Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Pediatr Infect Dis J. 2017 Aug;36(8):751–757. doi: 10.1097/INF.0000000000001573

Roles of Medication Responsibility, Executive and Adaptive Functioning in Adherence for Children and Adolescents with Perinatally Acquired HIV

Patricia A Garvie 1, Sean S Brummel 2, Susannah M Allison 3, Kathleen Malee 4, Claude A Mellins 5, Megan L Wilkins 6, Lynnette L Harris 7, E Doyle Patton 8, Miriam Chernoff 2, Richard M Rutstein 9, Mary E Paul 10, Sharon L Nichols 11, for the Pediatric HIV/AIDS Cohort Study
PMCID: PMC5512435  NIHMSID: NIHMS851070  PMID: 28709161

Abstract

Background

Medication adherence is a critical but challenging developmental task for children and adolescents with perinatally acquired HIV (PHIV). Understanding how medication responsibility, executive functions (EF) and adaptive functioning (AF) influence adherence may help prepare adolescents for transition to adulthood.

Methods

Participants included PHIV children and adolescents 7–16 years of age enrolled in the Pediatric HIV/AIDS Cohort Study Adolescent Master Protocol, who were prescribed antiretroviral medications. Measures included: Caregiver report and child self-report measures of adherence, medication responsibility, and EF, caregiver report of child AF, examiner-administered tests of EF and processing speed, and demographic and health characteristics.

Results

256 participants with PHIV (mean age 12 years), were 51% female, 80% black, and 79% non-Hispanic. Per 7-day recall, 72% were adherent (no missed doses). Children/adolescents self-reported that 22% had sole and 55% had shared medication responsibility. Adjusted logistic models revealed significantly higher odds of adherence with sole caregiver responsibility for medication (odds ratio (OR)=4.10, confidence interval (CI [1.43,11.8], p=0.009)), child nadir CD4% <15% (OR=2.26, CI [1.15,4.43], p=0.018), better self-reported behavioral regulation (OR=0.65 CI[0.44,0.96], p=0.029), and slower processing speed (OR=0.54, CI[0.38,0.77], p<0.001), adjusting for demographic variables (age, race, caregiver education).

Conclusions

Among children and adolescents with PHIV, continued caregiver medication management, especially during adolescence, is essential. Although global executive and adaptive functioning were not significantly associated with adherence, behavioral regulation was. Given that executive and adaptive functioning develop throughout adolescence, their relationships to adherence should be evaluated longitudinally, especially as youth transition to adulthood and caregiver responsibility diminishes.

Keywords: perinatal HIV, children, adolescents, executive functioning, adherence

INTRODUCTION

Children with perinatally acquired Human Immunodeficiency Virus (PHIV) in the United States (US) are aging into adolescence and assuming greater responsibility for their own HIV medication management, especially as they approach young adulthood. High, sustained antiretroviral (ARV) medication adherence remains necessary for viral suppression.14 Understanding factors that influence medication adherence among children and adolescents with PHIV is critical to promote optimal ARV adherence, health outcomes, and intervention development.

Barriers to ARV adherence for children/adolescents with PHIV include poor medication palatability, pill burden, dietary restrictions, acute and long-term side effects, coordination with daily schedules, forgetting, and treatment fatigue.58 Child, caregiver and family factors associated with PHIV adherence/non-adherence include older child/adolescent age, behavioral functioning, knowledge of HIV status, psychological adjustment, education and problem-solving skills, and caregiver relationship to the child.918 Children/adolescents with PHIV face multiple psychosocial stressors that may negatively affect adherence including poverty, parental HIV disease and death, disclosure, stigmatization, and limited social support.19

Among adults with HIV, decreased medication adherence is related to impaired cognition, particularly in the domain of executive functions (EF).2024 EFs are involved in goal-directed behavior and include the abilities to initiate behavior, inhibit competing actions, select task goals, plan, organize, shift mental set, problem-solve, and self-monitor.2527 Although research suggests global cognitive functioning does not share the same relationship with adherence in children/adolescents as in adults with HIV,10 EF has not been examined. EFs are related to treatment adherence in other childhood chronic illnesses (e.g., diabetes),2829 and may play an important role as PHIV adolescents assume medication responsibility, potentially providing important intervention targets to improve adherence.

Executive functions are complex and involve multiple cognitive components such as processing speed that may clarify associations of EF with adherence. Thus, evaluating component skills may be important. Processing speed is associated with HIV disease severity in youth30 and is implicated in ARV adherence in adults with HIV.21 Similarly, adaptive skills, or the ability to self-manage activities of daily life, may be crucial for successful medication adherence and medication responsibility, especially as caregiver adherence support diminishes.

The effect of cognition on adherence in PHIV children/adolescents may be mitigated by caregiver factors, including medication support.9 Others have suggested that primary caregiver responsibility for ARVs is essential for sustained adherence throughout adolescence.17 However, caregiver medication support often wanes in adolescence as youth assume responsibility for their own ARV adherence. With increased child/adolescent shared or sole responsibility for ARVs, it is essential to evaluate the influence of executive and adaptive functioning on the degree of youth assumed medication responsibility.

To our knowledge, no prior studies have investigated the relationship of executive functions, processing speed and adaptive functioning to ARV adherence in children/adolescents with PHIV, even though adherence involves complex behaviors that rely on problem-solving and adaptive skills. Additionally, the relationship of child/adolescent executive and adaptive functioning to medication responsibility has not been studied among PHIV children/adolescents. The aims of this study were to determine whether executive functions, adaptive skills and processing speed influence medication adherence and degree of medication responsibility among PHIV children/adolescents.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

The Pediatric HIV/AIDS Cohort Study (PHACS) Adolescent Master Protocol (AMP), a prospective cohort study of the long-term effects of PHIV and treatment on pre-adolescents and adolescents biomedical and neurobehavioral outcomes (https://phacsstudy.org/), enrolled participants between March 2007 and October 2009 at 15 AMP sites throughout the US and Puerto Rico. Eligibility criteria included perinatal HIV infection or exposure, age 7 to <16 years at enrollment, and accessible treatment history. To assess adherence outcomes, analyses presented herein included AMP PHIV participants who were prescribed ARVs and had available valid data through January 1, 2012 for: 1) caregiver-report and/or child/adolescent self-report of medication adherence and responsibility; 2) caregiver-report and child/adolescent self-report (participants ≥11) of executive function; 3) caregiver report of child/adolescent adaptive functioning; and, 4) examiner administered pencil-and-paper tests of child/adolescent executive functions and processing speed as described below.

Procedure

Institutional Review Boards at the Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health and each participating PHACS site approved the study. Written informed consent and age-appropriate assent were obtained for all participants according to institutional guidelines.

Measures

Primary Outcomes

Adherence

ARV adherence assessment was based on the work of the Adult and Pediatric AIDS Clinical Trials Groups, modified by the AMP team.12,3133 Caregivers and children/adolescents (whose caregivers provided consent) were interviewed separately and asked to report ARVs prescribed, primary person responsible for administering ARVs, and number of doses missed in the past 7 days. Consistent with prior studies,15 non-adherence was defined as either child or caregiver report of ≥1 missed dose of any ARV in the prior seven days. Significant associations of caregiver and child/adolescent 7-day adherence recall with concurrent HIV RNA viral load (VL) has been established in AMP,32 thus validation was not repeated herein.

Medication Responsibility

During the adherence interview, children/adolescents and/or caregivers independently reported whether the: 1) Caregiver was solely responsible for the child’s/adolescent’s ARV adherence; 2) child/adolescent and caregiver shared responsibility; or, 3) child/adolescent was solely responsible.

Potential Predictors of Adherence and Medication Responsibility

Adaptive Functioning (AF)

The Adaptive Behavior Assessment System-Second Edition (ABAS-II) Parent Report34 is a comprehensive, norm-referenced proxy inventory that assesses an individual’s adaptive skills for ages 5–21 years. The General Adaptive Composite (GAC) was evaluated as a global predictor of adherence; Conceptual, Social, and Practical Composites also were explored. Composites have a standard score mean (M)=100 and standard deviation (SD)=15.

Executive Function (EF)

The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Parent-Report Form (BRIEF-PR)26 and the BRIEF-Self-Report (BRIEF-SR)27 are standardized age-normed inventories for rating EF in the performance of everyday tasks across multiple domains. The Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI), Metacognition Index (MI), and Global Executive Composite (GEC) were examined in relation to adherence. T-scores (M=50, SD=10) are computed. Caregivers completed the BRIEF-PR regardless of participant age. Per standard administration guidelines, children/adolescents ≥11 years of age completed the BRIEF-SR. Because the BRIEF was not available in Spanish during data collection, it was not administered to children or caregivers without English proficiency.

The Children’s Color Trails Test (CCTT)35 is a standardized paper-and-pencil assessment of alternating and sustained visual attention, sequencing, psychomotor speed, cognitive flexibility, planning and inhibition-disinhibition. Age-norm-referenced scores include T-scores (M=50; SD=10) for each trial (CCTT-1 and CCTT-2) completion time (i.e., processing speed), and percentile ranges for the Interference Index, which assesses inhibition and cognitive flexibility.

Processing Speed

The Processing Speed Index (PSI) of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th Edition (WISC-IV)36 was included, and is normed with M=100, SD=15.

Potential Confounding Variables

Demographic, Caregiver and Health Information

Demographic information collected via structured interview with the primary caregiver included child age, sex, race/ethnicity, and primary language, household income, child HIV disclosure status, and caregiver relationship to child, HIV status, and education. Medical chart abstraction provided current (at study entry) and peak HIV-1 RNA VL; current and nadir CD4+ T-lymphocyte count (CD4) and percent (CD4%); Centers for Disease Control (CDC) HIV disease classification and age at classification; diagnosis of encephalopathy and age at diagnosis; and current ARV regimen.

Other Measures of Child and Caregiver Functioning

The following measures of child academic and child and caregiver cognitive functioning administered within AMP were included as potential confounders: Child/adolescent WISC-IV Full Scale IQ (FSIQ),36 Wechsler Individual Achievement Test, 2nd Edition, Abbreviated (WIAT-II-A)37 Word Reading subtest score, and caregiver FSIQ (Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI)38).

Data Collection

Administration of baseline measures was staggered in AMP to minimize participant burden. Semi-annual visits included physical exam, chart review of health and medication status, and demographic interviews. At study entry, child/adolescent participants were administered the WISC-IV, caregivers completed the ABAS-II, and child medical and developmental histories were assessed. At the 6-month visit, caregivers were administered the WASI. At the 1-year visit, children/adolescents completed the CCTT, WIAT-II-A, and BRIEF-SR; caregivers completed the BRIEF-PR. At the 1.5-year visit, adherence self- and caregiver-reports were obtained. All cognitive, behavioral, and health status variable data of interest preceded the adherence assessments.

Statistical Analyses

Descriptive statistics were used to summarize demographic variables. The odds of 7-day adherence and the child/adolescent having sole responsibility for ARVs were modeled. Logistic regression was used to estimate odds ratios (OR) while adjusting for potential confounders selected a priori based on associations within the literature and previous AMP analyses. Covariates included the participant’s age, sex, race, ethnicity, WISC-IV FSIQ, WIAT-II-A reading level, peak HIV RNA VL, nadir CD4%, degree of medication responsibility, and history of encephalopathy. Caregiver characteristics included: Education, FSIQ, and biological relationship to the participant. Regression analyses were used to reduce the number of potential confounders. Core model adjustment variables were selected as follows: Variables with a p-value <0.20 in univariate models were included in a core multivariable adjustment model, and retained if p-value was <0.15. Analyses with p-values <0.05 were considered statistically significant; confidence intervals (CI) are reported as 95%. SAS 9.2 and 9.4 (Cary, NC) were used for the analyses.

RESULTS

Participant Demographics (Tables 1 and 2)

Table 1.

Child and Caregiver Characteristics

Characteristic Total (N=256)
n (%)
Child Age at Study Entry Mean (SD) 11.99 (2.51)
Child Gender Female 130 (50.8%)
Child Race Black 194 (80.2%)
White 47 (19.4%)
Other 1 (0.4%)
Missing1 (n) 14
Child Ethnicity Hispanic 53 (20.7%)
Child Primary Language English 220 (88.0%)
Spanish 12 (4.8%)
French 2 (0.8%)
Bilingual, English/Spanish 14 (5.6%)
Other, specify 2 (0.8%)
Missing1 (n) 6
English Primarily Spoken at Home English 208 (82.2%)
Bilingual or other Language 45 (17.8%)
Missing1 (n) 3
Child Full Scale IQ Mean (SD) 85 (15.4)
Child Word Reading Standard Score Mean (SD) 86 (18.2)
Caregiver Income <= $20k 108 (45.4%)
> $20k 130 (54.6%)
Missing1 (n) 18
Caregiver Education Level < High School Completion 67 (26.5%)
≥ High School Diploma 186 (73.5%)
Missing1 (n) 3
Caregiver is Biological Parent No 144 (56.9%)
Yes 109 (43.1%)
Missing1 (n) 3
Caregiver HIV Status HIV+ 91 (43.5%)
HIV- 118 (56.5%)
Missing1 (n) 47
Caregiver Race White 73 (30.4%)
Black 166 (69.2%)
Other 1 (0.4%)
Missing1 (n) 16
Caregiver Ethnicity Non-Hispanic 198 (79.5%)
Hispanic 51 (20.5%)
Missing1 (n) 7
Caregiver Full Scale IQ Mean (SD) 89 (15.2)

Note: IQ = Intelligence Quotient; SD = Standard deviation

1

Missing n not included in denominator to calculate characteristic percentages.

Table 2.

Child HIV Disease Characteristics

Characteristic Total (N=256)
n (%)
Youth Knows HIV Status Yes 163 (64.7%)
No 89 (35.3%)
Missing1 (n) 4
CDC Class C (at study entry) 68 (26.6%)
History of Encephalopathy 33 (12.9%)
Nadir CD4% Mean (SD) 17.6 (9.0)
Age at Nadir CD4% Mean (SD) 5.2 (4.0)
CD4% at Study Entry Mean (SD) 32.0 (9.6)
Peak log RNA VL Mean (SD) 5.4 (0.7)
Age at Peak RNA VL Mean (SD) 4.2 (4.1)
log RNA VL at Study Entry Mean (SD) 2.6 (0.9)
VL at Study Entry <400 181 (71.3%)
≥400 73 (28.7%)
Missing1 (n) 2
HAART Lifetime Duration (years) Mean (SD) 7.75 (2.7)

Note: CDC – Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; RNA – ribonucleic acid;

VL – Viral load; HAART – Highly active antiretroviral therapy.

1

Missing n not included in denominator to calculate characteristic percentages.

Child/adolescent PHIV participants included in the analyses (n=256) were of average age 12 years (SD=2.51; range, 7–16 years), primarily black race (80%), and non-Hispanic (79%), with both genders equally represented (female, 51%).

Adherence

Available adherence reports included in the analysis were as follows: Caregiver only, n=51, child/adolescent self-report only, n=20, both caregiver and child/adolescent reports, n=185. Seventy-two percent of participants were identified as adherent by child self- and/or caregiver report (no missed doses) with moderate agreement between child/adolescent and caregiver reports of adherence (Kappa=0.53, CI[0.38,0.68], p<0.0001).

Predictors of Adherence (Table 3)

Table 3.

Adjusted and Unadjusted Logistic Regression Analyses Modeling the Odds of 7-day Adherence

Adherence Predictor Measurement Unadjusted Odds Ratios (95% CI) P-value Adjusted 2 Odds Ratios (95% CI) P-value
Adaptive Behavior (ABAS-II)
General Adaptive Composite (GAC) Standard Score 1 0.78 (0.59, 1.05) 0.105 0.76 (0.56, 1.05) 0.100
Conceptual composite Standard Score 1 0.84 (0.63, 1.12) 0.233 0.83 (0.61, 1.13) 0.244
Social Composite Standard Score 1 0.89 (0.67, 1.18) 0.412 0.82 (0.60, 1.13) 0.221
Practical Composite Standard Score 1 0.71 (0.52, 0.96) 0.029 0.71 (0.50, 1.00) 0.053
Executive Function BRIEF
Caregiver Report
Global Executive Composite (GEC) T Score1 1.03 (0.76, 1.40) 0.853 1.06 (0.76, 1.49) 0.721
Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) T Score1 1.04 (0.76, 1.42) 0.811 1.03 (0.73, 1.46) 0.853
Metacognition Index (MI) T Score1 1.02 (0.75, 1.39) 0.900 1.08 (0.78, 1.51) 0.645
Child Report
Global Executive Composite (GEC) T Score1 0.72 (0.50, 1.02) 0.061 0.73 (0.49, 1.08) 0.111
Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) T Score1 0.65 (0.46, 0.93) 0.018 0.65 (0.44, 0.96) 0.029
Metacognition Index (MI) T Score1 0.80 (0.57, 1.14) 0.216 0.83 (0.56, 1.23) 0.363
Children Color Trails Test (CCTT)
CCTT Trial 1 T Score1 0.53 (0.38, 0.73) <.001 0.54 (0.38, 0.77) <.001
CCTT Trial 2 T Score1 0.72 (0.53,0.98) 0.037 0.75 (0.54, 1.05) 0.095
CCTT Interference Index < 6th percentile Reference Reference
6–16 percentile 1.09 (0.34, 3.54) 0.881 0.98 (0.27, 3.55) 0.975
>16th percentile 1.73 (0.73, 4.10) 0.211 1.36 (0.52, 3.58) 0.535
Processing Speed
WISC-IV Processing Speed Index (PSI) Composite Score 1 0.63 (0.46,0.87) 0.004 0.63 (0.44, 0.89) 0.009

Note: ABAS-II – Adaptive Behavior Assessment System, Second Edition; BRIEF – Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; WISC-IV – Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition.

1

Estimates are displayed for a one unit change in the observed standard deviation.

2

Adjusted only for Caregiver Education, Child Race, Nadir CD4%, Age and Medication Responsibility.

Executive Function

BRIEF

Based on child/adolescent self-report (n=156), lower odds of adherence were associated with higher behavioral regulation T-scores (greater impairment; OR=0.65 CI[0.44,0.96], p=0.029). There were no significant relationships of adherence with caregiver (n=200) or self-reported metacognition or GEC, or with caregiver-reported behavioral regulation.

CCTT

The relationship of the CCTT Interference Index, a measure of EF, with adherence was non-significant.

Adaptive Functioning

ABAS-II

In adjusted models there were no significant relationships of adherence with caregiver-reported Global Adaptive functioning or Conceptual, Social or Practical subdomains with adherence.

Processing Speed

CCTT

In adjusted analyses, significantly higher odds of adherence was associated with slower CCTT Trial 1 psychomotor speed (OR=0.54, CI[0.38,0.77], p<0.001), consistent with observed lower WISC-IV Processing Speed Index scores (OR=0.63, CI[0.44,0.89], p=0.009). The relationship of adherence with CCTT Trial 2 was nonsignificant.

Demographic Covariates of Adherence

In unadjusted analyses of caregiver characteristics (not shown), participants with HIV+ caregivers were less likely to be adherent (p=0.045). Adherent participants had lower nadir CD4% (p=0.02) and were more likely to have controlled VL at study entry (p<0.001). In adjusted models, after selection of potential confounders, the core model adjustment variables for multivariable analyses included age, race, caregiver education, caregiver relationship, medication responsibility, and nadir CD4%. In a multivariable model (see Table, Supplemental Digital Content 1), black race was associated with lower odds of adherence (OR=0.39, CI[0.18,0.86], p=0.02). Higher odds of adherence were associated with younger child age (OR=3.94, CI[1.15,10.320], p=0.005), caregivers being high school graduates (OR=2.80, CI[1.41,5.56], p=0.003), caregiver solely responsible for medications (OR=4.10, CI[1.43,11.81], p=0.009), and participant nadir CD4% <15% vs. >15% (OR=2.26, CI[1.15,4.43] p=0.018). Child history of encephalopathy (p=0.837) and knowledge of HIV status (p=0.856) were not associated with adherence.

Medication Responsibility

Overall, there was 73% child-caregiver congruence for medication responsibility (Kappa=0.56, CI[0.46,0.66], p<0.0001). Per participant self-report, 22% reported sole medication responsibility, 21% sole caregiver responsibility, and 55% shared responsibility. By caregiver report, 15% reported sole child/adolescent responsibility, 34% sole caregiver responsibility, and 47% shared responsibility. A significantly higher proportion of adherent (39% and 26%) than non-adherent (20% and 9%) children/adolescents had caregivers with sole responsibility for medication management per both caregiver (p=0.017) and child reports (p=0.012), respectively.

Predictors of Medication Responsibility

Core model adjustment variables for child/adolescent medication responsibility included child/adolescent’s age and FSIQ (see table, Supplemental Digital Content 2). Adolescents ≥12 years of age had ten-fold higher odds of being solely responsible for medication administration. Although nonsignificant, FSIQ was marginally higher (OR=1.40, CI[0.96,2.06], p=0.083) for children/adolescents with sole responsibility for their medications. Given the moderate agreement between child/adolescent and caregiver reports of medication responsibility, and the greater availability of caregiver reports, modeling for this analysis was based on caregiver report. In adjusted models, there were no statistically significant associations of executive or adaptive functioning or encephalopathy with caregiver report of medication responsibility (Table 4). Additionally, there was no other significant demographic, HIV health status, or cognitive predictor of medication responsibility.

Table 4.

Adjusted and Unadjusted Logistic Regression Analyses Modeling the Odds That the Child is Solely Responsible for Medication

Adherence Predictor Measurement Unadjusted Odds Ratios (95% CI) P-value Adjusted 2 Odds Ratios (95% CI) P-value
Adaptive Behavior (ABAS-II)
General Adaptive Composite (GAC) Standard Score 1 1.06 (0.74, 1.52) 0.765 0.98 (0.63, 1.53) 0.933
Conceptual composite Standard Score 1 1.17 (0.81, 1.69) 0.403 0.98 (0.61, 1.58) 0.927
Social Composite Standard Score 1 0.96 (0.67, 1.38) 0.843 0.91 (0.58, 1.42) 0.665
Practical Composite Standard Score 1 1.07 (0.74, 1.54) 0.728 1.12 (0.72, 1.73) 0.621
Executive Function (BRIEF)
Caregiver Report
Global Executive Composite (GEC) T Score1 0.87 (0.59, 1.28) 0.465 0.78 (0.50, 1.20) 0.258
Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) T Score1 0.89 (0.60, 1.32) 0.560 0.81 (0.52, 1.26) 0.342
Metacognition Index (MI) T Score1 0.86 (0.59, 1.27) 0.456 0.78 (0.51, 1.21) 0.274
Child Self-Report
Global Executive Composite (GEC) T Score1 0.76 (0.50, 1.15) 0.197 0.82 (0.52, 1.30) 0.404
Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) T Score1 0.74 (0.49, 1.13) 0.164 0.76 (0.48, 1.21) 0.249
Metacognition Index (MI) T Score1 0.80 (0.53, 1.20) 0.275 0.90 (0.57, 1.42) 0.651
Children Color Trails Test (CCTT)
Processing Speed
CCTT Trial 1 T Score1 1.13 (0.76, 1.68) 0.536 1.11 (0.70, 1.77) 0.645
CCTT Trial 2 T Score1 0.93 (0.63, 1.38) 0.730 0.69 (0.42, 1.13) 0.139
Interference Index < 6th percentile Reference Reference
6–16 percentile 0.53 (0.11, 2.41) 0.408 0.65 (0.12, 3.65) 0.628
>16th percentile 0.45 (0.16, 1.26) 0.131 0.49 (0.15, 1.62) 0.243
WISC-IV Processing Speed Index (PSI) Composite Score 1 1.13 (0.78, 1.65) 0.51 1.27 (0.87, 1.83) 0.210

Note: ABAS-II – Adaptive Behavior Assessment System, Second Edition; BRIEF – Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function;

WISC-IV – Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition.

1

Estimates are displayed for a one unit change in the observed standard deviation.

2

Adjusted only for child age and Full Scale IQ.

DISCUSSION

Sustaining optimal adherence to ARV treatment in children/adolescents with PHIV is crucial. The roles of executive functions, adaptive functioning, processing speed, and cognitive and behavioral constructs not previously examined were assessed as potential predictors of PHIV ARV adherence. Similar to other studies of children with HIV in the US,39 72% of children/adolescents in our sample were identified as adherent. While these results demonstrate that optimal adherence is attainable by many PHIV children/adolescents, a still sizable number experience adherence difficulties and are at risk for suboptimal viral suppression. Nearly one-third of participants had detectable VL at study entry, which is of great clinical concern.

A critical finding in this study is the role of medication responsibility in relation to ARV adherence. The odds of adherence were significantly higher when caregivers maintained sole responsibility for their child’s/adolescent’s medication management. Per both caregiver and participant reports, significantly greater numbers of adherent than non-adherent children/adolescents had caregivers with sole responsibility for their ARVs. Relationships between child age and adherence also were observed; older children/adolescents were less likely to be adherent. Additionally, those >12 years of age were 10 times more likely to be solely responsible for their medications. Findings are consistent with prior reports demonstrating that PHIV adherence is better with parent involvement,4043 even in adolescence, highlighting the need for sustained participation of caregivers in medication management, careful evaluation of readiness for transition of responsibility from caregiver to youth, and ongoing caregiver support and monitoring even after youth assume sole responsibility. No relationships of child/adolescent executive or adaptive functioning were observed in relation to degree of medication responsibility further supporting the notion that responsibility for ARVs likely is shifted based on child/adolescent age rather than readiness.

Caregivers who were high school graduates were significantly more likely to have youth who were adherent, suggesting that at minimum high school completion may contribute to overall health literacy and consistent access to care. On the other hand, black race was significantly associated with non-adherence, raising questions regarding contributing roles of minority status, health disparities, and stigmatization; limited access to adequate educational and community-based social resources also may be implicated.

The importance of examining components of EF was supported by the increased likelihood of nonadherence among children/adolescents with self-reported behavioral dysregulation. Adherence may be challenging among children/adolescents who experience difficulties with aspects of behavioral regulation, such as inhibition, cognitive shift, and emotional control, as suggested in prior studies of caregiver-reported behavioral difficulties and adherence.10 Routine assessment of behavioral regulation may provide information to guide behavioral interventions for youth with adherence difficulties.

A significant relationship between adherence and processing speed was observed, albeit in the opposite direction of that seen in adults. One possible explanation is that for “slower processors,” caregivers may be aware of their child’s limitations, and may intentionally maintain adherence support, despite their child’s advancing age or expectations for increased autonomy. However, this requires further investigation to better understand the observed counter-intuitive relationship.

Contrary to expectations, no significant effects of global executive or global adaptive functioning on adherence were observed. Global indices may obscure the effects of selective skills that influence performance on a specific task and may not be adequately sensitive to detect subtle executive or adaptive differences. It is also important to consider that health and developmental consequences of intermittent or persistently poor adherence may have an impact on the developmental trajectory of executive functions, adaptive skills, and processing speed in children/adolescents with PHIV. Clarifying this relationship warrants future longitudinal investigation.

Regardless of executive or adaptive functioning and observed adherence behaviors, caregivers of children/adolescents with PHIV may assume their child is ready for autonomous medication management by virtue of their age and expect pre-adolescents/adolescents to assume sole or minimally supervised joint management of ARVs. Children/adolescents with “age-typical” organizational skills and working memory who have moderate supervision around medication management still may experience fluctuations in adherence behaviors related to emerging executive skills and/or other challenges, including emotional/behavioral concerns, burgeoning peer relationships and associated pressures to fit in,44 or acute or chronic psychosocial stressors that limit independence. Thus, it is important to counsel caregivers that age is not a sufficient indicator of readiness for sole medication responsibility, and to transfer medication responsibility based on youth age alone may be unwise. Ideally, caregivers should be encouraged to calibrate supervision over time, congruent with youth’s emerging autonomy, demonstrated cognitive and behavioral capabilities, and emotional health necessary for sustained success as they approach young adulthood.

This study is not without limitations, including those inherent with self-report measures, such as recall bias and social desirability. This sample may not reflect the population of children/adolescents with PHIV who do not participate in research studies like AMP that involve extensive monitoring. Those who kept study visits may be more likely to adhere to ARV medications than those who did not enroll, missed appointments, or were lost-to-follow-up. While executive and adaptive skills develop throughout adolescence and into adulthood, the mean age of our sample is one at which executive and adaptive skills are still emerging and may be better developed in later adolescence, thus limiting findings. Further, given the spacing of initial assessments, we cannot account for potential changes over the 1.5 year timeframe.

Conclusions

Among children/adolescents with PHIV, continued caregiver involvement in medication management during adolescence is essential, despite emerging expectations of autonomy. Global ratings of executive function and adaptive skills were not significantly associated with medication adherence, but self-reported behavioral regulation was. Therefore, components of executive functions should be monitored as youth age into young adulthood. In contrast to observations of adults with HIV, slower processing speed was associated with better adherence, which may reflect ongoing caregiver support resulting from perceptions of youth impairment, which warrants further examination. Given that adaptive behaviors and executive functions develop throughout adolescence and young adulthood, relationships with adherence should be evaluated longitudinally to guide development and implementation of effective adherence-promoting interventions.

Acknowledgments

Financial Support: The Pediatric HIV/AIDS Cohort Study (PHACS) was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development with co-funding from the National Institute on Drug Abuse, the National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases, the Office of AIDS Research, the National Institute of Mental Health, the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, the National Heart Lung and Blood Institute, the National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research, and the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, through cooperative agreements with the Harvard University T. H. Chan School of Public Health (HD052102) (Principal Investigator: George Seage; Project Director: Julie Alperen) and the Tulane University School of Medicine (HD052104) (Principal Investigator: Russell Van Dyke; Co-Principal Investigator: Kenneth Rich; Project Director: Patrick Davis). Data management services were provided by Frontier Science and Technology Research Foundation (PI: Suzanne Siminski), and regulatory services and logistical support were provided by Westat, Inc (PI: Julie Davidson).

We thank the children and families for their participation in PHACS, and the individuals and institutions involved in the conduct of PHACS. The study was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development with co-funding from the National Institute on Drug Abuse, the National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases, the Office of AIDS Research, the National Institute of Mental Health, the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, the National Heart Lung and Blood Institute, the National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research, and the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, through cooperative agreements with the Harvard University School of Public Health (HD052102) (Principal Investigator: George Seage; Project Director: Julie Alperen) and the Tulane University School of Medicine (HD052104) (Principal Investigator: Russell Van Dyke; Co-Principal Investigator: Kenneth Rich; Project Director: Patrick Davis). Data management services were provided by Frontier Science and Technology Research Foundation (PI: Suzanne Siminski), and regulatory services and logistical support were provided by Westat, Inc (PI: Julie Davidson). The following institutions, clinical site investigators and staff participated in conducting PHACS AMP in 2013, in alphabetical order: Ann & Robert H. Lurie Children’s Hospital of Chicago: Ram Yogev, Margaret Ann Sanders, Kathleen Malee, Scott Hunter; Baylor College of Medicine: William Shearer, Mary Paul, Norma Cooper, Lynnette Harris; Bronx Lebanon Hospital Center: Murli Purswani, Mahboobullah Baig, Anna Cintron; Children’s Diagnostic & Treatment Center: Ana Puga, Sandra Navarro, Patricia Garvie, James Blood; Children’s Hospital, Boston: Sandra Burchett, Nancy Karthas, Betsy Kammerer; Jacobi Medical Center: Andrew Wiznia, Marlene Burey, Molly Nozyce; Rutgers - New Jersey Medical School: Arry Dieudonne, Linda Bettica, Susan Adubato; St. Christopher’s Hospital for Children: Janet Chen, Maria Garcia Bulkley, Latreaca Ivey, Mitzie Grant; St. Jude Children’s Research Hospital: Katherine Knapp, Kim Allison, Megan Wilkins; San Juan Hospital/Department of Pediatrics: Midnela Acevedo-Flores, Heida Rios, Vivian Olivera; Tulane University Health Sciences Center: Margarita Silio, Medea Jones, Patricia Sirois; University of California, San Diego: Stephen Spector, Kim Norris, Sharon Nichols; University of Colorado Denver Health Sciences Center: Elizabeth McFarland, Alisa Katai, Jennifer Dunn, Suzanne Paul; University of Miami: Gwendolyn Scott, Patricia Bryan, Elizabeth Willen.

Footnotes

Note: The conclusions and opinions expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect those of the National Institutes of Health or U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

The authors have no conflicts of interest to report related to the work contained herein.

References

  • 1.Bangsberg DR. Less than 95% adherence to nonnucleoside reverse-transcriptase inhibitor therapy can lead to viral suppression. Clin Infect Dis. 2006;43:939–41. doi: 10.1086/507526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Rao D, Kekwaletswe TC, Hosek SG, et al. Stigma and social barriers to medication adherence with urban youth living with HIV. AIDS Care. 2007;19(1):28–33. doi: 10.1080/09540120600652303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Parienti JJ, Ragland K, Lucht F, et al. Average adherence to boosted protease inhibitor therapy, rather than the pattern of missed doses, as a predictor of HIV RNA replication. Clin Infect Dis. 2010;50:1192–7. doi: 10.1086/651419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kobin AB, Sheth NU. Levels of adherence required for virologic suppression among newer antiretroviral medications. Ann Parmacother. 2011;45(3):372–9. doi: 10.1345/aph.1P587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Buchanan AL, Montepiedra G, Sirois PA, et al. Barriers to medication adherence in HIV-infected children and youth based on self-and caregiver report. Pediatrics. 2012;129(5):e1244–e1251. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-1740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Byrne M, Honig J, Jurgrau A, et al. Achieving adherence with antiretroviral medications for pediatric HIV disease. AIDS Read. 2002;12:151–164. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Dolezal C, Mellins C, Brackis-Cott E, et al. The reliability reports of medical adherence from HIV+ children and their adult caregivers. J Pediatr Psychol. 2003;28:355–361. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsg025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Pontali E. Facilitating adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy in children with HIV infection: What are the issues and what can be done? Paediatr Drugs. 2005;7:137–149. doi: 10.2165/00148581-200507030-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Malee K, Williams PL, Montepiedra G, et al. The role of cognitive functioning in medication adherence of children and adolescents with HIV infection. J Pediatr Psychol. 2009;34(2):164–175. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsn068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Malee K, Williams P, Montepiedra G, et al. Medication adherence in children and adolescents with HIV infection: Associations with behavioral impairment. AIDS Patient Care ST. 2011;25(3):191–200. doi: 10.1089/apc.2010.0181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Simoni JM, Montgomery A, Martin E, et al. Adherence to antiretroviral therapy for pediatric HIV infection: A qualitative systematic review with recommendations for research and clinical management. Pediatrics. 2007;119:e1371–e1383. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Williams PL, Storm D, Montepiedra G, et al. for the PACTG 219C Team. Predictors of adherence to antiretroviral medications in children and adolescents with HIV infection. Pediatrics. 2006;118:e1745–57. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-0493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Hammani N, Nostlinger C, Hoeree T, et al. Integrating adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy into children’s daily lives: A qualitative study. Pediatrics. 2004;114:591–597. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Martin S, Elliot-DeSorbo DK, Wolters PL, et al. Patient, caregiver and regimen characteristics associated with adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy among HIV-infected children and adolescents. Pediatr Infect Dis J. 2007;26:61–67. doi: 10.1097/01.inf.0000250625.80340.48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Mellins C, Brackis-Cott E, Dolezal C, et al. The role of psychosocial and family factors in adherence to antiretroviral treatment in human immunodeficiency virus-infected children. Pediatr Infect Dis J. 2004;23:1035–1041. doi: 10.1097/01.inf.0000143646.15240.ac. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Naar-King S, Arfken C, Frey M, et al. Psychosocial factors and treatment adherence in paediatric HIV/AIDS. AIDS Care. 2006;18:621–628. doi: 10.1080/09540120500471895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Naar-King S, Montepiedra G, Garvie PA, et al. Social ecological predictors of longitudinal HIV treatment adherence in youth with perinatally acquired HIV. J Pediatr Psychol. 2013 Jul;38(6):664–74. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jst017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Nichols SL, Montepiedra G, Farley JJ, et al. Cognitive, academic and behavioral correlates of medication adherence in children and adolescents with perinatally acquired HIV-1 infection. J of Dev Behav Pediatr. 2012 May;33(4):298–308. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31824bef47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mellins CA, Malee KM. Understanding the mental health of youth living with perinatal HIV infection: lessons learned and current challenges. J Internat AIDS Soc. 2013;16:1–19. doi: 10.7448/IAS.16.1.18593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Albert SM, Weber CM, Todak G, et al. An observed performance test of medication management ability in HIV: Relation to neuropsychological status and medication outcomes. AIDS Behav. 1999;3:121–128. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ettenhofer ML, Foley J, Castellon SA, et al. Reciprocal prediction of medication adherence and neurocognition in HIV/AIDS. Neurology. 2010;74:1217–1222. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181d8c1ca. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hinkin CH, Castellon SA, Durvasula RS, et al. Medication adherence among HIV+ adults: Effects of cognitive dysfunction and regimen complexity. Neurology. 2002;59:1944–1950. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000038347.48137.67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hinkin CH, Hardy DJ, Mason KI, et al. Medication adherence in HIV-infected adults: Effect of patient age, cognitive status, and substance abuse. AIDS. 2004;18(1):S19–S25. doi: 10.1097/00002030-200418001-00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Thames AD, Arentoft A, Rivera-Mindt M, et al. Functional disability in medication management and driving among individuals with HIV: A 1-year follow-up study. J Clin Exp Neuropsyc. 2013;35(1):49–58. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2012.747596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Lezak M, Howieson D, Loring D, et al. Neuropsychological assessment. 4. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. p. 35. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, et al. BRIEF: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: PAR, Inc; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Guy SC, Isquith PK, Gioia GA. BRIEF-SR: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Self-report Version, Professional Manual. Lutz FL: PAR, Inc; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Bagner D, Williams L, Geffken G, et al. Type 1 diabetes in youth: The relationship between adherence and executive functioning. Child Health Care. 2007;36(2):169–179. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.McNally K, Rohan J, Pendley J, et al. Executive functioning, treatment adherence, and glycemic control in children with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2010;33(6):1159–1162. doi: 10.2337/dc09-2116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Nichols SL, Brummel SS, Smith RA, et al. Executive functioning in children and adolescents with perinatal HIV infection. Pediatr Infect Dis J. 2015 Sep;34(9):969–75. doi: 10.1097/INF.0000000000000809. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Van Dyke RB, Lee S, Johnson GM, et al. Reported adherence as a determinant of response to highly active antiretroviral therapy in children who have Human Immunodeficiency Virus Infection. Pediatrics. 2002;109:e61. doi: 10.1542/peds.109.4.e61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Usitalo A, Leister E, Tassiopoulos K, et al. Relationship between viral load and self-report measures of medication adherence among youth with perinatal HIV infection. AIDS Care. 2014 Jan;26(1):107–15. doi: 10.1080/09540121.2013.802280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Chesney MA, Ickovics JR, Chambers DB, et al. for the Patient Care Committee & Adherence Working Group of the Outcomes Committee of the Adult AIDS Clinical Trials Group (AACTG) Self-reported adherence to antiretroviral medications among participants in HIV clinical trials: The AACTG adherence Instruments. AIDS Care. 2000;12:255–266. doi: 10.1080/09540120050042891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Harrison PL, Oakland T. Adaptive Behavior Assessment System. 2. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessment; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Llorente AM, Williams J, Satz P, et al. Children’s Color Trails Test. Lutz, FL: PAR, Inc; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Wechsler D. Technical and Interpretive Manual. 4. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2003. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wechsler D. Wechsler Individual Achievement Test 2nd Edition (WIAT II) Examiner’s Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2002. 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Vreeman RC, Wiehe SE, Pearce EC, et al. A systematic review of pediatric adherence to antiretroviral therapy in low- and middle-income countries. Pediatr Infect Dis J. 2008;27(8):686–69. doi: 10.1097/INF.0b013e31816dd325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Reed-Knight B, Lewis JD, Blount RL. Association of disease, adolescent, and family factors with medication adherence in pediatric inflammatory bowel disease. J Pediatr Psychol. 2011;36(3):308–17. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsq076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Wysocki T, Nansel TR, Holmbeck GN, et al. Collaborative involvement of primary and secondary caregivers: Associations with youths’ diabetes outcomes. J Pediatr Psychol. 2009;34(8):869–81. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsn136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ellis DA, Podolski CL, Frey M, et al. The role of parental monitoring in adolescent health outcomes: Impact on regimen adherence in youth with type 1 diabetes. J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32(8):907–17. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsm009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Naar-King S, Montepiedra G, Nichols S, et al. Allocation of family responsibility for illness management in pediatric HIV. J Pediatr Psychol. 2009;34(2):187–94. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsn065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Havens JF, Mellins CA. Psychiatric aspects of HIV/AIDS. In: Rutter M, Taylor E, editors. Rutter’s Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 5. Oxford, UK: Blackwell; 2008. pp. 945–955. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES