Abstract
The chemotherapy drug, 5-fluorouracil (5-FU), has been reported to cause cognitive impairments in cancer patients. The drug also reduces cell proliferation and survival in the brain. Asiatic acid (AA) is a triterpene compound found in Centella asiatica that can protect against reduction of neurogenesis in the hippocampus and memory deficits induced by valproic acid (VPA). In the present study, we investigated the preventive effects of AA on the deficits in spatial working memory and cell proliferation and survival caused by 5-FU chemotherapy in a rat model. Male Sprague Dawley rats received 5-FU (5 i.v. injections, 25 mg/kg) on day 8, 11, 14, 17 and 20 of the study. This was co-administered with AA (30 mg/kg, oral gavage tube) either 20 days before receiving 5-FU (preventive), after receiving 5-FU (recovery), or for the entire period of the experiment (throughout). Spatial working memory was determined using the novel object location (NOL) test and hippocampal cell proliferation and survival of dividing cells were quantified using immunohistochemistry. Rats in the 5-FU alone and recovery groups showed memory deficits in the NOL test and reductions in cell proliferation and cell survival in the subgranular zone (SGZ) of the hippocampal dentate gyrus. Rats in the control, AA alone, and both preventive and throughout co-administration groups, however, did not exhibit these characteristics. The results showed that 5-FU chemotherapy impaired memory and reduced cell proliferation and cell survival in the SGZ of the hippocampal dentate gyrus. However, these impairments in the animals receiving 5-FU chemotherapy were restored to control levels when AA was co-administered before and during 5-FU treatment. These data demonstrate that AA can prevent the spatial working memory and hippocampal neurogenesis impairments caused by 5-FU chemotherapy.
Introduction
Chemotherapy drugs are widely used to treat cancer patients, including those suffering from breast cancer. One of its side effects is memory impairment, such as problems with memory formation, attention, and concentration. Memory deficits impact patients quality of life and ability to return to work [1, 2] and have shown to last for many years after chemotherapy treatment [3, 4]. The chemotherapy agent, 5-fluorouracil (5-FU), is commonly used in combination with other agents to treat various cancers [1]. Previous studies have found that 5-FU treatment induces cognitive deficits, including impairments in attention, visual memory, and speed of information processing [5–7]. This drug can cross the blood-brain barrier through simple diffusion and is able to disrupt cell proliferation by blocking the enzyme thymidylate synthase (TS). This enzyme is required for DNA replication [8], which is associated with cell proliferation.
The subgranular zone (SGZ) in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus is one of two main neurogenic regions where adult neurogenesis takes place and continues throughout life [9–11]. The process of neurogenesis can be influenced by various factors, for example, drugs and environmental stimuli [12, 13]. Furthermore, previous studies have shown that chronic stress decreases neurogenesis [14, 15] and induces apoptosis of new neurons in the hippocampus [16]. The decline in neurogenesis induced by chronic stress can be reversed by the administration of antidepressants [14] Previous investigations have reported that decreases in memory and cell proliferation and survival caused by chemotherapy agents can be mitigated after co-administration with fluoxetine [17]. Moreover, Kaempferia parviflora extract can prevent memory deficits and reduction in cell proliferation in the SGZ of the hippocampus [18]. It would be beneficial for cancer sufferers if an agent could be found to ameliorate the side effects of treatment, especially cognitive deficits, which can severely affect these patients quality of life.
Asiatic acid (AA) is a triterpene derived from Centella asiatica (L.) Urban. Asiatic acid has been reported to exhibit biological effects, including neuroprotective properties [19–21]. In traditional medicine, Centella asiatica has been utilized to restore decreased cognitive function. Several studies have demonstrated that AA enhances learning and memory, which are associated with hippocampal neurogenesis [21–23]. Acute administration of AA (30 mg/kg) has been shown to stimulate learning and memory as measured by a passive avoidance test [24]. In addition, subchronic and chronic administration of AA (30 mg/kg) has been shown to increase cell proliferation in the hippocampus and stimulate spatial working memory [25]. Furthermore, AA (30 mg/kg) can prevent neurogenesis and spatial memory impairment caused by valproic acid (VPA) [26]. Therefore, the present study aims to investigate the biological effects of AA in preventing and restoring cognitive deficit and decreases in hippocampal cell proliferation and survival induced by 5-FU chemotherapy in a rat model.
Materials and methods
Animals and drug administration
Adult male Sprague-Dawley rats (weight 180-220g, National Laboratory Animal Center, Mahidol University, Salaya, Nakornpathom) were housed four to five per cage and in a 12 h (7:00 AM to 7:00 PM) light-dark cycle with food and water available ad libitum. Animals were weighed daily from arrival and allowed to habituate in the animal facility for one week prior to the start of the procedures. Laboratory animal care in this study was conducted in accordance with the Ethics for Animal Experimentation put forth by the National Research Council of Thailand and with approval from the Animal Ethics Committee of Khon Kaen University under permit number AEKKU 25/2557.
