from five representative slices spaced 250 microns apart (Figure 4B). Average counts per slice were considered as independent samples.

Figure 4. Corticospinal sprouting into the thoracic spinal cord.

Figure 4.

(A) Diagram illustrating anterograde tracing experiments. (B) Histology of a sample coronal slice of the thoracic spinal hemi-cord taken from above the level of the spinal lesion and a magnified view of the sprouting into the grey matter in (C) Naïve-Intact (D) Sham therapy and (E) Complete therapy groups. (F) Bar graphs showing the average count of corticospinal axons sprouting per mm2 of gray matter of the spinal hemi-cord for the different groups. *p<0.05, **p<0.01. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23532.010

There was a rostro-caudal gradient of axonal innervation from the hindlimb motor cortex into the spinal cord (two-way ANOVA, level: F(2,81)=7.3, p=0.0012), with more counts at T7 level compared to T4 (Tukey, p=0.0054) and T1 (p=0.0030). More importantly, therapy after spinal cord transection promoted corticospinal sprouting (therapy, F(2,81)=6.4, p=0.0027). Specifically, more axons were counted in the gray matter of the thoracic spinal cord in transected animals that underwent complete therapy compared to both transected animals that underwent sham therapy (p=0.0111) and naïve intact animals (p=0.0055) (Figure 4C–F).

These results show that our therapy regimen promotes sprouting of corticospinal fibers, originating in the deafferented hindlimb cortex, into the gray matter of the spinal cord above the level of the lesion. This corticospinal sprouting could allow animals to gain greater control of trunk musculature, thus contributing to the observed motor recovery.

Combined therapeutic interventions induce reorganization of the deafferented somatosensory cortex after complete spinal cord transection

Recovery of hindlimb function after spinal cord transection likely relies on somatosensory inputs from the intact forelimbs, suggesting that motor cortical reorganization might be paralleled by somatosensory cortical reorganization in our animals. To test this prediction, in a third set of experiments we chronically and bilaterally implanted 16-channel arrays of microelectrodes (4-by-4) in the hindlimb somatosensory cortex of 14 animals, which were then spinally transected and received either complete therapy (n = 8) or sham therapy (n = 6). We mapped the responses of cortical neurons in the deafferented hindlimb cortex to tactile sensory stimuli delivered to seven fixed locations on each forelimb, pre (week 0) and post spinal cord injury (week 4, 8, 12). Cortical reorganization was measured as the percentage responding of cells, i.e. cells recorded in the deafferented hindlimb cortex that significantly responded to tactile stimulation of at least one location on the contralateral forelimb (Figure 5A,B). Note that neurons in the hindlimb cortex did not have significant responses to trunk stimulation either before or after spinal cord injury, likely because the trunk sensory area is not topographically adjacent to the deafferented hindlimb region.

Figure 5. Reorganization of the hindlimb sensory cortex induced by combined therapeutic interventions.

(A) Example showing single unit discrimination using waveform-based cluster analysis (left panel) and exemplar peri-stimulus time rasters and histograms of (a) a neuron which is unresponsive to sensory stimulation and (b) a neuron showing a significant response to sensory stimulation measured in a window extending 100 ms before and after the stimulus. (B) Proportion of neurons recorded from sham therapy group (black) that had a significant response to forepaw sensory stimulation compared to that of complete therapy group (red) pre- transection (0w) and weeks 4 (4w), 8 (8w) and 12 (12w) post-transection. (C) Heat maps indicating distribution of forepaw sensory responses recorded on each of the wires of the 4 × 4 microwire electrode array implanted into the hindlimb sensorimotor cortex pre- (week 0) and post- (weeks 4, 8, 12) spinal transection. (D) Average magnitude of the neural responses to forepaw stimulation broken down into four quadrants based on the location of the electrode wire (top left inset: quadrant locations), pre- and post- spinal transection. (E) Depiction of the rat sensorimotor cortex after complete spinal transection followed by 12 weeks of combined therapies. X-axis is the rostrocaudal distance from bregma, y-axis is the mediolateral distance from bregma. Red represents expansion of motor cortex, purple represents expansion of forelimb somatosensory cortex into the deafferented hindlimb cortex. Red dashed lines represent the extent of sensorimotor overlap within the confines of our recorded regions. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23532.011

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Characterization of neuronal responses to forepaw placement on the treadmill during locomotion.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

The top panel shows three representative peri-stimulus time histograms (PSTHs) generated around forepaw placement on the treadmill during locomotion. Note that these responses indicate that the neurons can have very different types of responses during normal locomotion. The peak latency represents the time of the bin containing the peak of the response relative to foot placement (which occurs at time T = 0, red arrow). The bottom panel shows the spread of the peak latencies of all neurons recorded from the hindlimb sensorimotor cortex to forepaw placement during normal locomotion.

We recorded an average of 33.4 ± 9.7 neurons per day per animal, with no differences in the number of neurons recorded between animals that received complete or sham therapy (two-way ANOVA, therapy: F(1,43)=0.1, p=0.77) or across evaluation days (time: F(3,43)=0.3, p=0.79). However, the percentage of responding cells was highly dependent on whether transected animals received complete or sham therapy (two-way GZLM, therapy x time, Wald χ2(3)=12.7, p=0.0054). Namely, even though the two groups were expectedly not different at week zero before the spinal transection (Tukey, p=0.42), the percentage of responding cells became significantly higher in animals that received complete therapy compared to sham therapy at week 4 (p=0.0128), week 8 (p<0.0001) and week 12 (p<0.0001) after spinal cord transection (Figure 5B).

We then exploited the 4-by-4-matrix arrangement of our electrode arrays to test whether this increased responsiveness of the hindlimb somatosensory cortex to forelimb stimuli, induced by therapy, followed a specific spatial topography. We found that in animals that received complete therapy after spinal cord transection, the probability to record responsive cells expanded toward more medial and more rostral electrodes (Figure 5C). In fact, the response magnitude (spikes/stimulus) of the recorded cells specifically increased in the medial-rostral quadrant of the array (Q2; t-test, p=0.002; bonferroni-corrected alpha = 0.0125) but not in the other quadrants (p>0.118; Figure 5D).

This medial-rostral expansion of the forelimb somatosensory representation nicely overlaps with the expanded trunk motor representation we described above, suggesting that integrated sensorimotor cortical reorganization is likely to contribute to the motor recovery induced by therapy after spinal cord transection (Figure 5E). Importantly, neurons in the hindlimb cortex were responsive to forepaw placement during normal treadmill locomotion, with their peak response latency being distributed across a broad range of phases in the locomotion cycle (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). This broad normal responsiveness provides a possible functional substrate for the cortical reorganization after spinal cord injury

Discussion

The main result of the present work is that completely spinalized adult rats can recover unassisted weight-supported hindlimb stepping without explicit spinal transmission of motor commands through the lesion. This apparently counterintuitive locomotor recovery was achieved with combinations of therapies that lead to substantial reorganization of the motor cortex, including significant expansion of the trunk representation into deafferented hindlimb cortex. This cortical reorganization is responsible for the motor recovery, as supported both by indirect correlative evidence and more directly by the observation that lesioning the reorganized cortex reverses the motor recovery (experiment 1; Figures 13). We further show that this cortical reorganization is associated with sprouting of cortico-spinal axons within the cord above the level of the lesion (experiment 2; Figure 4) and expansion of intact forelimb somatosensory representation toward the reorganized hindlimb motor cortex (experiment 3; Figure 5). These results suggest the creation of a novel cortical sensorimotor circuit that responds to inputs from the ventral forepaws and activates trunk muscles that likely span the lesion, causally subserving the observed behavioral recovery via putative biomechanical coupling. The adult nervous system is thus able to develop a strategy to bypass the lesion and enable – at least partial – recovery of hindlimb locomotion after complete spinal cord injury.

How can weight-supported hindlimb stepping be recovered after complete spinal cord injury?

Several previous studies were able to improve hindlimb locomotion after complete spinal cord injury in adult rats, either using sensorimotor rehabilitation(de Leon et al., 2002; Alluin et al., 2015), administration of serotonergic agonists (Feraboli-Lohnherr et al., 1999; Antri et al., 2002, Antri et al., 2005), transplants below the level of the lesion (Giménez Y Ribotta et al., 2000, Sławińska et al., 2013) or combined pharmacological and electrical stimulation (Gerasimenko et al., 2007; Ichiyama et al., 2008; Courtine et al., 2009; Musienko et al., 2011). However, all previous interventions required either tail pinching, perineal stimulation and/or some level of assisted vertical weight support to achieve hindlimb locomotion in adult spinalized rats. In our study, during treadmill testing and open-field evaluation our animals received no form of peripheral stimulation or assisted vertical weight support.

At first glance, this recovery of weight-supported hindlimb stepping after complete spinal cord transection seems paradoxical. Apart from improbable regeneration across the lesion, the immediate most logical explanation is to think about residual connections (Waxman, 1989; Courtine et al., 2008; Cowley et al., 2010; Flynn et al., 2011; Zaporozhets et al., 2011; Shah et al., 2013; Filli et al., 2014), but this possibility is excluded by both physiological and anatomical evidence in our experiments: (a) no movement whatsoever could be elicited below the level of the lesion by microstimulation of the hindlimb motor cortex; (b) the completeness of the spinal lesion was verified by both nissl and the more sensitive 5-HT staining in all animals at the end of the study.

An alternative explanation is suggested by the functional expansion of the trunk cortical representation. Trunk muscles do extend from above to below the level of the injury, thus providing a biomechanical substrate for bypassing the lesion. The idea is that our treated animals learned to activate the trunk musculature to reduce the load on the hindlimbs while balancing on the forelimbs, which allowed the central-pattern-generators below the level of the lesion to be activated, resulting in weight-supported hindlimb stepping. This explanation is supported by previous observations in neonatally spinalized rats (Giszter et al., 2007, 2008, 2010), which can develop unassisted weight-supported stepping (Stelzner et al., 1975; Weber and Stelzner, 1977; Commissiong and Sauve, 1993; Kim et al., 2001). Importantly, the motor recovery of neonatally spinalized rats is not due to any reconnection within the spinal cord (Cummings et al., 1981; Tillakaratne et al., 2010), but is instead causally related to sensorimotor cortical reorganization (Giszter et al., 1998, 2008; Kao et al., 2009, 2011; Moxon et al., 2013; Oza and Giszter, 2015; Udoekwere et al., 2016). Until now, recovery of unassisted weight-supported hindlimb stepping after complete spinal cord injury was believed to remain restricted to neonatally spinalized rats, due to the higher cortical plasticity at neonatal age compared to adulthood (Oza and Giszter, 2015; Udoekwere et al., 2016). Our results show that carefully designed combinations of pharmaceutical and physical therapies after spinal cord transection can produce sufficient cortical plasticity to recover unassisted weight supported hindlimb stepping after complete spinal cord injury in adult rats.

Cortical plasticity after spinal cord injury

Plasticity can refer to neural reorganization for at least two levels: (i) structural changes at the synaptic-to-cellular level, or (ii) changes in receptive field properties of neurons at the cellular-to-population level (Buonomano and Merzenich, 1998; Moxon and Foffani, 2015). Our results clarify how cortical plasticity after spinal cord injury encompasses both levels.

Cellular-to-population plasticity is demonstrated in our experiments by the expansion of both the trunk motor representation and the forelimb somatosensory representation into the deafferented hindlimb cortex. This overlap of sensorimotor reorganization unifies previous studies on somatosensory (Endo et al., 2007; Kao et al., 2009; Ghosh et al., 2010; Ganzer et al., 2013; Graziano et al., 2013; Humanes-Valera et al., 2017) or motor (Fouad et al., 2001; Giszter et al., 2008; van den Brand et al., 2012; Oza and Giszter, 2014, 2015; Ganzer et al., 2016) reorganization into a joint framework, and is likely to be a key factor for the observed functional recovery. Indeed, lesioning the reorganized cortex reversed the recovery of hindlimb function, without affecting forelimb function. Intriguingly, our cortical lesion did not produce any evident functional impairment in naïve non-spinal-cord-injured animals, in agreement with recent data suggesting that motor cortex is required for learning but not for executing a learned motor skill in normal rats (Kawai et al., 2015) and with the view that in rodents the motor cortex is not required for normal locomotion (Courtine et al., 2007). Crucially, our data suggest that the motor cortex is indeed required to sustain the recovered locomotion after complete spinal cord injury.

We admittedly did not perform motor and somatosensory maps in the same animals, nor did we attempt to selectively deactivate motor or somatosensory circuits, so we could not disentangle motor vs. sensory contributions of cortical reorganization to the observed recovery. This disentanglement might be important to understand why some animals achieved weight-supported stepping and other did not, which currently remains unclear. In any case, the relationship between cortical plasticity and functional recovery in our adult spinalized rats is very similar to neonatally spinalized rats, suggesting that combinations of therapies after spinal cord injury might be able to rescue at least some features of critical period plasticity (Pizzorusso et al., 2002; Maya Vetencourt et al., 2008; Murphy and Corbett, 2009; Yamahachi et al., 2009).

Synaptic-to-cellular plasticity is demonstrated in our experiments by the sprouting of cortico-spinal axons within the cord above the level of the lesion. This cortico-spinal sprouting is in agreement with many previous studies in several models of spinal cord injury (Fouad et al., 2001; Weidner et al., 2001; Bareyre et al., 2004; Barritt et al., 2006; Vavrek et al., 2006; Girgis et al., 2007; García-Alías et al., 2009; Sasaki et al., 2009; Ghosh et al., 2010; Lee et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2011; Floriddia et al., 2012; Scali et al., 2013; Du et al., 2015). Future work should expand on the molecular mechanisms of this cortical plasticity, establishing the role of pre-synaptic and post-synaptic processes as well as the possible contribution of astrocytes and neuron-glia interaction in the cortical reorganization observed here. Overall, even though subcortical plasticity is likely to have contributed to the observed functional recovery (Kambi et al., 2014; Alonso-Calviño et al., 2016), our results suggest that at least part of the plasticity induced by our therapies after spinal cord injury is genuinely cortical.

Pathophysiological implications

Our main result that cortical plasticity, induced in adult spinalized rats, supports recovery of weight-supported hindlimb stepping has several pathophysiological implications.

First, it is important to acknowledge the potential translational limitation of our study focusing on quadrupedal instead of bipedal stepping. Indeed, the novel cortical sensorimotor circuit developed in our rats – integrating sensory information from the ventral forepaws and motor control to the muscles of the spine – suggests that the closed-loop control of trunk musculature that is sufficient for quadrupedal functional improvement might be irrelevant for bipedal locomotion. However, it can be argued that patients attempting to recover locomotion after a spinal cord injury make significant use of the upper limbs to handle their walking aids, so that locomotion behavior switches from the normal bipedal pattern to an assisted quasi-quadrupedal pattern (Del-Ama et al., 2014). From this quasi-quadrupedal perspective, this novel supraspinal control of trunk stability – with possible involvement of intersegmental reflexes (Tani et al., 1997) – might become critical to achieve locomotion in recovering patients with spinal cord injury, as in our animals.

Second, from a reductionist perspective, it is tempting – and indeed valuable – to investigate the potential functional impact of different therapies delivered one at a time. However, therapeutic interventions after spinal cord injury can act at multiple levels of the sensorimotor system (Onifer et al., 2011; Musienko et al., 2012; van den Brand et al., 2012), including the skeletomuscular system (Hangartner et al., 1994; Lauer et al., 2011), muscular-spinal reflex circuits (Mello et al., 2004; Hamid and Hayek, 2008; Phadke et al., 2009; Rayegani et al., 2011), and neural circuits within the spinal cord (Liu et al., 2010a; Côté et al., 2011; Keeler et al., 2012). Consequently, when combined together, different therapies can have redundant or synergistic effects on cortical plasticity and recovery (Foffani et al., 2016). The present results support the view that well-designed combinations of therapies should be employed to maximize corticospinal plasticity and functional outcome (Thuret et al., 2006; Hollis et al., 2016).

Finally, our study substantially raises the standard of behavioral recovery that adult rats can achieve after a spinal cord injury without any spinal transmission of signals through the lesioned cord (and without any external support or sensory stimulation). Any work attempting to restore sensorimotor communication through the lesion after spinal cord injury should carefully consider the possibility that at least part of the functional recovery might be unrelated to the restored spinal communication.

Conclusion

Overall, our results show that careful combinations of pharmacological and physical therapies in adult rats can create a novel cortical sensorimotor circuit that is able to bypass the lesion – probably through biomechanical coupling – to partly recover unassisted hindlimb locomotion after complete spinal cord injury. These results demonstrate the importance of taking advantage of plasticity along the entire neural axis when developing therapies to optimize recovery of function after severe spinal cord injury.

Materials and methods

Experimental design

A total of 74 adult female Sprague-Dawley rats weighing 250–300 g were used in this study, divided in three sets of experiments: 54 for experiment 1 (behavioral assessment followed by motor mapping followed by cortical lesioning), 6 for experiment 2 (tract tracing) and 14 for experiment 3 (somatosensory maps). Animals were single housed and experiments were performed during the light cycle. All animal procedures were conducted in accordance with Drexel University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee-approved protocols.

Experiment 1

Animals received a complete spinal cord transection at thoracic level T8/T9 and were divided randomly into three groups: (1) animals treated with drugs and passive hindlimb bike exercise (‘partial therapy’, n = 15); (2) animals treated with drugs, passive hindlimb bike exercise and active treadmill training (‘complete therapy’, n = 15); (3) animal that receive sham drug (saline) and sham exercise therapy (‘sham therapy’, n = 15). Note that animals that received partial therapy also received sham-treadmill therapy. Hindlimb motor function was assessed as percentage of weight supported step cycles (%WSS) during treadmill locomotion (at 4, 8 and 12 weeks post transection), and in the open field using BBB scores (at 2, 4, 8 and 12 weeks post transection, normalized to group-averages at assess effects of therapy). One animal that underwent sham therapy died at week-12, so it was excluded from the analyses. Two animals under partial therapy had no treadmill data at week-8, so they were excluded from the %WSS analysis. During week-13 after the spinal transection, a subset of these animals were anesthetized and the motor cortex was mapped using intracortical micro-stimulation to identify changes in motor representations (complete therapy, n = 14; partial therapy, n = 12, sham therapy, n = 10). Reorganized areas were identified in this manner and bilaterally lesioned (complete therapy, n = 12; partial therapy, n = 10). All animals that received cortical lesions were given therapy for an additional 2 weeks and then were behavioral re-evaluated with %WSS and BBB scores. Two additional groups of intact naïve animals were used for control experiments (n = 5 for motor maps, n = 4 for cortical lesions).

Experiment 2

Animals received a complete spinal cord transection at thoracic level (T8/T9) and either complete therapy (n = 2) or sham therapy (n = 2). Anterograde tract tracing was performed by injecting a tracer into the reorganized deafferented hindlimb motor cortex 9 weeks after the spinal transection. Animals continued therapy (complete or sham) and were perfused 3 weeks later. Axons were counted in the thoracic gray matter of the spinal hemicord above the level of the lesion, contralateral to the injected cortex. A group of naïve intact animals (n = 2) was used as a control.

Experiment 3

Animals were chronically and bilaterally implanted with 4 × 4 16-channels arrays of microelectrodes in the hindlimb somatosensory cortex, were then spinally transected and received either complete therapy (n = 8) or sham therapy (n = 6). Somatosensory maps of the responses of hindlimb neurons to forelimb stimuli were performed in anesthetized conditions before (week 0) and 4, 8, and 12 weeks after spinal cord transection. Note that five animals were excluded from the study between week-8 and week-12 due to experimental complications (loss of head-cap, bladder infection or severe skin lesions). 11 animals were also recorded during treadmill locomotion.

Spinal cord transection and animal care

Complete mid-thoracic spinal cord transection was performed with similar methods as in our previous studies (Knudsen et al., 2012; Manohar et al., 2012; Ganzer et al., 2013; Graziano et al., 2013; Foffani et al., 2016; Ganzer et al., 2016). Animals were anesthetized with 5% isoflurane and 2 L/min of oxygen and maintained at 2–3% isoflurane with 1-liter oxygen for the duration of the surgery. A laminectomy at the T8/9 level exposed one spinal cord segment. A #10 scalpel blade was used to open the dura and pia mater and #11 scalpel blade was used to make the complete transection of the spinal cord. A fine-tipped microaspiration device was then used to remove 2–3 mm of spinal cord. A collagen matrix, Vitrogen (Cohesion Technology, Encinitas, CA), was injected into the site of the transection. Following recovery from surgery, animals were given an IM injection of the antibiotic Pen-G and 5 ml of lactated ringer subcutaneously animals and returned to their home cages.

Animals were housed two per cage with highly absorbent Alpha-Dri bedding (Shepherd Specialty Papers Inc. Kalamazoo, MI) and cages were kept on warm water blankets. Animals were housed under a 12 hr light/dark cycle (lights on at 07:00) with ad libitum access to food and water. In addition, bladder care was given three times daily for 2 weeks or until bladder control was regained. At the sign of infection, the rats were given subcutaneous injections of Baytril (0.06 mg/kg) once a day for 7 days.

Therapies

Drug administration

Drug administration was similar to our previous studies (Ganzer et al., 2013, 2016). 5-HT agonists were dissolved in sterile physiological saline. Quipazine was injected intraperitoneally and 8-OH-DPAT subcutaneously. Animals were injected once per day 5 days per week beginning 2 weeks after the spinal transection and continuing until week-12. The 2 week lag time post injury allowed time for 5-HT receptor upregulation in the spinal cord caudal to the lesion (Kim et al., 2001). Our chosen dose of drug was a combined injection of 0.125 mg/kg of quipazine (1 mg/ml) and 0.125 mg/kg of 8-OH-DPAT (1 mg/ml), as this dose has a maximal effect on cortical reorganization (Ganzer et al., 2013). Sham therapy animals received an equivalent volume of saline injections.

Passive hindlimb bike exercise

Passive hindlimb bike exercise was similar to our previous studies (Ganzer et al., 2013; Graziano et al., 2013; Foffani et al., 2016; Ganzer et al., 2016). Animals received this exercise three times a week (Monday, Wednesday and Friday) starting 1 week after the spinal transection, using a custom built motor-driven cycling apparatus. Rats were suspended horizontally with their feet secured to the pedals. Cycling speed was maintained at 45 revolutions/min and each exercise bout consisted of two 30 min exercise periods with a 10 min rest period in between. Sham exercise consisted of placing the animals on the bicycles for 70 min while the pedals were stationary.

Active treadmill training

Active treadmill training was an improved version of what we used in previous studies (Kao et al., 2009; Foffani et al., 2016), such that the ability of the rats to accept hindlimb loading was tested weekly (Timoszyk et al., 2005). Animals received treadmill training 10 min after the drugs were injected each day. Rats were placed in a cloth harness with a Velcro strap and attached to an arm of a device that provided lateral and vertical weight assist. The arm was attached to a spring on the other end, which could be extended by turning a knob (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). In order to vertically support the rat’s weight the spring was extended until the moment of the forces acting on either ends of the arm were balanced. The device was calibrated to obtain the percentage of the rat’s body weight supported. At the start of each week, vertical weight assist was ramped down. As the hindlimbs were loaded with more of the animal’s weight, they typically started taking steps. At a certain level of weight assist, the animal was unable to bear its own weight and failed to step with its hindlimbs for more than three consecutive step cycles. The assisted vertical weight support was set right above this value, which was defined as load-bearing failure point (Timoszyk et al., 2005). Two weeks after the spinal transection, animals were not able to step with their hindlimbs, so the initial failure point was set at the assisted weight support that allowed animals to maintain a quadrupedal position on the treadmill, supporting part of their weight with the forelimbs. This initial failure point at week-2 corresponded to the weight of the animal, used to calibrate the device, less the amount of vertical support provided by the forelimbs on the treadmill, which we could not measure. Therefore, the assisted vertical weight support at failure point was expressed as a percentage of the initial failure point at week-2 (note that 100% assisted weight support is upper bounded by the weight of the animal). Failure point was evaluated in all animals (i.e. also in animals that received sham therapy or partial therapy and thus did not receive treadmill training).

Behavioral assessment

Behavioral measures in all groups were always performed on drugs (i.e. after acute administration of the same 5-HT agonists used for therapy), in order to guarantee the functional state of the cord below the level of the lesion (Barbeau and Rossignol, 1990; Jackson and White, 1990). We did not use tail pinch or perineal stimulation.

Treadmill testing (% weight-supported step cycles)

Treadmill testing was performed without vertical support for all therapy groups, after administration of drug. Testing sessions were video-taped to determine the number of step cycles for which the animal could support its own weight at weeks 4, 8 and 12 post spinal cord injury (SCI). The recovery of hindlimb locomotor function was assessed using the number of weight supported step cycles (WSS) taken by the rat when attached to the weight assist device and placed in a neutral position on a moving treadmill. At the neutral position, the height of the rat above the treadmill was adjusted till the forepaws made complete plantar contact with the surface, and the length of the spring was adjusted till the arm was level and provided no additional force on the rat in the vertical direction (i.e. no vertical assist). The %WSS was calculated as the number of weight supported step cycles over the first 100 step cycles taken. The start of a step cycle was determined from forepaw footfalls. A cycle was defined as weight supported if for each hindpaw step the following criteria were fulfilled: (a) the hindquarters were elevated off the treadmill surface, (b) the hindpaw was placed underneath with visible muscle contraction, (c) the knee did not touch the treadmill belt, (d) the plantar surface of the hindpaw made contact with the treadmill during lift off and (e) again reestablished after the hindlimb was advanced. The experimenter performing the assessment from the videotape was blind to the group of the animal. During treadmill testing, animals did not receive any assisted vertical weight support, so the %WSS was used to directly measured the ability of animals to support their weight during hindlimb locomotion.

Open field locomotion (open field score)

All therapy groups were evaluated for recovery of locomotor function during overground-locomotion using the Basso, Beattie and Bresnahan (BBB) open field score at weeks 2, 4, 8 and 12 post- SCI (Basso et al., 1995). The open field score, which was always estimated over 4 min, was normalized at weeks 4, 8 and 12 using the average score at week two for each group (after verifying that the was no difference among groups at week 2). During each evaluation week, the animals were tested 5 min after drug administration. Sham therapy animals received a challenge dose of drug prior to BBB testing. BBB scores from 0 to 8 are non-weight supporting, while scores of 9 to 21 indicate hindquarter weight support. Three experimenters performed the evaluation, one placed the animal in the open field and the other two were blind to the group of the animal and performed the scoring.

Motor mapping

Motor mapping was performed during week-13 after the spinal transection using intracortical microstimulation with methods similar to our previous study (Ganzer et al., 2016). Animals were anesthetized with a combination of Ketamine (50 mg/kg), Xylazine (5 mg/kg) and Acepromazine (0.75 mg/kg) and placed in a stereotaxic frame. Supplemental doses of anesthetic were administered as needed. Craniotomies were performed over the right cortex to expose the medial post-bregma area. Electrode penetrations were defined using stereotaxic coordinates (Leergaard et al., 2004) on a cortical grid (coordinates AP: −0.5 to 2 mm posterior to bregma; ML: 1 to 3.5 lateral to midline). Electrode penetrations were made at 500–600 µm intervals within the medial post-bregma area. Care was taken to avoid surface cortical vasculature during mapping. A low impedance glass-insulated tungsten microelectrode (500 kΩ; FHC Inc.; Bowdoin, ME) was mounted on a stereotaxic electrode manipulator. In order to assess microstimulation waveform quality, a 100 kΩ resistor was connected to a grounding screw adjacent to the craniotomies in series with the stimulator (AM systems; Sequim, WA). The dura was removed and the microelectrode was lowered, perpendicular to the surface of the brain, to penetrate the pia. The microelectrode was then slowly inserted into the brain to a depth of ~1,600 µm, corresponding to cortical layer V/VI. Stimulation parameters consisted of 0.2 ms duration constant current bipolar pulses (anodal leading), at 333 Hz in trains of 300 ms duration. At each penetration site, the stimulation current gradually increased from 0 µA until a movement was evoked (current threshold). Movement was assessed by visual inspection. Penetrations were marked as non-responsive when no movement could be evoked with stimulation up to 100 µA. Motor maps were conducted with two experimenters. The first experimenter placed the electrode and recorded the data for each site. The second experimenter was kept blind to the experimental group of the animal and electrode placement to avoid potential biasing. The second experimenter delivered stimuli while observing which parts of the body moved in response to stimulation.

The expansion of intact motor representations into the deafferented hindlimb motor cortex was assessed by calculating the area (mm2) devoted to a movement type (trunk, hindlimb, forelimb, vibrissae) using custom MATLAB algorithms. Small shifts in this grid to avoid surface cortical vasculature during mapping were corrected for during processing. For each animal, the area of specific representations (trunk, hindlimb, forelimb, vibrissae) was determined by multiplying the number of responsive sites evoking the corresponding movement type by 0.25 mm2, using the method of (Ramanathan et al., 2006).

Electrolytic cortical lesions

At the end of the motor mapping procedure, the reorganized area was bilaterally lesioned. Twisted pair stainless steel electrodes of 0.35 mm diameter insulated except at the tip was lowered to a depth of 1.65 mm below the surface of the brain at a stereotaxic coordinate that is 2 mm lateral to the midline and 1 mm posterior to bregma. A constant current of amplitude 500μA for 10 s was sent through the bipolar leads of the electrode. The electrodes were removed and the surface of the brain was covered with gelfoam and dental acrylic. Lesions were also performed in the same stereotactic location for naïve intact animals.

Anterograde axonal tract tracing

Anterograde tract tracing was performed 9 weeks after the spinal transection. Rats were anesthetized and fixed in a stereotactic frame. A craniotomy was performed over the medial post-bregma area in the right hemisphere (0–2 mm posterior and 1–3 mm lateral to bregma). Six injections of the anterograde tracer, fluorescein-biotin-dextran (10% in PBS, 10 kDa molecular weight BDA, Mini Emerald, Invitrogen) was made to cover the de-afferented hindlimb region. All injections were at a depth of 1.5 mm below the surface of the cortex. Small holes in the dura were made before inserting the 33 gauge, 10 μl syringe (Hamilton Co.) controlled by an electrical pump (World Precision Instruments). The needle was left in place 5 min after insertion, followed by injection of tracer and again for 10 min after each injection to prevent backflow of tracer. Each injection consisted of 400 nl of tracer at a flow rate of 15 nl/min. After the last injection, the brain was covered with gelfoam and dental acrylic. Animals were returned to their therapy regime (sham, partial or complete therapy) and the tracer was allowed to be transported for 3 weeks before the animal was perfused at the end of 12 weeks after the spinal transection.

Animals were perfused transcardially with buffered saline (100 ml), followed by buffered 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.2 M PBS (500 ml). The thoracic cord (T1-T8) was removed, retaining the T1, T4 and T7 roots and post-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 24 hr and finally cryoprotected in 30% sucrose until the tissue sank. Specimens were frozen in Shandon M-1 Embedding Matrix compound (Thermo Scientific) and sectioned on a freezing microtome at 25 µm. Labeled axons exiting the corticospinal tract were counted from five slices for each animal around T1, T4 and T7 similar to previous studies (Carmel et al., 2010; van den Brand et al., 2012). Slices used for counting were spaced at least 250 microns apart. For each slice, a region of interest was selected that included 2/3 of the spinal hemicord gray matter contralateral to the injected cortex, excluding only the most dorsal aspects of the dorsal horn and the most ventral aspect of the ventral horn (no labeled axons were found in these regions for any animal group). Stereo Investigator stereology program (MBF Bioscience) was used to sample the grey matter and count axons. Because cortico-spinal tract BDA-labeled terminations were sparse, we used relatively dense sampling parameters: square counting frames 50 µm on each side, resulting in sampling 50% of the region of interest per section. Data are reported as cumulative axon counts per area per slice. A group of naïve intact animals (n = 2) was used as a control.

Somatosensory mapping

Somatosensory mapping was performed with similar techniques as in our previous studies (Foffani et al., 2004; Tutunculer et al., 2006; Moxon et al., 2008; Kao et al., 2011; Knudsen et al., 2012; Manohar et al., 2012; Knudsen et al., 2014). Microelectrode arrays were chronically implanted under general anesthesia (2–3% isoflurane in O2 delivered via orotracheal intubation) and aseptic conditions. More specifically, 4 × 4 arrays of 50 μm Teflon-insulated, stainless steel microwires (MicroProbes for Life Sciences) were bilaterally implanted to the infragranular layers (1.3–1.5 mm) of the rat hindlimb representation within the sensorimotor cortex (Leergaard et al., 2004).

On the days of somatosensory mapping, animals were lightly anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (Nembutal) with an induction dosage of 35 mg/kg and maintained at stage III-2 level of anesthesia (Friedberg et al., 1999; Erchova et al., 2002). They were responsive to toe pinch and corneal reflexes. Seven sparse locations were stimulated on each forelimb including two digits, two palm pads on the forepaw and wrist, elbow and shoulder. Each location was consecutively tapped 100 times at 0.5 Hz with a fine-tipped metal probe, controlled by a precision stepper motor (Gemini GV6). To ensure that only tactile receptors at the sight of contact were activated and to control the magnitude of the stimuli at each location, the metal probe was first positioned on the skin, ensuring contact but no visual indentation under 10X magnification. The metal probe was then moved 0.5 mm away from the skin, and the stimulation was started. The effect of the stimulus was viewed under 10X magnification to ensure no movement of the digits or limb. All locations were tapped within the same recording session to ensure that the same neurons were recorded in response to stimulation of all locations. All 100 stimuli were given to a location, and then the stimulator was moved to the next location. The motor stimulator simultaneously sent a TTL-pulse to the data acquisition system to record the timestamp of the stimulus onset. Single neurons were discriminated by hand using a combined thresholding and real-time PCA analysis of waveform features. Neurons were re-discriminated before every recording session to ensure only single units were recorded. Neural signals were acquired (40 kHz) with a neurophysiological recording system (Plexon). The waveforms and action potential timestamps of all the discriminated neurons were recorded, and stored for further analysis.

The responses of neurons to sensory stimuli were quantified using peri-stimulus time histograms (PSTHs,[Tutunculer et al., 2006]). A PSTH is built as a histogram of spike times relative to the sensory stimulus (time-zero) averaged across trials (100 trials, 1 ms binsize). A neural response was considered significant if (a) it exceeded a threshold set as the average background activity of the neuron (evaluated from 100 to 5 ms before the stimulus) plus three standard deviations, (b) at least three bins (at 1 ms binsize) were over the threshold, and (c) the spiking activity between the first and the last significant bin was significantly greater than the background activity (non-paired t-test, p<0.001). Response magnitude was quantified as the background-subtracted average number of spikes per stimulus in the 5–50 ms post-stimulus window of the PSTH.

Awake recordings

Neuronal activity was also recorded while animals locomoted on the treadmill (n = 11). Chronically implanted electrodes were connected to headstages and tethered to the Plexon recording system. Neurons were discriminated before each recording session. A mirror was placed behind the animal and the entire session was videotaped in order to extract the timestamps of paw placements. The recording lasted 10 mins. The timestamps on the video recording were synchronized with the neural recording system.

PSTHs were generated around the forepaw footfalls using timestamps from the video analysis of treadmill locomotion using a 10 ms bin size. Background firing activity of each neuron was calculated as the firing rate in the entire recording duration. The PSTHs were smoothed in a 500 ms window (250 ms before and after paw placement) using a sliding-window zero-phase filter of length five bins. To see if the neuron was responsive to paw placements, a threshold was defined as the 99% confidence interval above the mean firing rate of the cell. The peak of the response was the highest bin that crossed the threshold and the response was defined as all the consecutive bins around the peak bin that were over the threshold. The response of the cell was calculated between these bins from the unsmoothed PSTH. Responses were considered significant if at at least three consecutive bins exceeded the 99% confidence interval threshold.

Perfusion and histology of the spinal cord

Animals for Experiments 1 and 3 were perfused transcardially with buffered saline (100 ml), followed by buffered 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.2 M PBS (500 ml). For animals that underwent cortical lesioning, the brain and spinal cord were then removed, post-fixed in the same buffered 4% paraformaldehyde for 24 hr and finally cryoprotected in 30% sucrose until the tissue sank. Specimens were frozen in Shandon M-1 Embedding Matrix compound (Thermo Scientific, Waltham, MA) and sectioned on a freezing microtome at 40 µm. The transection segments of the spinal cords were sectioned horizontally into five sets. One set was Nissl–myelin stained and the resulting sections were examined under a microscope to confirm completeness of the transection. A second set was stained with a polyclonal antibody to 5-HT. Frozen sections were incubated at 4°C with the primary antibody (diluted 1:40,000 Immunostar, Stillwater, MN) for 16 hr, with biotinylated goat anti-rabbit IgG for 2 hr, and with avidin-biotinylated horseradish peroxidase complex for 2 hr, as specified by the manufacturer (ABC Standard Kit; Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA). Peroxidase reactivity was visualized with 0.05% diaminobenzidine tetrahydrochloride and 0.01% hydrogen peroxide in 0.05 mM Tris buffer.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using two-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) or one-way multivariate ANOVA. A squared-root transformation was used when necessary to guarantee normality and homoscedasticity. Binary neural data (responding/ nonresponding neurons, sample size is number of neurons) were entered into generalized linear models (GZLM) with binomial distribution and logit link function. GZLMs allow binary data to be rigorously analyzed with ANOVA-like designs. Tukey’s test or Fisher’s test were used for post-hoc comparisons. Correlations were assessed with Pearson correlation coefficient and with robust regression methods, using a bi-square fit. All results were considered significant at p<0.05.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by grant NS096971 by the National Institutes of Health,grant 1402984 by National Science Foundation and grant 85900 by the Shriner's Hospital for Children. Thanks to Gary Blumenthal, Chimela Nwaobasi and Timur Litvinov and for assistance with animal care, behavior and video analysis.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Science Foundation CBET 1402984 to Karen A Moxon.

  • National Institutes of Health NS096971 to Karen A Moxon.

  • Shriners Hospital for Children 85900 to Karen A Moxon.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

AM, Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

GF, Formal analysis, Supervision, Investigation, Visualization, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

PDG, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

JRB, Supervision, Investigation, Writing—review and editing.

KAM, Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All animal procedures were conducted in accordance with Drexel University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee-approved protocols (Protocol# 20069 and Protocol#18374).

References

  1. Alluin O, Delivet-Mongrain H, Rossignol S. Inducing hindlimb locomotor recovery in adult rat after complete thoracic spinal cord section using repeated treadmill training with perineal stimulation only. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2015;114:1931–1946. doi: 10.1152/jn.00416.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alonso-Calviño E, Martínez-Camero I, Fernández-López E, Humanes-Valera D, Foffani G, Aguilar J. Increased responses in the somatosensory thalamus immediately after spinal cord injury. Neurobiology of Disease. 2016;87:39–49. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2015.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Antri M, Barthe JY, Mouffle C, Orsal D. Long-lasting recovery of locomotor function in chronic spinal rat following chronic combined pharmacological stimulation of serotonergic receptors with 8-OHDPAT and quipazine. Neuroscience Letters. 2005;384:162–167. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.04.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Antri M, Orsal D, Barthe JY. Locomotor recovery in the chronic spinal rat: effects of long-term treatment with a 5-HT2 agonist. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;16:467–476. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02088.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barbeau H, Rossignol S. The effects of serotonergic drugs on the locomotor pattern and on cutaneous reflexes of the adult chronic spinal cat. Brain Research. 1990;514:55–67. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90435-E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bareyre FM, Kerschensteiner M, Raineteau O, Mettenleiter TC, Weinmann O, Schwab ME. The injured spinal cord spontaneously forms a new intraspinal circuit in adult rats. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:269–277. doi: 10.1038/nn1195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Barritt AW, Davies M, Marchand F, Hartley R, Grist J, Yip P, McMahon SB, Bradbury EJ. Chondroitinase ABC promotes sprouting of intact and injured spinal systems after spinal cord injury. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:10856–10867. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2980-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Basso DM, Beattie MS, Bresnahan JC. A sensitive and reliable locomotor rating scale for open field testing in rats. Journal of Neurotrauma. 1995;12:1–21. doi: 10.1089/neu.1995.12.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bensmaia SJ, Miller LE. Restoring sensorimotor function through intracortical interfaces: progress and looming challenges. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2014;15:313–325. doi: 10.1038/nrn3724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bouton CE, Shaikhouni A, Annetta NV, Bockbrader MA, Friedenberg DA, Nielson DM, Sharma G, Sederberg PB, Glenn BC, Mysiw WJ, Morgan AG, Deogaonkar M, Rezai AR. Restoring cortical control of functional movement in a human with quadriplegia. Nature. 2016;533:247–250. doi: 10.1038/nature17435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Buonomano DV, Merzenich MM. Cortical plasticity: from synapses to maps. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1998;21:149–186. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.21.1.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carmel JB, Berrol LJ, Brus-Ramer M, Martin JH. Chronic electrical stimulation of the intact corticospinal system after unilateral injury restores skilled locomotor control and promotes spinal axon outgrowth. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:10918–10926. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1435-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Commissiong JW, Sauve Y. Neurophysiological basis of functional recovery in the neonatal spinalized rat. Experimental Brain Research. 1993;96:473–479. doi: 10.1007/BF00234114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Courtine G, Bunge MB, Fawcett JW, Grossman RG, Kaas JH, Lemon R, Maier I, Martin J, Nudo RJ, Ramon-Cueto A, Rouiller EM, Schnell L, Wannier T, Schwab ME, Edgerton VR. Can experiments in nonhuman primates expedite the translation of treatments for spinal cord injury in humans? Nature Medicine. 2007;13:561–566. doi: 10.1038/nm1595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Courtine G, Gerasimenko Y, van den Brand R, Yew A, Musienko P, Zhong H, Song B, Ao Y, Ichiyama RM, Lavrov I, Roy RR, Sofroniew MV, Edgerton VR. Transformation of nonfunctional spinal circuits into functional states after the loss of brain input. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:1333–1342. doi: 10.1038/nn.2401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Courtine G, Song B, Roy RR, Zhong H, Herrmann JE, Ao Y, Qi J, Edgerton VR, Sofroniew MV. Recovery of supraspinal control of stepping via indirect propriospinal relay connections after spinal cord injury. Nature Medicine. 2008;14:69–74. doi: 10.1038/nm1682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cowley KC, Zaporozhets E, Schmidt BJ. Propriospinal transmission of the locomotor command signal in the neonatal rat. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1198:42–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05421.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Côté MP, Azzam GA, Lemay MA, Zhukareva V, Houlé JD. Activity-dependent increase in neurotrophic factors is associated with an enhanced modulation of spinal reflexes after spinal cord injury. Journal of Neurotrauma. 2011;28:299–309. doi: 10.1089/neu.2010.1594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cummings JP, Bernstein DR, Stelzner DJ. Further evidence that sparing of function after spinal cord transection in the neonatal rat is not due to axonal generation or regeneration. Experimental Neurology. 1981;74:615–620. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(81)90196-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. David S, Aguayo AJ. Axonal elongation into peripheral nervous system "bridges" after central nervous system injury in adult rats. Science. 1981;214:931–933. doi: 10.1126/science.6171034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. de Leon RD, Kubasak MD, Phelps PE, Timoszyk WK, Reinkensmeyer DJ, Roy RR, Edgerton VR. Using robotics to teach the spinal cord to walk. Brain Research Reviews. 2002;40:267–273. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(02)00209-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Del-Ama AJ, Gil-Agudo A, Pons JL, Moreno JC. Hybrid gait training with an overground robot for people with incomplete spinal cord injury: a pilot study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014;8:298. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Du K, Zheng S, Zhang Q, Li S, Gao X, Wang J, Jiang L, Liu K. Pten deletion promotes regrowth of corticospinal tract axons 1 year after spinal cord Injury. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:9754–9763. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3637-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Endo T, Spenger C, Tominaga T, Brené S, Olson L. Cortical sensory map rearrangement after spinal cord injury: fMRI responses linked to Nogo signalling. Brain. 2007;130:2951–2961. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Erchova IA, Lebedev MA, Diamond ME. Somatosensory cortical neuronal population activity across states of anaesthesia. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;15:744–752. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2002.01898.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ethier C, Oby ER, Bauman MJ, Miller LE. Restoration of grasp following paralysis through brain-controlled stimulation of muscles. Nature. 2012;485:368–371. doi: 10.1038/nature10987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Feraboli-Lohnherr D, Barthe JY, Orsal D. Serotonin-induced activation of the network for locomotion in adult spinal rats. Journal of Neuroscience Research. 1999;55:87–98. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4547(19990101)55:1&#x0003c;87::AID-JNR10&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-#. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Filli L, Engmann AK, Zörner B, Weinmann O, Moraitis T, Gullo M, Kasper H, Schneider R, Schwab ME. Bridging the gap: a reticulo-propriospinal detour bypassing an incomplete spinal cord injury. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:13399–13410. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0701-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Floriddia EM, Rathore KI, Tedeschi A, Quadrato G, Wuttke A, Lueckmann JM, Kigerl KA, Popovich PG, Di Giovanni S. p53 regulates the neuronal intrinsic and extrinsic responses affecting the recovery of motor function following spinal cord injury. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:13956–13970. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1925-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Flynn JR, Graham BA, Galea MP, Callister RJ. The role of propriospinal interneurons in recovery from spinal cord injury. Neuropharmacology. 2011;60:809–822. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Foffani G, Shumsky J, Knudsen EB, Ganzer PD, Moxon KA. Interactive effects between exercise and serotonergic pharmacotherapy on cortical reorganization after spinal cord Injury. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair. 2016;30:479–489. doi: 10.1177/1545968315600523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Foffani G, Tutunculer B, Moxon KA. Role of spike timing in the forelimb somatosensory cortex of the rat. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:7266–7271. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2523-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fouad K, Pedersen V, Schwab ME, Brösamle C. Cervical sprouting of corticospinal fibers after thoracic spinal cord injury accompanies shifts in evoked motor responses. Current Biology. 2001;11:1766–1770. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00535-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Friedberg MH, Lee SM, Ebner FF. Modulation of receptive field properties of thalamic somatosensory neurons by the depth of anesthesia. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1999;81:2243–2252. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.5.2243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ganzer PD, Manohar A, Shumsky JS, Moxon KA. Therapy induces widespread reorganization of motor cortex after complete spinal transection that supports motor recovery. Experimental Neurology. 2016;279:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2016.01.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Ganzer PD, Moxon KA, Knudsen EB, Shumsky JS. Serotonergic pharmacotherapy promotes cortical reorganization after spinal cord injury. Experimental Neurology. 2013;241:84–94. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2012.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. García-Alías G, Barkhuysen S, Buckle M, Fawcett JW. Chondroitinase ABC treatment opens a window of opportunity for task-specific rehabilitation. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:1145–1151. doi: 10.1038/nn.2377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Gerasimenko YP, Ichiyama RM, Lavrov IA, Courtine G, Cai L, Zhong H, Roy RR, Edgerton VR. Epidural spinal cord stimulation plus quipazine administration enable stepping in complete spinal adult rats. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;98:2525–2536. doi: 10.1152/jn.00836.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ghosh A, Haiss F, Sydekum E, Schneider R, Gullo M, Wyss MT, Mueggler T, Baltes C, Rudin M, Weber B, Schwab ME. Rewiring of hindlimb corticospinal neurons after spinal cord injury. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:97–104. doi: 10.1038/nn.2448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Giménez Y Ribotta M, Langa F, Menet V, Privat A. Comparative anatomy of the cerebellar cortex in mice lacking vimentin, GFAP, and both vimentin and GFAP. Glia. 2000;31:69–83. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1136(200007)31:1&#x0003c;69::AID-GLIA70&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-W. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Girgis J, Merrett D, Kirkland S, Metz GA, Verge V, Fouad K. Reaching training in rats with spinal cord injury promotes plasticity and task specific recovery. Brain. 2007;130:2993–3003. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Giszter S, Davies MR, Ramakrishnan A, Udoekwere UI, Kargo WJ. Trunk sensorimotor cortex is essential for autonomous weight-supported locomotion in adult rats spinalized as P1/P2 neonates. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2008;100:839–851. doi: 10.1152/jn.00866.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Giszter SF, Davies MR, Graziani V. Motor strategies used by rats spinalized at birth to maintain stance in response to imposed perturbations. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;97:2663–2675. doi: 10.1152/jn.00308.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Giszter SF, Hockensmith G, Ramakrishnan A, Udoekwere UI. How spinalized rats can walk: biomechanics, cortex, and hindlimb muscle scaling--implications for rehabilitation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1198:279–293. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05534.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Giszter SF, Kargo WJ, Davies M, Shibayama M. Fetal transplants rescue axial muscle representations in M1 cortex of neonatally transected rats that develop weight support. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998;80:3021–3030. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.6.3021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Graziano A, Foffani G, Knudsen EB, Shumsky J, Moxon KA. Passive exercise of the hind limbs after complete thoracic transection of the spinal cord promotes cortical reorganization. PLoS One. 2013;8:e54350. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hamid S, Hayek R. Role of electrical stimulation for rehabilitation and regeneration after spinal cord injury: an overview. European Spine Journal. 2008;17:1256–1269. doi: 10.1007/s00586-008-0729-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hangartner TN, Rodgers MM, Glaser RM, Barre PS. Tibial bone density loss in spinal cord injured patients: effects of FES exercise. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development. 1994;31:50–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hollis ER, Ishiko N, Yu T, Lu CC, Haimovich A, Tolentino K, Richman A, Tury A, Wang SH, Pessian M, Jo E, Kolodkin A, Zou Y. Ryk controls remapping of motor cortex during functional recovery after spinal cord injury. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:697–705. doi: 10.1038/nn.4282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Humanes-Valera D, Foffani G, Alonso-Calviño E, Fernández-López E, Aguilar J. Dual cortical plasticity after spinal cord Injury. Cerebral Cortex. 2017;27:2926–2940. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Ichiyama RM, Courtine G, Gerasimenko YP, Yang GJ, van den Brand R, Lavrov IA, Zhong H, Roy RR, Edgerton VR. Step training reinforces specific spinal locomotor circuitry in adult spinal rats. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:7370–7375. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1881-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Jackson DA, White SR. Receptor subtypes mediating facilitation by serotonin of excitability of spinal motoneurons. Neuropharmacology. 1990;29:787–797. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(90)90151-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kambi N, Halder P, Rajan R, Arora V, Chand P, Arora M, Jain N. Large-scale reorganization of the somatosensory cortex following spinal cord injuries is due to brainstem plasticity. Nature Communications. 2014;5:3602. doi: 10.1038/ncomms4602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kao T, Shumsky JS, Knudsen EB, Murray M, Moxon KA. Functional role of exercise-induced cortical organization of sensorimotor cortex after spinal transection. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;106:2662–2674. doi: 10.1152/jn.01017.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kao T, Shumsky JS, Murray M, Moxon KA. Exercise induces cortical plasticity after neonatal spinal cord injury in the rat. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:7549–7557. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2474-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kawai R, Markman T, Poddar R, Ko R, Fantana AL, Dhawale AK, Kampff AR, Ölveczky BP. Motor cortex is required for learning but not for executing a motor skill. Neuron. 2015;86:800–812. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Keeler BE, Liu G, Siegfried RN, Zhukareva V, Murray M, Houlé JD. Acute and prolonged hindlimb exercise elicits different gene expression in motoneurons than sensory neurons after spinal cord injury. Brain Research. 2012;1438:8–21. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kim D, Murray M, Simansky KJ. The serotonergic 5-HT(2C) agonist m-chlorophenylpiperazine increases weight-supported locomotion without development of tolerance in rats with spinal transections. Experimental Neurology. 2001;169:496–500. doi: 10.1006/exnr.2001.7660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Knudsen EB, Flint RD, Moxon KA. Encoding of temporal intervals in the rat hindlimb sensorimotor cortex. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2012;6:67. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2012.00067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Knudsen EB, Powers ME, Moxon KA. Dissociating movement from movement timing in the rat primary motor cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:15576–15586. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1816-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Lauer RT, Smith BT, Mulcahey MJ, Betz RR, Johnston TE. Effects of cycling and/or electrical stimulation on bone mineral density in children with spinal cord injury. Spinal Cord. 2011;49:917–923. doi: 10.1038/sc.2011.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lee H, McKeon RJ, Bellamkonda RV. Sustained delivery of thermostabilized chABC enhances axonal sprouting and functional recovery after spinal cord injury. PNAS. 2010;107:3340–3345. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905437106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Leergaard TB, Alloway KD, Pham TA, Bolstad I, Hoffer ZS, Pettersen C, Bjaalie JG. Three-dimensional topography of corticopontine projections from rat sensorimotor cortex: comparisons with corticostriatal projections reveal diverse integrative organization. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2004;478:306–322. doi: 10.1002/cne.20289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Liu G, Keeler BE, Zhukareva V, Houlé JD. Cycling exercise affects the expression of apoptosis-associated microRNAs after spinal cord injury in rats. Experimental Neurology. 2010a;226:200–206. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2010.08.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Liu K, Lu Y, Lee JK, Samara R, Willenberg R, Sears-Kraxberger I, Tedeschi A, Park KK, Jin D, Cai B, Xu B, Connolly L, Steward O, Zheng B, He Z. PTEN deletion enhances the regenerative ability of adult corticospinal neurons. Nature Neuroscience. 2010b;13:1075–1081. doi: 10.1038/nn.2603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Manohar A, Flint RD, Knudsen E, Moxon KA. Decoding hindlimb movement for a brain machine interface after a complete spinal transection. PLoS One. 2012;7:e52173. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Maya Vetencourt JF, Sale A, Viegi A, Baroncelli L, De Pasquale R, O'Leary OF, Castrén E, Maffei L. The antidepressant fluoxetine restores plasticity in the adult visual cortex. Science. 2008;320:385–388. doi: 10.1126/science.1150516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Mello DM, Nielsen BD, Peters TL, Caron JP, Orth MW. Comparison of inhibitory effects of glucosamine and mannosamine on bovine articular cartilage degradation in vitro. American Journal of Veterinary Research. 2004;65:1440–1445. doi: 10.2460/ajvr.2004.65.1440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Moxon KA, Foffani G. Brain-machine interfaces beyond neuroprosthetics. Neuron. 2015;86:55–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Moxon KA, Hale LL, Aguilar J, Foffani G. Responses of infragranular neurons in the rat primary somatosensory cortex to forepaw and hindpaw tactile stimuli. Neuroscience. 2008;156:1083–1092. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Moxon KA, Kao T, Shumsky JS. Role of cortical reorganization on the effect of 5-HT pharmacotherapy for spinal cord injury. Experimental Neurology. 2013;240:17–27. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2012.10.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Murphy TH, Corbett D. Plasticity during stroke recovery: from synapse to behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:861–872. doi: 10.1038/nrn2735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Musienko P, Heutschi J, Friedli L, van den Brand R, Courtine G. Multi-system neurorehabilitative strategies to restore motor functions following severe spinal cord injury. Experimental Neurology. 2012;235:100–109. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2011.08.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Musienko P, van den Brand R, Märzendorfer O, Roy RR, Gerasimenko Y, Edgerton VR, Courtine G. Controlling specific locomotor behaviors through multidimensional monoaminergic modulation of spinal circuitries. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:9264–9278. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5796-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Onifer SM, Smith GM, Fouad K. Plasticity after spinal cord injury: relevance to recovery and approaches to facilitate it. Neurotherapeutics. 2011;8:283–293. doi: 10.1007/s13311-011-0034-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Oza CS, Giszter SF. Plasticity and alterations of trunk motor cortex following spinal cord injury and non-stepping robot and treadmill training. Experimental Neurology. 2014;256:57–69. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2014.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Oza CS, Giszter SF. Trunk robot rehabilitation training with active stepping reorganizes and enriches trunk motor cortex representations in spinal transected rats. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:7174–7189. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4366-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Phadke CP, Flynn SM, Thompson FJ, Behrman AL, Trimble MH, Kukulka CG. Comparison of single bout effects of bicycle training versus locomotor training on paired reflex depression of the soleus H-reflex after motor incomplete spinal cord injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2009;90:1218–1228. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2009.01.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Pizzorusso T, Medini P, Berardi N, Chierzi S, Fawcett JW, Maffei L. Reactivation of ocular dominance plasticity in the adult visual cortex. Science. 2002;298:1248–1251. doi: 10.1126/science.1072699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Ramanathan D, Conner JM, Tuszynski MH. A form of motor cortical plasticity that correlates with recovery of function after brain injury. PNAS. 2006;103:11370–11375. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601065103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Rayegani SM, Shojaee H, Sedighipour L, Soroush MR, Baghbani M, Amirani OB. The effect of electrical passive cycling on spasticity in war veterans with spinal cord injury. Frontiers in Neurology. 2011;2:39. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2011.00039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Richardson PM, McGuinness UM, Aguayo AJ. Axons from CNS neurons regenerate into PNS grafts. Nature. 1980;284:264–265. doi: 10.1038/284264a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Ruschel J, Hellal F, Flynn KC, Dupraz S, Elliott DA, Tedeschi A, Bates M, Sliwinski C, Brook G, Dobrindt K, Peitz M, Brüstle O, Norenberg MD, Blesch A, Weidner N, Bunge MB, Bixby JL, Bradke F. Axonal regeneration. systemic administration of epothilone B promotes axon regeneration after spinal cord injury. Science. 2015;348:347–352. doi: 10.1126/science.aaa2958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Sasaki M, Radtke C, Tan AM, Zhao P, Hamada H, Houkin K, Honmou O, Kocsis JD. BDNF-hypersecreting human mesenchymal stem cells promote functional recovery, axonal sprouting, and protection of corticospinal neurons after spinal cord injury. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:14932–14941. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2769-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Scali M, Begenisic T, Mainardi M, Milanese M, Bonifacino T, Bonanno G, Sale A, Maffei L. Fluoxetine treatment promotes functional recovery in a rat model of cervical spinal cord injury. Scientific Reports. 2013;3:2217. doi: 10.1038/srep02217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Selzer ME. Promotion of axonal regeneration in the injured CNS. The Lancet Neurology. 2003;2:157–166. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(03)00322-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Shah PK, Garcia-Alias G, Choe J, Gad P, Gerasimenko Y, Tillakaratne N, Zhong H, Roy RR, Edgerton VR. Use of quadrupedal step training to re-engage spinal interneuronal networks and improve locomotor function after spinal cord injury. Brain. 2013;136:3362–3377. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Sławińska U, Miazga K, Cabaj AM, Leszczyńska AN, Majczyński H, Nagy JI, Jordan LM. Grafting of fetal brainstem 5-HT neurons into the sublesional spinal cord of paraplegic rats restores coordinated hindlimb locomotion. Experimental Neurology. 2013;247:572–581. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2013.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Stelzner DJ, Ershler WB, Weber ED. Effects of spinal transection in neonatal and weanling rats: survival of function. Experimental Neurology. 1975;46:156–177. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(75)90039-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Tani T, Yamamoto H, Ichimiya M, Kimura J. Reflexes evoked in human erector spinae muscles by tapping during voluntary activity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Electromyography and Motor Control. 1997;105:194–200. doi: 10.1016/S0924-980X(97)00017-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Thuret S, Moon LD, Gage FH. Therapeutic interventions after spinal cord injury. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7:628–643. doi: 10.1038/nrn1955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Tillakaratne NJ, Guu JJ, de Leon RD, Bigbee AJ, London NJ, Zhong H, Ziegler MD, Joynes RL, Roy RR, Edgerton VR. Functional recovery of stepping in rats after a complete neonatal spinal cord transection is not due to regrowth across the lesion site. Neuroscience. 2010;166:23–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Timoszyk WK, Nessler JA, Acosta C, Roy RR, Edgerton VR, Reinkensmeyer DJ, de Leon R. Hindlimb loading determines stepping quantity and quality following spinal cord transection. Brain Research. 2005;1050:180–189. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.05.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Tutunculer B, Foffani G, Himes BT, Moxon KA. Structure of the excitatory receptive fields of infragranular forelimb neurons in the rat primary somatosensory cortex responding to touch. Cerebral Cortex. 2006;16:791–810. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Udoekwere UI, Oza CS, Giszter SF. Teaching adult rats spinalized as neonates to walk using trunk robotic rehabilitation: elements of success, failure, and dependence. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:8341–8355. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2435-14.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. van den Brand R, Heutschi J, Barraud Q, DiGiovanna J, Bartholdi K, Huerlimann M, Friedli L, Vollenweider I, Moraud EM, Duis S, Dominici N, Micera S, Musienko P, Courtine G. Restoring voluntary control of locomotion after paralyzing spinal cord injury. Science. 2012;336:1182–1185. doi: 10.1126/science.1217416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. van Middendorp JJ, Hosman AJ, Donders AR, Pouw MH, Ditunno JF, Curt A, Geurts AC, Van de Meent H, EM-SCI Study Group A clinical prediction rule for ambulation outcomes after traumatic spinal cord injury: a longitudinal cohort study. The Lancet. 2011;377:1004–1010. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)62276-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Vavrek R, Girgis J, Tetzlaff W, Hiebert GW, Fouad K. BDNF promotes connections of corticospinal neurons onto spared descending interneurons in spinal cord injured rats. Brain. 2006;129:1534–1545. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Wang D, Ichiyama RM, Zhao R, Andrews MR, Fawcett JW. Chondroitinase combined with rehabilitation promotes recovery of forelimb function in rats with chronic spinal cord injury. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:9332–9344. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0983-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Waxman SG. Demyelination in spinal cord injury. Journal of the Neurological Sciences. 1989;91:1–14. doi: 10.1016/0022-510X(89)90072-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Weber ED, Stelzner DJ. Behavioral effects of spinal cord transection in the developing rat. Brain Research. 1977;125:241–255. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90618-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Weidner N, Ner A, Salimi N, Tuszynski MH. Spontaneous corticospinal axonal plasticity and functional recovery after adult central nervous system injury.  PNAS. 2001;98:3513–3518. doi: 10.1073/pnas.051626798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Yamahachi H, Marik SA, McManus JN, Denk W, Gilbert CD. Rapid axonal sprouting and pruning accompany functional reorganization in primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2009;64:719–729. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Zaporozhets E, Cowley KC, Schmidt BJ. Neurochemical excitation of propriospinal neurons facilitates locomotor command signal transmission in the lesioned spinal cord. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;105:2818–2829. doi: 10.1152/jn.00917.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2017 Jun 29;6:e23532. doi: 10.7554/eLife.23532.013

Decision letter

Editor: Ole Kiehn1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Cortex-dependent recovery of hindlimb locomotion after complete spinal cord injury" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Eve Marder as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Arko Ghosh (Reviewer #1); Reggie Edgerton (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary

The present work investigates the neuronal plasticity following pharmacological and physical therapy leading to weight-supported hind limb stepping after complete spinal cord transection at the thoracic level in rats. The authors show that full weight-supported stepping is followed by plasticity in motor cortex that involves increased motor output to trunk muscles with a possible increased integration of sensory input to these areas from the forelimbs during training. The essential new idea is that improved locomotor function after a complete spinal cord transection can occur with interventions previously shown to result in improved locomotor function, but it is not via any crossing of fibers over the lesion but due to improved control of trunk muscles that act as a mechanical bridge to allow weight-supported stepping in the hind limbs. The study is comprehensive and the central hypothesis is substantiated by electrophysiological, lesion and anatomical findings. The study has the potential to provide important new insight to the functional recovery following intervention therapies after spinal cord injury. There are, however, a number of concerns that should be addressed to improve the presentation, make the conclusions of the paper stronger and clarify several issues.

Title: The title should include the animal used.

Essential revisions

1) A major concern raised by all reviewers is how the results are interpreted and discussed. The paper emphasizes throughout the observation that these animals, with appropriate rehabilitation and serotonergic administration, can recover function including weight supported stepping after complete spinal cord injury. Although this is an important point and bears repeating: it is not new. This result is well established in the literature, by several studies (from Rossignol, Courtine, Edgerton and others) in rats, cats, and mice using a variety of interventions. The point is still important to make as it is not always appreciated, but the paper has to be reworked throughout to avoid repeated overstatements/overselling with regard to these findings and by including appropriate acknowledgement and reference to previous work. On the other hand, the authors should focus in the writing on the real significance of the study: namely that cortical plasticity might mediate functional recovery after SCI via biomechanical coupling across the lesion – which is not well appreciated in the field. Also here the wording should be appropriate not overstate the finding.

2) The authors need to substantiate that they obtain WSS after training. All analysis for WSS is expressed as% of 100 steps that met the criteria of a step cycle on the treadmill and the criteria in the open field. These data therefore provide no direct evidence of absolute levels of weight support generated. All measures are% of the number of steps with the criteria for a step adjusted to the level of a constant criteria for treadmill and over-ground open field as well as for training. No absolute values even for the BBB tests are provided. BBB scores are informative as to the functional level of each animal and the scale is clearly not linear; an increase from 2 to 3 might mean something very different from an increase from 10 to 11. All changes are in% for each rat with adjusted levels of support as the point of reference. The subjective scores should be reported – so as to demonstrate that each group had similar functional levels after spinal cord injury.

3) The biomechanical coupling across the lesion is the core idea put forward in the study. Yet, there is little explanation or mechanistic insights into how this is accomplished. It will be useful to have a description of what animals actually did to recover weight bearing stepping. How different are the trunk-supported steps post-lesion from the steps pre-lesion? How did animals with good recovery control their trunk muscles differently than animals with poor recovery? The authors should be able to extract biomechanical data from video analysis of animals without further experimentation to address these questions. The open field-scores were estimated over the first 100 steps. Did the animals in all of the groups perform the tests as quickly and was this used to adjust for the corresponding statistics. It will also be useful to discuss possible mechanisms for the recovery across the lesion. There have been reports of intersegmental reflexes among trunk muscles that should be cited. Altogether the authors should better separate their work from previous research with a more in-depth consideration of what has been already done regarding trunk supported steps and the proposed mechanisms.

4) Changes in cortical plasticity. More excitable vs. more area? The authors argue the area occupied by the trunk region expands following the lesion and supports the treatment-induced recovery. However, it remains possible that the cortex is more excitable post-lesion – as suggested by some of the previous work performed by some of the co-authors of this very report. To resolve this, the data from the different threshold currents used in the ICMS experiments could be simply shown. Essentially, a heat map showing the current thresholds should settle this.

5) The cortical lesion experiments are incomplete unless the functional specificity of the lesion is clarified. The lesion experiments bear a strong importance to the study so it is important to address if the impact of the lesion is specific to the hind limb. This would be expected from the apparently accurate lesion location; but a control measure used to explore if the forelimb weight support remains unobstructed should be presented to exclude a general impact of cortical lesion (brain swelling etc.). In particular the authors should pay attention to the cortical lesion on normal locomotor behavior in non-spinal-cord-injured animals. This relates to their findings that cortical lesions in control animals have no effect on motor behavior. At the moment this is just mentioned in one sentence and would need some sort of qualification and discussed in relationship to previous findings in rats (e.g. Ölvecky's or Courtin's).

6) The sensory cortical recordings should be better aligned with the rest of the paper to support the central claim by adding trunk sensory simulations or at least discuss their role in the recovery. Secondly, in Figure 5D the 'quarter' division of the grid seems a bit arbitrary. It might be more informative to divide sites according to their motor representation. I.e. is the increased forelimb representation mainly at sites that encode the trunk or is it general across all representations? Finally, was there a phase relationship between the activity of these cells and forelimb movements during normal walking that could be explained by their new receptive field? This latter analysis of neural coding is beyond the scope of this paper, but it would be helpful if the authors could comment on it since it would clearly help in explaining the recovery strategy.

7) While the measurements of the protein levels for a number of molecules linked to plastic changes are interesting they presence or absence cannot be linked to any specific plasticity. This part of the results should be deleted from the study.

8) The Discussion needs to be reworked to take into account the new data analysis and to tone down the strong statements in the first paragraph. It should also include a discussion of other studies that have shown cortical plasticity after spinal cord injury and the effect of cortical lesions in intact animals.

9) The logic of tying this work to a "brain machine interface" seemed off track and should be avoided.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "Cortex-dependent recovery of unassisted hindlimb locomotion after complete spinal cord injury in adult rats" for further consideration at eLife. Your revised article has been favorably evaluated by Eve Marder (Senior editor), a Reviewing editor, and three reviewers.

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed before acceptance, as outlined below:

This revised study addresses most of the concerns raised by the reviewers. It has clarified issues regarding the biomechanical coupling and unassisted weight support which further helps to highlight the novelty of the study. The major strength of this report is in its focus on the circuits that are above the injury which may biomechanically influence the motor functions originally controlled by the injured cord. Although there is not a clear mechanistic explanation for the link between behavioral recovery, changes in cortical structures and the mechanical coupling the authors have gone a long way in the revision to explain how animals achieve training+treatment induced recovery and discussed the findings appropriately.

There remain some questions that the authors needs to respond to before the paper can be published:

1) There are significant concerns about the final figure showing the Western blots. Comments about this were made by reviewers in the previous round but it did not make it into the decision letter. First, several β actin control blots are reused across figures. I.e. in Figure B, the β actin control for PSD95 is the same as the control for synaptophysin. The authors should clarify that they were all taken from the same sheet. If they're not, then it's not appropriate to use the same β actin blot as a control for both cases. More confusing, however, are the blots in Figure C and D. The β actin controls appear to be the same for GLAST and ephrin A3, except that the final sample is missing in D. As for Figure B, if they were all on the same sheet, then this might be ok. But it appears to be more complicated. In C there are 5 samples for partial and 5 samples for complete; in D there are 4 samples for partial and 5 for complete. This explains why there's one less sample in the β actin control in D. However, this implies that there is a mismatch between columns in C and D, with the 5 complete animals matching up with different columns in the β actin controls (matching with 10-14 in C and with 9-13 in D). One can also see part of a blot clipped at the end of the ephrinA3 row. Please explain 1) whether the reused β actin controls were obtained from the sheet, 2) why there are 4 partial animals in some samples and 5 partials in others, 3) the apparent mismatch between columns in C and D, and 4) the apparently clipped blot in D.

Added by Senior Editor: it would be best if independent controls were used for each run. If they were run on one gel, then redesign the figure to make that clear and only show the control once if it was the same actual run used for multiple samples. fix this in some scientifically unassailable way or we won't be able to proceed to publication. Incorrectly replicated gel panels are one of the most common errors found in published papers, and we are starting to monitor papers for this kind of issue.

2) Please add the results from the bi-square vs. OLS regressions as a supplementary figure, along with your argument as to why the 'OLS' is preferred.

3) There is an opportunity to refer to a paper about intersegmental reflexes:

Reflexes evoked in human erector spinae muscles by tapping during voluntary activity. Tani T1, Yamamoto H, Ichimiya M, Kimura J.

eLife. 2017 Jun 29;6:e23532. doi: 10.7554/eLife.23532.014

Author response


Title: The title should include the animal used.

The title was changed to “Cortex-dependent recovery of unassisted hindlimb locomotion after complete spinal cord injury in adult rats”

Essential revisions

1) A major concern raised by all reviewers is how the results are interpreted and discussed. The paper emphasizes throughout the observation that these animals, with appropriate rehabilitation and serotonergic administration, can recover function including weight supported stepping after complete spinal cord injury. Although this is an important point and bears repeating: it is not new. This result is well established in the literature, by several studies (from Rossignol, Courtine, Edgerton and others) in rats, cats, and mice using a variety of interventions. The point is still important to make as it is not always appreciated, but the paper has to be reworked throughout to avoid repeated overstatements/overselling with regard to these findings and by including appropriate acknowledgement and reference to previous work. On the other hand, the authors should focus in the writing on the real significance of the study: namely that cortical plasticity might mediate functional recovery after SCI via biomechanical coupling across the lesion – which is not well appreciated in the field. Also here the wording should be appropriate not overstate the finding.

We agree with the reviewers that several previous studies were able to improve hindlimb locomotion after complete spinal cord injury in adult rats, either using sensorimotor rehabilitation (de Leon et al., 2002; Allouin, Delivet-Mongrain and Rossignol, 2015), administration of serotonergic agonists (Feraboli-Lohnherr, Barthe and Orsal, 1999; Antri, Orsal and Barthe, 2002; Antri, Barthe, Mouffle and Orsal, 2005), transplants below the level of the lesion (Jimenez y Ribotta et al., J Neurosci 2000; Sławińska et al., 2013) or combined pharmacological and electrical stimulation (Gerasimenko et al., 2007; Ichiyama et al., 2008; Courtine et al., 2009; Musienko et al., 2011). However, all previous interventions required either tail pinching, perineal stimulation and/or some level of assisted vertical weight support to achieve hindlimb locomotion in adult spinalized rats. In our study, during treadmill testing and BBB evaluation our animals received no form of peripheral stimulation or vertical weight support. This absence of external aid is important per se and it strengthens the finding that cortical plasticity might mediate functional recovery after spinal cord injury via biomechanical coupling across the lesion. This was clarified in the Discussion of the revised manuscript.

2) The authors need to substantiate that they obtain WSS after training. All analysis for WSS is expressed as% of 100 steps that met the criteria of a step cycle on the treadmill and the criteria in the open field. These data therefore provide no direct evidence of absolute levels of weight support generated. All measures are% of the number of steps with the criteria for a step adjusted to the level of a constant criteria for treadmill and over-ground open field as well as for training.

During testing, we did not provide any assisted vertical weight support to the animals, either on the treadmill or in the open field. The% WSS – which was assessed on the treadmill – thus reflects the ability gained by the animals to support their weight during hindlimb locomotion. This was clarified in the Materials and methods and Results.

During training, we did provide assisted vertical weight support to the animals through an external device that was controlled based on the animal’s ability to take steps (Timoszyk et al., 2005). At some level of weight support the animal is unable to bear its own weight and fails to step with its hindlimbs for more than 3 consecutive step cycles. The assisted weight support was set right above this value, which was defined as load-bearing failure point (Timoszyk et al., 2005). Two weeks after the spinal transection, animals were not able to step with their hindlimbs, so the initial failure point was set at the assisted weight support that allowed animals to maintain a quadrupedal position on the treadmill, supporting part of their weight with the forelimbs. This initial failure point at week-2 corresponded to the weight of the animal, used to calibrate the device, less the amount of vertical support provided by the forelimbs on the treadmill, which we could not measure. Therefore, the assisted weight support at failure point was expressed as a percentage of the initial failure point at week-2 (note that 100% assisted weight support is upper bounded by the weight of the animal). The failure point was evaluated in all animals (i.e. also in animals that received sham or partial therapy and thus did not receive treadmill training). The assisted weight support at failure point decreased with partial and complete therapy as a function of time after the spinal transection, providing direct evidence of increasing levels of weight support achieved by the animals. This was clarified in the Materials and methods and Results. We also added a figure (Figure 1—figure supplement 1) to show the evolution of the failure point after the spinal transection.

No absolute values even for the BBB tests are provided. BBB scores are informative as to the functional level of each animal and the scale is clearly not linear; an increase from 2 to 3 might mean something very different from an increase from 10 to 11. All changes are in% for each rat with adjusted levels of support as the point of reference. The subjective scores should be reported – so as to demonstrate that each group had similar functional levels after spinal cord injury.

Following the reviewers’ suggestions, we added in the Results the absolute values of the BBB scores at the beginning of therapy (i.e. 2 weeks after spinal cord injury): 6.5 ± 1.3 for sham, 5.7 ± 2.0 for passive, 5.2 ± 2.4 for full. All groups had similar functional levels at the beginning of therapy (1-way ANOVA, F(2,41)=1.7, p=0.19). Please note that, as stated in the original Materials and methods, these BBB scores were obtained on drugs (i.e. after acute administration of the same 5-HT agonists used for therapy).

3) The biomechanical coupling across the lesion is the core idea put forward in the study. Yet, there is little explanation or mechanistic insights into how this is accomplished. It will be useful to have a description of what animals actually did to recover weight bearing stepping. How different are the trunk-supported steps post-lesion from the steps pre-lesion? How did animals with good recovery control their trunk muscles differently than animals with poor recovery? The authors should be able to extract biomechanical data from video analysis of animals without further experimentation to address these questions. The open field-scores were estimated over the first 100 steps. Did the animals in all of the groups perform the tests as quickly and was this used to adjust for the corresponding statistics. It will also be useful to discuss possible mechanisms for the recovery across the lesion. There have been reports of intersegmental reflexes among trunk muscles that should be cited. Altogether the authors should better separate their work from previous research with a more in-depth consideration of what has been already done regarding trunk supported steps and the proposed mechanisms.

Our videos do not allow us to perform formal biomechanical analyses. Moreover, the animals were in a harness so we could not extract biomechanical data from the trunk. However, we did perform new video analyses from the treadmill testing data in animals that subsequently received cortical lesion (complete therapy, n=12; partial therapy, n=10), during testing at week-12 (pre-cortical lesion) and at week-14 (post-cortical lesion). Each of the 100 step cycles used for the% WSS was categorized – separately for each hindpaw – as no step (i.e. dragging), dorsal step (when the dorsal surface of the paw makes contact with the treadmill during paw placement), lateral step (when the animal performs a lateral sweep like motion without plantar contact) or plantar step (when the plantar surface of the paw comes in contact with the treadmill entirely during paw placement). Animals were categorized in two groups based on an arbitrary recovery threshold of 10% WSS at week-12: animals with good recovery (performer,% WSS>10, average 28.4 ± 11.1%, n=11) and animals with poor recovery (non-performer,% WSS≤10, average 2.8 ± 3.8%, n=11). Left and right hindpaw steps were separated as an independent factor. As expected, the percentage of no steps was lower in performers than in non-performers (3-way ANOVA, group: F(1,40)=10.1, p=0.0028) and increased after cortical lesion in both groups (lesion: F(1,40)=6.8, p=0.0129). The percentage of dorsal steps was similar in the two groups (group: F(1,40)=0.05, p=0.82), with a tendency to increase after cortical lesion in performers but not in non-performers (group x lesion: F(1,40)=5.3, p=0.0261). The percentage of lateral steps was again similar in the two groups (group: F(1,40)=1.2, p=0.28) and increased after cortical lesion in both groups (lesion: F(1,40)=6.8, p=0.0127). Finally, the percentage of plantar steps was markedly higher in performers (group: F(1,40)=95.7, p<0.0001) and significantly decreased after the lesion in performers (lesion x group: F(1,40)=19.9, p<0.0001; Tukey: p=0.0002) but not in non-performers (Tukey: p=0.11). These results clarify that the ability to make plantar contact with the treadmill during stepping is key to achieve good recovery and that the reorganized motor cortex plays a critical role in this recovery. This was added in a new figure (Figure 2—figure supplement 1)

Open field scores (i.e. BBB) were not estimated over these first 100 steps on the treadmill The scores were determined from a separate observation of the animals in the open field over a 4 minute period, according to standard procedures. This was clarified in the Materials and methods.

We could not find convincing references on intersegmental reflexes among trunk muscles. The only references we could find on intersegmental reflexes are related to respiration (McBain et al., Exp Physiol 2016 and references therein), which do not seems particularly relevant. Propiospinal connections (see e.g. Courtine et al., 2008; Filli et al., 2014) do not seem relevant either due to the completeness of our spinal cord transection. Suggestions are very welcome.

Unassisted trunk-supported steps have been described in detail in neonatally spinalized rats (Giszter, Davies and Graziani, 2007; Giszter, Davies and Ramakrishnan, 2008; Giszter, Hockensmith, Ramakirshnan and Idoekwere, 2010). A critical novelty of the present work is that we were able to achieve unassisted trunk-supported steps in adult spinalized rats. This was clarified in the Discussion.

4) Changes in cortical plasticity. More excitable vs. more area? The authors argue the area occupied by the trunk region expands following the lesion and supports the treatment-induced recovery. However, it remains possible that the cortex is more excitable post-lesion – as suggested by some of the previous work performed by some of the co-authors of this very report. To resolve this, the data from the different threshold currents used in the ICMS experiments could be simply shown. Essentially, a heat map showing the current thresholds should settle this.

As suggested by the reviewer, we included a figure (Figure 1—figure supplement 2) with the heat maps of the threshold currents used to evoke trunk movements for all three groups. There were no differences between the threshold currents across the three groups (1-way ANOVA, F(2,85)=0.751, p=0.475). Note that penetrations, rather than animals, were considered as individual samples for this analysis to maximize power. The heat maps indicate an expansion of the trunk area, particularly for animals that received complete therapy, while using equivalent threshold currents across all groups. This was clarified in the Results. Also note that our previous works only showed increased excitability in the somatosensory system. We have never found increased excitability in the motor system (Ganzer et al. 2014; Ganzer, Manohar, Shumsky and Moxon et al. 2016)

5) The cortical lesion experiments are incomplete unless the functional specificity of the lesion is clarified. The lesion experiments bear a strong importance to the study so it is important to address if the impact of the lesion is specific to the hind limb. This would be expected from the apparently accurate lesion location; but a control measure used to explore if the forelimb weight support remains unobstructed should be presented to exclude a general impact of cortical lesion (brain swelling etc.). In particular the authors should pay attention to the cortical lesion on normal locomotor behavior in non-spinal-cord-injured animals. This relates to their findings that cortical lesions in control animals have no effect on motor behavior. At the moment this is just mentioned in one sentence and would need some sort of qualification and discussed in relationship to previous findings in rats (e.g. Ölvecky's or Courtin's).

We agree that the specificity of the cortical lesion to hindlimb function is important. The% WSS for the forelimbs was 100% for all animals at week-12 and remained 100% for all animals at week-14, after the cortical lesion. Furthermore, from the new video analyses we confirmed that 100% of forelimb steps were plantar steps both before and after the cortical lesion. Forelimb weight support thus remained unobstructed by the cortical lesion, supporting the specificity of the cortical lesion to hindlimb function. This was clarified in the Results.

With our measures of% WSS we could not find any difference in hindlimb locomotion after cortical lesion in naïve non-spinal-cord-injured rats (i.e. 100% WSS). Our cortical lesion was rather focal, but we cannot exclude more subtle kinematic changes, which could be previously observed after extensive lesions of the forelimb and hindlimb sensorimotor cortex in mice (Ueno and Yamashita, Exp Neurol 2011). Nevertheless, the negligible effects of our cortical lesion in naïve animals are in agreement with recent data suggesting that motor cortex is required for learning but not for executing a learned motor skill in normal rats (Kawai et al., 2015), supporting the view that in rodents the motor cortex is not required for normal locomotion (Courtine et al., 2007). Importantly, our data suggest that the motor cortex is indeed required to sustain the recovered locomotion after complete spinal cord injury. This was clarified in the Results and Discussion.

6) The sensory cortical recordings should be better aligned with the rest of the paper to support the central claim by adding trunk sensory simulations or at least discuss their role in the recovery.

Neurons recorded from the putative hindlimb sensory region did not have significant responses to trunk stimulation neither before nor after spinal cord injury. The likely reason is that the trunk sensory area is not topographically adjacent to the deafferented hindlimb region. In normal animals the trunk sensory region is further caudal and lateral, well separated from the motor region. The novel sensorimotor map in the cortex that develops after therapy shows an overlapping expansion of the trunk motor (not sensory) and forelimb sensory into the deafferent hindlimb cortex. This speaks towards sensorimotor integration of the sensation in the forepaws that cues enhanced motor control of trunk musculature as a rehabilation strategy used by the animals. This was clarified in the Results

Secondly, in Figure 5D the 'quarter' division of the grid seems a bit arbitrary. It might be more informative to divide sites according to their motor representation. I.e. is the increased forelimb representation mainly at sites that encode the trunk or is it general across all representations?

In Figure 5D, the quarters have been named according to their identity as forelimb/hindlimb/trunk sensory or motor representations in normal animals (changed in figure)

Finally, was there a phase relationship between the activity of these cells and forelimb movements during normal walking that could be explained by their new receptive field? This latter analysis of neural coding is beyond the scope of this paper, but it would be helpful if the authors could comment on it since it would clearly help in explaining the recovery strategy.

We performed awake recordings from the hindlimb sensorimotor cortex during treadmill locomotion in 11 intact rats. We recorded an average of 33.8 ± 8.3 neurons per animal. Of these cells, 24.81 ± 11.56% significantly responded to contralateral forepaw placement, with a broad distribution of peak latencies (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). This suggests that neurons in the hindlimb sensorimotor cortex are responsive to all phases of normal locomotion, providing a functional substrate for the reorganization of this cortical region – with proper treatment – after spinal cord injury. This was added in the Materials and methods and Results.

7) While the measurements of the protein levels for a number of molecules linked to plastic changes are interesting they presence or absence cannot be linked to any specific plasticity. This part of the results should be deleted from the study.

We now make clear that the measured proteins are related to synaptic plasticity and are specific to mechanisms of long-term potentiation (Filosa et al., Nat Neurosci 2009). Importantly, this allows our cortical reorganization to be linked to specific synaptic plasticity within the cortex and not just a response to subcortical changes. This was clarified in the Discussion.

8) The Discussion needs to be reworked to take into account the new data analysis and to tone down the strong statements in the first paragraph. It should also include a discussion of other studies that have shown cortical plasticity after spinal cord injury and the effect of cortical lesions in intact animals.

The discussion was reworked. See also response to comment 1.

9) The logic of tying this work to a "brain machine interface" seemed off track and should be avoided.

The wording “brain machine interfaces” was avoided.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

There remain some questions that the authors needs to respond to before the paper can be published:

1) There are significant concerns about the final figure showing the Western blots. Comments about this were made by reviewers in the previous round but it did not make it into the decision letter. First, several β actin control blots are reused across figures. I.e. in Figure B, the β actin control for PSD95 is the same as the control for synaptophysin. The authors should clarify that they were all taken from the same sheet. If they're not, then it's not appropriate to use the same β actin blot as a control for both cases. More confusing, however, are the blots in Figure C and D. The β actin controls appear to be the same for GLAST and ephrin A3, except that the final sample is missing in D. As for Figure B, if they were all on the same sheet, then this might be ok. But it appears to be more complicated. In C there are 5 samples for partial and 5 samples for complete; in D there are 4 samples for partial and 5 for complete. This explains why there's one less sample in the β actin control in D. However, this implies that there is a mismatch between columns in C and D, with the 5 complete animals matching up with different columns in the β actin controls (matching with 10-14 in C and with 9-13 in D). One can also see part of a blot clipped at the end of the ephrinA3 row. Please explain 1) whether the reused β actin controls were obtained from the sheet, 2) why there are 4 partial animals in some samples and 5 partials in others, 3) the apparent mismatch between columns in C and D, and 4) the apparently clipped blot in D.

We agree with the reviewer’s original suggestion and have removed this figure (and two authors) from the paper.

2) Please add the results from the bi-square vs. OLS regressions as a supplementary figure, along with your argument as to why the 'OLS' is preferred.

Added these results as Figure 2—figure supplement 2, including further explanation with our argument for preferring OLS.

3) There is an opportunity to refer to a paper about intersegmental reflexes:

Reflexes evoked in human erector spinae muscles by tapping during voluntary activity. Tani T1, Yamamoto H, Ichimiya M, Kimura J.

We have now included this citation in the Discussion.


Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

RESOURCES