Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Exp Brain Res. 2017 Apr 28;235(7):2243–2258. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-4971-y

Motor Equivalence and Structure of Variance: Multi-Muscle Postural Synergies in Parkinson's Disease

Ali Falaki 1, Xuemei Huang 2,3,4,5, Mechelle M Lewis 2,3, Mark L Latash 1
PMCID: PMC5507367  NIHMSID: NIHMS872366  PMID: 28455740

Abstract

We explored posture-stabilizing multi-muscle synergies with two methods of analysis of multielement, abundant systems: (1) Analysis of inter-cycle variance; and (2) Analysis of motor equivalence, both quantified within the framework of the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) hypothesis. Data collected in two earlier studies of patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) were re-analyzed. One study compared synergies in the space of muscle modes (muscle groups with parallel scaling of activation) during tasks performed by early-stage PD patients and controls. The other study explored the effects of dopaminergic medication on multi-muscle-mode synergies. Inter-cycle variance and absolute magnitude of the center of pressure displacement across consecutive cycles were quantified during voluntary whole-body sway within the UCM and orthogonal to the UCM space. The patients showed smaller indices of variance within the UCM and motor equivalence compared to controls. The indices were also smaller in the off-drug compared to on-drug condition. There were strong across-subject correlations between the intercycle variance within/orthogonal to the UCM and motor equivalent/non-motor equivalent displacements. This study has shown that, at least for cyclical tasks, analysis of variance and analysis of motor equivalence lead to metrics of stability that correlate with each other and show similar effects of disease and medication. These results show, for the first time, intimate links between indices of variance and motor equivalence. They suggest that analysis of motor equivalence, which requires only a handful of trials, could be used broadly in the field of motor disorders to analyze problems with action stability.

Keywords: synergy, posture, motor equivalence, Parkinson's disease, stability

Introduction

The term “synergy” has been used in the literature in different ways. In clinical literature, synergy usually means a pathological, stereotypical pattern of muscle activations interfering with purposeful actions (DeWald et al. 1995). Another meaning of synergy is a group of variables with proportional scaling of changes over time or with changes in task parameters (d'Avella et al. 2003; Ivanenko et al. 2004; Ting and Macpherson 2005). To avoid terminological confusion, we prefer to address such groups as modes (Krishnamoorthy et al. 2003) and use them as elemental variables in the analysis of synergies defined as neural organizations of elements (such as joints, limbs, digits, muscle groups, etc.) ensuring task-specific stability of salient performance variables (reviewed in Latash 2008, 2016).

A number of recent studies quantified multi-element motor synergies in different populations, including patients with Parkinson's disease (PD, reviewed in Latash and Huang 2015). These studies defined synergies as neural organizations of abundant sets of elements whose purpose is to provide stability of salient performance variables (reviewed in Latash 2012, 2016). In particular, synergies in the space of activations of muscle groups (M-modes) stabilizing coordinates of the center of pressure (COP) were studied (Falaki et al. 2016). This analysis was performed using the framework of the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) hypothesis (Scholz and Schöner 1999; Latash et al. 2007) to quantify two components of inter-trial (or inter-cycle, for cyclical tasks) variance within the space of M-modes. The variance component within the UCM space (VUCM) had no effect on the COP coordinate, whereas the variance component within the space orthogonal to the UCM (VORT) did. The normalized difference between VUCM and VORT, quantified per dimension in each space, has been used as an index of stability (a synergy index). In this context, stability means an ability to return to a state or trajectory following a small transient perturbation or a change in initial state. Analysis of inter-cycle variance may be viewed as a method producing a proxy of stability (reviewed in Latash 2008, 2016). In particular, repeating a task from slightly different initial states is expected to lead to trajectories converging in more stable directions and diverging in unstable directions resulting in different variance indices within the UCM and ORT.

Two aspects of impaired control of stability in PD patients have been identified, namely, impaired stability and impaired agility (reviewed in Latash and Huang 2015). In particular, PD patients typically show weaker synergies stabilizing COP coordinates during steady-state tasks interpreted as signs of impaired stability. Weaker synergies in postural tasks have been documented in early-stage PD patients with no identifiable clinical signs of postural instability (Falaki et al. 2016). These changes in the synergy index show sensitivity to dopamine-replacement drugs (Park et al. 2014; Falaki et al. 2017).

One of the problems with using analysis of inter-trial variance in patient populations is the necessity to collect multiple trials to obtain clouds of data points (illustrated in Figure 1) to estimate VUCM and VORT. Recently, another set of metrics has been developed within the framework of the UCM hypothesis, which can be measured in single trials. This analysis quantifies the magnitudes of motion of an abundant system within the UCM and ORT spaces (Scholz et al. 2007; Mattos et al. 2011, 2013, 2015). The within-UCM component, by definition, has no effect on the salient performance variable; it has been addressed as motor equivalent (ME). The ORT component, non-motor equivalent (nME), changes the performance variable.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

An illustration of the main notions for a simple task of producing a constant sum with two effectors: E1 + E2 = C. For a given C, the solution space is shown with the slanted dashed line (UCM1). The cloud of data points measured in consecutive trials is elongated along the UCM (VUCM > VORT). If the subject of this experiment is asked to produce a transient quick change in C and then come back to the initial value, motion to a new UCM (UCM2) will be followed by a return motion ending up in a new point (2) that deviates from the original point (1) along the UCM more than along the orthogonal to the UCM direction (ME > nME).

Figure 1 illustrates the notions of VUCM, VORT, ME and nME using a task of producing a particular magnitude of the sum of two variables: E1 + E2 = C. For a given C, the solution space is shown with the slanted dashed line (UCM1). A synergy stabilizing (E1 + E2) is reflected in the cloud of data points measured in consecutive trials elongated along the UCM (VUCM > VORT). If the subject of this experiment is asked to produce a transient quick change in C and then return to the initial value, motion to a new UCM (UCM2) will be followed by a return motion ending up at a new point (2) that deviates from the original point (1) along the UCM more than along the orthogonal to the UCM direction (ME > nME).

Both the inter-trial variance analysis and the ME analysis provide proxies of stability of the performance variable (reviewed in Latash 2016, 2017). They are not identical, however, and a few recent studies have documented different statistical effects on the corresponding sets of metrics (Mattos et al. 2011, 2015). Note that ME analysis has never been performed in the space of muscle activations (or M-modes) during whole-body tasks. This analysis also has never been applied to data in neurological patients. In this study, we explore the links between the ME and variance analyses, and the potential usefulness of the ME analysis for clinical studies by reanalyzing data from two earlier experiments (Falaki et al. 2016, Falaki et al. 2017). Specifically, we addressed the following hypotheses. Hypothesis-1: PD patients will show smaller ME components during voluntary cyclical sway compared to control subjects (cf. smaller VUCM in PD, Falaki et al. 2016). Hypothesis-2: PD patients will show smaller ME components off-drugs compared to the on-drug state (cf. smaller VUCM off-drugs, Falaki et al. 2016). Hypothesis-3: the ME component will correlate with VUCM, whereas the nME component will correlate with VORT. The last hypothesis is based on statistical considerations described in more detail in the Discussion.

We also explored the time profiles in the outcome indices within the sway cycle using a factor Phase. This exploration was based on earlier studies showing correlations of VORT with the rate of change of the salient performance variable and correlation of VUCM with the magnitude of that variable (Latash et al. 2002; Scholz et al. 2002). These findings were interpreted later within a model of synergic control (Goodman et al. 2005), which assigned VORT to variance in setting timing parameters during a cyclical action and VUCM to variance in setting spatial parameters. We expected similar relations between the rate of change of COP and nME, and between its magnitude and ME.

Methods

This paper reports results of additional analysis of the data collected in two studies and published earlier (Falaki et al. 2016, 2017). Here we briefly describe the materials and methods of the two studies.