Sixty rats were randomly divided into six groups (n = 10 animals/group). In the first (control) group, rats were administered 0.9% sterile saline 5 times by intravenous (i.v.) injection and received propylene glycol (1 ml/kg/day, Ajax Finechem Pty Ltd., Australia) by oral gavage for 20 days. The second (5-FU) group received 5-FU (25 mg/kg, Boryung pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Korea) 5 times by i.v. injection on day 8, 11, 14, 17, and 20 of the experiment. Rats in the third (AA) group were given AA (30 mg/kg, dissolved in propylene glycol, Faces Biochemical Co., Ltd., China) administered orally for 20 days. Those in the fourth (preventive) group, animals were administered AA (30 mg/kg) via oral gavage tube for 20 days (from day 1 to day 20) and received 5-FU (5 times by i.v. injection) at an equivocal dose as the rats in group two. The fifth (recovery) group, animals were administered 5-FU (5 times by i.v. injection) at an equivocal dose as the rats in group two and received AA 1 day after the last 5-FU injection (from day 21 to day 40) for 20 days of the experiment. Rats in the sixth (throughout) group, animals were administered 5-FU (5 times by i.v. injection) at an equivocal dose as the rats in group two and received AA for the entire period of the experiment (from day 1 to day 40), a total of 40 days. Animals in all groups were given 3 BrdU injections (100 mg/kg at a volume of 4 ml/kg, Sigma Aldrich, Inc., St. Louis, USA) by intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection. These BrdU injections were administered 24 hours apart beginning 2 days before their 5-FU/saline injection.
Behavioral testing
Novel object location (NOL)
The NOL test was used to determine spatial working memory in the present study. The protocol was adapted from a previous method [27] and carried out as previously described [25, 28]. The task apparatus consisted of an arena (36 cm x 50 cm x 30 cm) and plastic bottles filled with water to weigh them down. The experiment was conducted at an illumination of 350–400 lux and recorded by VDO camcorder Version-052, OKER (Crown computer Co., Ltd, Bangkok, Thailand). One day prior to testing, each animal was habituated to an arena for 30 min.
The NOL test began 3 days after the end of the drug treatment and was composed of two-trials (familiarization and choice trials). In the familiarization trial, two identical objects were randomly placed in separate locations in the arena and the rats were allowed to explore the objects for 3 min. Then, the rats were returned to their home cages for a 15 min inter-trial interval. Meanwhile the objects and the arenas were cleaned with 20% ethanol to eliminate olfactory cues. During the choice trial, one object was returned to the same location while the other one was moved to a new (novel) location, after which the rats were returned to the arena and allowed to explore the objects for 3 min. The exploratory activity that was measured was time spent actively exploring the object by sniffing, licking, chewing, or directing the nose towards the object at a distance less than 2 cm [27]. The preference index (PI) was defined as the percentage of exploration time spent examining the object in the novel location over the total exploration time (spent with objects in both novel and familiar locations) during the choice trial as described by Sirichoat et al. (2015) [25] and Umka Welbat et al. (2016) [26].
Tissue preparation
After the behavioral test, rats were euthanized by rapid stunning and cervical dislocation. Brains from each group were cryoprotected in a 30% sucrose solution for 3 hours at 4°C and then embedded in Optimal Cutting Temperature (OCT) compound (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Germany). They were then snap-frozen in liquid nitrogen-cooled isopentane and stored at −80°C prior to sectioning.
Immunohistochemistry
Ki-67 was used to quantify cell proliferation in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus. Frozen brains were serially sectioned (20 μm) in the coronal plane from the Bregma point -2.3 to -6.3 mm to include the entire dentate gyrus using a cryostat. Sections were mounted on 3-aminopropyl-methoxysilane (APS)-coated slides and stored at −20°C for Ki-67 staining. Every 15th section from the length of the dentate gyrus was used to select a total of 9 sections per brain using a systemic random sampling technique [29]. Ki-67 staining was performed as previously described [28]. Sections were incubated with monoclonal mouse anti Ki-67 primary antibody (1:150, Vector Laboratory, Inc., USA.) for 1 h. Sections were then incubated with 488 Rabbit Anti-mouse IgG (1:300,Invitrogen, USA) for 40 min and counter-stained with propidium iodide (1:6000, Sigma Aldrich, Inc., St. Louis, MO, USA) for 30 sec.
Cell survival was investigated using BrdU immunostaining. Frozen brains were serially sectioned (40 μm) in the coronal plane from the Bregma point -2.3 to -6.3 mm to include the entire dentate gyrus using a freezing microtome. Sections were kept in a cryoprotective buffer at 4°C. Every 8th section from the length of dentate gyrus was used to select a total of 9 sections per brain. Sections were then incubated with sheep primary anti-BrdU (1:100, Abcam, UK) at 4°C overnight. Following this, they were incubated with anti-sheep secondary Alexa Fluor 488 (1:300, Invitrogen, USA) for 1 h and counter-stained with propidium iodide (1:6000, Sigma Aldrich, Inc., St. Louis, MO, USA) for 30 sec.
All sections were viewed at X40 on a Nikon ECLIPSE 80i fluorescence microscope running NIS-Element AR 3.2 software (Melville, NY, USA). Then Ki-67 rand BrdU positive cells within the SGZ (defined as 3 cell diameters from inside of the dentate gyrus) were counted for both blades of the dentate gyrus [1, 17, 30]. The numbers of Ki-67 and BrdU positive cells in each hippocampus were produced by combining the cell counts per section for the entire dentate gyrus and multiplying the results by 15 and 8, respectively.
Statistical analysis
All statistical parameters were calculated using GraphPad Prism (Ver. 5.0) software, and expressed as mean±standard error of mean (SEM). A probability level of p<0.05 was considered statistically significant. Data were analyzed via a paired Student’s t-test and an ANOVA (analysis of variance) test when appropriate. All parameters were performed blind.
Results
Effect of 5-FU and AA on spatial working memory
The behavioural effect of 5-FU and AA on spatial working memory was determined using the NOL test three days after the end of the drug administration. During the familiarization trial, the animals explored two identical objects. There were no significant differences among the six groups regarding to exploration time for either object in the arena (mean±SEM; location A, control: 7.013±0.907 sec, 5-FU: 10.33±1.226 sec, AA: 8.296±1.437 sec, preventive: 8.551±1.024 sec, throughout: 7.557±0.578 sec, recovery: 6.196±0.710 sec.; location B, control: 5.433±0.631 sec, 5-FU: 9.263±0.897 sec, AA: 8.413±1.071 sec, preventive: 7.387±0.823 sec, throughout: 8.873±0.433 sec, recovery: 6.570±0.772 sec., p>0.05, paired Student t-test, Fig 1A), indicating that the animals did not display a preference for either object’s location over the other. In the choice trial, one object was moved to a novel location and animals were tested for their ability to discriminate between the objects placed in the familiar and novel locations. Animals in the control, AA, 5-FU+AA (preventive), and 5-FU+AA (throughout) groups spent a significantly longer time exploring the object in the novel location compared to the object in the familiar location (mean±SEM; familiar location, control: 4.839±0.268 sec, AA: 9.390±0.877 sec, preventive: 7.859±1.072 sec, throughout: 7.164±0.883 sec.; novel location, control: 11.320±1.593 sec, AA: 13.260±1.235 sec, preventive: 13.540±1.345 sec, throughout: 11.440±1.039 sec., p<0.05, paired Student t-test, Fig 1B), indicating unaffected spatial working memory. In contrast, animals treated with 5-FU alone and 5-FU with AA (recovery) could not discriminate between the object placed in the novel location and that in the familiar location (mean±SEM; familiar location, 5-FU: 7.627±1.088 sec, recovery: 6.951±0.530 sec.; novel location, 5-FU: 10.160±1.736 sec, recovery: 8.363±1.121 sec., p>0.05, paired Student t-test), suggesting that 5-FU induced a deficit in spatial working memory.
The exploration times in the choice trial were converted into a preference index (PI). The mean PI of the control, AA, 5-FU+AA (preventive), and 5-FU+AA (throughout) groups was significantly higher than 50% chance (mean±SEM; control: 67.95±4.37, AA: 58.21±3.06, preventive: 63.16±3.32, throughout: 61.78±2.04., p<0.05; one-sample t-test, Fig 2A), suggesting that these animals did not exhibit memory deficit. In contrast, the PI of the 5-FU and 5-FU+AA (recovery) groups did not differ from 50% chance, indicating memory impairment (mean±SEM; 5-FU: 56.41±2.99, recovery: 53.52±3.45., p>0.05; one-sample t-test). Moreover, there was no significant difference among the groups in terms of total exploration time (mean±SEM; control: 28.60±1.74, 5-FU: 37.37±3.68, AA: 39.35±3.24, preventive: 37.33±3.30, throughout: 35.03±2.30, recovery: 28.07±1.70., p>0.05, one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni’s post-hoc test, Fig 2B), indicating that animals did not suffer from impairment of locomotor activity.
Effect of 5-FU and AA on proliferating cell counts in the dentate gyrus
Fig 3A–3F show representative images of proliferating cells within the SGZ of the dentate gyrus obtained using Ki-67 immunohistochemistry. The number of Ki-67 positive cells differed significantly among the groups. Animals receiving 5-FU alone showed a significant decrease in Ki-67 positive cells compared with the controls (mean±SEM; control: 2605±84.02 cells, 5-FU: 1668±41.25 cells, p<0.05, one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni’s post-hoc test, Fig 3G). The Ki-67 positive cell numbers in the group treated with AA on its own did not differ significantly compared to the control (mean±SEM; AA: 2895±61.36 cells, p>0.05, one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni’s post-hoc test), but 5-FU group (p<0.05, one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni’s post-hoc test). The Ki-67 positive cell numbers of the groups in which 5-FU and AA were co-administered (preventive and throughout) did not differ from the controls (mean±SEM; preventive: 2320±90.80 cells, throughout: 2360±85.18 cells, p>0.05, one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni’s post-hoc test), but were significantly higher than in the 5-FU group (p<0.05, one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni’s post-hoc test). However, subjects in the 5-FU and AA (recovery) group had a significantly lower numbers of Ki-67 positive cells compared with controls (mean±SEM; recovery: 1913±33.49 cells, p<0.05, one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni’s post-hoc test). These results indicate that 5-FU induced a reduction of cell proliferation in the SGZ of the dentate gyrus. This effect can be prevented by AA if it is administered before or during the 5-FU treatment period, but not after.
Effect of 5-FU and AA on cell survival in the dentate gyrus
In order to study cell survival, BrdU injections were administered daily for three days prior to 5-FU treatment. BrdU positive cells were counted in the SGZ of the dentate gyrus (Fig 4A–4F). There was a significant decrease of BrdU positive cells in animals treated with 5-FU alone compared with controls (mean±SEM; control: 1193±85.62 cells, 5-FU: 677.5±92.34 cells, p<0.05, one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni’s post-hoc test, Fig 4G). Animals receiving AA alone had significantly higher numbers of BrdU positive cell compared to the 5-FU group (mean±SEM; AA: 1305±103.4 cells, p<0.05, one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni’s post-hoc test). The BrdU-positive cell numbers in two of the groups treated with both 5-FU and AA (preventive and throughout) did not differ significantly from the control group (mean±SEM; preventive: 1088±75.37 cells, throughout: 990.0±21.56 cells, p>0.05, one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni’s post-hoc test), but were significantly higher than those in the 5-FU group (p<0.05, one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni’s post-hoc test). In contrast, animals treated with both 5-FU and AA (recovery) had significantly fewer BrdU positive cells when compared with controls (mean±SEM; recovery: 715.0±39.62 cells, p<0.05, one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni’s post-hoc test). These results demonstrated that AA can protect against decreased cell survival when administered before or during 5-FU treatment, but not after.
Discussion
The data presented shows that treatment with 5-FU chemotherapy caused cognitive deficits, which were associated with decreases in the rate of neurogenesis in the SGZ of the dentate gyrus. These impairments were mitigated through co-administration of 5-FU with AA before (preventive) and during (throughout) but not after (recovery) 5-FU treatment.
Several studies have indicated that 5-FU administered at a high single dose [31–33] or repeated doses [1, 30, 34] can produce cognitive impairment. These effects can persist for several years after treatment [3, 4, 6, 35–37] and the cause is still unclear [38]. The NOL test was chosen as a test of spatial working memory as it is a hippocampal-dependent task. This test relies on the spontaneous preference of rats to explore an object location without requiring positive or negative reinforcements [27, 39]. Our results showed that control animals were able to discriminate between objects in novel and familiar locations. In contrast, animals which had received 5-FU showed no significant difference in the time spent on the two objects, indicating a deficit in spatial working memory. These results are consistent with earlier reports that treatment with 5-FU induces cognitive impairment in both animals [30, 34, 40] and humans [41, 42]. Although the preference index of the AA group did not differ from control group, animals administered with AA alone showed an improvement in their ability to distinguish between objects placed in familiar and novel locations. This suggests that treatment with AA alone for 20 days did improve spatial working memory. Previous studies have found that treatment with AA alone for 28 days show preference index higher than control, indicating that AA alone can enhance spatial working memory when compared with control [25]. However, in the present study, the preference index in animals receiving only AA was not different from control. It might be due to a shorter period (20 days) of AA administration. In addition, AA has cognitive benefits for animals treated with VPA when administered prior to VPA treatment [26]. Animals receiving AA before and during 5-FU treatment showed a significant preference for the object in the novel location compared with that in the familiar location. However, animals receiving 5-FU alone or that were co-treated with AA after 5-FU treatment did not differentiate between the objects in the novel and familiar locations. This suggests that AA provides cognitive benefits and can protect against the some of the negative effects of this chemotherapy agent, but only if administered before or during (not after) chemotherapy treatment.
New neurons are continuously produced throughout life from a population of dividing cells within the adult rat brain [43]. New neurons in the hippocampus have the ability to proliferate, differentiate and migrate into the SGZ of the hippocampal dentate gyrus in order to connect into the brain’s neural network [44]. Moreover, hippocampal neurogenesis is associated with learning and memory [45, 46]. The production of new neurons in the SGZ of the dentate gyrus is important for spatial memory formation [47, 48]. Reductions in cell proliferation and survival in the SGZ are influenced by various factors such as environmental and pharmacological stimuli [44, 49], irradiation [50, 51], or chemotherapy drugs [2, 17, 34, 52]. The main function of 5-FU is to inhibit thymidylate synthase, which is required for thymidine synthesis during the S-phase of the cell cycle [53, 54]. In the present study, the effects of 5-FU and AA on production and survival of new hippocampal cells were examined using immunohistochemistry to analyze Ki-67 and BrdU, respectively. The results showed that the number of Ki-67 and BrdU positive cells in 5-FU-treated animals was significantly lower than in the control animals. These results confirmed previous reports that 5-FU treatment reduced cell proliferation and cell survival in the SGZ of the dentate gyrus [1, 2]. The animals receiving AA after 5-FU treatment (recovery) showed the same reduction in both the cell proliferation and cell survival as the group treated 5-FU alone. While AA on its own had no effect on cell proliferation and cell survival, co-administration of AA before (preventive) and during (throughout) 5-FU treatment led to there being no reduction in cell proliferation or cell survival after the end of the experiment when compared to controls. This indicates that AA is able to prevent the cognitive deficits associated with a reduction of hippocampal neurogenesis, but that these deficits cannot be reversed after 5-FU chemotherapy treatment.
Previous reports have demonstrated that AA has antioxidant and neuroprotective activities [55] which have been shown to protect against neuronal damage in cell cultures [22]. Similarly, AA has been shown to protect against mitochondria injury from cerebral ischemia in animal models [21]. A recent study has found that AA can also prevent spatial working memory and hippocampal neurogenesis impairment caused by VPA [26]. Our results show that co-administration of AA and 5-FU ameliorated the reduction in cell proliferation and survival in the SGZ and improved memory as measured by the NOL test. This change may be due to the neuroprotective and antioxidant properties of AA.
The extract of Centella asiatica can increase levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) [56], which is associated with increasing the proliferation of neural stem cells [57]. A previous study showed that AA, a compound extracted from Centella asiatica, can increase levels of doublecortin (DCX) and Notch1 protein expression within the hippocampus [25] DCX is a microtubule-associated protein that is highly expressed in immature neurons [58, 59]. It is also required for neuronal migration, differentiation, and plasticity of the cerebral cortex [58, 60] Notch1 plays an important role in the process of neurogenesis and is also expressed in the dendrites of mature neurons [61, 62, 63]. Therefore, the enhancement of cell proliferation and survival and spatial working memory by asiatic acid in this study may be via the mechanism of BDNF, DCX and Notch1 protein expression.
In conclusion, the results of this study show that co-administration of AA can counteract the memory impairment associated with a reduction in new neuron generation in the SGZ of the hippocampus caused by 5-FU. Our results provide evidence that AA might be useful in preventing cognitive impairment in patients treated with chemotherapy drugs. Investigations of other mechanisms of AA on neurogenesis and its interaction with chemotherapy agents will help doctors better understand how to counteract memory deficits in patients undergoing these treatments.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank The Thailand Research Fund (TRG5780086), Invitation Research of the Faculty of Medicine (grant number: IN59312), and the Neuroscience Research and Development Group at Khon Kaen University for funding this project. The authors also wish to thank Mr.Dylan Southard for critical reading of the manuscript.
Data Availability
All relevant data are publicly available in Figshare, as follows: 10.6084/m9.figshare.4810762 <https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.4810762>; 10.6084/m9.figshare.4810765 <https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.4810765>; 10.6084/m9.figshare.5097154 <https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.5097154>; 10.6084/m9.figshare.5097169 <https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.5097169>.
Funding Statement
The Thailand Research Fund (TRG5780086), Invitation Research of the Faculty of Medicine (grant number: IN59312), and the Neuroscience Research and Development Group at Khon Kaen University funded this project.
References
- 1.Lyons L, ElBeltagy M, Bennett G, Wigmore P. Fluoxetine counteracts the cognitive and cellular effects of 5-fluorouracil in the rat hippocampus by a mechanism of prevention rather than recovery. PloS one. 2012;7(1):e30010 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030010 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3260195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.ELBeltagy M, Mustafa S, Umka J, Lyons L, Salman A, Dormon K, et al. The effect of 5-fluorouracil on the long term survival and proliferation of cells in the rat hippocampus. Brain research bulletin. 2012;88(5):514–8. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Matsuda T, Takayama T, Tashiro M, Nakamura Y, Ohashi Y, Shimozuma K. Mild cognitive impairment after adjuvant chemotherapy in breast cancer patients—evaluation of appropriate research design and methodology to measure symptoms. Breast cancer. 2005;12(4):279–87. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ahles TA, Saykin AJ. Breast cancer chemotherapy-related cognitive dysfunction. Clinical breast cancer. 2002;3 Suppl 3:S84–90. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Foley JJ, Raffa RB, Walker EA. Effects of chemotherapeutic agents 5-fluorouracil and methotrexate alone and combined in a mouse model of learning and memory. Psychopharmacology. 2008;199(4):527–38. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1175-y ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3263345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Falleti MG, Sanfilippo A, Maruff P, Weih L, Phillips KA. The nature and severity of cognitive impairment associated with adjuvant chemotherapy in women with breast cancer: a meta-analysis of the current literature. Brain and cognition. 2005;59(1):60–70. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.05.001 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Winocur G, Vardy J, Binns MA, Kerr L, Tannock I. The effects of the anti-cancer drugs, methotrexate and 5-fluorouracil, on cognitive function in mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2006;85(1):66–75. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.07.010 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Chu E, Callender MA, Farrell MP, Schmitz JC. Thymidylate synthase inhibitors as anticancer agents: from bench to bedside. Cancer Chemother Pharmacol. 2003;52 Suppl 1:S80–9. doi: 10.1007/s00280-003-0625-9 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lucassen PJ, Meerlo P, Naylor AS, van Dam AM, Dayer AG, Fuchs E, et al. Regulation of adult neurogenesis by stress, sleep disruption, exercise and inflammation: Implications for depression and antidepressant action. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;20(1):1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2009.08.003 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Altman J, Das GD. Autoradiographic and histological evidence of postnatal hippocampal neurogenesis in rats. The Journal of comparative neurology. 1965;124(3):319–35. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ehninger D, Kempermann G. Neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus. Cell and tissue research. 2008;331(1):243–50. doi: 10.1007/s00441-007-0478-3 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ming GL, Song H. Adult neurogenesis in the mammalian brain: significant answers and significant questions. Neuron. 2011;70(4):687–702. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.001 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3106107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.van Praag H, Christie BR, Sejnowski TJ, Gage FH. Running enhances neurogenesis, learning, and long-term potentiation in mice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1999;96(23):13427–31. ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC23964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Crupi R, Mazzon E, Marino A, La Spada G, Bramanti P, Battaglia F, et al. Hypericum perforatum treatment: effect on behaviour and neurogenesis in a chronic stress model in mice. BMC complementary and alternative medicine. 2011;11:7 doi: 10.1186/1472-6882-11-7 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3041724. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Lagace DC, Donovan MH, DeCarolis NA, Farnbauch LA, Malhotra S, Berton O, et al. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis is functionally important for stress-induced social avoidance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107(9):4436–41. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910072107 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2840117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lucassen PJ, Heine VM, Muller MB, van der Beek EM, Wiegant VM, De Kloet ER, et al. Stress, depression and hippocampal apoptosis. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets. 2006;5(5):531–46. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lyons L, ElBeltagy M, Umka J, Markwick R, Startin C, Bennett G, et al. Fluoxetine reverses the memory impairment and reduction in proliferation and survival of hippocampal cells caused by methotrexate chemotherapy. Psychopharmacology. 2011;215(1):105–15. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2122-2 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3072503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Welbat JU, Chaisawang P, Chaijaroonkhanarak W, Prachaney P, Pannangrong W, Sripanidkulchai B, et al. Kaempferia parviflora extract ameliorates the cognitive impairments and the reduction in cell proliferation induced by valproic acid treatment in rats. Annals of anatomy = Anatomischer Anzeiger: official organ of the Anatomische Gesellschaft. 2016;206:7–13. doi: 10.1016/j.aanat.2016.04.029 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Shukla A, Rasik AM, Jain GK, Shankar R, Kulshrestha DK, Dhawan BN. In vitro and in vivo wound healing activity of asiaticoside isolated from Centella asiatica. Journal of ethnopharmacology. 1999;65(1):1–11. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Huang SS, Chiu CS, Chen HJ, Hou WC, Sheu MJ, Lin YC, et al. Antinociceptive activities and the mechanisms of anti-inflammation of asiatic Acid in mice. Evidence-based complementary and alternative medicine: eCAM. 2011;2011:895857. doi: 10.1155/2011/895857 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3092715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Krishnamurthy RG, Senut MC, Zemke D, Min J, Frenkel MB, Greenberg EJ, et al. Asiatic acid, a pentacyclic triterpene from Centella asiatica, is neuroprotective in a mouse model of focal cerebral ischemia. Journal of neuroscience research. 2009;87(11):2541–50. doi: 10.1002/jnr.22071 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2941770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Xu MF, Xiong YY, Liu JK, Qian JJ, Zhu L, Gao J. Asiatic acid, a pentacyclic triterpene in Centella asiatica, attenuates glutamate-induced cognitive deficits in mice and apoptosis in SH-SY5Y cells. Acta pharmacologica Sinica. 2012;33(5):578–87. doi: 10.1038/aps.2012.3 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4010358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Orhan IE. Centella asiatica (L.) Urban: From Traditional Medicine to Modern Medicine with Neuroprotective Potential. Evidence-based complementary and alternative medicine: eCAM. 2012;2012:946259. doi: 10.1155/2012/946259 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3359802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Nasir MN, Habsah M, Zamzuri I, Rammes G, Hasnan J, Abdullah J. Effects of asiatic acid on passive and active avoidance task in male Spraque-Dawley rats. Journal of ethnopharmacology. 2011;134(2):203–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jep.2010.12.010 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Sirichoat A, Chaijaroonkhanarak W, Prachaney P, Pannangrong W, Leksomboon R, Chaichun A, et al. Effects of Asiatic Acid on Spatial Working Memory and Cell Proliferation in the Adult Rat Hippocampus. Nutrients. 2015;7(10):8413–23. doi: 10.3390/nu7105401 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4632421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Umka Welbat J, Sirichoat A, Chaijaroonkhanarak W, Prachaney P, Pannangrong W, Pakdeechote P, et al. Asiatic Acid Prevents the Deleterious Effects of Valproic Acid on Cognition and Hippocampal Cell Proliferation and Survival. Nutrients. 2016;8(5). doi: 10.3390/nu8050303 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Dix SL, Aggleton JP. Extending the spontaneous preference test of recognition: evidence of object-location and object-context recognition. Behavioural brain research. 1999;99(2):191–200. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Umka J, Mustafa S, ElBeltagy M, Thorpe A, Latif L, Bennett G, et al. Valproic acid reduces spatial working memory and cell proliferation in the hippocampus. Neuroscience. 2010;166(1):15–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.11.073 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Mayhew TM, Burton GJ. Methodological problems in placental morphometry: apologia for the use of stereology based on sound sampling practice. Placenta. 1988;9(6):565–81. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.ElBeltagy M, Mustafa S, Umka J, Lyons L, Salman A, Chur-yoe GT, et al. Fluoxetine improves the memory deficits caused by the chemotherapy agent 5-fluorouracil. Behavioural brain research. 2010;208(1):112–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.11.017 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Moore DH, Fowler WC Jr., Crumpler LS. 5-Fluorouracil neurotoxicity. Gynecologic oncology. 1990;36(1):152–4. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Atkins JN, Muss HB, Case D, Brockschmidt J, Schnell FM, O'Rourke M, et al. High-dose 24-hour infusion of 5-fluorouracil in metastatic prostate cancer. A phase II trial of the Piedmont Oncology Association. American journal of clinical oncology. 1991;14(6):526–9. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Yeh KH, Cheng AL. Acute confusion induced by a high-dose infusion of 5-fluorouracil and folinic acid. Journal of the Formosan Medical Association = Taiwan yi zhi. 1994;93(8):721–3. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Mustafa S, Walker A, Bennett G, Wigmore PM. 5-Fluorouracil chemotherapy affects spatial working memory and newborn neurons in the adult rat hippocampus. The European journal of neuroscience. 2008;28(2):323–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06325.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bower JE. Behavioral symptoms in patients with breast cancer and survivors. Journal of clinical oncology: official journal of the American Society of Clinical Oncology. 2008;26(5):768–77. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2007.14.3248 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3057774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Fan HG, Houede-Tchen N, Yi QL, Chemerynsky I, Downie FP, Sabate K, et al. Fatigue, menopausal symptoms, and cognitive function in women after adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer: 1- and 2-year follow-up of a prospective controlled study. Journal of clinical oncology: official journal of the American Society of Clinical Oncology. 2005;23(31):8025–32. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2005.01.6550 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kreukels BP, van Dam FS, Ridderinkhof KR, Boogerd W, Schagen SB. Persistent neurocognitive problems after adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer. Clinical breast cancer. 2008;8(1):80–7. doi: 10.3816/CBC.2008.n.006 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Soussain C, Ricard D, Fike JR, Mazeron JJ, Psimaras D, Delattre JY. CNS complications of radiotherapy and chemotherapy. Lancet. 2009;374(9701):1639–51. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61299-X . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Dere E, Huston JP, De Souza Silva MA. The pharmacology, neuroanatomy and neurogenetics of one-trial object recognition in rodents. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 2007;31(5):673–704. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.01.005 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Fardell JE, Vardy J, Shah JD, Johnston IN. Cognitive impairments caused by oxaliplatin and 5-fluorouracil chemotherapy are ameliorated by physical activity. Psychopharmacology. 2012;220(1):183–93. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2466-2 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.de Ruiter MB, Reneman L, Boogerd W, Veltman DJ, van Dam FS, Nederveen AJ, et al. Cerebral hyporesponsiveness and cognitive impairment 10 years after chemotherapy for breast cancer. Human brain mapping. 2011;32(8):1206–19. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21102 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Vardy J, Tannock I. Cognitive function after chemotherapy in adults with solid tumours. Critical reviews in oncology/hematology. 2007;63(3):183–202. doi: 10.1016/j.critrevonc.2007.06.001 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Abrous DN, Koehl M, Le Moal M. Adult neurogenesis: from precursors to network and physiology. Physiological reviews. 2005;85(2):523–69. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00055.2003 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Zhao C, Deng W, Gage FH. Mechanisms and functional implications of adult neurogenesis. Cell. 2008;132(4):645–60. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.01.033 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Epp JR, Chow C, Galea LA. Hippocampus-dependent learning influences hippocampal neurogenesis. Frontiers in neuroscience. 2013;7:57 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00057 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3627134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Gould E, Beylin A, Tanapat P, Reeves A, Shors TJ. Learning enhances adult neurogenesis in the hippocampal formation. Nature neuroscience. 1999;2(3):260–5. doi: 10.1038/6365 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Dupret D, Revest JM, Koehl M, Ichas F, De Giorgi F, Costet P, et al. Spatial relational memory requires hippocampal adult neurogenesis. PloS one. 2008;3(4):e1959 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001959 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2396793. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Imayoshi I, Sakamoto M, Ohtsuka T, Kageyama R. Continuous neurogenesis in the adult brain. Development, growth & differentiation. 2009;51(3):379–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-169X.2009.01094.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bruel-Jungerman E, Laroche S, Rampon C. New neurons in the dentate gyrus are involved in the expression of enhanced long-term memory following environmental enrichment. The European journal of neuroscience. 2005;21(2):513–21. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03875.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Madsen TM, Kristjansen PE, Bolwig TG, Wortwein G. Arrested neuronal proliferation and impaired hippocampal function following fractionated brain irradiation in the adult rat. Neuroscience. 2003;119(3):635–42. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Snyder JS, Hong NS, McDonald RJ, Wojtowicz JM. A role for adult neurogenesis in spatial long-term memory. Neuroscience. 2005;130(4):843–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.10.009 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Seigers R, Schagen SB, Beerling W, Boogerd W, van Tellingen O, van Dam FS, et al. Long-lasting suppression of hippocampal cell proliferation and impaired cognitive performance by methotrexate in the rat. Behavioural brain research. 2008;186(2):168–75. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.08.004 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Pinedo HM, Peters GF. Fluorouracil: biochemistry and pharmacology. Journal of clinical oncology: official journal of the American Society of Clinical Oncology. 1988;6(10):1653–64. doi: 10.1200/JCO.1988.6.10.1653 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Rustum YM. Thymidylate synthase: a critical target in cancer therapy? Frontiers in bioscience: a journal and virtual library. 2004;9:2467–73. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Lee MK, Kim SR, Sung SH, Lim D, Kim H, Choi H, et al. Asiatic acid derivatives protect cultured cortical neurons from glutamate-induced excitotoxicity. Res Commun Mol Pathol Pharmacol. 2000;108(1–2):75–86. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Khotimah H, Sumitr S, Ali M, Widodo M. Standardized Centella Asiatica Increased Brain- Derived Neurotrophic Factor and Decreased Apoptosis of Dopaminergic Neuron in Rotenone-Induced Zebrafish. Journal of Psychology. 2015;2(1):22–7. doi: 10.5176/2345-7872_2.1_25 [Google Scholar]
- 57.Chiou SH, Chen SJ, Peng CH, Chang YL, Ku HH, Hsu WM, et al. Fluoxetine up-regulates expression of cellular FLICE-inhibitory protein and inhibits LPS-induced apoptosis in hippocampus-derived neural stem cell. Biochemical and biophysical research communications. 2006;343(2):391–400. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2006.02.180 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Manohar S, Paolone NA, Bleichfeld M, Hayes SH, Salvi RJ, Baizer JS. Expression of doublecortin, a neuronal migration protein, in unipolar brush cells of the vestibulocerebellum and dorsal cochlear nucleus of the adult rat. Neuroscience. 2012;202:169–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.12.013 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3268848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Rao MS, Shetty AK. Efficacy of doublecortin as a marker to analyse the absolute number and dendritic growth of newly generated neurons in the adult dentate gyrus. The European journal of neuroscience. 2004;19(2):234–46. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Wakabayashi T, Kosaka J, Mori T, Takamori Y, Yamada H. Doublecortin expression continues into adulthood in horizontal cells in the rat retina. Neuroscience letters. 2008;442(3):249–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.030 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Alberi L, Liu S, Wang Y, Badie R, Smith-Hicks C, Wu J, et al. Activity-induced Notch signaling in neurons requires Arc/Arg3.1 and is essential for synaptic plasticity in hippocampal networks. Neuron. 2011;69(3):437–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.004 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3056341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Breunig JJ, Silbereis J, Vaccarino FM, Sestan N, Rakic P. Notch regulates cell fate and dendrite morphology of newborn neurons in the postnatal dentate gyrus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007;104(51):20558–63. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710156104 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2154470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Wang Y, Chan SL, Miele L, Yao PJ, Mackes J, Ingram DK, et al. Involvement of Notch signaling in hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2004;101(25):9458–62. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308126101 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC438998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are publicly available in Figshare, as follows: 10.6084/m9.figshare.4810762 <https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.4810762>; 10.6084/m9.figshare.4810765 <https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.4810765>; 10.6084/m9.figshare.5097154 <https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.5097154>; 10.6084/m9.figshare.5097169 <https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.5097169>.