Subjects

In the first experiment, 11 patients (6 females) diagnosed with PD at stage-II according to Hoehn and Yahr (HY; Hoehn and Yahr 1967), age 69 ± 6.3 years (mean ± standard deviation), and 11 healthy adults (age 65 ± 8.1 years, 5 females) volunteered to participate. In the second experiment, 10 PD patients (6 HY stage II and 4 HY stage III; age 69 ± 6.3; 5 females) participated. Three patients took part in both experiments. All of the participants were right-hand dominant according to their preferential hand use during writing and eating, except for one ambidextrous control subject (Sub #C07, experiment 1) and one ambidextrous patient (Sub #P17, experiment 2). Table 1 presents the general characteristics of the participants, as well as the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS-III) motor scores, disease duration since diagnosis, and the Levodopa equivalent dose (LED; Tomlinson et al. 2010). Control subjects were free from any diagnosed neurological or musculoskeletal disorder, and patients had no other known neurological or motor disorder except PD. None of the HY stage II patients showed signs of clinically identifiable postural instability (falls or positive responses to the shoulder pull test described in UPDRS) and no patient showed drug-induced dyskinesia on their prescribed medications. Participants were recruited from a large subject pool of an ongoing clinical study at the Penn State Milton S. Hershey Medical Center and were referred by their neurologist. All participants gave written informed consent according to the protocol approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Hershey Medical Center.

Table 1. Description of the participants.

Patients

Sub ID Sex Age Symptom Onset Duration (y) UPDRS motor (Off) UPDRS motor (On) LED, mg/day Experiment 1 Experiment 2
P01 F 72 R 3.5 11 300 3 mo later
P02 F 73 L 4.5 18 480
P03 F 72 L 4 38 300
P04 F 70 Bilateral 0.8 13 500 2.5 mo later
P05 M 72 R 4.6 15 167
P06 M 69 Bilateral 1.6 8 195
P07 M 79 R 2.2 8 500 1 mo later
P08 M L 7.6 5 738
P09 M 71 R 3.1 21 400
P10 F 55 R 2.2 2 250
P11 F 63 R 4.7 18 700
P12 M 63 L 11.7 21 13 667
P13 M 64 R 8.6 46 30 635
P14 M 71 Bilateral 1.5 19 8 250
P15 F 64 R 12.2 16 9 550
P04 F 70 Bilateral 1.1 37 33 500
P17 F 67 L 3.3 27 8 750
P01 F 73 R 3.8 10 6 480
P19 F 67 R 4.8 38 25 700
P20 M 75 L 8.6 31 18 650
P07 M 79 R 2.3 29 25 600
Controls

Sub ID Sex Age

C01 M 56
C02 F 58
C03 F 67
C04 F 54
C05 M 77
C06 M 78
C07 M 69
C08 F 71
C09 M 64
C10 M 63
C11 F 61

Parkinson's disease participants who were common in both experiments are shown in bold. M/F: male/females, R/L: right/left, LED: levodopa equivalent dose

Apparatus

Subjects stood on a force platform (AMTI, OPTIMA) that recorded the horizontal and vertical components of the ground reaction force (FX, FY, and FZ, respectively), as well as the three moments of force about the XYZ axes (MX, MY, and MZ). In order to compute the coordinates of the center of pressure (COP), we used the platform coordinate system in which the X-axis was a horizontal axis pointing in the anterior direction, the Y-axis represented a horizontal axis pointing toward the right side of the subject's body, and the Z-axis was a vertical axis pointing downward (Fig. 2). Participants were provided with visual feedback on their COP coordinates in the anterior-posterior direction (COPAP) and in the medial-lateral direction (COPML) computed using standard methods (Winter et al., 1996). This feedback was presented as a 5-mm white circle on a 23″ monitor mounted at eye level about 1.5 m away from the subject. To ensure safety and comfort, participants wore a safety harness affixed to the ceiling and an experimenter stood by them during the course of the experiment.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Experimental setup. A: Participants stood on the force platform with arms crossed over the chest and swayed mainly about ankle joints in the anterior-posterior direction. The platform coordinate system also is shown. A safety harness always was worn by participants (not drawn). B: Visual feedback of both COPAP and COPML. Participants shifted their center of pressure between targets shown on the screen as two horizontal lines (6 cm peak-to-peak and symmetrical with respect to the initial standing position).

Surface electromyography (EMG) was recoded using rectangular-shaped active electrodes that housed built-in pre-amplifiers on the recording heads (Delsys, Trigno Wireless EMG). Electrodes were placed over the bellies of 13 leg and trunk muscles on the right side of the body: tibialis anterior (TA), soleus (SOL), gastrocnemius medialis (GM), gastrocnemius lateralis (GL), biceps femoris (BF), semitendinosus (ST), rectus femoris (RF), vastus lateralis (VL), vastus medialis (VM), tensor fasciae latae (TFL), lumbar erector spinae (ESL), thoracic erector spinae (EST), and rectus abdominis (RA). EMG sensors were placed according to published guidelines (Criswell and Cram, 2011) and subsequently confirmed by observing EMG patterns of related isometric contractions and free movements. We recorded EMGs on one side of the body based on earlier studies documenting bilateral changes in indices of synergies with no side differences in PD patients, even in those at Hoehn & Yahr stage-I that is defined clinically as the stage at which PD symptoms are limited to one side of the body (Park et al. 2012, 2014).

A data collection computer (Dell, Xenon 2 GHz) with a customized LabVIEW-based program (LabVIEW 2013, National Instruments) was used to digitize the EMG and force platform data at the sampling frequency of 1 KHz with a 16-bit resolution (PCI-6225, National Instruments).

Procedures

Initially, participants were asked to stand barefoot on the platform while keeping their feet parallel at shoulder width. We enforced the same initial posture throughout different trials by marking the position of each foot on the platform surface and asking subjects to reproduce this initial position at the beginning of each trial.

Quiet standing (QS) task

Each participant stood quietly on the force platform—with arms crossed over the chest and fingertips rested on the shoulders—for 30 s and tried to avoid body movements. This was the only task without visual feedback on the COP coordinates. The purpose of this task was to measure the baseline activity of recorded muscles for further correction and normalization of EMG signals (see ‘Data processing’) to facilitate between-subjects comparisons.

Voluntary sway (VS) task

Each participant stood on the platform and held the initial posture with arms crossed over the chest. The task was to perform continuous voluntary whole-body sway about the ankle joints in the anterior-posterior direction, shifting COPAP between two targets shown as two horizontal lines on the screen, while trying to minimize COPML deviations (Klous et al. 2010; Falaki et al. 2016). During this task, visual feedback of both COPAP and COPML was available on the screen (Fig. 2B). Positions of the targets were set at 3-cm posterior and 3-cm anterior to the neutral standing position (6 cm peak-to-peak amplitude). Each participant performed the VS task twice while being paced by a sound metronome at 0.5 Hz. The participants kept their feet in full contact with the platform during the course of the task, and each trial lasted 30 s. There was a 30-s rest period between trials. In case of excessive hip and/or knee joint involvement, the trial was repeated. Participants performed two practice trials, each lasting 15 s, prior to data collection. For the purposes of analysis, the time interval between two consecutive anterior-most COPAP coordinates was defined as a VS cycle.

Experiment 1

In this experiment, patients were tested on their normal schedule of medication. The experiment started with measuring UPDRS-III motor scores by a trained clinician, after which all participants performed the QS task followed by the VS task.

Experiment 2

This experiment involved two sessions. The first session started early in the morning following an overnight withdrawal of the prescribed PD medication, prior to taking the first daily dose (off-drug condition). When participants finished the first session, they took their prescribed medication. The second session started one hour after taking the medication (on-drug condition). Each session started with measuring UPDRS-III motor scores followed by performing the QS and VS tasks.

Data processing

EMG and force platform signals were processed offline using a customized Matlab R2015a program. Prior to computing time-varying COPAP coordinates (Winter et al, 1996), force platform signals were filtered with a 10 Hz low-pass, forth-order, zero-lag Butterworth filter. For VS trials, to avoid edge effects and incomplete cycles at the beginning and end of each trial, only the data within {3 s; 28 s} time interval were accepted. The time interval between two consecutive anterior-most COPAP coordinates was defined as a sway cycle. On average, each participant performed 10 cycles within each VS trial. There was no statistically significant difference between the average number of cycles performed by PD and CO groups (Experiment 1), or between on-drug and off-drug conditions (Experiment 2). For the analysis of variance and motor equivalence, cycles that had an extra local COPAP peak and those with the most anterior (posterior) COPAP coordinates that differed from the corresponding means by more than ±2 SD were removed from further analysis. Such aberrant cycles likely reflected distracted attention by the subject; they had to be removed to satisfy one of the main assumption of the analysis, namely that the subjects were trying “to do the same” across cycles. The total number of rejected cycles varied between 0 and 3 per subject.

Raw EMG data were shifted 50 ms backward with respect to the force platform data to compensate for the electro-mechanical delay (Corcos et al. 1992; Cavanagh and Komi 1997), band-pass filtered (20-350 Hz) with a fourth-order, zero-lag Butterworth filter, and then rectified. A 100-ms window moving average filter then was used to create the envelope of the rectified EMG signals. For each participant, filtered EMG of each muscle was corrected for the background activity by removing the baseline muscle activity during the QS trial (EMGQS) defined as the average value within the time period {13 s; 17 s}. Further EMGs were normalized by the peak value (EMGPEAK) of the corresponding muscle across all sways.

EMGNORM=EMGEMGQSEMGPEAK (1)

Defining muscle modes

We assumed that the central nervous system (CNS) organizes muscles into groups with close to parallel scaling of activation levels within each group (cf. d'Avella et al. 2003; Krishnamoorthy et al. 2003). We have defined these muscle groups as muscle modes or M-modes. To identify M-modes for each subject, the correlation matrix of EMG data normalized and averaged over each 50-ms time window (IEMGNORM) from the VS trials was subjected to principal component analysis (PCA) with Varimax rotation and factor extraction (Krishnamoorthy et al. 2003; Danna-Dos-Santos et al. 2007). The first four PCs were selected, in the order of total variance for which they accounted, based on the Kaiser criterion, and confirmed by visual inspection of the inflection point of the scree plot. Additional criteria were that each muscle had to be significantly loaded (loading beyond ±0.5) on at least one of the four PCs and there could be no PCs without at least one significantly loaded muscle.

As in the aforementioned earlier studies, we decided to accept a fixed number of M-modes across groups and conditions rather than aim for a similar amount of total variance accounted for by the selected modes. Note that adding an M-mode to a set increases the UCM dimensionality without affecting ORT dimensionality (see later). Since variance magnitudes are normalized per dimension (whereas ME and nME indices are normalized per square root of dimensionality), this would result in unfair comparisons. Indeed, adding an M-mode that accounts for little variance would reduce variance within the UCM (and ME) after normalization without affecting variance within ORT (and nME). Moreover, adding a fifth M-mode would create more modes with only one muscle significantly loaded, which goes against the basic idea of identifying muscle groups with parallel scaling of activation.

We used PCA rather than the widely used non-negative matrix factorization method (NMF; Ivanenko et al. 2004; Ting and McKay 2007; d'Avella and Bizzi 2005) because PCA generates orthogonal eigenvectors while NMF generates a set of independent but not orthogonal vectors. Note that orthogonal bases are much more convenient for the analysis of variance and motor equivalence (see later). Also, note that an earlier study has shown comparable results from the PCA analysis with Varimax rotation and factor extraction and from NMF (Tresch et al. 2006).

Defining the Jacobian matrix

Linear relations between small changes in elemental variables (ΔM) and COPAP shifts (ΔCOPAP) were assumed (Krishnamoorthy et al. 2003; Danna-Dos-Santos et al. 2007). Averaged over each 50-ms time window, EMG and COPAP data from all cycles during the VS task were used to run multiple linear regression analysis without intercept. For each participant, coefficients of the regression analysis defined the Jacobian (J) matrix:

ΔCOPAP=k1ΔM1+k2ΔM2+k3ΔM3+k4ΔM4J=[k1k2k3k4] (2)

The J matrix determines how small deviations of M-mode magnitudes affect the selected performance variable (the COPAP coordinate). Prior to the regression analysis, a fourth-order, zero-lag Butterworth filter was used to low-pass filter ΔCOPAP and ΔM at 5 Hz to remove effects of high-frequency data components that were unrelated to voluntary body sway.

Analysis of variance

According to the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) hypothesis (Scholz and Schöner, 1999), elemental variables (in this study M-modes) are organized to provide stability of a potentially important performance variable (in this study, COPAP). Within this approach, two inter-trial (inter-cycle) variance components are quantified in the M-mode space. The first component is within the UCM (VUCM, variance preserving the COPAP coordinate), whereas the second component is in the orthogonal to the UCM sub-space (VORT, variance affecting the COPAP coordinate). The M-mode space had the dimensionality of n = 4. Since the hypothesis of COPAP stabilization accounted for one degree of freedom (d = 1), the UCM sub-space had the dimensionality of three.

For each participant, the duration of all of the accepted sway cycles during the VS task was normalized to 100%. For each time-normalized step (1% of the sway cycle), the mean vector of M-mode magnitudes across cycles () was subtracted from vectors of individual M-mode magnitudes from each cycle resulting in mean-free values of M-modes changes, ΔMdemeaned:

ΔMdemeaned=MM¯ (3)

To compute VUCM and VORT, ΔMdemeaned was projected onto the UCM (fUCM) and the ORT space (fORT). The null-space of the J matrix served as a linear approximation of the UCM. Variance within the UCM and ORT, normalized by dimension, was computed as:

VUCM=σUCM2=i=1N|fUCM|2(nd)NtrialsVORT=σORT2=i=1N|fORT|2dNtrials (4)

Analysis of motor equivalence

The motor equivalence analysis (Mattos et al. 2011, 2013, 2015) quantifies the amount of deviations in the space of elemental variables (M-modes) that occurs in directions preserving (motor equivalent, ME), changing (non-motor equivalent, nME) the performance variable, COPAP. For each subject and each time-normalized step, deviations of M-modes from one sway cycle to the next were projected onto the null-space of J (fME) and its orthogonal complement (fnME):

ΔM=MiM¯(i+1)fME=i=1nd(ɛiTΔM)TɛiTfnME=ΔM(fME)T

In equation (5), εi stands for basis vectors that spanned the null-space of J. The length of this projection within the UCM sub-space was the ME component, whereas the length of the projection within the ORT reflected the nME component. For quantitative comparisons, ME and nME components were normalized by the square root of the dimension in the respective spaces (cf. Mattos et al. 2011):

ME=fME(nd)ME=fnMEd (6)

For each subject, the average of the ME and nME components across time-normalized cycles was computed for statistical analyses. Note that we used average indices over all the cycles for this analysis. We viewed this as appropriate for the first step in using the ME analysis to identify postural stability problems in PD. In future, ME and nME indices can be computed as averages over 3-4 trials only, particular for non-cyclical tasks.

Statistics

Statistical analysis was performed with SPSS 21.0 (IBM Corp.). In the text and figures, data are shown as means ± standard errors (SE). To explore the dependence of outcome variables (kinematics of sway, VUCM, VORT, ME and nME) on the phase of swaying, the factor Phase with four levels was used to represent four windows with a length equal to 11% of the sway cycle and centered about the maximum, minimum, and in-between phases of the cycle: ‘95-5%’ (representing 95-100% of cycle n-1 and 1-5% of cycle n), ‘20-30%’, ‘45-55%, and ‘70-80%’. In Experiment 1, two-way repeated measures ANOVAs were used to explore the effects of factor Phase (four levels) and Group (two levels: PD and CO) on the outcome variables. Repeated measures ANOVAs with factors Group, Variance (two levels: VUCM and VORT), and/or Motor equivalence (two levels: ME and nME) were used to investigate effects of PD on the averaged across cycle variances and motor equivalence components. In Experiment 2, factor Medication (two levels: off-drug and on-drug) replaced factor Group in order to explore effects of the dopamine-replacement drug on the outcome variables. Data were checked for normality and sphericity. In case of violation of the assumption of sphericity, Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used for degrees-of-freedom adjustments. To explore significant effects at the level of p < 0.05, pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni corrections were performed. Correlations between variance components (VUCM and VORT) and motor equivalence components (ME and nME) were explored using Pearson correlation coefficients. We present median values and inter-quartile ranges (IQR) for coefficients of determination (R2).

Results

Voluntary sway kinematics

All participants, including patients in the off-drug condition, were able to perform qualitatively similar ‘sine-like’ time-profile of COPAP during the voluntary body sway task. Figure 3 shows the mean (top panels) and SD (bottom panels) of the COPAP averaged over VS cycles and across participants separately for Experiment 1 and Experiment 2. Participants were at the anterior-most position at 0% and 100% of the time-normalized sway cycle, whereas they were at the posterior-most position approximately at 50% of the sway time. There was no statistically significant difference in the COPAP coordinate at different sway phases between PD and control participants (Experiment 1), and between the off-drug and on-drug conditions (Experiment 2) in the two-way ANOVAs with factors Phase and Group (Medication). The ANOVA showed only the trivial main effect of Phase. The average peak rate of COPAP shifts, averaged across forward and backward phases, (dCOP/dt)MAX, however, was significantly smaller in PD subjects (19.4 ± 1.65 cm/s) than controls (25.9 ± 1.61 cm/s), confirmed by a oneway ANOVA with factor Group (F(1,20) = 7.98; p < 0.05). Although the SD of the COPAP was larger in controls, the group difference was non-significant (F(1,20) = 3.83; p > 0.05). In Experiment 2, there was no statistically significant differences between (dCOP/dt)MAX in the off-drug (19.1 ± 1.80 cm/s) and on-drug (20.9 ± 1.49 cm/s) conditions, and no difference between the SD of the COPAP (off-drug: 0.82 ± 0.05; on-drug: 0.92 ± 0.07).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Time-normalized profiles of the center of pressure coordinate in the anterior-posterior direction (COPAP; A and B) and the standard deviation of COPAP (SD-COP; C and D) with standard error shades. Participants were at the anterior-most position at 0% and 100%, of the sway cycle. At about 50% of the sway cycle, participants were at the posterior-most position. A and C: Experiment 1. The dashed line represents the mean data for PD patients and the solid line represents the mean data for the controls. B and D: Experiment 2. The dashed line represents the mean data for the off-drug condition and the solid line represents the mean data for the on-drug condition.

Defining M-modes and Jacobians

Based on the criteria described in the Methods, four M-modes were identified in all subjects. Table 2 represents a typical set of muscle loadings for a PD participant in the on-drug and off-drug conditions. M1-mode and M2-mode typically involved significantly loaded dorsal or ventral muscles and are called either dorsal or ventral M-mode. The composition of those modes was similar in all subjects and did not differ in PD patients based on the side of symptom onset. The other two M-modes compositions were more variable across subjects. Loadings over 0.5 are shown in bold.

Table 2. Muscle loadings for the first four M-modes in a typical PD patient.

Muscle Off-drug On-drug


M1 M2 M3 M4 M1 M2 M3 M4
TA 0.75 -0.38 -0.17 -0.18 0.73 0.40 -0.25 -0.03
SOL -0.45 0.72 0.30 0.13 -0.37 -0.86 0.22 -0.03
GM -0.46 0.77 0.26 0.10 -0.29 -0.91 0.14 -0.01
GL -0.42 0.74 0.33 0.09 -0.23 -0.86 0.31 -0.01
BF -0.01 0.90 0.03 -0.03 -0.37 -0.74 0.34 -0.04
ST 0.81 -0.09 -0.01 0.02 -0.17 -0.08 0.76 -0.07
RF 0.86 -0.31 -0.20 -0.06 0.81 0.43 -0.22 0.02
VL 0.78 -0.17 -0.29 0.15 0.82 0.40 -0.15 0.03
VM 0.88 -0.34 -0.09 -0.03 0.88 0.26 -0.14 0.04
TFL 0.87 -0.08 -0.18 0.03 0.85 0.08 -0.25 -0.01
ESL -0.16 0.16 0.86 0.04 -0.21 -0.35 0.75 0.00
EST -0.17 0.20 0.85 0.02 -0.23 -0.30 0.73 0.03
RA 0.02 0.08 0.04 0.98 0.02 0.03 -0.04 0.99

Loading factors after Varimax rotation for a typical Parkinson's disease patient in the off-drug and on-drug conditions. Significant loadings (greater than 0.5) are shown in bold. TA - tibialis anterior, SOL - soleus, GM - gastrocnemius medialis, GL - gastrocnemius lateralis, BF - biceps femoris, ST - semitendinosus, RF - rectus femoris, VL - vastus lateralis, VM - vastus medialis, TFL - tensor fasciae latae, ESL - lumbar erector spinae, EST - thoracic erector spinae, RA -rectus abdominis.

In Experiment 1, the four M-modes accounted for, on average, significantly less variance in the muscle activation space in the PD group compared to controls (71.5 ± 1.74% vs. 78.3 ± 1.74%; F(1,20) = 7.969; p < 0.05). In Experiment 2, the M-modes accounted for significantly less variance in the muscle activation space in the off-drug condition compared to its respective value in the on-drug condition (68.6 ± 2.2% vs. 74.7 ± 2.4%; t(9) = -4.51, p < 0.01). In both Experiments, all M-modes were significant predictors of COPAP shifts for each subject (p < 0.001).

Analysis of variance and motor equivalence

For each subject and/or medication condition, we used the framework of the UCM hypothesis to quantify two components of the inter-cycle variance in the M-mode space: the component affecting (VORT) and not affecting (VUCM) the COPAP coordinate during the VS task. The same data also were used to quantify the deviations in the M-mode space across consecutive cycles that preserved (ME) or changed (nME) the COPAP coordinate. The time profiles of VUCM and ME were similar to each other, whereas the time profiles of VORT and nME were aligned closely. Figures 4 and 5 present averaged time profiles over cycles of ME and nME (top panels) and VUCM and VORT (bottom panels) for the two Experiments. Note the qualitative similarity of the top and bottom graphs. In particular, the nME and VORT plots show a two-peak profile resembling that expected for the absolute value of dCOP(t)/dt if COP(t) were a perfect sine wave (compare to the bottom panels in Fig. 3).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Results of the motor equivalence and variance analyses in Experiment 1. Time-normalized profiles of the motor equivalent (ME, panel A) and non-motor equivalent (nME, panel B) components, variance within the UCM (VUCM, panel C), and within ORT (VORT, panel D) for PD patients (dashed line) and controls (CO, solid line). Note the higher magnitudes of all indices in the controls and the typical double-peaked patterns of nME and VORT.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Results of the motor equivalence and variance analyses in Experiment 2. Time-normalized profiles of the motor equivalent (ME, panel A) and non-motor equivalent (nME, panel B) components, variance within the UCM (VUCM, panel C), and within ORT (VORT, panel D) for PD patients off-drug (dashed line) and on-drug (solid line). Note the higher magnitudes of all indices on-drug and the typical double-peaked patterns of nME and VORT.

To explore possible links of the outcome variables to parameters of the task, such as COP(t) and dCOP(t)/dt, we used linear regression analysis. To explore the similarity between the pairs of outcome variables, VUCM vs. ME and VORT vs. nME, we performed statistical analyses on the effects of the main factors (Phase, Group, and Medication) and correlation analyses within these pairs of variables.

Experiment 1

A two-way ANOVA, Group × Variance, showed that control subjects had significantly larger amounts of total inter-cycle variance in the M-mode space compared to PD patients (0.023 ± 0.002 vs. 0.016 ± 0.002; effect of Group F(1,20) = 10.32; p < 0.01). Both VUCM and VORT in the PD group (Vucm: 0.012 ± 0.001; VORT: 0.020 ± 0.003) were smaller compared to the control group (VUCM: 0.018 ± 0.001; VORT: 0.029 ± 0.003). VORT was, on average, larger than Vucm (effect of Variance; F(1,20) = 23.15; p < 0.001). A two-way ANOVA, Phase × Group, performed separately on VUCM and VORT showed no significant modulation of VUCM across the four phases of the sway cycle, whereas VORT showed a significant Phase effect (F(3,60) = 12.80; p < 0.001), and a significant Group × Phase interaction (F(3,60) = 3.39; p < 0.05). Pairwise comparisons revealed that VORT in phases 1 and 3 was smaller than its respective values in phases 2 and 4 (p < 0.05). The interaction reflected the fact that the effect of phase on VORT was significant in the control group only.

Hypothesis 1

Similar results were obtained in the analysis of motor equivalence. In particular, both ME and nME in PD (ME: 0.121 ± 0.005; nME: 0.139 ± 0.10) were significantly smaller than their respective values in controls (ME: 0.138 ± 0.005; nME: 0.171 ± 0.010). This was confirmed by a significant effect of Group (F(1,20) = 8.24; p < 0.01) in a two-way ANOVA Group × Motor equivalence. The nME component was larger than the ME component in both groups (F(1,20) = 14.64; p < 0.001) without an interaction. A two-way ANOVA, Group × Phase, on nME showed significant effects of Group (F(1,20) = 5.50; p < 0.05), Phase (F(3,60) = 14.54; p < 0.001), and a Group × Phase interaction (F(3,60) = 2.97; p < 0.05). Similar to VORT, pairwise comparisons revealed smaller magnitudes of nME in phases 1 and 3 compared with phases 2 and 4 (p < 0.05). In PD patients, only the amount of nME in phase 2 was larger than in phase 3 (p < 0.05).

Further, we explored correlations between the nME (and VORT) indices and the derivative of the COPAP (t) for each subject using a liner regression analysis: VORT (nME) = a + b |dCOP/dt|. The values of VORT (nME) and |COPap (t)/dt| were taken at each 1% from the time-normalized cycles. Table 3 summarizes the median, the first quartile, and the third quartile values. In 10 out of 11 controls, VORT was correlated linearly with |dCOP/dt|. A similar relationship existed between nME and |dCOP/dt|. For the PD group, these correlations were significant in seven out of 11 patients.

Table 3. Results of the correlation analysis.
Group VORT vs. |dCOP/dt| nME vs. |dCOP/dt| VUCM vs. COP(t) ME vs. COP(t)
Median Q1, Q3 Median Q1, Q3 Median Q1, Q3 Median Q1, Q3
PD patients 0.11 0.04; 0.26 0.07 0.03; 0.20 0.17 0.13; 0.21 0.11 0.05; 0.19
Controls 0.37 0.24; 0.42 0.31 0.24; 0.47 0.26 0.05; 0.39 0.10 0.08; 0.18
PD off-drug 0.08 0.02; 0.16 0.07 0.04; 0.29 0.27 0.10; 0.38 0.09 0.02; 0.36
PD on-drug 0.06 0.02; 0.09 0.07 0.05; 0.11 0.18 0.04; 0.26 0.14 0.07; 0.24

The median and inter-quartile values of the correlation coefficients between the VORT (nME) indices and the derivative of the COPAP (t), and between the VUCM (ME) indices and COPAP(t). Q1: the first quartile, Q3: the third quartile

The correlation between VUCM and COPAP (t) was also investigated using linear regression: VUCM (ME) = a + b COP(t). The values of VUCM and COP(t) were taken at each 1% of the sway cycle. VUCM was correlated linearly with COPAP (t) in nine controls and 10 PD patients. Comparing ME and COPAP (t), a linear relation was observed in nine controls and nine PD patients.

Experiment 2

Dopaminergic medications led to an increase in the amount of VUCM (0.017 ± 0.002 vs. 0.012 ± 0.002 on- and off-drug, respectively; F(1,9) = 6.08; p < 0.05) without a change in VORT (0.021 ± 0.004 vs. 0.021 ± 0.003; on- and off-drug, respectively). There also was an effect of Phase (F(3,54) = 3.96; p < 0.05) on VUCM: On average, VUCM in phase 2 (0.012 ± 0.001) was smaller than phase 3 (0.017 ± 0.002), as confirmed by pairwise comparisons. There were no significant effects of Phase on VORT.

Hypothesis 2

Similar results were obtained in the analysis of the ME and nME components. In particular, ME on-drug was larger compared to the off-drug condition (0.134 ± 0.005 vs. 0.118 ± 0.006; F(1,9) = 17.60; p < 0.01). In contrast, there were no effects of Medication on the nME component (on-drug: 0.143 ± 0.008; off-drug 0.141 ± 0.010). There were no significant Phase effects on either ME or nME.

Overall, correlations between ME(t) and COPAP(t) and between nME(t) and dCOPAP(t)/dt were weak, although statistically significant in some of the patients. On-drug, the absolute value of dCOP(t)/dt was related linearly to VORT indices in seven patients and to the nME in eight patients. Off-drug, these relationships were significant for VORT in six patients and for nME in seven patients. In addition, there was a significant relationship between VUCM and COPAP (t) in seven patients and between ME and COPAP (t) in nine patients in the on-drug condition. Off-drug, COPAP (t) was correlated with VUCM indices in nine patients and with ME in seven patients. A summary of the median and inter-quartile values is presented in Table 3.

Hypothesis 3

Correlations between ME and VUCM, and between nME and VORT were explored using Pearson correlation coefficients. For each subjects, time-normalized data points of variance components (VUCM and VORT) were averaged over the cycle and plotted against the respective motor equivalence components (ME and nME), also averaged over the cycle. Figure 6 represents scatter plots of VUCM and ME, as well as VORT and nME for Experiment 1 (panels A and B) and Experiment 2 (panels C and D) with the lease squares line of best fit. In this Figure, data points refer to individual subjects. There was a significant correlation between VUCM and ME (PD: R2 = 0.49; CO: R2 = 0.4; p < 0.05), and between VORT and nME component (PD: R2 = 0.87; CO: R2 = 0.84; p < 0.05) in Experiment 1. In Experiment 2, there also was a significant correlation between VUCM and ME (off-drug: R2 = 0.76; on-drug: R2 = 0.42; p < 0.05), and between VORT and nME (off-drug: R2 = 0.48; on-drug: R2 = 0.91; p < 0.05).

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Scatter plots of the variance and motor equivalence indices across subjects. Correlations between VUCM and the ME component for Experiment 1 (panel A) and Experiment 2 (panel C) and between VORT and the nME component for Experiment 1 (panel B) and Experiment 2 (panel D) are presented with regression lines and coefficients of determination.

We also quantified correlations between the same pairs of variables within each participant using cross-correlation analysis of time-normalized data points. Peak correlation coefficients were large for both Experiment 1 and Experiment 2. In Experiment 1, peak correlation coefficients between VUCM and ME ranged from 0.35 to 0.87 for PD subjects (median: 0.77; IQR: 0.67, 0.84) and from 0.39 to 0.91 for controls (median: 0.73; IQR: 0.65, 0.79). For the analysis of VORT vs. nME, peak correlation coefficients ranged from 0.56 to 0.93 for PD subjects (median: 0.75; IQR: 0.70, 0.86) and from 0.55 to 0.96 for controls (median: 0.87; IQR: 0.76, 0.92). The time-lag of the peak correlation coefficient was close to zero in all comparisons (note that we used cycle percentage as a unit of time): The mean ± SE of the time-lag for the controls was –3.6 ± 3.35 in % cycle for the ME-VUCM analysis and 0 ± 0 for the nME-VORT analysis. For PD patients, the time-lag of cross-correlation analysis was –1.9 ± 1.91 for the ME-VUCM analysis and –1.3 ± 1.27 for the nME-VORT analysis. There was no statistically significant difference between the groups.

In Experiment 2, the time-lag was zero for all participants. For the off-drug condition, correlation coefficients between VUCM and ME ranged from 0.68 to 0.90 (median: 0.84; IQR: 0.78, 0.88) and between VORT and nME ranged from 0.54 to 0.91 (median: 0.83; IQR: 0.77, 0.87). For the on-drug condition, correlation coefficients between VUCM and ME ranged between 0.62 and 0.94 (median: 0.77; IQR: 0.70, 0.87) and between VORT and nME from 0.54 to 0.88 (median: 0.80; IQR: 0.76, 0.86). There was no statistically significant difference between correlation coefficients due to the medication that was confirmed by a one-way ANOVA.

Discussion

The three main hypotheses formulated in the Introduction have been supported by the data. Indeed, in Experiment-1, PD patients showed smaller motor equivalent (ME) components within the space of muscle modes during voluntary cyclical sway compared to control subjects in support of our Hypothesis-1. In Experiment-2, PD patients showed smaller ME components off-drugs compared to the on-drug state in support of Hypothesis-2. Both qualitative comparisons (Figures 4 and 5) and correlation analyses confirmed similar patterns between the ME component and the variance within the UCM (VUCM) and between the nME component and the variance within the ORT space (VORT). Effects of the main factors on the metrics computed in the analysis of variance and in the analysis of motor equivalence were similar in both experiments. These observations support Hypothesis-3.

Our comparisons of the two types of indices, those reflecting inter-cycle variance structure (VUCM and VORT) and those reflecting motor equivalence estimated across consecutive cycles (ME and nME), have shown that they both are sensitive to PD and dopamine-replacement medications. Note that our Experiment 1 involved PD patients at stage II according to Hoehn and Yahr (1967), which is defined as a stage without clinically identifiable postural instability. Nevertheless, both ME and variance indices showed significant differences between the PD and control groups. Both pairs of indices are proxies for the stability of a functionally important variable (COPAP) estimated within the muscle mode space. The results suggest that indices of motor equivalence may be sensitive to emerging problems with postural stability even at stages when clinical examination fails to detect those problems. Experiment 2 confirmed that these indices also are sensitive to effects of dopamine-replacement medication. The fact that these two indices are correlated closely suggests that ME indices may become a practical measurement for clinical research to gauge synergic control of postural stability. Although we used averages over numerous cycles to produce ME indices in the current study, theoretically a few trials may be sufficient. This is something to explore in the future.

Synergies – mechanisms of task-specific stability

Our study is based on the concept of multi-element synergies stabilizing important, task-specific variables in a hierarchical system (reviewed in Latash 2010, 2016, 2017). This general theoretical framework is based on the idea of parametric control, i.e., the neural control of movements based on specifying time patterns of parameters of natural laws. It suggests that neural variables at a hierarchically high, task-specific level encode referent coordinates (RCs) for salient performance variables. Further, as a result of several few-to-many transformations, these task-specific RCs are transformed into RCs for individual effectors, joints, and muscles. At the muscle level, RC is equivalent to threshold of the tonic stretch reflex, as in the classical equilibrium-point hypothesis (Feldman 1966, 1986). The transformations are organized in such a way that spontaneous deviations of performance at the level of elements (due to changing intrinsic neural states and external forces) co-vary to keep variability of the salient variable relatively low. Such organizations have been addressed as synergies stabilizing those performance variables (Latash et al. 2007).

Note that proper stability, as ensured by synergies, is a crucial feature of all functional actions. For example, during steady-state performance, low stability may lead to failure and/or necessity to monitor performance and make corrections at all times. Another important aspect of synergies is their task-specificity: Different performance variables can be stabilized by the same set of elemental variables (e.g., M-modes) depending on the task. This allows several tasks to be performed with a single set of effectors and to avoid interference among salient performance variables when a few tasks are conducted simultaneously.

The story about synergies and their changes in neurological disorders would be incomplete without mentioning the phenomenon of anticipatory synergy adjustments (ASAs, Olafsdottir et al. 2005), which represents attenuation of a synergy in preparation for a quick action. This important functional mechanism allows a person to avoid fighting one's own synergies during quick actions. ASAs have been documented across systems and tasks (Shim et al. 2005; Klous et al. 2011; Krishnan et al. 2011, 2012). This mechanism is impaired in a variety of patients with both subcortical and cortical disorders (Park et al. 2012; Jo et al. 2016; reviewed in Latash and Huang 2015), which may lead to difficulty with movement initiation and even episodes of freezing.

Whereas mechanisms of synergies (those responsible for organizing muscles into M-modes and those responsible for M-mode synergies stabilizing salient performance variables) remain largely unknown, recent studies in neurological patients provide insights into possible neurophysiological mechanisms of synergic control of different sets of effectors, from multi-finger actions, to multi-joint reaching, and to multi-muscle whole-body actions (reviewed in Latash and Huang 2015). In particular, those studies have documented significantly impaired synergy indices during steady-state tasks in patients with subcortical disorders, but not following cortical stroke (Reisman and Scholz 2003; Jo et al. 2016). These findings are compatible with the idea of distributed processing modules (Houk 2005) involving loops through the basal ganglia and the cerebellum. In contrast, ASAs are reduced and decreased in magnitude in patients with both cortical and subcortical disorders (Park et al. 2012; Jo et al. 2016). Indices of synergic control are sensitive to very early stages of PD; they can be seen in the apparently non-involved hands of patients at HY stage-I (Park et al. 2012, 2014) and even in asymptomatic persons at elevated risk for parkinsonism, such as professional welders (Lewis et al. 2016).

So far, all the clinical studies of synergies have used analysis of the structure of inter-trial variance in two sub-spaces, the UCM where the salient performance variable does not change, and the ORT where it changes (see Fig. 1). These methods have provided a wealth of useful information. Their broad application, however, has been slowed by the necessity to analyze clouds of data points that requires collecting numerous trials in patients. This is not always feasible given that many patient populations are characterized by quick fatigue. One of the goals of our study has been to define whether a method that is capable of producing indices of stability based on a few trials, i.e. analysis of motor equivalence, is comparably sensitive to changes in synergies resulting from PD and/or dopaminergic medication. Whereas our experiment used indices of motor equivalence averaged over all of the cycles, such indices can be computed using individual cycles/trials and then averaged over only a few cycles or trials.

Indices of variance and motor equivalence

During cyclical tasks, analyses of inter-cycle variance and inter-cycle mean distances are linked if one assumes a perfectly normal distribution of the data. Indeed, if we assume that each measurement at a particular phase of the cycle represents a sample from a normal distribution, the difference between two random samples will be another normal distribution with the mean μD = 0 and standard deviation σD linearly related to standard deviation of the original distribution, σD =σ√2. In the analysis of motor equivalence, we computed absolute distances between data points in consecutive cycles (see Methods). This makes the distribution of those distances non-negative (folded) with a new mean (μX) and new standard deviation (σX). From classical statistics (Leone et al. 1961):

μX=σ2/πσX2=σ2μX2 (7)

These equations can be applied separately to data in different sub-spaces of the original space. They suggest, in particular, that ME is expected to be proportional to the standard deviation within the UCM (σUCM), whereas nME is expected to be proportional to the standard deviation within the ORT space (σORT). Correlations with squared magnitudes of standard deviations, VUCM and VORT, also are expected, although they may be weaker.

Indeed, our analysis has shown that ME correlated with VUCM, whereas nME correlated with VORT (see also the similar shapes of VUCM and ME and of VORT and nME in Figs. 4 and 5). Although the current data set is limited and does not allow distinguishing between correlations with standard deviation and with variance (we explored both and found no significant differences), they allow drawing an important conclusion: At least in some tasks, indices of motor equivalence (ME and nME) can be used as synergy indices instead of metrics of intercycle variance. This conclusion is corroborated by the observations of similar significant differences in both variance indices and motor equivalence indices between the PD and control groups in Experiment 1 and between the PD patients on- and off-drug in Experiment 2. This result allows collecting only a handful of cycles/trials (< 5 trials) to obtain quantitative estimates of synergies stabilizing salient variables based on the ME and nME indices. As a result, analysis of motor equivalence may be applicable more easily to studies of populations with movement disorders who may be able to perform only a few cycles/trials of a given motor task.

The aforementioned statistical analysis potentially also can be applied to non-cyclical data. In discrete actions, however, it is more likely that individual trials differ from each other more than cycles within a continuous cyclical task due to the larger variance in setting central parameters that define the neural control process (cf. Friedman et al. 2009). As a result, a relatively small set of data points may deviate more from what is expected regarding a perfectly normal distribution. This may be the reason why, in some earlier studies, statistical effects on variance and motor equivalence indices were different (Mattos et al. 2011, 2015).

The time profiles of the ME and variance indices showed characteristic shapes, consistent across all subjects (Figures 4 and 5). In particular, ME and VUCM showed a relatively flat time profile with a small peak in the middle, whereas nME and VORT showed double-peaked profiles with the peaks approximating the times of maximal COPAP rate shifts. Such time profiles of VORT were reported earlier in studies of multi-finger force production (Latash et al. 2001, 2002; Scholz et al. 2002). Our correlation analyses confirmed associations between the COPAP coordinate and both VUCM and ME, and between the absolute magnitude of COP(t) rate and both VORT and nME in support of the model introduced earlier (Goodman et al. 2005; see Introduction).

The double-peaked profile of VORT and nME was more pronounced in the control subjects possibly reflecting two factors. First, the controls showed larger peak rates of COPAP shift, which are expected to lead to higher VORT even if variance in setting the timing parameter is unchanged between the groups (see equations in Friedman et al. 2009). Second, variance in setting timing parameters across cycles also may be higher in PD as suggested by observations of higher temporal variability in motor performance in PD subjects (Wing et al. 1984; Pastor et al. 1992; Almeida et al. 2002).

Analysis of synergies as a sensitive clinical tool

Studies of action stability during whole-body movements have used a variety of methods applied to different patient populations and functional tasks including vertical posture (Hausdorff et al. 1985; Cavanaugh et al. 2005; Riva et al. 2013; van Emmerik et al. 2014). Studies of patients with postural disorders explored analysis of postural sway during quiet standing (Visser et al. 2008), postural preparation to self-initiated action (King et al. 2010; Rogers et al. 2011), and responses to unexpected postural perturbations (Boonstra et al. 2014). Some of these studies produced indices sensitive to early stages of postural problems and effects of treatment.

We would like to emphasize the following advantages of our approach based on quantifying multi-muscle synergies during postural tasks. First, this approach is theory-based and rooted deeply in both physics and physiology. It is linked intimately to the theory of control of movements with time-varying spatial referent coordinates organized in a hierarchical way, to the principle of abundance, to the uncontrolled manifold hypothesis, and to the idea of control with spatial RCs (reviewed in Latash 2010, 2016; Feldman 2015). Second, the approach has proven its specificity to subcortical neurological disorders (reviewed in Latash and Huang 2015) in support of the idea of distributed processing modules involving loops through the cerebellum and the basal ganglia (Houk 2005). Third, the approach is highly sensitive to very early stages of disorders (such as PD subjects in HY stage-I, Park et al. 2012, 2013, 2014) and even to subclinical changes in persons at elevated risk for parkinsonism (Lewis et al. 2016). Other mentioned approaches are typically computational, use behavioral indices with no clear links to physics and physiology, and/or are not as sensitive to disease-related changes.

Given the obvious importance of proper control of action stability for motor function, we believe that analysis of synergies should be incorporated broadly into clinical research. So far, applications of this analysis have been limited, partly due to the necessity to collect numerous trials for analysis of inter-trial variance. We hope that the current study will pave the way toward broader use of this method by showing that, at least for some tasks, analysis of only a few trials may be able to provide data sensitive to early-stage PD and the effects of medication. We would like to emphasize that our methods were able to detect significant changes in indices of postural stability in PD patients at HY stage II, i.e., those who show no detectable problems with postural stability during neurological examination.

Concluding comments

This study has lessons for both basic and clinical research. It shows that indices of inter-cycle (potentially also inter-trial) variance and those of motor equivalence are strongly correlated with each other and show similar statistical effects. Within the idea of movement control with changes in referent body configurations (RC, Latash 2010, Feldman 2015), muscle modes may be viewed as reflections of elemental changes in the body RC, which are used as the basis for a variety of whole-body actions (Latash 2016). The elemental RC changes are used as elemental variables to produce synergies stabilizing task-specific salient variables. Both inter-trial variance structure and ME deviations are natural consequences of this scheme: An input from hierarchically higher levels leads to deviations in less stable directions, i.e. within the UCM, rather than in directions leading to shifts of the salient variable (i.e., within ORT). This is true for spontaneous, natural variations in the task-specific input as well as for its purposeful changes. Within this scheme, loss of stability produced by synergies is expected to be reflected in both variance and ME indices, as demonstrated in our study. This makes both indices potentially sensitive to neurological disorders characterized by reduced action stability such as PD. The fact that ME indices can be measured in individual trials makes this index very attractive for clinical studies, particularly when examination time may be a limiting factor.

We would like to acknowledge limitations of our study. In particular, we recorded and analyzed EMG signals from one side of the body only. This approach was chosen based on earlier studies showing bilateral changes in synergy indices that demonstrated no side differences in PD patients, even in those with major bilateral differences in clinical symptoms (Park et al. 2012, 2014). As a result, we opted to analyze more muscles on one side of the body only given the limited number of EMG channels.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the participants in the study. XH and MML were supported by NIH grants NS060722, ES019672, and NS082151 during the past 12 months. MML also has an internal grant from Penn State University but received no salary support as part of the award. MLL, AF, XH, and MML were supported by NIH grants NS035032 and NS095873. This publication was also supported, in part, by Grants UL1 TR000127 and TL1 TR000125 from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS).

Footnotes

Disclosures: No conflicts of interest are claimed by any of the authors.

References

  1. Almeida QJ, Wishart LR, Lee TD. Bimanual coordination deficits with Parkinson's disease: the influence of movement speed and external cueing. Mov Disord. 2002;17:30–37. doi: 10.1002/mds.10030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Boonstra TA, van Vugt JPP, van der Kooij H, Bloem BR. Balance asymmetry in Parkinson's disease and its contribution to freezing of gait. PLoS One. 2014;9:e102493. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0102493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Cavanaugh JT, Guskiewicz KM, Stergiou N. A nonlinear dynamic approach for evaluating postural control: new directions for the management of sport-related cerebral concussion. Sports Med. 2005;35:935–950. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200535110-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Corcos DM, Gottlieb GL, Latash ML, Almeida GL, Agarwal GC. Electromechanical delay: An experimental artifact. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. 1992;2:59–68. doi: 10.1016/1050-6411(92)90017-D. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Criswell E, Cram JR. Cram's introduction to surface electromyography. 2nd. Jones and Bartlett; Sudbury, MA: 2011. [Google Scholar]
  6. Danna-Dos-Santos A, Slomka K, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Muscle modes and synergies during voluntary body sway. Exp Brain Res. 2007;179:533–550. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0812-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. d'Avella A, Saltiel P, Bizzi E. Combinations of muscle synergies in the construction of a natural motor behavior. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:300–308. doi: 10.1038/nn1010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. d'Avella A, Bizzi E. Shared and specific muscle synergies in natural motor behaviors. PNAS. 2005;102:3076–3081. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500199102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. DeWald JP, Pope PS, Given JD, Buchanan TS, Rymer WZ. Abnormal muscle coactivation patterns during isometric torque generation at the elbow and shoulder in hemiparetic subjects. Brain. 1995;118:495–510. doi: 10.1093/brain/118.2.495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Falaki A, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML. Impaired synergic control of posture in Parkinson's patients without postural instability. Gait Posture. 2016;44:209–215. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.12.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Falaki A, Huang X, Lewis MM, Latash ML. Dopaminergic modulation of multi-muscle synergies in postural tasks performed by patients with Parkinson's disease. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2017.01.002. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Feldman AG. Functional tuning of the nervous system with control of movement or maintenance of a steady posture. II. Controllable parameters of the muscle. Biophysics. 1966;11:565–578. [Google Scholar]
  13. Feldman AG. Once more on the equilibrium-point hypothesis (λ-model) for motor control. Journal of Motor Behavior. 1986;18:17–54. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1986.10735369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Feldman AG. Referent control of action and perception: Challenging conventional theories in behavioral science. Springer, NY: 2015. [Google Scholar]
  15. Friedman J, SKM V, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. The sources of two components of variance: An example of multifinger cyclic force production tasks at different frequencies. Exp Brain Res. 2009;196:263–277. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1846-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Goodman SR, Shim JK, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Motor variability within a multi-effector system: Experimental and analytical studies of multi-finger production of quick force pulses. Exp Brain Res. 2005;163:75–85. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-2147-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hausdorff JM, Purdon PL, Peng CK, Ladin Z, Wei JY, Goldberger AL. Fractal dynamics of human gait: stability of long-range correlations in stride interval fluctuations. J Appl Physiol. 1985;80:1448–1457. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1996.80.5.1448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hoehn M, Yahr M. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology. 1967;17:427–442. doi: 10.1212/wnl.17.5.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Houk JC. Agents of the mind. Biol Cybern. 2005;92:427–437. doi: 10.1007/s00422-005-0569-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ivanenko YP, Poppele RE, Lacquaniti F. Five basic muscle activation patterns account for muscle activity during human locomotion. J Physiol. 2004;556:267–282. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.057174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jo HJ, Maenza C, Good DC, Huang X, Park J, Sainburg RL, Latash ML. Effects of unilateral stroke on multi-finger synergies and their feed-forward adjustments. Neurosci. 2016;319:194–205. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.01.054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. King LA, St George RJ, Carlson-Kuhta P, Nutt JG, Horak FB. Preparation for compensatory forward stepping in Parkinson's disease. Arch Phys Med Rehab. 2010;91:1332–1338. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2010.05.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Klous M, Mikulic P, Latash ML. Two aspects of feed-forward postural control: Anticipatory postural adjustments and anticipatory synergy adjustments. J Neurophysiol. 2011;105:2275–2288. doi: 10.1152/jn.00665.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Krishnamoorthy V, Latash ML, Scholz JP, Zatsiorsky VM. Muscle synergies during shifts of the center of pressure by standing persons. Exp Brain Res. 2003;152:281–292. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1574-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Krishnan V, Aruin AS, Latash ML. Two stages and three components of postural preparation to action. Exp Brain Res. 2011;212:47–63. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2694-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Krishnan V, Latash ML, Aruin AS. Early and late components of feed-forward postural adjustments to predictable perturbations. Clin Neurophysiol. 2012;123:1016–1026. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Latash ML. Synergy. Oxford Univ. Press; New York, NY: 2008. [Google Scholar]
  28. Latash ML. Motor synergies and the equilibrium-point hypothesis. Motor Control. 2010;14:294–322. doi: 10.1123/mcj.14.3.294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Latash ML. The bliss (not the problem) of motor abundance (not redundancy) Exp Brain Res. 2012;217:1–5. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3000-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Latash ML. Towards physics of neural processes and behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;69:136–146. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Latash ML. Biological movement and laws of physics. Motor Control. 2017 doi: 10.1123/mc.2016-0016. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Latash ML, Huang X. Neural control of movement stability: Lessons from studies of neurological patients. Neurosci. 2015;301:39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.05.075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Latash ML, Scholz JF, Danion F, Schöner G. Structure of motor variability in marginally redundant multi-finger force production tasks. Exp Brain Res. 2001;141:153–165. doi: 10.1007/s002210100861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Latash ML, Scholz JF, Danion F, Schöner G. Finger coordination during discrete and oscillatory force production tasks. Exp Brain Res. 2002;146:412–432. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1196-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Latash ML, Scholz JP, Schöner G. Toward a new theory of motor synergies. Motor Control. 2007;11:276–308. doi: 10.1123/mcj.11.3.276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Leone FC, Nottingham RB, Nelson LS. The folded normal distribution. Technometrics. 1961;3:543–550. [Google Scholar]
  37. Lewis MM, Lee EY, Jo HJ, Park J, Latash ML, Huang X. Synergy as a new and sensitive marker of basal ganglia dysfunction: A study of asymptomatic welders. Neurotoxicology. 2016;56:76–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2016.06.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Mattos D, Latash ML, Park E, Kuhl J, Scholz JP. Unpredictable elbow joint perturbation during reaching results in multijoint motor equivalence. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:1424–1436. doi: 10.1152/jn.00163.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Mattos D, Kuhl J, Scholz JP, Latash ML. Motor equivalence (ME) during reaching: Is ME observable at the muscle level? Motor Control. 2013;17:145–175. doi: 10.1123/mcj.17.2.145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Mattos D, Schöner G, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Motor equivalence during accurate multi-finger force production. Exp Brain Res. 2015;233:487–502. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4128-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Olafsdottir H, Yoshida N, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Anticipatory covariation of finger forces during self-paced and reaction time force production. Neurosci Lett. 2005;381:92–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Park J, Jo HJ, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML. Effects of Parkinson's disease on optimization and structure of variance in multi-finger tasks. Exp Brain Res. 2013;231:51–63. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3665-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Park J, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML. Dopaminergic modulation of motor coordination in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Rel Disord. 2014;20:64–68. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2013.09.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Park J, Wu YH, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML. Changes in multi-finger interaction and coordination in Parkinson's disease. J Neurophysiol. 2012;108:915–924. doi: 10.1152/jn.00043.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Pastor MA, Jahanshahi M, Artieda J, Obeso JA. Performance of repetitive wrist movements in Parkinson's disease. Brain. 1992;115:875–891. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.3.875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Reisman D, Scholz JP. Aspects of joint coordination are preserved during pointing in persons with post-stroke hemiparesis. Brain. 2003;126:2510–2527. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Riva F, Bisi MC, Stagni R. Orbital stability analysis in biomechanics: a systematic review of a nonlinear technique to detect instability of motor tasks. Gait Posture. 2013;37:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Rogers MW, Kennedy R, Palmer S, Pawar M, Reising M, Martinez KM, Simuni T, Zhang Y, MacKinnon CD. Postural preparation to stepping in patients with Parkinson's disease. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:915–924. doi: 10.1152/jn.00005.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Scholz JP, Schöner G. The uncontrolled manifold concept: Identifying control variables for a functional task. Exp Brain Res. 1999;126:289–306. doi: 10.1007/s002210050738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Scholz JP, Danion F, Latash ML, Schöner G. Understanding finger coordination through analysis of the structure of force variability. Biol Cybern. 2002;86:29–39. doi: 10.1007/s004220100279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Scholz JP, Schöner G, Hsu WL, Jeka JJ, Horak F, Martin V. Motor equivalent control of the center of mass in response to support surface perturbations. Exp Brain Res. 2007;180:163–179. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0848-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Shim JK, Olafsdottir H, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. The emergence and disappearance of multi-digit synergies during force production tasks. Expl Brain Res. 2005;164:260–270. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2248-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ting LH, McKay JL. Neuromechanics of muscle synergies for posture and movement. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17:622–628. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Tomlinson CL, Stowe R, Patel S, Rick C, Gray R, Clarke CE. Systematic review of Levodopa dose equivalency report in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2010;25:2649–2685. doi: 10.1002/mds.23429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Tresch MC, Cheung VC, d'Avella A. Matrix factorization algorithms for the identification of muscle synergies: evaluation on simulated and experimental data sets. J Neurophysiol. 2006;95:2199–2212. doi: 10.1152/jn.00222.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. van Emmerik RE, Jones SL, Busa MA, Remelius JG, Averill JL. Enhancing postural stability and adaptability in multiple sclerosis. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2014;826:251–276. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-1338-1_15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Visser JE, Carpenter MG, van der Kooij H, Bloem BR. The clinical utility of posturography. Clin Neurophysiol. 2008;119:2424–2436. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.07.220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Wing AM, Keele S, Margolin DI. Motor disorder and the timing of repetitive movements. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1984;423:183–192. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23428.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Winter DA, Prince F, Frank JS, Powell C, Zabjek KF. Unified theory regarding A/P and M/L balance in quiet stance. J Neurophysiol. 1996;75:2334–2343. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.6.2334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES