Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2017 Jun 15;6:e22861. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22861

A subset of ipRGCs regulates both maturation of the circadian clock and segregation of retinogeniculate projections in mice

Kylie S Chew 1,2,, Jordan M Renna 3,, David S McNeill 1, Diego C Fernandez 1,, William T Keenan 1, Michael B Thomsen 1, Jennifer L Ecker 1, Gideon S Loevinsohn 4, Cassandra VanDunk 5,6, Daniel C Vicarel 3, Adele Tufford 7, Shijun Weng 4, Paul A Gray 5,8, Michel Cayouette 7,9, Erik D Herzog 10, Haiqing Zhao 1, David M Berson 4,*, Samer Hattar 1,*,
Editor: Joseph S Takahashi11
PMCID: PMC5513697  PMID: 28617242

Abstract

The visual system consists of two major subsystems, image-forming circuits that drive conscious vision and non-image-forming circuits for behaviors such as circadian photoentrainment. While historically considered non-overlapping, recent evidence has uncovered crosstalk between these subsystems. Here, we investigated shared developmental mechanisms. We revealed an unprecedented role for light in the maturation of the circadian clock and discovered that intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs) are critical for this refinement process. In addition, ipRGCs regulate retinal waves independent of light, and developmental ablation of a subset of ipRGCs disrupts eye-specific segregation of retinogeniculate projections. Specifically, a subset of ipRGCs, comprising ~200 cells and which project intraretinally and to circadian centers in the brain, are sufficient to mediate both of these developmental processes. Thus, this subset of ipRGCs constitute a shared node in the neural networks that mediate light-dependent maturation of the circadian clock and light-independent refinement of retinogeniculate projections.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22861.001

Research Organism: Mouse

Introduction

Light is detected by the eye for image-forming functions, including conscious perception of the visual scene, and for non-image-forming (NIF) functions, such as synchronization of circadian rhythms to the solar day (circadian photoentrainment) and the pupillary light reflex. These diverse visual functions, both image- and non-image-forming, require the retina for the detection and the initial processing of light signals, which are then relayed to the brain via the output neurons of the eye, retinal ganglion cells (RGCs). The majority of RGCs project to image-forming centers in the brain, such as the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (dLGN) and the superior colliculus (SC). A subset of RGCs, however, is intrinsically photosensitive (Berson, 2003; Berson et al., 2002; Hattar et al., 2002; Provencio et al., 1998), in addition to receiving indirect light signals from the classical photoreceptors, rods and cones (Lucas et al., 2003; Mrosovsky and Hattar, 2003; Panda et al., 2002; Ruby et al., 2002; Schmidt et al., 2008). These intrinsically photosensitive RGCs (ipRGCs) constitute the sole conduit of light information to non-image-forming centers in the brain, such as the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) (Güler et al., 2008; Hattar et al., 2003; Panda et al., 2003). ipRGCs drive non-image-forming behaviors even in the absence of rods and cones (Czeisler et al., 1995; Foster et al., 1991; Freedman et al., 1999; Lucas et al., 2001, 1999). Early reports envisioned a strict separation between the image- and non-image-forming visual networks (Dreher and Robinson, 1991; Moore, 1997; Young and Lund, 1994), but this view has begun to be challenged by recent evidence for functional crosstalk between the two systems (Ecker et al., 2010; Estevez et al., 2012; Renna et al., 2011; Schmidt et al., 2014; Zhang et al., 2008).

During development, all RGCs must achieve precise central connections in the brain that are necessary for the generation of visual behaviors. Precise visual circuits emerge in a multistep process: axon guidance pathways establish a coarse level of organization, which are then refined in an activity-dependent manner (Arroyo and Feller, 2016; Oster and Sretavan, 2003; Osterhout et al., 2014; Wong, 1999). The image-forming visual system has been a classic model of activity-dependent refinement of neuronal circuits, and both light-dependent and independent mechanisms play critical roles (Hubel et al., 1977; Meister et al., 1991; Shatz and Stryker, 1988). Refinement of the coarse projections of the RGCs to the dLGN is dependent on spontaneously generated neural activity, termed retinal waves. These retinal waves, with defined properties, sweep across the retina to instruct segregation of eye-specific projections to the dLGN and SC (Ackman et al., 2012; Chandrasekaran et al., 2005; Feller, 2002, 2009; Firth et al., 2005; McLaughlin et al., 2003; Meister et al., 1991; Mrsic-Flogel et al., 2005; Muir-Robinson et al., 2002; Shatz and Stryker, 1988; Stellwagen and Shatz, 2002; Wong, 1999; Xu et al. 2011; Zhang et al., 2011). Interestingly, light detection through ipRGCs influences retinal waves (Renna et al., 2011), although, eye-specific segregation still proceeds normally in the absence of light (Demas et al., 2006). In turn, retinal waves have been shown to modulate the intraretinal gap junction network of ipRGCs (Arroyo et al., 2016). Furthermore, light detection by ipRGCs has also been implicated in regulating developmental vascularization in the eye (Rao et al., 2013). However, it has not been determined whether ipRGCs have light-independent developmental roles or whether the developmental roles of ipRGCs have permanent functional consequences.

ipRGCs are best known for their ability to synchronize the circadian clock in the SCN to the solar day (Chen et al., 2011; Güler et al., 2008), a process known as circadian photoentrainment. The circadian clock contains an intrinsic genetic program that, in the absence of environmental light input, produces molecular and physiological rhythms with periods close to, but not exactly, 24 hr (Harmer et al., 2001; Menaker et al., 1978). Due to the autonomous nature of the circadian clock, the prevailing view has been that the fundamental features of the clock in the SCN, such as period length, do not require environmental input for maturation (Davis and Menaker, 1981; Davis and Gorski, 1985; Jud and Albrecht, 2006; Pittendrigh, 1954; Richter, 1971; Vallone et al., 2007; Yamazaki et al., 2002). However, there is intriguing evidence that animals that do not form eyes or the optic nerve due to genetic defects exhibit a lengthened circadian period (Laemle and Ottenweller, 1998; Wee et al., 2002). Since ipRGCs are the major, if not the sole, source of retinal input to the SCN (Baver et al., 2008; Berson, 2003; Hattar et al., 2006, 2002), these observations implicate a possible role for ipRGCs in the maturation of the circadian clock.

ipRGCs have now been shown to also project to the image-forming visual system (Ecker et al., 2010). These projections appear to arise from subtypes of ipRGCs that are morphologically and physiologically distinct from the originally identified ipRGCs, which are now known as M1 ipRGCs (Ecker et al., 2010; Schmidt et al., 2011; Schmidt and Kofuji, 2009; Schmidt et al., 2008). M1 ipRGCs predominantly innervate non-image-forming centers and are molecularly differentiated based on the expression of a transcription factor, Pou4f2 (also called Brn3b) (Chen et al., 2011). Specifically, Pou4f2-negative M1 ipRGCs project exclusively to circadian centers in the brain and send intraretinal axonal collaterals (Chen et al., 2011), whereas the majority of M1 ipRGCs and all non-M1 ipRGCs express Pou4f2. The non-M1 ipRGC subtypes (M2-M5) send their axons to the dLGN and SC and are capable of supporting coarse pattern vision in animals lacking functional rod and cone phototransduction pathways (Brown et al., 2010; Ecker et al., 2010). In addition, the M4 subtype of ipRGCs has been recently shown to influence vision by specifically mediating contrast sensitivity (Estevez et al., 2012; Schmidt et al., 2014).

Here, we used a variety of mutant mouse lines in which different proportions of ipRGCs are ablated at different ages to investigate developmental roles for ipRGCs and the long-term functional consequences. We show that a subset of ipRGCs (Pou4f2-negative M1 ipRGCs) influence the development of both the circadian and the visual systems. Furthermore, we show that light plays an essential role in the circadian but not the image-forming visual process.

Results

Developmental ablation of ipRGCs using Diphtheria toxin A subunit

We utilized two allelic changes at the melanopsin locus to ablate ipRGCs at different ages. As we have published previously, expression of attenuated diphtheria toxin (aDTA) from the melanopsin (Opn4) locus (Opn4aDTA) results in ablation of mostly the M1 ipRGCs and does so only at adult ages (Güler et al., 2008) (Figure 1A,B,D,E). A newly generated line, expresses the full-strength version of diphtheria toxin (DTA) (Opn4DTA) and causes ablation of ipRGCs at early postnatal stages (Figure 1; Figure 1—figure supplement 1A–B; Supplementary file 1). To quantify ipRGC loss in the aDTA and DTA lines, we used two genetic labeling methods. The Opn4Cre; Z/AP mice, in which alkaline phosphatase (AP) expression is dependent on Cre expression in ipRGCs, labels all subtypes of ipRGCs (Ecker et al., 2010). Whereas in Opn4LacZ mice only M1 ipRGCs are labeled following X-Gal staining for β-galactosidase activity (Hattar et al., 2002; McNeill et al., 2011). In animals heterozygous for DTA and Cre (Opn4Cre/DTA; Z/AP mice; Supplementary file 1), ipRGCs were reduced in number at birth (Figure 1A,C). Total ipRGC number declined until P14, when approximately 500 cells survived, and then remained constant thereafter through 1year of age (Figure 1A,C and E). Using the LacZ locus with the aDTA or DTA loci, we show that at 6 months of age, about 75 M1 ipRGCs survived in Opn4LacZ/DTA mice (Supplementary file 1), and consistent with our previous report, about 125 M1 ipRGCs survived in Opn4LacZ/aDTA mice (Güler et al., 2008) (Figure 1F; Supplementary file 1). These results show that even some ipRGCs that express high levels of melanopsin (M1s) can survive the presence of a single dose of the full strength DTA.

Figure 1. Developmental ablation of ipRGCs in the mouse retina.

(A) Developmental time course of ipRGC innervation of the SCN, visualized by AP staining, in Opn4Cre/+; Z/AP and Opn4Cre/DTA; Z/AP mouse. For comparison, SCN staining from Opn4Cre/aDTA; Z/AP mice at P14 are also shown. Scale bar = 200 μm. (B) SCN innervation in P7 WT, Opn4DTA/DTA, and Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice revealed by CTB injections into the eyes. Scale bar = 100 μm. (C) Developmental time course of ipRGC (all subtypes) cell density visualized by AP staining of retina from Opn4Cre/+ Z/AP (control) and Opn4Cre/DTA; Z/AP mice at P0 (control n = 3, DTA n = 7), P3 (control n = 7, DTA n = 5), P5 (control n = 6, DTA n = 4), P9 (control n = 4, DTA n = 4), and P14 (control n = 3, DTA n = 6, aDTA n = 5). Cell counts from P14 retinas of Opn4Cre/aDTA; Z/AP mice are also shown for comparison. Using a two-way ANOVA, we found a strongly significant effect of genotype. A t-test for P0, P3, P5, and P9 time points, and a one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni's post-hoc analysis for P14 revealed a significant cell loss at each time point. (D) SCN innervation revealed by CTB injections into the eyes of 6-month-old WT, Opn4DTA/+, Opn4DTA/DTA, and Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice. Scale bar = 200 μm. (E) Total cell counts of ipRGCs (all subtypes) revealed by alkaline phosphatase staining at P14 and 1 year of age in Opn4Cre/+; Z/AP (control; P14: n = 5, 1 year: n = 4), Opn4Cre/aDTA; Z/AP (P14: n = 5; 1 year: n = 3), and Opn4Cre/DTA; Z/AP (P14: n = 6; 1 year: n = 6). Two-way ANOVA, Bonferroni's multiple comparisons test and adjusted p values. (F) Total cell counts of M1 ipRGCs, identified by x-Gal staining of retinas from 6 month old Opn4LacZ/+ (control; n = 4), Opn4LacZ/aDTA, and Opn4LacZ/DTA mice (n = 4). One-way ANOVA, Bonferroni's multiple comparisons test and adjusted p values. Error bars represent s.e.m. for all graphs. See also Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22861.002

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Generation and characterization of mice with an Opn4DTA allele.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Targeting construct for inserting the coding sequence of DTA into the melanopsin (Opn4) locus. (B) Southern blot analysis confirming homologous recombination of the targeting construct at Opn4 locus in ES cells. (C) Hematoxylin and eosin staining on retinal sections from WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice. (D) Quantification of the retinal layer thickness based on hematoxylin and eosin staining. No significant differences were found by a Student’s t-test. (E) Staining with fluorescently conjugated peanut agglutinin to label cones (red), and immunohistochemistry for caleretinin-positive amacrine and ganglion cells (green), and Brn3a-positive ganglion cells (blue) on retinal sections from WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice. Scale bar = 100 µm. (F) Staining with fluorescently conjugated peanut agglutinin to label cones (red) and immunohistochemistry for γ−13 to label ON-bipolar cells (green) on retinal sections from WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice. Scale bar = 100 µm. (G and H) We used two markers that label distinct populations of conventional RGCs: Brn3a is a marker for ~80% of conventional RGCs and it does not colocalize with melanopsin. SMI-32 labels alpha-like RGCs, a subset of which are M4 ipRGCs. Both RGC labeling methods showed no significant difference between Opn4DTA/DTA and WT. These data indicate that conventional RGCs and even a subtype of ipRGCs that expresses low levels of melanopsin are not ablated in Opn4DTA/DTA mice. Scale bar = 100 µm. (I) Density of cones, identified by staining with fluorescently conjugated peanut agglutinin, was not significantly different between Opn4DTA/DTA and WT. Scale bar = 50 µm. (J) We used ChAT staining to label Starburst amacrine cells, which are important for retinal waves. We found that the density of ChAT positive amacrine cells was similar between WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice. Scale bar = 100 µm. (G–J) No significant differences were found by a Student’s t-test.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. The number of cells in the SCN is unaffected in Opn4DTA/DTA mice.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(A) Number of DAPI-labeled nuclei in the SCN of WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice graphed rostral to caudal based on 25 μm coronal sections. (B) Total number of DAPI-labeled nuclei in the SCN of WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice. (A) Number of neurons (identified by anti-Hu immunohistochemistry) in the SCN of WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice by 25 μm coronal section graphed rostral to caudal based on 25 μm coronal sections. (B) Total number of neurons in the SCN of WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice. (A and C) No significant differences by two-way ANOVA, Bonferroni's multiple comparisons test and adjusted p values. (B and D) No significant differences were found by a Student’s t-test. Error bars for all graphs represent s.e.m.

We then sought to determine whether two alleles of DTA would result in a more complete ipRGC ablation. Since melanopsin is the only known marker for all ipRGCs, either an allele of Cre or LacZ is required at the melanopsin locus in order for ipRGCs to be labeled and quantified. Neither an anti-melanopsin antibody nor in situ hybridization with a melanopsin probe can be utilized because Cre, LacZ, aDTA, and DTA replace the melanopsin gene and thus result in a knockout for melanopsin. However, retinal innervation of the SCN originates exclusively from ipRGCs and thus the extent of innervation can be used as a proxy for ipRGC loss. To determine whether two copies of DTA (Opn4DTA/DTA; Supplementary file 1) would produce further ablation of ipRGCs, we therefore examined retinal innervation of the SCN as an indirect measure of ipRGC loss. We labeled all retinal projections by injecting fluorescently labeled cholera toxin subunitβ (CTB, a neuronal tracer) into the eyes and compared SCN innervation in 6-month-old wild-type, heterozygous (Opn4DTA/+), homozygous attenuated-DTA (Opn4aDTA/aDTA), and homozygous DTA (Opn4DTA/DTA) mice (Figure 1D; Supplementary file 1). Sparse retinal fibers were found in the SCN of heterozygous DTA mice (Opn4DTA/+) as observed in Opn4Cre/DTA; Z/AP mice (Figure 1A) but these retinal fibers were entirely absent in Opn4DTA/DTA mice (Figure 1D) suggesting that Opn4DTA/DTA mice have more extensive ipRGC loss than Opn4DTA/+ mice. The more substantial and earlier ipRGC loss in Opn4DTA/DTA mice was also confirmed by examining SCN innervation using CTB injections in P7 wild-type, Opn4aDTA/aDTA, and Opn4DTA/DTA mice (Figure 1B).

Given the strength of DTA as a toxin, we assessed Opn4DTA/DTA mice for off-target effects. General retinal structure was evaluated by staining retinal sections from wild-type and Opn4DTA/DTA mice with hematoxylin and eosin stains and additionally fluorescent staining for various retinal cell types, including cones, ON bipolar cells, calretinin-positive amacrine and ganglion cells, and Brn3a positive ganglion cells (Figure 1—figure supplement 1C,E–F). Wild-type and Opn4DTA/DTA mice appeared similar by these stains, and quantification of total retinal thickness, as well as the thickness of each of the retinal layers, based on hematoxylin and eosin staining showed no difference between wild-type and Opn4DTA/DTA mice (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D). We also counted the number of SMI-32-positive ganglion cells, Brn3a-positive ganglion cells, cones, and starburst amacrine cells (Figure 1—figure supplement 1G–J). When we compared wild-type and Opn4DTA/DTA mice, there was no difference in cell number of any of these cell types. It is relevant to mention that about 50% of SMI-32 positive ganglion cells are actually M4 ipRGCs (Estevez et al., 2012; Schmidt et al., 2014), which express lower levels of melanopsin compared to M1 ipRGCs (Ecker et al., 2010; Sexton et al., 2015). Thus, expression of two copies of DTA from the melanopsin locus does not even succeed in killing all ipRGCs. Nonetheless, since we lack specific molecular markers for the other non-M1 ipRGC subtypes, we have no means of knowing the exact time course and extent of ablation for each of those subtypes. To assess potential off target effects in relevant brain regions, we counted the total number of nuclei (based on DAPI staining) and the total number of neurons (identified by antibody staining for Hu) in the SCN of wild-type and Opn4DTA/DTA mice. There was no difference between wild-type and Opn4DTA/DTA mice in the number of nuclei or neurons when compared in total or section by section through the SCN (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Furthermore, the total size of the dLGN was also not different between wild-type and Opn4DTA/DTA mice or among any other tested mouse lines (Figure 4—figure supplement 4E). These data combined demonstrate that DTA efficiently and selectively ablated ipRGCs at early postnatal ages.

ipRGCs are necessary to set the period length of the circadian clock

We recorded wheel-running activity under a 12:12-LD cycle, to measure circadian photoentrainment, and under constant darkness, to measure the intrinsic properties of the circadian clock (Figure 2A–C). We assessed wild-type and Opn4LacZ/LacZ (a null allele for melanopsin) mice as controls and also recorded wheel-running activity of Opn4aDTA/aDTA, Opn4DTA/DTA, Opn4DTA/+, and Opn4DTA/LacZ mice (Figure 2A; Figure 2—figure supplement 1; Supplementary file 1). Opn4LacZ/LacZ and wild-type mice showed normal photoentrainment, phase shifting (at CT16), and free-running periods (Figure 2A–C). As previously published, Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice were unable to photoentrain or phase shift and free-ran under all lighting conditions with a period comparable to controls (Güler et al., 2008) (Figure 2A–C). Opn4DTA/+ mice photoentrained, phase shifted (at CT16), and free-ran in constant darkness with a normal circadian period (Figure 2A–C) indicating that residual retinal input to the SCN in heterozygotes is sufficient for circadian photoentrainment. In the Opn4DTA/LacZ mice, the ~500 remaining ipRGCs (~75 M1 ipRGCs) in heterozygous DTA animals, further lose their intrinsic photoresponses due to the loss of both melanopsin alleles and thus can only relay rod/cone input to the brain. In these mice, circadian responses to light were highly attenuated, with inconsistent entrainment and phase-shifting, and exhibited a free-running period comparable to control mice (Figure 2A–C, Figure 2—figure supplement 1A). Similar to Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice, Opn4DTA/DTA mice did not photoentrain; however, they free-ran with an abnormally lengthened period (Figure 2A,B; Figure 2—figure supplement 1B). Because full-strength DTA, but not aDTA, kills ipRGCs during early development, these findings suggested the hypothesis that ipRGCs act during early postnatal ages to establish a normal circadian period length. We performed a general examination of expression of transcription factors important for SCN development and a neuropeptide relevant for clock function to examine the SCN, and found no apparent disruption in the SCN of Opn4DTA/DTA mice (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). Thus, loss of ipRGC innervation does not drastically alter SCN development, and it is possible the lengthened period stems from a loss of direct ipRGC-dependent regulation of SCN neurons. Alternatively, this effect could be due to altered input from the intergeniculate leaflet (IGL), which also receives innervation by ipRGCs.

Figure 2. Developmental ablation of ipRGCs results in a lengthened circadian period.

(A) Representative actograms of wild-type, Opn4LacZ/LacZ, Opn4DTA/+, Opn4DTA/LacZ, Opn4DTA/DTA and Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice under 12:12-LD cycle and constant darkness (DD). White background indicates light and grey background indicates darkness. (B) The circadian period length of all tested genotypes (wild-type: n = 18, Opn4LacZ/LacZ: n = 17, Opn4DTA/+: n = 8, Opn4DTA/LacZ: n = 7, Opn4DTA/DTA: n = 7, and Opn4aDTA/aDTA: n = 18). (C) For phase-shifting experiments, a subset of wild-type, Opn4LacZ/LacZ, Opn4aDTA/aDTA were assessed, and the total mice analyzed was as follows: wild-type (n = 7), Opn4LacZ/LacZ (n = 8), Opn4DTA/+ (n = 8), Opn4DTA/LacZ (n = 7), Opn4DTA/DTA (n = 7), and Opn4aDTA/aDTA (n = 8). As expected from substantial ipRGC ablation, phase shifting was significantly reduced in Opn4DTA/LacZ, Opn4DTA/DTA and Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice. (D) Representative actograms of wild-type mice enucleated at either P0 (n = 8) or P60 (n = 7), under 12:12-LD and DD. (E) The circadian period length for both enucleation groups. Only mice enucleated at P0 exhibited a lengthened circadian period (B and C) One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni's multiple comparisons test and adjusted p values. (E) Two-way ANOVA, Bonferroni's multiple comparisons test and adjusted p values. Error bars represent s.e.m. for all graphs. See also Figure 2—figure supplements 12.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22861.005

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Additional actograms for Opn4DTA/LacZ and Opn4DTA/DTA mice.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Actograms of all tested Opn4DTA/LacZ. (B) Actograms of all tested Opn4DTA/DTA mice.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Expression pattern of transcription factors critical for SCN development.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

In situ hybridization for several developmentally important transcription factors, Rora, Six3, and Lhx1 as well as the functionally important neuropeptide vasopressin (AVP) in control (Opn4DTA/+) and Opn4DTA/DTA mice to test whether the lengthened circadian period exhibited by Opn4DTA/DTA (but not Opn4DTA/+) mice could be attributed to gross changes in the SCN. Each row represents one animal. The expression patterns of these genes were similar between these two groups of mice.

To assess the relevance of early ablation of ipRGCs in causing the lengthened circadian period of Opn4DTA/DTA mice, we removed both eyes from mice at either P0 or P60 to mimic the killing of ipRGCs early (Opn4DTA/DTA) versus late (Opn4aDTA/aDTA). Starting at P74, we recorded wheel-running activity under a 12:12-LD cycle and constant darkness. Since the mice lack eyes, they lacked photic effects on the clock (Figure 2D,E). However, mice enucleated at P0 exhibited a lengthened circadian period similar to Opn4DTA/DTA mice, whereas mice enucleated at P60 phenocopied Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice exhibiting a period comparable to intact mice (Figure 2A–B,D–E). Circadian period length has been shown to be stable in adult mice kept under constant darkness for 2 months (Campuzano et al., 1999). Thus, these data corroborate results from Opn4DTA/DTA mice indicating that ipRGCs function postnatally to set the length of the circadian period.

Light is required to set the length of the intrinsic circadian period

We asked whether light-driven input from ipRGCs is important for establishing circadian period length. Wild-type animals were raised under either constant darkness or a 12:12-LD cycle. At P60, we assessed the intrinsic circadian period of these animals by recording their wheel-running activity in constant darkness (Figure 3A,B; Figure 3—figure supplement 1). While the phenotype was less penetrant than that the Opn4DTA/DTA mice and P0 enucleates, most (9 of 16) dark-reared mice exhibited a longer intrinsic circadian period than mice raised in a 12:12-LD cycle and this was stable for duration of our recordings (up to 60 days; Figure 3A,B; Figure 3—figure supplement 1). The lengthened period of the dark-reared animals was comparable to that exhibited by mice enucleated at P0 and mice with early postnatal ablation of ipRGCs (Opn4DTA/DTA) (Figures 2 and 3; Figure 3—figure supplement 1). It has been thought that the mammalian circadian period length was established independent of sensory input, but our data show that light contributes to this process. In zebrafish, it is known that light is required to initiate expression of clock genes and for generation of rhythms (Ben-Moshe et al., 2014; Kazimi and Cahill, 1999; Ziv et al., 2005); however, this is the first evidence to indicate that light is also required for mammalian clock maturation.

Figure 3. Light sets the circadian period length, even during adulthood.

(A and B) Representative actograms of wild-type mice raised in either a 12:12-LD cycle (n = 13) or darkness (n = 16), and exposed to 12:12-LD cycle for 1 month. The circadian period of dark-reared mice is lengthened initially, but rescued and is no longer significantly different from 12:12-LD reared mice after light exposure. (C and D) Representative actograms of wild-type mice raised in either a 12:12-LD cycle (n = 17) or in darkness (n = 12), and then exposed to a 3 hr light pulse. Dark-reared mice exhibited a longer period. Following the 3 hr light pulse, the circadian period length of dark-reared animals shortened. (B and D) Two-way ANOVA, Bonferroni's multiple comparisons test and adjusted p values. Error bars represent s.e.m. See also Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22861.008

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Actograms for all dark reared mice tested.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

The lengthened period phenotype exhibited by dark-reared animals was less penetrant than in Opn4DTA/DTA or P0 enucleated mice. Though a majority of dark-reared animals (9 of 16; 56%;) exhibited a lengthened period, the remainder did not. This is likely due to the process of setting the circadian period being highly sensitive to light.

Upon exposure to a 12:12-LD-cycle, dark-reared mice photoentrained, and light exposure during photoentrainment was sufficient to normalize the circadian period length (Figure 3A,B; Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Furthermore, a single short light pulse (3 hr), which did not cause photoentrainment, (Figure 3C,D) was also sufficient to set the circadian period of dark-reared animals (Figure 3C,D). These data indicate that brief light exposure is sufficient to set the circadian period and can even occur in adults, thus, indicating a lack of a critical developmental window for this process.

Developmental ablation of ipRGCs disrupts axonal segregation of retinogeniculate projections

By P7, ipRGCs have reached their central targets in the brain (Figure 1A–B) (McNeill et al., 2011) and a subset of ipRGCs forms intra-retinal collateral axons that terminate in the inner plexiform layer, where they synapse onto dopaminergic amacrine cells (Joo et al., 2013; Prigge et al., 2016) (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). P7 is in the middle of a critical developmental period for the image-forming visual system. From P0-P14, the retinotopic map is being refined by spontaneously generated neural activity in the retina, termed retinal waves (Ackman et al., 2012; Feller, 2009; Feller et al., 1996; McLaughlin et al., 2003; Meister et al., 1991; Ramoa et al., 1989; Shatz, 1990; Stellwagen and Shatz, 2002; Xu et al. 2011; Zhang et al., 2011). The presence of intra-retinal collaterals on ipRGCs at early postnatal ages provides anatomical means for ipRGCs to influence the retina during an important developmental window. The Opn4DTA/DTA mice allowed us the opportunity to directly test the involvement of ipRGCs, not just melanopsin-based light sensitivity, in the development and refinement of the image-forming visual system. To investigate this, we traced the axonal projections of retinal ganglion cells in adult wild-type and Opn4DTA/DTA mice, and observed that, while retinal innervation of the superior colliculus in Opn4DTA/DTA mice was comparable to wild-type (Figure 4—figure supplement 2), eye-specific axonal segregation in the dLGN was severely disrupted (Figure 4A,B; Figure 4—figure supplements 3 and 4). In Opn4DTA/DTA mice, the ipsilateral zone had poorly defined boundaries and substantial contralateral innervation (Figure 4A). We quantified eye-specific axonal segregation in the dLGN using two distinct methods (Datwani et al., 2009; Demas et al., 2006; Renna et al., 2011) (Figure 4B; Figure 4—figure supplement 3). In Opn4DTA/DTA mice, there was a significant increase in the percentage of pixels with overlapping input from the two eyes, and this eye-specific segregation defect was most severe in the caudal dLGN (Figure 4A, Figure 4—figure supplement 4F). The total amount of ipsilateral and contralateral input and total dLGN size were similar across all tested genotypes (Figure 4C,D, Figure 4—figure supplement 4C–E). Abnormal eye-specific segregation was not observed in either melanopsin knockout mice (Opn4LacZ/LacZ) or mice with ipRGCs ablated during adulthood (Opn4aDTA/aDTA) (Figure 4—figure supplement 4A–D). Importantly, Opn4DTA/+ mice, which have substantial ipRGC loss, although to lesser degree than Opn4DTA/DTA mice, exhibited disrupted eye-specific segregation that was intermediate between wild-type and Opn4DTA/DTA mice (Figure 4A,B, Figure 4—figure supplement 4F). Combined, these data reveal an developmental role for ipRGCs in the segregation of all RGC inputs to the dLGN.

Figure 4. Adult Opn4DTA/DTA mice display deficits of eye-specific axonal segregation and visual acuity.

(A) RGC axonal innervation of the adult dLGN of wild-type (n = 12), Opn4DTA/+ (n = 8), Opn4DTA/DTA (n = 10) and Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2zDTA/+ (n = 6; previously published as Opn4Cre/+; Brn3bzDTA/+). The rightmost images represent binarized version of the merged images to visualize the overlap between contralateral and ipsilateral RGC projections. Representative images were taken from the region of the dLGN indicated by the blue arrow in Figure 4—figure supplement 4F. (B) Opn4DTA/DTA mice exhibited a significantly higher percentage of overlapping pixels relative to the total number of LGN than any other tested genotype. Opn4DTA/+ mice exhibited levels of overlapping pixels that were intermediate compared to Opn4DTA/DTA and control mice. (C and D) The percentage of the total number of pixels in the dLGN from ipsilateral and contralateral fibers is similar among all tested genotypes. (E and F) The virtual optokinetic system and visual water task were used to assess visual function wild-type (virtual optokinetic system: n = 16, visual water task: n = 10), Opn4LacZ/LacZ (virtual optokinetic system: n = 4, visual water task: n = 6), Opn4DTA/+ (virtual optokinetic system: n = 9, visual water task: n = 6), Opn4DTA/DTA (virtual optokinetic system: n = 7, visual water task: n = 10) mice. Wild-type and Opn4LacZ/LacZ mice were indistinguishable. Opn4DTA/DTA mice exhibited reduced visual acuity compared to wild-type mice, and Opn4DTA/+ mice exhibited intermediate visual acuity. (B–F) One-way ANOVA, Bonferroni's multiple comparisons test and adjusted p values. Error bars represent s.e.m. See also Figure 4—figure supplements 15.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22861.010

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. ipRGC intra-retinal axonal collaterals are present by P7.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) Alkaline phosphatase (AP) staining in P7 Opn4CreERT2/+ retinas (Chen et al., 2013; Joo et al., 2013) from mice that had been injected with a low dose of tamoxifen at P0, which resulted in sparse labeling of ipRGCs with AP. This image shows an ipRGC axon branching with one branch (green arrows) projecting to the optic disk and the other (blue arrows) to the inner plexiform layer (IPL). Red arrow indicates the cell body, and the black arrowhead indicates the branching point of the axon. (B) A trace of the ipRGC shown in (A) that projects an intra-retinal axon collateral. The trace was compiled from images of consecutive focal planes. Red lines indicate dendrites and black lines indicate the axon.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Retinal innervation of the SC in Opn4DTA/DTA mice is indistinguishable from wild-type mice.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(A) Representative images of RGC axonal innervation of the adult SC in WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice. Alexa Fluor 594- (purple) and 488-(green) conjugated CTB were injected into the right and left eye, respectively, to label RGC projections. Images are of the left SC. (B) Heat map showing a reconstruction of the dorsal view of retinal innervation of the SC.
Figure 4—figure supplement 3. Quantification of disruption in eye-specific axonal segregation in the dLGN of adult Opn4DTA/DTA mice.

Figure 4—figure supplement 3.

(A and B) The averaged distribution of pixel intensity ratios (R) for all dLGN pixels in the right (A) and left (B) dLGN of adult WT (black curve) and Opn4DTA/DTA (red curve) mice are plotted. The mice were raised in 12:12 LD cycle. Opn4DTA/DTA mice have more unsegregated pixels (gray shaded area-see methods) than WT mice. (C) Pooled data normalized to WT mice indicated Opn4DTA/DTA mice have 16% more unsegregated pixels, and a 9% decrease in the variance of the R distribution (n = 4 WT, 4 Opn4DTA/DTA, 15 sections/mouse/hemisphere). Student-t test. Error bars represent s.e.m.
Figure 4—figure supplement 4. Eye-specific axonal segregation is normal in Opn4Cre/+, Opn4LacZ/LacZ, and Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice.

Figure 4—figure supplement 4.

(A) RGC axonal innervation of adult dLGN in Opn4Cre/+, Opn4LacZ/LacZ, and Opn4aDTA/aDTA. Images are of the right dLGN. The rightmost images are a binarized version of the merged images to visualize the overlap between contralateral and ipsilateral RGC projections. Representative images were taken from the region of the dLGN indicated in D (blue arrow). Opn4LacZ/LacZ and Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice served as additional controls for Opn4DTA/DTA mice and were indistinguishable from WT. Opn4Cre/+ mice served as an additional control for Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2zDTA/+ mice (previously published as Opn4Cre/+; Brn3bzDTA/+) and were indistinguishable from each other as well as not significantly different from WT mice. (B) Percentage of overlapping pixels in all tested genotypes. (C, D) The percentage of the total number of pixels in the dLGN from ipsilateral and contralateral fibers was similar among all tested genotypes. (E) Size of the dLGN is comparable across all tested genotypes. In B–D, values for WT, Opn4DTA/+, Opn4DTA/DTA and Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2zDTA/+ mice were re-plotted from Figure 4B–D for comparison. * indicates p<0.05 and ** indicates p<0.001 with one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni's multiple comparisons test. (F) The rostral to caudal distribution of percent overlap between contralateral and ipsilateral projections through the dLGN. The blue arrow indicates the section of the dLGN presented in A) and in Figure 4A. Error bars represent s.e.m.
Figure 4—figure supplement 5. All but ~200 ipRGCs are ablated by P7 in Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2zDTA/+ mice.

Figure 4—figure supplement 5.

(A) Cell counts of total number of ipRGCs in Opn4Cre/+; Z/AP (control) and Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2zDTA/+; Z/AP mice at P7 (control: n = 5; Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2zDTA/+; Z/AP: n = 5) and P14 (control: n = 5; Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2zDTA/+; Z/AP: n = 2). ipRGCs were identified by AP staining, which labels all subtypes. In Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2zDTA/+; Z/AP mice, there was a significant loss of ipRGCs by P7 and no further loss at P14 suggesting that the loss of all but ~200 ipRGCs is complete by P7. One-way ANOVA Bonferroni's multiple comparisons test and adjusted p values. (B) Total retinal innervation of the SCN visualized with Alexa Fluor 594- (purple) and 488-(green) conjugated CTB in Opn4Cre/+ (control), Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2zDTA/+, and Opn4DTA/DTA mice. Scale bar = 200 µm.

To conclusively determine whether ipRGCs function during early postnatal ages to regulate refinement of retinogeniculate projections, we examined eye-specific segregation at P8 and, similar to adult mice, found an increase in overlapping ipsilateral and contralateral projections in Opn4DTA/DTA mice, but no difference in total retinal input compared to wild-type (Figure 5A–D). These data indicate that ipRGCs function at early postnatal ages to mediate refinement of the image-forming visual system by regulating eye-specific segregation of retinogeniculate projections.

Figure 5. Opn4DTA/DTA mice display disrupted eye-specific axonal segregation as early as P8 and exhibit altered RGC firing properties.

Figure 5.

(A) RGC axonal innervation of the dLGN in P8 wild-type (n = 4) and Opn4DTA/DTA (n = 4) mice as in Figure 3A. (B) At P8, Opn4DTA/DTA mice exhibit a significantly higher percentage of overlapping pixels relative to the total number of LGN pixels than wild-type mice. (C and D) The percentage of the total number of pixels in the dLGN from ipsilateral and contralateral fibers is similar among all tested genotypes. * indicates p<0.05 with Student’s t-test. Error bars represent s.e.m. for all graphs. (E) RGC spiking properties. Opn4DTA/DTA mice exhibit longer WABs and had a higher firing rate, with more spikes and a shorter inter-burst interval. There were also significantly more spikes outside of WABs. (F) Spike Time Tiling Coefficient (STTC) versus distance demonstrating that correlated spiking activity between neurons was similar in Opn4DTA/DTA mice compared to controls. 275 spike trains from wild-type/control, 245 from DTA. * represent statistically significant differences after Student's T-tests with a Holm-Bonferroni correction, α = 0.05, m = 22 for spiking properties, m = 10 for STTC.’ Exact p values are listed in Supplementary file 2. (G) Representative raster plots from multielectrode array recording of retinal waves in P6 WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice in the dark. Each row represents the activity on a single electrode. (H) Expansion of one wave (identified by the red box in (G)). Error bars represent s.e.m. for all graphs.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22861.016

Mice with early ablation of ipRGCs have deficits in visual acuity

To examine the functional consequence of altered eye-specific segregation, we compared wild-type, Opn4LacZ/LacZ, and Opn4DTA/DTA mice in two behavioral tests of visual acuity: the virtual optomotor system and the visual water task (Douglas et al., 2005; Prusky et al., 2000). By both measures, Opn4DTA/DTA animals exhibited reduced visual acuity compared to controls (wild-type and Opn4LacZ/LacZ; Figure 4E,F).

Heterozygous DTA animals (Opn4DTA/+) have less ipRGC loss than homozygous mice (Opn4DTA/DTA) (Figure 1D; Supplementary file 1) and have intermediate deficits in eye-specific segregation compared to homozygous and wild-type mice (Figure 4A,B; Figure 4—figure supplement 4F). Remarkably, Opn4DTA/+ mice also exhibited an intermediate reduction in visual acuity (Figure 4E,F). Thus, the severity of the deficits in eye-specific segregation and visual functions were correlated with the extent of ipRGC loss. While many factors including circadian time and the pupillary light reflex contribute to visual acuity, these factors cannot explain the deficits in visual acuity observed in Opn4DTA/+ and Opn4DTA/DTA mice. First, the loss of acuity in Opn4DTA/DTA cannot be explained by a loss of photoentrainment and the possibility that we are testing acuity at different circadian times, because Opn4DTA/+ mice, which photoentrain also exhibit deficits in visual acuity. In addition, Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice, which free run, do not exhibit the substantial reduction in acuity observed in Opn4DTA/DTA mice. Furthermore, loss of the pupillary light reflex cannot fully explain the deficits in visual acuity because when the pupil is fully dilated with atropine there is only a minor reduction in acuity and it is not comparable to the substantial deficits observed in Opn4DTA/+ and Opn4DTA/DTA mice (Güler et al., 2008). However, it is important to note that we cannot rule out contributions from direct effects of ipRGCs on retinal functions. For example, the role of M1 ipRGCs in regulating dopamine in the retina, could contribute (Dkhissi-Benyahya et al., 2013; Prigge et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2012).

Spontaneous retinal activity is altered in Opn4DTA/DTA mice

Eye-specific segregation deficits in Opn4DTA/DTA mice were observed by P8, and since light through ipRGCs modulates retinal wave activity and retinal waves drive ipRGC spiking (Renna et al., 2011), we hypothesized that Opn4DTA/DTA have altered spontaneous retinal activity in darkness. We recorded wave activity at P6 in the dark on a multielectrode array in wild-type and Opn4DTA/DTA retinas (Figure 5E–H; Supplementary file 2). Spiking properties of RGCs during waves were significantly altered in Opn4DTA/DTA mice. Wave-associated bursts (WABs) were significantly longer in duration, and had higher firing rates, and shorter inter-burst intervals (Figure 5E,G–H, Supplementary file 2) than they did in wild-type controls. There were also significantly more total spikes as well as more spikes outside of bursts (Figure 5E,G–H, Supplementary file 2). Correlated spiking activity between neurons was very similar between the genotypes, as measured by the spike time tiling coefficient (STTC; [Cutts and Eglen, 2014])(Figure 5F). These data indicate that ipRGCs are critical for normal retinal wave activity, even in darkness, and together with our anatomical findings (Figure 4A–D; Figure 4—figure supplements 34), allow us to suggest that ipRGCs mediate eye-specific segregation of RGC projections to the dLGN by regulating the spiking properties of conventional RGCs during retinal waves.

200 M1 ipRGCs are sufficient for both setting the circadian period and refining the imaging-forming visual system

There are currently five identified subtypes of ipRGCs. M1 ipRGCs project to non-image-forming brain centers, while M2-M5 ipRGCs project at least in part to image-forming targets (Ecker et al., 2010; Schmidt et al., 2011; Schmidt and Kofuji, 2009). The M1 subtype can be further subdivided based on expression of the transcription factor Pou4f2 (also referred to as Brn3b), which is also expressed in all non-M1 ipRGCs (Chen et al., 2011).~200 Pou4f2-negative-M1 ipRGCs project exclusively to circadian centers (predominantly to the SCN) (Figure 4—figure supplement 5B) (Chen et al., 2011) and are the subset of ipRGCs that have intra-retinal collateral axons (data not shown). This population of cells are sufficient for circadian photoentrainment in the presence of one copy of melanopsin (Chen et al., 2011). As described previously, Pou4f2-positive M1 ipRGCs and non-M1 ipRGCs can be selectively ablated by crossing Opn4Cre/+ mice with Pou4f2Z-DTA/+ mice (previously published as Brn3bZ-DTA/+ mice), in which a floxed stop cassette followed by DTA was inserted into the Pou4f2 locus (Chen et al., 2011; Mu et al., 2005). In doubly heterozygous offspring (Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2Z-DTA/+, previously published as Opn4Cre/+;Brn3bZ-DTA/+ mice, Supplementary file 1), Pou4f2-positive ipRGCs (some M1 and all non-M1 ipRGCs) are ablated by P7, leaving a subset of M1 Pou4f2-negative ipRGCs (~200 cells) and all conventional RGCs (Chen et al., 2011) (Figure 4—figure supplement 5A, Supplementary file 1). In these mice, the SCN remains innervated by ipRGCs, the IGL receives partial innervation, and ipRGC innervation of image-forming brain regions is entirely abolished (Chen et al., 2011) (Figure 4—figure supplement 5B).

In Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2Z-DTA/+ mice, ablation of Pou4f2-positive ipRGCs is complete by P7, and at this age only the ~200 Pou4f2-negative ipRGCs remain (Figure 4—figure supplement 5A; Supplementary file 1). Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2Z-DTA/+ mice can photoentrain and free-run with a circadian period indistinguishable from controls (Chen et al., 2011), indicating that the remaining Pou4f2-negative-M1 ipRGCs are sufficient to set the circadian period. Organization of eye-specific retinogeniculate projections in Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2Z-DTA/+ is also indistinguishable from controls (Figure 4A–D; Figure 4—figure supplement 4A–D). Together these results demonstrate that a single subpopulation of ipRGCs are critically involved in the development of networks devoted to image-forming vision and the circadian clock. This small population of cells is sufficient for setting of the circadian period as well as proper segregation of retinogeniculate circuitry.

Discussion

In this study, we identified a shared developmental mechanism for the maturation of the circadian clock and refinement of eye-specific segregation. Our work reveals the unprecedented role that light plays in setting the circadian period and identified a regulatory component of the neural networks that regulate refinement of RGC projections to the thalamus.

The methods we used to ablate ipRGCs involve some inherent uncertainties. We used two different versions of diphtheria toxin subunit A with different potencies. As expected, the full-strength DTA was more efficient than was the attenuated form at killing ipRGCs, but it also ablated ipRGCs earlier in development. One concern of using two copies of the toxin gene is that even low leaky expression could lead to off-target cell death. We think this is unlikely since the toxin failed to ablate at least one ipRGC subtypes with low levels of melanopsin expression—the M4 cells. Moreover, loss of cells in the retina and retinofugal projections in the brain were consistent with selective loss of ipRGCs. However, it is always possible that DTA expression from the melanopsin locus causes a reduction in cell populations that we did not examine. Further, because DTA blocks protein translation, it could disrupt the function of some cells without actually killing them; the assays we performed do not distinguish between these forms of toxin action. In addition, technical limitations precluded full assessment the degree of cell loss for many ipRGC subtypes. We currently lack specific molecular markers for M2, M3, and M5 cells. Furthermore, dendritic stratification is key to distinguishing ipRGC subtypes, but DTA-dependent ipRGC loss occurs before dendritic arborizations are mature (between P10-P15, Coombs et al., 2007). However, it is clear that, in Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2Z-DTA/+ mice, Pou4f2-positive ipRGCs, which include all non-M1 ipRGCs, die early during development and yet, their death does not affect the circadian period or the segregation of RGC projections in the dLGN. Thus, despite some uncertainty about which ipRGCs types die in these mouse lines and when they do so, taken together our data support the view that early ablation of Pou4f2-negative M1 ipRGCs are sufficient to induce the effects reported here.

Our study indicates that contrary to the prevailing view that the circadian clock develops independent of environmental input, light is necessary for setting the intrinsic period of the circadian clock. When wild-type animals were raised in constant darkness, they exhibited a lengthened circadian period as was observed when mice were enucleated at P0 and when ipRGCs were ablated at early postnatal ages (Opn4DTA/DTA) but not when mice were enucleated at P60 or when ipRGCs were only ablated during adulthood (Opn4aDTA/aDTA; Figures 2 and 3). Remarkably, when dark-reared mice were exposed to light for the first time during adulthood, their period length became indistinguishable from mice reared under a 12:12 LD cycle (Figure 3). This rescue indicates there is no critical developmental window during which the intrinsic properties of the circadian clock must be set by light. Moreover, once animals are exposed to light, the circadian period is irreversibly set. It will be of interest to investigate the molecular and cellular mechanisms underlying the lengthened period in Opn4DTA/DTA, P0 enucleates, and dark reared mice, and how light detection by ipRGCs induces a permanent change in the period length of the circadian clock.

We noticed that the phenotype exhibited by dark-reared animals was less penetrant than that of Opn4DTA/DTA and P0 enucleates. Though a majority of dark-reared animals (9 of 16) exhibited a lengthened period, the remainder did not (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). By contrast, virtually all Opn4DTA/DTA mice and P0 enucleates exhibited lengthened circadian periods. One possible interpretation of this variability could be that the process of setting the circadian period is highly sensitive to light. This is supported by the fact that merely 3 hr of light exposure is sufficient to set the circadian period (Figure 3C,D). Alternatively, some dark-reared animals could have been exposed to, say, very weak light occasionally leaking from night vision goggles or it is possible that, since ipRGCs depolarize during retinal waves, spontaneously derived activity can partially suffice for period setting. It would be interesting, in future studies, to dark rear mice that lack retinal waves and determine whether a higher proportion exhibit a lengthened period than dark-reared wild-type mice.

A surprising finding was that early genetic ablation of ipRGCs led to disrupted eye-specific segregation in the dLGN and reduced visual acuity (Figures 4 and 5; Figure 4—figure supplements 3 and 4). Even more surprisingly, our data implicated Pou4f2-negative M1 ipRGCs, which have been viewed as circadian photoreceptors, as being involved in the development of the image-forming visual system (Figure 4A,B; Figure 4—figure supplement 4A–B). In Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2Z-DTA/+ mice, ipRGC subtypes known to innervate the dLGN (i.e., M2-M5 cells) are developmentally ablated (Figure 4—figure supplement 5) and only ~200 Pou4f2-negative, circadian center projecting-M1 ipRGCs remain (Chen et al., 2011). In these animals, geniculate organization and visual acuity are normal (Figure 4A–D) (Chen et al., 2011). The further loss of these 200 Pou4f2-negative M1 ipRGCs in Opn4DTA/DTA mice (Figure 1) resulted in severe deficits in image-forming visual system (Figures 4 and 5; Figure 4—figure supplements 3 and 4). These results also suggested that the defects observed in Opn4DTA/DTA mice are not due to the loss of direct ipRGC innervation of the dLGN, nor to a generalized reduction in the total number of RGCs.

It is possible that the segregation deficits observed in Opn4DTA mice are due to the altered spontaneous retinal activity that occurs in P6 Opn4DTA/DTA mice. The salient features of retinal waves appear to be comprised of correlated, patterned activity sweeping across the retina, instructive for the formation of retinofugal circuits in the superior colliculus (Xu et al. 2015; Xu et al. 2011), whereas the individual spiking properties of ganglion cells drive eye-specific segregation within the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (Speer et al., 2014). Many previous studies have examined mice with genetically or pharmacologically disrupted spontaneous activity (Blankenship et al., 2011; Rossi et al., 2001; Stellwagen et al., 1999; Torborg et al., 2005; Xu et al. 2011), yet, Opn4DTA/DTA mice are a unique instance of a manipulation that caused an increase in non-WAB firing while WAB activity remains highly correlated between pairs of RGCs. Connexin36/45 double knockouts, which have disrupted eye-specific segregation, exhibit increased tonic firing in RGCs, although correlated firing between RGCs is also highly reduced (Blankenship et al., 2011). In ferret, disruption of patterned activity by the ablation of starburst amacrine cells altered correlated activity and a number of WAB bursting properties, yet eye-specific segregation proceeded normally (Speer et al., 2014). Our data are consistent with this study, suggesting changes in spiking activity outside of WABs is critical for eye specific sectors within the dLGN.

The contribution of disrupted eye-specific segregation to reduced visual acuity has yet to be thoroughly investigated likely because many mutations or manipulations that cause disrupted eye-specific segregation also disrupt other functions of the visual system. For example, no b-wave (nob) mice have disrupted eye-specific segregation (Demas et al., 2006) and have behavioral deficits in visual acuity, but this more likely explained by their substantially altered ERG responses (Neuillé et al., 2014). Similarly eye injections of TTX or epibatidine would disrupt acuity for reasons other than disrupting eye-specific segregation. Nonetheless, β2 KO mice, which have eye-specific segregation deficits at P8 but not at P28 (Feller, 2002), have a substantial deficit in visual acuity in the optomotor response and by recordings of visually evoked potentials (Rossi et al., 2001), but exhibit no deficit in the Y-maze (Wang et al., 2009). Thus, it is plausible that the disrupted eye-specific segregation in Opn4DTA/DTA and Opn4DTA/+ causes the reduction in visual acuity, but as discussed above, we cannot rule out the contribution of other roles for ipRGCs in retina.

Pou4f2-negative M1 ipRGCs project intra-retinal axonal collaterals that synapse onto dopaminergic amacrine cells, and these projections are present by P7 (Prigge et al., 2016) (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Thus, it is probable that ipRGCs regulate the spiking properties of RGCs through intra-retinal signaling via axonal collaterals.

In this study, we found that maturation of the circadian clock requires light input and setting of the circadian period can occur even in adulthood. We also showed that ipRGCs are necessary for refinement of eye-specific axonal segregation in the LGN as well as normal visual acuity. Our work reveals that while the networks underlying the image-forming and NIF systems have long been viewed as distinct, ipRGCs constitute a shared node in the neural circuits that mediate light-dependent maturation of the circadian clock and refinement of retinogeniculate projections.

Materials and methods

Animal models

Animals were housed and treated in accordance with NIH and IACUC guidelines, and used protocols approved by the Johns Hopkins University and Brown University Animal Care and Use Committees (Protocol numbers MO16A212, and 1010040).

Statistical analysis

All statistical tests were performed in Graphpad Prism 6, except for retinal wave analysis the detail of which are described below. Specific statistical comparisons are listed in the figure captions.

Generation of Opn4DTA mice

To generate Opn4DTA mice, we used the targeting arms and general strategy detailed in (Ecker et al., 2010; Güler et al., 2008; Hattar et al., 2002). The construct contained a 4.4 kb sequence immediately 5’ of the start codon for mouse melanopsin, followed by the coding sequence for diphtheria toxin A (DTA) subunit, an internal ribosomal entry site (IRES), the coding sequence for tauLacZ, and a self-excising neomycin resistance construct (loxP-tAce-Cre-Pol II-Neo-loxP) (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). Embryonic stem (ES) cells were first screened for homologous recombination by PCR, and then homologous recombination was confirmed with Southern blot analysis (a restriction digestion with SpeI resulted in a 10.7 kb band for the wild-type allele and a 5.3 kb band for the recombined allele) (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B). The blastocyst injection was performed by the Johns Hopkins transgenic core facility. The germline transmission was obtained by crossing chimeric males with C57Bl/6J females. The genotyping was done by PCR. The DTA allele was detected with the primers: AACTTTTCTTCGTACCACGG (forward) and ACTCATACATCGCATCTTGG (reverse), and the wild-type allele was detected with the primers: CCCCTGCTCATCATCATCTTCTG (forward) and TGACAATCAGTGCGACCTTGGC (reverse). Opn4DTA/+ and Opn4DTA/DTA mice are viable, fertile, and do not exhibit any gross abnormalities in size.

Alkaline phosphatase staining

A cre-mediated alkaline phosphatase (AP) reporter, provided by Tudor Badea in Jeremy Nathan’s lab, was expressed in conjunction with Opn4Cre (Ecker et al., 2010). Mice were deeply anesthetized with 30 ml/kg Avertin and then intracardially perfused with phosphate-buffered saline for 3 min followed by 40 ml of 4% paraformaldehyde. Brains and retinas were isolated and post-fixed for 40 min in 4% paraformaldehyde. Brains were mounted in 3% agarose and then cut into 200 µm sections on a vibrating microtome (Vibra-tome 1000 Plus). Tissue was heat-inactivated for overnight at 65°C. Alkaline phosphatase histochemistry was performed using NBT/BCIP tablets (Roche) for 2–4 hr in the dark with constant shaking. Tissue was washed three times with phosphate-buffered saline containing 0.1% Tween-20 (Sigma-Aldrich). Retinas were mounted immediately and imaged. Brains were fixed 3 hr in 4% paraformaldehyde at 4°C, then counterstained with 1:5 Fast Red nuclear stain (Vector Laboratories) in water for 7 min. The sections were then dehydrated in an ethanol series, and after at least in hour in 100% ethanol, the sections were cleared in a 2:1 mixture of benzyl benzoate:benzyl alcohol (Sigma-Aldrich), mounted in glycerol, and imaged immediately. To measure cell density, we counted the number of ipRGCs, in four representative areas of each retina, and calculated the density of ipRGCs per mm2.

X-gal staining

Mice were deeply anesthetized with 30 ml/kg Avertin followed by cervical dislocation. Eyes were isolated and fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 10 min. Retinas were dissected out and then incubated in buffer B (100 mM phosphate buffer at pH 7.4, 2 mM MgCl2, 0.01% sodium deoxycholate, 0.02% IGEPAL) then stained for 3 days in buffer B plus 5 mM potassium ferricyanide, 5 mM potassium ferrocyanide and 1 mg/ml X-gal as described in Hattar et al. (2002).

Hematoxilin and eosin staining

Animals were anesthetized with 30 ml/kg Avertin, and eyes were removed fixed in 4% PFA for 1 hr. Retinas were dissected in PBS, placed in cartridges (Tissue-Tek Biopsy Uni-Cassette), and processed and embedded in paraffin overnight. Eyecups were sectioned at 6 μm. Resulting sections were deparaffinize by immersion in two changes of xylene for 10 min each. Sections were then rehydrated in descending series of ethanol ending in water for 5 min. Sections were stained with hematoxylin for 30 s, washed with tap water for 10 dips, placed briefly in 0.1% sodium bicarbonate, and then rinsed in clean tap water for 10 dips. Sections were rinsed in 70% ethanol for five dips and stained in eosin for 20 s. Sectioned were dehydrated with an ascending series of ethanol, ending with two washes of 100% ethanol. Sections were placed in two washes of xylene (5 min each), and mounted in Permount.

Immunohistochemistry

Whole eyes were fixed for 15 min in 2% paraformaldehyde (PFA) diluted in PBS and were then dissected to remove the cornea and lens. Whole eyecups were fixed for additional 45 min in 2% PFA diluted in PBS. Further dissection was done to release the retinas from the RPE, and four nicks were made so that the retina would lay flat. Whole retinas were blocked in 500 mL of PBS containing 0.3% Triton X100% and 3% goat serum for 2 hr at RT. Either mouse anti-Brn3a (Millipore, cat #AB5945, RRID:AB_92154) (1:250), rabbit anti-γ13 (generously provided by Robert Margolskee, RRID: AB_2314434)(1:500), rabbit anti-calretinin (Swant, cat #CR 7699/3 hr, RRID: AB_10000321)(1:500), goat anti-ChAT (Millipore, cat #Ab144p, RRID: AB_262156) (1:200), or mouse anti-SMI-32 (Covance, cat #SMI-32R, RRID: AB_2315331) (1:500) was diluted in blocking solution and incubated overnight at 4°C. Retinas were washed 10 min in three changes of PBS, then placed in 1:500 Alexa Fluor secondary antibody (Invitrogen) overnight at 4°C. Retinas were washed as above and mounted flat on slides in VectaShield (Vector Labs, RRID: AB_2336789). To measure cell density was measured by counting cells in four representative areas of each retina, and density was calculated as the number of cells per mm2.

For SCN cell counts, mice were perfused with cold 4% PFA, brains were dissected out and then cryoprotected in 30% sucrose, frozen in OCT, and 25 μm serial coronal sections containing the SCN were taken. Slides were blocked in PBS containing 0.3% Triton X100 and 3% goat serum for 3 hr at RT and then incubated overnight at 4°C in 1:200 monoclonal mouse immunoglobulin G (IgG) 2b anti-human-HuC/D (Thermofisher, cat# A-21271, RRID: AB_2096358). Slides were then washed in PBS and then incubated in 1:500 goat anti-mouse IgG2b (isotype of secondary antibody is critical (Thermofisher, RRID: AB_429670)) overnight at 4°C. Slides were then washed in PBS and mounted in VectaShield (Vector Labs, RRID: AB_2336789) with DAPI. For counting of DAPI-labeled nuclei and Hu-labeled cell bodies, we used a program that was coded in Mathmatica (Wolfram) and was previously described in Martinelli et al. (2016).

PNA staining

Mice were deeply anesthetized with 30 ml/kg Avertin followed by cervical dislocation. Eyes were isolated and fixed in 4% PFA for 1 hr. Retinas were dissected out and then incubated for 2 hr in Alexa 488-conjugated lectin peanut agglutinin (PNA) (Invitrogen cat# L21409, RRID: AB_2315178) diluted 1:200 in PBS with 0.3% Triton X100% and 3% goat serum. Retinas were then washed in PBS and mounted in VectaShield (Vector Labs, RRID: AB_2336789).

In-situ hybridization

Mice were anesthetized with a ketamine/xylazine mixture before transcardially perfusing with 0.9% saline followed by 4% PFA in PBS, pH 7.4. All tissues were post fixed in 4% PFA overnight at 4°C, cryoprotected in 20% sucrose in PBS, frozen in OCT. Compound Embedding Medium (Tissue-Tek), and stored at −75°C. Serial sections (20 μm) were cut on a Hacker cryostat and thaw mounted on Superfrost Plus slides (Thermo Fisher Scientific). Five adjacent sets of sections were prepared from each postnatal age and stored at –20°C. Probes were generated as described in VanDunk et al. (2011). Slides were immersed in 4% PFA, permeabilized with proteinase K, returned to 4% PFA before being washed in 0.1 M triethanolamine-HCl with 0.25% acetic anhydride. Once blocked in hybridization buffer at 65°C slides were incubated in hybridization buffer containing 1–2 µg/ml DIG-labeled anti-sense cRNA overnight at 65°C. Slides were then washed in 2XSSC buffer at 62°C, washed in 0.2XSSC at 65°C, blocked with 10% normal horse serum (NHS) in 0.1M PBS, and incubated in alkaline phosphatase labeled anti-DIG antibody (1:2000 in 10% NHS; Roche, RRID: AB_514497) overnight. Sections were washed and color was visualized using Nitro blue tetrazolium and 5-Bromo-4-chloro-3-indolyl phosphate (Roche). Staining was stopped after visual inspection. Sections were washed, fixed in 4% PFA, and coverslipped in 90% glycerol, Vectashield Mounting Medium (Vector Laboratories, RRID: AB_2336789), or UltraCruz Mounting Media with DAPI (Santa Cruz Biotechnology).

Images were acquired using a Nikon Eclipse 90i microscope, Photometrics Coolsnap HQ2 camera with a Prior Scientific ProScan II motorized translation stage, and acquired in Volocity (PerkinElmer Life and Analytical Sciences). Images were exported as 8bit JPEG or TIFF files. All images were adjusted for clarity by filtering and/or modifying levels, as necessary, in Photoshop (Adobe Systems).

Enucleations

In order to remove the eyes, P0 mice were placed on ice for 2 min, and then a 1–2 mm incision was made across each eyelid using a sterile scalpel blade. The scalpel blade was then used to puncture the eyes and forceps were used to pull the eyes free of the orbitals. P60 mice were first anesthetized with intraperitoneal injection of 20 mL/kg of Avertin. Fingers were placed on either side of the eye causing it to bulge, a curved pair of scissors was placed between the eye and the skin, and the optic nerve was cut. Bleeding was controlled by orbital pressure. The animal was monitored over the next several days for signs of infection.

Wheel running behavior

Mice were placed in cages with a 4.5-inch running wheel, and their activity was monitored with VitalView software (Mini Mitter), and cages were changed at least every 2 weeks. All free-running periods and phase shifts were calculated with ClockLab (Actimetrics).

6-month-old wild-type (n = 18 mice), Opn4LacZ/LacZ (n = 17), Opn4DTA/+ (n = 8), Opn4DTA/LacZ (n = 7), Opn4DTA/DTA (n = 7), and Opn4aDTA/aDTA (n = 18) mice were placed in 12:12 LD for 10 days followed by constant darkness for 14 days. For phase-shifting experiments, a subset of wild-type, Opn4LacZ/LacZ, Opn4aDTA/aDTA were used. Wild-type (n = 7), Opn4LacZ/LacZ (n = 8), Opn4DTA/+ (n = 8), Opn4DTA/LacZ (n = 7), Opn4DTA/DTA (n = 7), and Opn4aDTA/aDTA (n = 8) mice were exposed to a light pulse (500 lx; CT16) for 15 min, after being in constant dark for 14 days.

P0 and P60 ennucleated animals were placed in 12:12 LD for 24 days followed by constant darkness for 14 days.

Dark-reared animals and control mice (raised in 12:12 LD) were placed in constant darkness without any exposure to light. Wheel running behavior was recorded in constant darkness for 1–3 months. Mice were then either given a 3-hr light pulse (LD reared: n = 17; DD reared: n = 12) or placed in 12:12 LD for 14 days and then presented with a 6-hr shift and allowed to re-entrain for 14 days (LD reared: n = 13; DD reared: n = 16). Mice were then returned to constant darkness for 1 month.

Cholera toxin injections

WT (n = 12), Opn4LacZ/LacZ (n = 6), Opn4aDTA/aDTA (n = 6), Opn4DTA/+ (n = 8), Opn4DTA/DTA (n = 10), and Opn4Cre/+ (n = 8), and Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2Z-DTA/+ (n = 6) mice used for examination of adult central projections were raised in a standard 12:12 LD cycle. 3-6-month-old mice were anaesthetized with 20 mL/kg of Avertin. Eyes were injected intravitreally using a glass pipet with approximately 2 μl of cholera toxin B subunit conjugated with Alexa Fluor 488 (Thermofisher, cat# C34775) or Alexa Fluor 594 (Thermofisher, cat# C22842) using a Harvard Apparatus HL-190 picospritzer. CTB-488 was used at a concentration of 6.25 µg/µL and CTB-594 was used at 5 µg/µL for all injections. Three days after injection, mice were perfused with 4% PFA, and brains were isolated, cryoprotected in 30% sucrose, frozen in OCT, and 40 µm sections were taken using a cryostat. Sections were dried overnight, mounted in VectaShield, and imaged on a Zeiss Imager M1 upright epifluorescence microscope (AxioVision). Retinas were also dissected, mounted in vectashield, and examined for good injection quality.

Mice used to examine LGN projections at P8 were born and raised in 12:12-LD cycle. At P7, mice were anaesthetized on ice, and ocular injections of contrasting fluorescent anterograde tracers (cholera toxin subunit B (CTB)-Alexa-488 and CTB-Alexa-594) were made. At P8, mice were perfused with 4% PFA, and brains were isolated, cryoprotected in 30% sucrose, frozen in OCT, and 40 µm sections were taken using a cryostat. Sections were dried overnight, mounted in VectaShield (Vector Laboratories, RRID: AB_2336789), and imaged on a Zeiss Imager M1 upright epifluorescence microscope (AxioVision). Retinas were also dissected, mounted in vectashield, and examined for good injection quality. Only animals with complete retinal labeling were assessed further. For assessment of SCN innervation at P7, mice were injected with CTB-Alexa-488 at P4. At P7, mice were perfused with 4% PFA, and brains were isolated, cryoprotected in 30% sucrose, frozen in OCT, and 40 µm sections were taken using a cryostat. Sections were dried overnight, mounted in VectaShield (Vector Laboratories, RRID: AB_2336789), and imaged on Zeiss LSM 700 Confocal.

Quantification of eye-specific segregation

Quantifications were performed with the analyzer blinded to the genotypes being measured.

Percent overlap between contralateral and ipsilateral projections

40 µm serial sections were taken spanning the dLGN. Images were taken of every section containing the dLGN. In ImageJ, for each image, a background threshold was determined by measuring the maximum intensity of pixels in regions of the section that contained no retinal innervation. Each image was binerized at this intensity threshold. Pixels from contralateral fibers were psedo-colored red and pixels from ipsilateral fibers were psedo-colored green. The whole dLGN was selected. Percent overlap represents ((number of yellow pixels/total number of pixels)*100)), percent ipsilateral represents ((number of green pixels/total number of pixels)*100)), and percent contralateral represents ((number of red pixels/total number of pixels)*100))(Datwani et al., 2009; Demas et al., 2006). Averages presented in bar graphs represent all sections from the dLGN pooled, and the distribution of segregation throughout the dLGN is shown in Figure 4—figure supplement 4F. Representative images were always taken from the region of the dLGN indicated by the blue arrow in Figure 4—figure supplement 4F.

Quantification of eye-specific segregation using the variance of R-value distribution (additional quantification method for eye specific segregation)

A complete series of 40 µm sections spanning the dLGN were digitally imaged with settings (gain, exposure time and gamma level) that were identical for all photomicrographs and for both tracers for all mice. Sections from each brain were aligned to ensure all images analyzed were within the same region of the dLGN. We were blind to genotype until data were processed and analyzed, as described by (Renna et al., 2011). For this quantification method, 15 sections from the middle of the LGN were analyzed per adult mouse, per dLGN hemisphere. R-values were calculated for all pixels within each dLGN. This was done by selecting a region of interest that was maximally innervated by the contralateral eye and a region of interest that was maximally innervated by the ipsilateral eye. The R-distribution for maximally ipsilateral segregated pixels and maximally contralateral pixels were calculated and averaged across all 15 dLGN sections. Pixels with an R-value less than 99.9% of the maximally contra segregated pixels and more than 99.9% of all maximally ipsilateral segregated pixels were termed unsegregated pixels.

Similar to (Renna et al., 2011), we calculated the variance for each dLGN R-distribution. Larger variances are indicative of a wider range of values (more ipsilateral dominant pixels and contralateral dominate pixels), and thus, fewer pixels with more balanced left and right eye input.

Reconstruction of retinal innervation of the SC

Similar to (Fernandez et al., 2012), Coronal sections were used for the SC reconstruction using Matlab (Math Work). For each section, the retino-recipient SC was outlined, and the total retinotopic area was calculated. Images were converted to 8-bits of grey scale and the optic density of CTB-staining was calculated. The total length was measured and divided in bins (4 µm) from the medial to lateral region. The CTB density was obtained by dividing the total pixel area by CTB+ pixels. Finally, a colorimetric thermal representation was applied (from 0% = blue to 100% = red). All sections containing the SC were used for a final reconstruction of the retinal projection to the SC.

Retinal wave recordings

Mice were shipped overnight at postnatal day 5 (Opn4DTA/DTA from Johns Hopkins University and wild-type control mice from Jackson Laboratories). All procedures involving the use of animals were in accordance with the National Institutes of Health and approved by the Brown University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Upon arrival at Brown University, the P6 mice were sacrificed via a lethal intraperitoneal injection of Beuthanasia. As previously described, retinas were extracted and placed ganglion-cell side down on the array under dim red light (Renna et al., 2011). The retinas were continuously superfused with oxygenated Ames solution maintained at 36–37°C, and were kept in constant darkness. Recordings were made after at least 30 min of dark adaptation.

The raw analog data were digitized using MC Rack software (Multi Channel Systems) before being filtered. Further processing was done in OfflineSorter (Plexon Inc.). In order to determine the number of neurons being sampled by each electrode, we filtered the raw digitized data using a 125 Hz high-pass filter. A threshold of 5 standard deviations from the baseline voltage was used as the criterion for a spike. The candidate spikes were then sorted using OfflineSorter (Plexon Inc.) for the first two principal components of the waveforms, using a standard T-distribution E-M algorithm (Shoham et al., 2003). Spikes appearing within a 1 ms window on 70% of channels were assumed to be artifacts (caused by bubbles or other disturbances) and were discarded.

Spike time tiling coefficients

To quantify the amount of correlation between two spike trains, we used the spike time tiling coefficient (STTC) (Cutts and Eglen, 2014). We calculated the STTC for every pair of units in a retina, using a correlation time interval of Δt = 50 ms, and took the median over the interelectrode distance to get a STTC vs Interelectrode Distance curve. We also identified units from the ground channel, and units from electrodes that had poor contact with the retina (had a mean firing rate lower than 0.25 Hz over the entire 10 min recording) for later removal.

Burst identification

Spike timestamps were exported to NeuroExplorer v4.133 (Nex Technologies) for burst analysis. We employed NeuroExplorer’s Surprise algorithm to detect bursts of spiking, using a minimum surprise value of 4. This value was based on (Kirkby et al., 2013), where spike trains were considered bursting if a Poisson probability dropped below 10−4, and surprise-values are the negative logarithm of that probability.

Calculation of firing properties

All spike timestamps and burst start- and end-timestamps from all sorted units were exported to text from NeuroExplorer. Firing properties were calculated from these data files using custom Visual Basic for Applications macros. Our calculations proceeded as follows, for each unit. First, units from invalid channels (as defined above) were excluded from further analysis. Second, bursts shorter than 0.75 s or longer than 15 s were also excluded, although their constituent spikes were kept for later firing rate calculations. Third, bursts that started less than half of the maximum burst duration (7.5 s) after the recording started or that ended less than that duration before the recording ended were excluded from further analysis. This ensured that interrupted WABs—WABs that occurred while the recording was being started or stopped—were not analyzed. The constituent spike timestamps of these bursts were also kept for later firing rate calculations.

WABs were identified as follows. All bursts were divided into 10 bins of equal duration. For a given burst, if the start of at least one of its bins was within a time interval Δt of the start of any bin of a different burst, then those two bursts were said to be ‘associated’. The interval Δt was equal to half of the sum of the two bursts’ bin-durations. If a burst was associated with a burst on at least two neighboring channels, then it was classified as a WAB, where a channel’s ‘neighboring channels’ were any of the eight channels physically surrounding it on the MEA grid. Bursts could not be associated with bursts on the same unit or the same channel, and a burst could not be associated with a burst from multiple units on a single neighboring channel. Finally, spiking property values for an entire retina were taken as the average over all of its units’ values.

Statistics

For spiking properties, we employed two-tailed Student’s T-tests assuming unequal variance for each property, with a significance level of α = 0.05. We used the Holm-Bonferroni method to control the familywise error rate of these multiple comparisons. To compare the dependency of STTC on distance between these populations, we employed a second round of Student’s T-tests, comparing the STTC-values at all interelectrode distances less than or equal to 800 μm.

Virtual optomotor system

Using the virtual optokinetic system (Douglas et al., 2005), we placed individual mice in a box created using four computer screens, which display sine wave gratings that move to create virtual cylinder. If mice can see the gratings, they track the moving bars by turning their head. To obtain an estimate of visual acuity using this setup, we increased the spatial frequency (SF) of the gratings until the mice no longer tracked the movement of the gratings.

The visual water task

In the visual water task (Prusky et al., 2000), mice are place in a trapezoidal shaped maze that contains water and has a divider down the middle to create two arms. At the end of one arm, a gray panel in displayed and in the other arm, sine wave grating is displayed. Mice were trained, using gratings with a low SF, to associate the gratings with a hidden platform that allows them to escape from the water. The location of the grey panel and gratings with the hidden platform were moved between the arms in a pseudorandom pattern that mice cannot memorize. Any entrances into the arm containing the grey panel were recorded as incorrect, after the mice could reliably swim to the platform with greater that 90% accuracy, the SF of the gratings was increased. An animal’s threshold was considered to have been reached once they failed to have better than 70% accuracy out of 10 trials. This threshold was confirmed by retesting the previous SF, which is 0.02cycles/degree lower, and then retesting the spatial frequency that the mice initially failed. If the mouse’s behavior repeated where they could see the lower SF, but not one increment higher, then the visual acuity of that mouse was recorded as the last spatial frequency where they had better than 70% accuracy.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Marnie Halpern, Carla Shatz, Rejji Kuruvilla, Tiffany Schmidt, and Alan Rupp for their critical reading and advice on the manuscript. We would also like to thank the mouse tri-lab for suggestions and advice. Funding was provided by The Johns Hopkins University-Dean's office funds, The David and Lucile Packard Foundation Fellowship, The Alfred P. Sloan Fellowship, National Institutes of Health Grants R01-GM076430 and R01-EY019053 (to SH); R01-EY017137 (to DMB); DC007395 (to HZ); R01-GM104991 (to EH); R01-HL089742 (to PAG); F32-EY20108 and R15EY026255 (to JMR); and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research MOP-77570 (to MC). In addition, this research was supported (in part) by the Intramural Research Program of the NIMH.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institute of General Medical Sciences GM076430 to Samer Hattar.

  • National Eye Institute R01-EY019053 to Samer Hattar.

  • David and Lucile Packard Foundation to Samer Hattar.

  • Alfred P. Sloan Foundation to Samer Hattar.

  • Johns Hopkins University to Samer Hattar.

  • National Eye Institute R01-EY017137 to David M Berson.

  • National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders DC007395 to Haiqing Zhao.

  • National Institute of General Medical Sciences R01-GM104991 to Erik D Herzog.

  • National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute R01-HL089742 to Paul A Gray.

  • National Eye Institute F32-EY20108 to Jordan M Renna.

  • National Eye Institute R15EY026255 to Jordan M Renna.

  • Canadian Institutes of Health Research MOP-77570 to Michel Cayouette.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

KSC, Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

JMR, Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

DSM, Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology.

DCF, Conceptualization, Resources, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Diego was the one who did crucial experiments now in Figure 1, to answer the reviewers' concerns about the strength of the DTA versus the aDTA in killing ipRGCs during development.

WTK, Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation.

MBT, Conceptualization, Investigation.

JLE, Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology.

GSL, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation.

CV, Resources, Data curation, Investigation, Visualization.

DCV, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology.

AT, Resources, Formal analysis, Investigation.

SW, Data curation, Investigation.

PAG, Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Investigation.

MC, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation.

EDH, Formal analysis, Supervision, Investigation, Writing—original draft.

HZ, Conceptualization, Resources, Formal analysis, Supervision, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—original draft.

DMB, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

SH, Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Animals were housed and treated in accordance with NIH and IACUC guidelines, and used protocols approved by the Johns Hopkins University and Brown University Animal Care and Use Committees (Protocol numbers MO16A212 and 1010040).

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Description of all mouse lines.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22861.017

elife-22861-supp1.docx (138.7KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.22861.017
Supplementary file 2. Values and statistics for retinal wave recordings.

Properties of spontaneous retinal activity in P6 WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice. Recordings were done in darkness. * represent statistically significant differences after Student's T-tests with a Holm-Bonferroni correction, α = 0.05, m = 22. Significance is determined when the p-value is less than or equal to the Holm-Bonferroni corrected p-value.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22861.018

elife-22861-supp2.docx (202.9KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.22861.018

References

  1. Ackman JB, Burbridge TJ, Crair MC. Retinal waves coordinate patterned activity throughout the developing visual system. Nature. 2012;490:219–225. doi: 10.1038/nature11529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arroyo DA, Feller MB. Spatiotemporal features of retinal waves instruct the wiring of the Visual Circuitry. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2016;10:54. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2016.00054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arroyo DA, Kirkby LA, Feller MB. Retinal waves modulate an Intraretinal Circuit of Intrinsically Photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:6892–6905. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0572-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baver SB, Pickard GE, Sollars PJ, Pickard GE. Two types of melanopsin retinal ganglion cell differentially innervate the hypothalamic suprachiasmatic nucleus and the olivary pretectal nucleus. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;27:1763–1770. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06149.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ben-Moshe Z, Foulkes NS, Gothilf Y. Functional development of the circadian clock in the zebrafish pineal gland. BioMed Research International. 2014;2014:235781–8. doi: 10.1155/2014/235781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Berson DM, Dunn FA, Takao M. Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the circadian clock. Science. 2002;295:1070–1073. doi: 10.1126/science.1067262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Berson DM. Strange vision: ganglion cells as circadian photoreceptors. Trends in Neurosciences. 2003;26:314–320. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00130-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blankenship AG, Hamby AM, Firl A, Vyas S, Maxeiner S, Willecke K, Feller MB. The role of neuronal connexins 36 and 45 in shaping spontaneous firing patterns in the developing retina. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:9998–10008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5640-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brown TM, Gias C, Hatori M, Keding SR, Semo M, Coffey PJ, Gigg J, Piggins HD, Panda S, Lucas RJ. Melanopsin contributions to irradiance coding in the thalamo-cortical visual system. PLoS Biology. 2010;8:e1000558. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Campuzano A, Cambras T, Vilaplana J, Canal MM, Carulla M, Díez-Noguera A. Period length of the light-dark cycle influences the growth rate and food intake in mice. Physiology & Behavior. 1999;67:791–797. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00196-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chandrasekaran AR, Plas DT, Gonzalez E, Crair MC. Evidence for an instructive role of retinal activity in retinotopic map refinement in the superior colliculus of the mouse. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:6929–6938. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1470-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chen SK, Badea TC, Hattar S. Photoentrainment and pupillary light reflex are mediated by distinct populations of ipRGCs. Nature. 2011;476:92–95. doi: 10.1038/nature10206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chen SK, Chew KS, McNeill DS, Keeley PW, Ecker JL, Mao BQ, Pahlberg J, Kim B, Lee SC, Fox MA, Guido W, Wong KY, Sampath AP, Reese BE, Kuruvilla R, Hattar S. Apoptosis regulates ipRGC spacing necessary for rods and cones to drive circadian photoentrainment. Neuron. 2013;77:503–515. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Coombs JL, Van Der List D, Chalupa LM. Morphological properties of mouse retinal ganglion cells during postnatal development. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2007;503:803–814. doi: 10.1002/cne.21429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cutts CS, Eglen SJ. Detecting pairwise correlations in spike trains: an objective comparison of methods and application to the study of retinal waves. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:14288–14303. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2767-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Czeisler CA, Shanahan TL, Klerman EB, Martens H, Brotman DJ, Emens JS, Klein T, Rizzo JF. Suppression of melatonin secretion in some blind patients by exposure to bright light. New England Journal of Medicine. 1995;332:6–11. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199501053320102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Datwani A, McConnell MJ, Kanold PO, Micheva KD, Busse B, Shamloo M, Smith SJ, Shatz CJ. Classical MHCI molecules regulate retinogeniculate refinement and limit ocular dominance plasticity. Neuron. 2009;64:463–470. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Davis FC, Menaker M. Development of the mouse circadian pacemaker: independence from Environmental cycles. Journal of Comparative Physiology ? A. 1981;143:527–539. doi: 10.1007/BF00609919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Davis FC, Gorski RA. Development of hamster circadian rhythms: prenatal entrainment of the pacemaker. Journal of Biological Rhythms. 1985;1:77–89. doi: 10.1177/074873048600100108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Demas J, Sagdullaev BT, Green E, Jaubert-Miazza L, McCall MA, Gregg RG, Wong RO, Guido W. Failure to maintain eye-specific segregation in Nob, a mutant with abnormally patterned retinal activity. Neuron. 2006;50:247–259. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dkhissi-Benyahya O, Coutanson C, Knoblauch K, Lahouaoui H, Leviel V, Rey C, Bennis M, Cooper HM. The absence of melanopsin alters retinal clock function and dopamine regulation by light. Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences. 2013;70:3435–3447. doi: 10.1007/s00018-013-1338-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Douglas RM, Alam NM, Silver BD, McGill TJ, Tschetter WW, Prusky GT. Independent visual threshold measurements in the two eyes of freely moving rats and mice using a virtual-reality optokinetic system. Visual Neuroscience. 2005;22:677–684. doi: 10.1017/S0952523805225166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dreher B, Robinson SR. Neuroanatomy of the Visual Pathways and Their Development. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  24. Ecker JL, Dumitrescu ON, Wong KY, Alam NM, Chen SK, LeGates T, Renna JM, Prusky GT, Berson DM, Hattar S. Melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion-cell photoreceptors: cellular diversity and role in pattern vision. Neuron. 2010;67:49–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Estevez ME, Fogerson PM, Ilardi MC, Borghuis BG, Chan E, Weng S, Auferkorte ON, Demb JB, Berson DM. Form and function of the M4 cell, an intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cell type contributing to geniculocortical vision. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:13608–13620. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1422-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Feller MB, Wellis DP, Stellwagen D, Werblin FS, Shatz CJ. Requirement for cholinergic synaptic transmission in the propagation of spontaneous retinal waves. Science. 1996;272:1182–1187. doi: 10.1126/science.272.5265.1182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Feller MB. The role of nAChR-mediated spontaneous retinal activity in visual system development. Journal of Neurobiology. 2002;53:556–567. doi: 10.1002/neu.10140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Feller MB. Retinal waves are likely to instruct the formation of eye-specific retinogeniculate projections. Neural Development. 2009;4:24. doi: 10.1186/1749-8104-4-24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fernandez DC, Pasquini LA, Dorfman D, Aldana Marcos HJ, Rosenstein RE. Ischemic conditioning protects from axoglial alterations of the optic pathway induced by experimental diabetes in rats. PLoS One. 2012;7:e51966. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Firth SI, Wang CT, Feller MB. Retinal waves: mechanisms and function in visual system development. Cell Calcium. 2005;37:425–432. doi: 10.1016/j.ceca.2005.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Foster RG, Provencio I, Hudson D, Fiske S, De Grip W, Menaker M. Circadian photoreception in the retinally degenerate mouse (rd/rd) Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 1991;169:39–50. doi: 10.1007/BF00198171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Freedman MS, Lucas RJ, Soni B, von Schantz M, Muñoz M, David-Gray Z, Foster R. Regulation of mammalian circadian behavior by non-rod, non-cone, ocular photoreceptors. Science. 1999;284:502–504. doi: 10.1126/science.284.5413.502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Güler AD, Ecker JL, Lall GS, Haq S, Altimus CM, Liao HW, Barnard AR, Cahill H, Badea TC, Zhao H, Hankins MW, Berson DM, Lucas RJ, Yau KW, Hattar S. Melanopsin cells are the principal conduits for rod-cone input to non-image-forming vision. Nature. 2008;453:102–105. doi: 10.1038/nature06829. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Harmer SL, Panda S, Kay SA. Molecular bases of circadian rhythms. Annual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology. 2001;17:215–253. doi: 10.1146/annurev.cellbio.17.1.215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hattar S, Liao HW, Takao M, Berson DM, Yau KW. Melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells: architecture, projections, and intrinsic photosensitivity. Science. 2002;295:1065–1070. doi: 10.1126/science.1069609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hattar S, Lucas RJ, Mrosovsky N, Thompson S, Douglas RH, Hankins MW, Lem J, Biel M, Hofmann F, Foster RG, Yau KW. Melanopsin and rod-cone photoreceptive systems account for all Major accessory visual functions in mice. Nature. 2003;424:75–81. doi: 10.1038/nature01761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hattar S, Kumar M, Park A, Tong P, Tung J, Yau KW, Berson DM. Central projections of melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion cells in the mouse. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2006;497:326–349. doi: 10.1002/cne.20970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hubel DH, Wiesel TN, LeVay S. Plasticity of ocular dominance columns in monkey striate cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1977;278:377–409. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1977.0050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Joo HR, Peterson BB, Dacey DM, Hattar S, Chen SK. Recurrent axon collaterals of intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Visual Neuroscience. 2013;30:175–182. doi: 10.1017/S0952523813000199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jud C, Albrecht U. Circadian rhythms in murine pups develop in absence of a functional maternal circadian clock. Journal of Biological Rhythms. 2006;21:149–154. doi: 10.1177/0748730406286264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kazimi N, Cahill GM. Development of a circadian melatonin rhythm in embryonic zebrafish. Developmental Brain Research. 1999;117:47–52. doi: 10.1016/S0165-3806(99)00096-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kirkby LA, Sack GS, Firl A, Feller MB. A role for correlated spontaneous activity in the assembly of neural circuits. Neuron. 2013;80:1129–1144. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Laemle LK, Ottenweller JE. Daily patterns of running wheel activity in male anophthalmic mice. Physiology & Behavior. 1998;64:165–171. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00045-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lucas RJ, Freedman MS, Muñoz M, Garcia-Fernández JM, Foster RG. Regulation of the mammalian pineal by non-rod, non-cone, ocular photoreceptors. Science. 1999;284:505–507. doi: 10.1126/science.284.5413.505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lucas RJ, Douglas RH, Foster RG. Characterization of an ocular photopigment capable of driving pupillary constriction in mice. Nature Neuroscience. 2001;4:621–626. doi: 10.1038/88443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lucas RJ, Hattar S, Takao M, Berson DM, Foster RG, Yau KW. Diminished pupillary light reflex at high irradiances in melanopsin-knockout mice. Science. 2003;299:245–247. doi: 10.1126/science.1077293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Martinelli DC, Chew KS, Rohlmann A, Lum MY, Ressl S, Hattar S, Brunger AT, Missler M, Südhof TC. Expression of C1ql3 in Discrete neuronal Populations Controls Efferent Synapse Numbers and Diverse Behaviors. Neuron. 2016;91:1034–1051. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. McLaughlin T, Torborg CL, Feller MB, O'Leary DD. Retinotopic map refinement requires spontaneous retinal waves during a brief critical period of development. Neuron. 2003;40:1147–1160. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00790-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. McNeill DS, Sheely CJ, Ecker JL, Badea TC, Morhardt D, Guido W, Hattar S. Development of melanopsin-based irradiance detecting circuitry. Neural Development. 2011;6:8. doi: 10.1186/1749-8104-6-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Meister M, Wong RO, Baylor DA, Shatz CJ. Synchronous bursts of action potentials in ganglion cells of the developing mammalian retina. Science. 1991;252:939–943. doi: 10.1126/science.2035024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Menaker M, Takahashi JS, Eskin A. The physiology of circadian pacemakers. Annual Review of Physiology. 1978;40:501–526. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ph.40.030178.002441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Moore RY. Circadian rhythms: basic neurobiology and clinical applications. Annual Review of Medicine. 1997;48:253–266. doi: 10.1146/annurev.med.48.1.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Mrosovsky N, Hattar S. Impaired masking responses to light in melanopsin-knockout mice. Chronobiology International. 2003;20:989–999. doi: 10.1081/CBI-120026043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Mrsic-Flogel TD, Hofer SB, Creutzfeldt C, Cloëz-Tayarani I, Changeux JP, Bonhoeffer T, Hübener M. Altered map of visual space in the superior colliculus of mice lacking early retinal waves. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:6921–6928. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1555-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Mu X, Fu X, Sun H, Liang S, Maeda H, Frishman LJ, Klein WH. Ganglion cells are required for normal progenitor- cell proliferation but not cell-fate determination or patterning in the developing mouse retina. Current Biology. 2005;15:525–530. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Muir-Robinson G, Hwang BJ, Feller MB. Retinogeniculate axons undergo eye-specific segregation in the absence of eye-specific layers. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:5259–5264. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-13-05259.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Neuillé M, El Shamieh S, Orhan E, Michiels C, Antonio A, Lancelot ME, Condroyer C, Bujakowska K, Poch O, Sahel JA, Audo I, Zeitz C. Lrit3 deficient mouse (nob6): a novel model of complete congenital stationary night blindness (cCSNB) PLoS One. 2014;9:e90342. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0090342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Oster SF, Sretavan DW. Connecting the eye to the brain: the molecular basis of ganglion cell axon guidance. British Journal of Ophthalmology. 2003;87:639–645. doi: 10.1136/bjo.87.5.639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Osterhout JA, El-Danaf RN, Nguyen PL, Huberman AD. Birthdate and outgrowth timing predict cellular mechanisms of axon target matching in the developing visual pathway. Cell Reports. 2014;8:1006–1017. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2014.06.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Panda S, Sato TK, Castrucci AM, Rollag MD, DeGrip WJ, Hogenesch JB, Provencio I, Kay SA. Melanopsin (Opn4) requirement for normal light-induced circadian phase shifting. Science. 2002;298:2213–2216. doi: 10.1126/science.1076848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Panda S, Provencio I, Tu DC, Pires SS, Rollag MD, Castrucci AM, Pletcher MT, Sato TK, Wiltshire T, Andahazy M, Kay SA, Van Gelder RN, Hogenesch JB. Melanopsin is required for non-image-forming photic responses in blind mice. Science. 2003;301:525–527. doi: 10.1126/science.1086179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Pittendrigh CS. On temperature independence in the clock system controlling emergence time in Drosophila. PNAS. 1954;40:1018–1029. doi: 10.1073/pnas.40.10.1018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Prigge CL, Yeh PT, Liou NF, Lee CC, You SF, Liu LL, McNeill DS, Chew KS, Hattar S, Chen SK, Zhang DQ. M1 ipRGCs influence visual function through Retrograde signaling in the Retina. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:7184–7197. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3500-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Provencio I, Jiang G, De Grip WJ, Hayes WP, Rollag MD. Melanopsin: an opsin in Melanophores, brain, and eye. PNAS. 1998;95:340–345. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.1.340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Prusky GT, West PW, Douglas RM. Behavioral assessment of visual acuity in mice and rats. Vision Research. 2000;40:2201–2209. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00081-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Ramoa AS, Campbell G, Shatz CJ. Retinal ganglion beta cells project transiently to the superior colliculus during development. PNAS. 1989;86:2061–2065. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.6.2061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Rao S, Chun C, Fan J, Kofron JM, Yang MB, Hegde RS, Ferrara N, Copenhagen DR, Lang RA. A direct and melanopsin-dependent fetal light response regulates mouse eye development. Nature. 2013;494:243–246. doi: 10.1038/nature11823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Renna JM, Weng S, Berson DM. Light acts through melanopsin to alter retinal waves and segregation of retinogeniculate afferents. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:827–829. doi: 10.1038/nn.2845. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Richter CP. Inborn nature of the rat's 24-hour clock. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1971;75:1–4. doi: 10.1037/h0030681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Rossi FM, Pizzorusso T, Porciatti V, Marubio LM, Maffei L, Changeux JP. Requirement of the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor beta 2 subunit for the anatomical and functional development of the visual system. PNAS. 2001;98:6453–6458. doi: 10.1073/pnas.101120998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Ruby NF, Brennan TJ, Xie X, Cao V, Franken P, Heller HC, O'Hara BF. Role of melanopsin in circadian responses to light. Science. 2002;298:2211–2213. doi: 10.1126/science.1076701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Schmidt TM, Taniguchi K, Kofuji P. Intrinsic and extrinsic light responses in melanopsin-expressing ganglion cells during mouse development. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2008;100:371–384. doi: 10.1152/jn.00062.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Schmidt TM, Kofuji P. Functional and morphological differences among intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:476–482. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4117-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Schmidt TM, Chen SK, Hattar S. Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells: many subtypes, diverse functions. Trends in Neurosciences. 2011;34:572–580. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Schmidt TM, Alam NM, Chen S, Kofuji P, Li W, Prusky GT, Hattar S. A role for melanopsin in alpha retinal ganglion cells and contrast detection. Neuron. 2014;82:781–788. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.03.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Sexton TJ, Bleckert A, Turner MH, Van Gelder RN. Type I intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells of early post-natal development correspond to the M4 subtype. Neural Development. 2015;10:17. doi: 10.1186/s13064-015-0042-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Shatz CJ, Stryker MP. Prenatal tetrodotoxin infusion blocks segregation of retinogeniculate afferents. Science. 1988;242:87–89. doi: 10.1126/science.3175636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Shatz CJ. Competitive interactions between retinal ganglion cells during prenatal development. Journal of Neurobiology. 1990;21:197–211. doi: 10.1002/neu.480210113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Shoham S, Fellows MR, Normann RA. Robust, automatic spike sorting using mixtures of multivariate t-distributions. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2003;127:111–122. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(03)00120-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Speer CM, Sun C, Liets LC, Stafford BK, Chapman B, Cheng HJ. Eye-specific retinogeniculate segregation proceeds normally following disruption of patterned spontaneous retinal activity. Neural Development. 2014;9:25. doi: 10.1186/1749-8104-9-25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Stellwagen D, Shatz CJ, Feller MB. Dynamics of retinal waves are controlled by cyclic AMP. Neuron. 1999;24:673–685. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81121-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Stellwagen D, Shatz CJ. An instructive role for retinal waves in the development of retinogeniculate connectivity. Neuron. 2002;33:357–367. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00577-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Torborg CL, Hansen KA, Feller MB. High frequency, synchronized bursting drives eye-specific segregation of retinogeniculate projections. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:72–78. doi: 10.1038/nn1376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Vallone D, Lahiri K, Dickmeis T, Foulkes NS. Start the clock! circadian rhythms and development. Developmental Dynamics. 2007;236:142–155. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.20998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. VanDunk C, Hunter LA, Gray PA. Development, maturation, and necessity of transcription factors in the mouse suprachiasmatic nucleus. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:6457–6467. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5385-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Wang L, Rangarajan KV, Lawhn-Heath CA, Sarnaik R, Wang BS, Liu X, Cang J. Direction-specific disruption of subcortical visual behavior and receptive fields in mice lacking the beta2 subunit of nicotinic acetylcholine receptor. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:12909–12918. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2128-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Wee R, Castrucci AM, Provencio I, Gan L, Van Gelder RN. Loss of photic entrainment and altered free-running circadian rhythms in math5-/- mice. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:10427–10433. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-23-10427.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Wong RO. Retinal waves and visual system development. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1999;22:29–47. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Xu HP, Furman M, Mineur YS, Chen H, King SL, Zenisek D, Zhou ZJ, Butts DA, Tian N, Picciotto MR, Crair MC. An instructive role for patterned spontaneous retinal activity in mouse visual map development. Neuron. 2011;70:1115–1127. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.04.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Xu HP, Burbridge TJ, Chen MG, Ge X, Zhang Y, Zhou ZJ, Crair MC. Spatial pattern of spontaneous retinal waves instructs retinotopic map refinement more than activity frequency. Developmental Neurobiology. 2015;75:621–640. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Yamazaki S, Alones V, Menaker M. Interaction of the retina with suprachiasmatic pacemakers in the control of circadian behavior. Journal of Biological Rhythms. 2002;17:315–329. doi: 10.1177/074873040201700405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Young MJ, Lund RD. The anatomical substrates subserving the pupillary light reflex in rats: origin of the consensual pupillary response. Neuroscience. 1994;62:481–496. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)90381-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Zhang DQ, Wong KY, Sollars PJ, Berson DM, Pickard GE, McMahon DG. Intraretinal signaling by ganglion cell photoreceptors to dopaminergic amacrine neurons. PNAS. 2008;105:14181–14186. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803893105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Zhang J, Ackman JB, Xu HP, Crair MC. Visual map development depends on the temporal pattern of binocular activity in mice. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;15:298–307. doi: 10.1038/nn.3007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Zhang DQ, Belenky MA, Sollars PJ, Pickard GE, McMahon DG. Melanopsin mediates retrograde visual signaling in the retina. PLoS One. 2012;7:e42647. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Ziv L, Levkovitz S, Toyama R, Falcon J, Gothilf Y. Functional development of the zebrafish pineal gland: light-induced expression of period2 is required for onset of the circadian clock. Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 2005;17:314–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2005.01315.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2017 Jun 15;6:e22861. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22861.019

Decision letter

Editor: Joseph S Takahashi1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "200 ipRGCs can regulate both maturation of the circadian clock and segregation of retinogeniculate projections in mice" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and a Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

This manuscript presents evidence that a small number of intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs) regulate development of the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) and eye specific segregation of projections to the geniculate in early development with consequences for the period of circadian clock and visual acuity. The primary evidence for this conclusion comes from comparisons between a number of transgenic mice using different versions of the diphtheria toxin cytotoxic lesion of ipRGCs. They present a new model – opn4DTA – and associated evidence that it has a more complete and earlier onset deletion of ipRGCs than is the case for opn4aDTA animals. In other respects, the phenotype of these two animals as hemizygotes is broadly similar but when homozygous for the toxin, DTA mice have longer circadian period and disrupted segregation of LGN projections compared to aDTA mice. The authors attribute these findings to an early loss of all ipRGCs in the DTA mice. Another model used (Opn4Cre Brn3bzDTA) allows 200 M1 ipRGCs targeting the SCN to survive. This animal has normal period and a normal thalamic projection. The authors conclude that these few ipRGCs can control development. They show that dark rearing (and early enucleation) also lengthens circadian period but does not phenocopy effects of ipRGC loss on the thalamic projection.

The differences in phenotype between the mouse lines appear robust and the authors are to be complimented on the care taken in their documentation and description. The demonstration that circadian period is impacted by dark rearing and that this effect is so readily reversed is very interesting in itself.

Essential revisions:

1) Enthusiasm for this work is constrained by the limited description of the phenotype of the DTA/DTA mouse. The manuscript is built around a comparison of this mouse with other transgenic lines yet we do not know how complete the lesion of ipRGCs is in this genotype, the developmental time course of their loss, or how specific the lesion is to ipRGCs. Without answers to these questions it is hard to draw definitive conclusions. The authors attribute differences reported between DTA/DTA and aDTA/aDTA or DTA mice to a more complete loss of ipRGCs. This may be a reasonable supposition but we do not know for sure either that there is a more complete loss early in development or that the DTA/DTA does not have off target effects on other important cell types. As the only marker of ipRGCs is melanopsin, there is no way of identifying ipRGCs in the DTA/DTA mice. The only way of measuring ipRGC loss in this genotype is by looking at projections to the SCN. These are lost at 6 months showing that lesion of this projection in adults is more complete, but we do not know whether that is true for other ipRGC types (which express less melanopsin and might therefore be more resistant) or whether the lesion is more complete also at early stages of development. More importantly, this method is also not appropriate for looking at off target effects. It is common for transgenes to have some 'leaky' expression in cells other than those targeted. How confident can the authors be that DTA is not also killing other cells? If it were then this could provide an alternative explanation for the observations.

2) Another important question is whether the DTA mice have the same strain background as other lines? This is not clear from the methods.

3) There are a couple of other instances of over interpretation. The evidence that 200 ipRGCs are sufficient to support development comes from lack of phenotype in Opn4Cre Brn3bzDTA. To interpret that observation, we need to know the time course of ipRGC loss in this genotype and in DTA/DTA. If brn3b+ve ipRGCs survive longer in Opn4Cre Brn3bzDTA than in DTA/DTA that would provide an alternative explanation for this effect.

4) The authors show deficits in acuity by optomotor and swim maze tests in DTA/DTA mice compared to wild type and Opn4LacZ/LacZmice. They interpret this as an impact of poor eye segregation. That is not the only possible explanation for those data. Loss of ipRGCs could impact acuity because lack of entrainment means that animals were tested at different circadian phases, the pupil is more dilated, direct influences of ipRGCs on retinal function are lost, or some as yet undefined impact of ipRGCs on vision is absent.

5) The authors encourage the reader to pull together the circadian and visual effects as instances of convergence between image-forming (IF) and non-image-forming (NIF) visual systems in development. Are effects on the clock and thalamic projections in any mechanistic sense linked? They both occur in DTA/DTA mice, but while the projection phenotype is clearly an error in optic nerve development, that seems not to be the case for the SCN. The SCN, but not LGN, effect can be phenocopied by dark rearing (unlike the projection deficit) and disappears immediately upon light exposure. As it is rapidly reversible, this not an error in development of anatomy or connectivity (authors present additional evidence that these are normal). If these are two quite separate effects on development how does presenting these in the IF vs NIF dichotomy help the reader to interpret?

6) The Introduction and Abstract present the logic for the study in terms of the growing evidence of blurred distinction between NIF and IF vision in adults and the need to ask whether this also the case in development? This leads the reader to link effects on the clock and thalamus in ways that may not be appropriate (see above). A much more direct motivation for the study is the growing reports that melanopsin and/or ipRGCs regulate retinal development (see papers by Copenhagen/Lang and Berson groups). Building from that literature would better place this work in context of the current state of knowledge.

7) It seems from the data presented that overlap of projections from 2 eyes is reduced in Opn4Cre Brn3bzDTA compared to wild types. Is this true?

8) The requirement for very few (200) ipRGCs to assist eye-specific segregation may not be very surprising given the dendritic coverage of these cells (big) and the fact that they are a "net", for photoreception, not a high acuity system for detailed analysis. The original "splash" regarding ipRGCs impacting eye-specific segregation (and retinal waves) was published by Renna et al., 2011. The results presented here are different but the novelty resides in the "200" aspect which, while interesting, is hard to set in context. What if it had been 300 or 500? Is the number past 1 relevant?

9) Given the retinal wave phenotype in the Renna et al., 2011 paper, do the authors have any evidence relevant to this phenotype?

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "A subset of ipRGCs regulates both maturation of the circadian clock and segregation of retinogeniculate projections in mice" for further consideration at eLife. Your revised article has been favorably evaluated by a Senior editor, a Reviewing editor, and two reviewers.

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed before acceptance, as outlined below:

This revision is much improved by additional data dealing with the major issues with the original submission and some changes in presentation. Phenotypes for the various mouse lines are clear and well described. We reiterate however that a weakness of the study design is that two important bits of information for interpreting the data are fundamentally unknowable – what happens to ipRGCs of all types in the DTA/DTA and aDTA/aDTA animals, and whether there are off target effects on other cells in the retina and/or brain of these transgenes. The authors have addressed these questions with more data that are consistent with their preferred interpretation, that the DTA/DTA ablation of ipRGCs occurs earlier and is more extensive than in aDTA/aDTA, and that there is no off target effect. We would prefer a more balanced coverage of these critical questions, the authors present the positive evidence for their interpretation but do not give the limitations of their data appropriate coverage. A paragraph in the Discussion describing what they cannot know and what alternative explanations for their data this allows would be helpful for the reader.

One note to authors: the reply to other reviews has in several places, a "we told you before, in the previous version" tone. This is a source of opaqueness in the paper too; we suggest the authors consider the possibility that the reviewers misunderstood in part because the presentation was not clear and/or there are a lot of conditional mutants. This is not a simple and straightforward study, which requires like any challenging conversation, more clarity, not less. We recommend they consider how the reader will interpret their findings and think hard about striving for more clarity about novelty and methodology for the non-expert.

eLife. 2017 Jun 15;6:e22861. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22861.020

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) Enthusiasm for this work is constrained by the limited description of the phenotype of the DTA/DTA mouse.

This is a surprising comment since Figure 1—figure supplement 1 of the original manuscript included a thorough description of the Opn4DTA/DTA mouse. Nevertheless, as described below, we have now added additional data for the Opn4DTA/DTA mice.

The manuscript is built around a comparison of this mouse with other transgenic lines yet we do not know how complete the lesion of ipRGCs is in this genotype, the developmental time course of their loss, or how specific the lesion is to ipRGCs. Without answers to these questions it is hard to draw definitive conclusions. The authors attribute differences reported between DTA/DTA and aDTA/aDTA or DTA mice to a more complete loss of ipRGCs. This may be a reasonable supposition but we do not know for sure either that there is a more complete loss early in development or that the DTA/DTA does not have off target effects on other important cell types.

Again, this is a surprising comment. It is true that we have not specifically tested the Opn4DTA/DTA mice, but we have tested the heterozygous mice extensively and across developmental timecourse (Figure 1A-B). In fact, in the original submission we included data documenting ipRGC loss in mice that expressed, from the melanopsin locus, one copy of the toxin (either aDTA or DTA) and one copy of a marker to identify ipRGCs and their projections (either Cre combined with an alkaline phosphatase reporter, or tau-LacZ). We used mice heterozygous for either aDTA or DTA to establish the extent and timing of ipRGC ablation, which in mice homozygous for the toxin should be either the same or faster and/or more extensive than in mice heterozygous for toxin. However, now, we have added data that directly assess the effect on the homozygous animals. As suggested, we injected CTB in P4 pups, harvested brain tissue at P7, and imaged retinal innervation of the SCN in WT, Opn4aDTA/aDTA, and Opn4DTA/DTA mice. These new data are shown in Figure 1 (new panel B), and clearly show the earlier, more substantial ablation of SCN projecting ipRGCs in Opn4DTA/DTA mice compared to both WT and Opn4aDTA/aDTA.

As the only marker of ipRGCs is melanopsin, there is no way of identifying ipRGCs in the DTA/DTA mice. The only way of measuring ipRGC loss in this genotype is by looking at projections to the SCN. These are lost at 6 months showing that lesion of this projection in adults is more complete, but we do not know whether that is true for other ipRGC types (which express less melanopsin and might therefore be more resistant) or whether the lesion is more complete also at early stages of development.

As described above, to specifically answer the early stages of development loss, we have now carried out the experiment examining SCN innervation in postnatal Opn4DTA/DTA mice (Figure 1 new panel B).

In terms of examining loss of specific ipRGCs subtypes, it is important to note that the reviewers are correct that assessment of ipRGC projections to the SCN specifically provides information regarding ablation of M1 ipRGCs. Determining the timeline of ablation of specific ipRGC subtypes during development is not possible since the subtypes cannot be accurately identified until dendrite arborization is complete, which is between P10-P15 (Coombs, Van Der List and Chalupa, 2007) and after cell loss is complete in Opn4DTA mice (Figure 1C and D). We can make inferences about the proportion M1 and non-M1 ipRGC subtypes that persist based on adult cell counts that were performed in mice heterozygous for toxin and included in the original submission. In mice with one copy of a toxin and one copy of Cre combined with an alkaline phosphatase reporter, we counted the total number of ipRGCs that survive toxin expression (~1500 cells in Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice and ~500 cells Opn4DTA/DTA mice). We also counted the number of M1 cells that survive, which is ~150 in aDTA mice and ~75 in DTA mice. Thus, about ~1350 non-M1 ipRGCs persist in mice with one copy of aDTA, and about ~425 non-M1 ipRGCs persist in mice with one of copy DTA. This is consistent with the knowledge that M1 ipRGCs express the most melanopsin and therefore express the most toxin and undergo the most substantial ablation.

However, the reviewers, of course, are asking about loss of specific non-M1 ipRGC subtypes in Opn4DTA/DTA animals. Without a copy of melanopsin (or a reporter for melanopsin expression), we have no way of labeling the ipRGCs and their dendrites in order to identify the number of remaining ipRGCs of each subtype. Thus, to address this question, we must use molecular markers for ipRGCs subtypes. While the field has been attempting to discover molecular markers for each ipRGC subtype for many years now, there has been minimal success. However, one marker, which identifies the M4 subtype, has been discovered: a protein called SMI-32. SMI-32 was historically a marker of ON and OFF α ganglion cells. With the realization that ON α cells are one and the same as M4 ipRGCs, a marker for M4 ipRGC was identified (Estevez et al., 2012; Schmidt et al., 2014). It is worth noting that all M4 cells are SMI-32 positive, but since SMI-32 also labels OFF α cells, about 50% of SMI-32 positive ganglion cells are melanopsin expressing (Schmidt et al., 2014). We actually included SMI-32 staining in WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice in the original submission, but did not highlight that it was also labeling M4 ipRGCs. We have done so now. We detected no difference in the number of SMI-32 positive cells in DTA/DTA compared to WT controls. Thus, many, if not all, M4 ipRGCs persist in Opn4DTA/DTA mice, which is consistent with there still being AP positive ipRGCs in Opn4Cre/DTA; Z/AP mice and M4 ipRGCs expressing less melanopsin than M1s (Sexton and Van Gelder 2015). We have included a new section in the Results to describe this point.

The major contribution of our data is the demonstration that Brn3b negative M1 ipRGCs are important for both the maturation of the circadian clock and eye-specific segregation. We now have demonstrated beyond reasonable doubt that these cells are ablated in Opn4DTA/DTA mice. Thus, we feel that an exhaustive assessment of which non-M1 ipRGCs die in the Opn4aDTA and Opn4DTA lines does not further augment the conclusions of this of study.

More importantly, this method is also not appropriate for looking at off target effects. It is common for transgenes to have some 'leaky' expression in cells other than those targeted. How confident can the authors be that DTA is not also killing other cells? If it were then this could provide an alternative explanation for the observations.

Off-target cell loss due to leaky expression of DTA was initially also a major concern for us, and thus, we conducted a myriad of experiments to prove to ourselves, and hopefully future readers, that this concern is unwarranted. We actually included much of this data in the original submission (shown in current Figure 1—figure supplement 1C, E-J). We have now include additional data (see Figure 1—figure supplement 1D and Figure 1—figure supplement 2). We have also expanded our description of these data in the main text of the manuscript. Since it is obviously not possible to account for every individual cell in the whole organism, we can most generally report that the Opn4DTA/DTA mice are viable, fertile, and do not exhibit any gross abnormalities in size. This information has been added to the paper. More specifically, we focused on evaluating the retina and the brain regions most relevant to this study. General retinal structure was assessed by staining retinal sections from WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice with H&E stain and additionally fluorescent staining for various retinal cells type, including cones, ON bipolar cells, calretinin positive amacrine and ganglion cells, and Brn3a positive ganglion cells. We have now quantified the total retinal thickness as well as the thickness of each of the retinal layers of WT and Opn4DTA/DTA based on H&E staining (see new Figure 1—figure supplement 1D). Comparison of WT and Opn4DTA/DTA retinas showed no difference in total retinal thickness or the thickness of any of the individual retinal layers. We also counted the number of SMI-32 positive ganglion cells, Brn3a positive ganglion cells, cones, and starburst amacrine cells. When we compared WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice, there was no difference in cell number of any of these cell types. Also, as mentioned above, about 50% of SMI-32 positive ganglion cells are actually M4 ipRGCs, which means that expression of two copies of DTA from the melanopsin locus does not even ablate all ipRGCs. This fact helped convince us that there is no off-target killing of non-ipRGC cells. Nonetheless, we did further tests to verify there were no off-target effects outside the retina. We counted the total number of nuclei (based on DAPI staining) and the total number of neurons (identified by antibody staining for Hu) in the SCN of WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice. There was no difference between WT and Opn4DTA/DTA in the number of nuclei or neurons when compared in total or section by section through the SCN. These new data are in new Figure 1—figure supplement 2. We have now also measured the total size of the dLGN, which was also not different between WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice, or between any other tested mouse line. These new data are in Figure 4—figure supplement 4 new panel E. We hope the reviewers agree that the combination of the data presented in the original submission and our new measurements thoroughly and rigorously demonstrates there is no off-target killing of non-ipRGCs in Opn4DTA/DTA mice.

2) Another important question is whether the DTA mice have the same strain background as other lines? This is not clear from the methods.

The reviewer is correct that this is an important point. We apologize for not including this information in the original submission. We strived to have all the mice on similar strain backgrounds as possible; although, it is challenging to have identical backgrounds considering the number of crosses performed for this study. All mice were either on C56BL/6J or a C56BL/6J – 129/J hybrid background. This information has been added to mouse strain table in the supplementary information (See updated table of mouse lines). We would like to stress that we always strain matched the control group and the most meaningful comparisons (for example WT, Opn4DTA, dark reared, and enucleated mice were all on the pure B6 background). In terms of circadian biology the C56BL/6 and 129/J strains are very similar (Schwartz and Zimmerman 1990), and there have been no reported differences in eye specific segregation nor did we observe any differences between pure C56BL/6J and mice on a mixed background.

3) There are a couple of other instances of over interpretation. The evidence that 200 ipRGCs are sufficient to support development comes from lack of phenotype in Opn4Cre Brn3bzDTA. To interpret that observation, we need to know the time course of ipRGC loss in this genotype and in DTA/DTA. If brn3b+ve ipRGCs survive longer in Opn4Cre Brn3bzDTA than in DTA/DTA that would provide an alternative explanation for this effect.

We actually included the developmental timecourse of ablation in the Opn4Cre/+;Brn3bzDTA/+ (now called Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2zDTA/+ in the revised manuscript) animals in the original submission (Figure 4—figure supplement 5) and showed that ipRGC ablation is already complete by P7. Thus, the timing brn3b positive ipRGCs cell loss is comparable in DTA and Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2zDTA/+ mice. ipRGC ablation may even be a bit faster in Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2zDTA/+ mice and is also known to include ablation of the M4 population (Schmidt et al., 2014). In fact, more ipRGCs are ablated in these animals than the DTA line and yet no segregation deficits are observed in the Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2zDTA/+ animals.

4) The authors show deficits in acuity by optomotor and swim maze tests in DTA/DTA mice compared to wild type and Opn4LacZ/LacZmice. They interpret this as an impact of poor eye segregation. That is not the only possible explanation for those data. Loss of ipRGCs could impact acuity because lack of entrainment means that animals were tested at different circadian phases, the pupil is more dilated, direct influences of ipRGCs on retinal function are lost, or some as yet undefined impact of ipRGCs on vision is absent.

The reviewers make a good point here, and thus, we have added a discussion of alternative explanations regarding the reduction in acuity. However, the loss of acuity in Opn4DTA/DTA mice cannot be explained by a loss of photoentrainment because Opn4aDTA/aDTA mice, which also free run, do not exhibit such a substantial reduction in acuity. Opn4DTA/+ mice, which do photoentrain also exhibited deficits in visual acuity. Furthermore, when the pupil is fully dilated with Carbocal there is only a minor reduction in acuity and it is not comparable to the substantial deficits that we observed in Opn4DTA/DTA and Opn4DTA/+ mice. Nonetheless, direct effects of ipRGCs on retinal functions, including the role of M1 ipRGCs in regulating dopamine in the retina, could contribute. We have now discussed these points in the manuscript.

5) The authors encourage the reader to pull together the circadian and visual effects as instances of convergence between image-forming (IF) and non-image-forming (NIF) visual systems in development. Are effects on the clock and thalamic projections in any mechanistic sense linked? They both occur in DTA/DTA mice, but while the projection phenotype is clearly an error in optic nerve development, that seems not to be the case for the SCN. The SCN, but not LGN, effect can be phenocopied by dark rearing (unlike the projection deficit) and disappears immediately upon light exposure. As it is rapidly reversible, this not an error in development of anatomy or connectivity (authors present additional evidence that these are normal). If these are two quite separate effects on development how does presenting these in the IF vs NIF dichotomy help the reader to interpret?

We presented the IF vs NIF dichotomy because we were very surprised that the population of ipRGCs (the Brn3b negative M1 ipRGCs), which project exclusively to NIF brain regions, are also sufficient for the refinement of retinal ganglion cell projections the LGN. This single population appears to have both light dependent functions (circadian period setting) and light independent functions (modulation of spontaneous retinal activity; see point 9). We feel this is a critical point from our work; however, we agree that we could make this much clearer and that our presentation in the original submission could mislead readers. We do not want readers to think there is mechanistic overlap between circadian period setting and refinement of eye-specific segregation, but rather that a single cell type acts through very different mechanisms (light dependent and light independent) to affect the development of both the NIF and IF systems. We think that the addition of retinal wave recordings (see point 9) will help make this clearer, but we have also modified the Introduction and Discussion to hopefully better highlight the conclusions that we aim to make.

6) The Introduction and Abstract present the logic for the study in terms of the growing evidence of blurred distinction between NIF and IF vision in adults and the need to ask whether this also the case in development? This leads the reader to link effects on the clock and thalamus in ways that may not be appropriate (see above). A much more direct motivation for the study is the growing reports that melanopsin and/or ipRGCs regulate retinal development (see papers by Copenhagen/Lang and Berson groups). Building from that literature would better place this work in context of the current state of knowledge.

This is an excellent point, and we now have modified the Introduction with the established roles for ipRGCs in retinal development in mind.

7) It seems from the data presented that overlap of projections from 2 eyes is reduced in opn4cre brn3bzdta compared to wild types. Is this true?

There is no statistical difference by a one-way ANOVA and this is now stated in the figure legend.

8) The requirement for very few (200) ipRGCs to assist eye-specific segregation may not be very surprising given the dendritic coverage of these cells (big) and the fact that they are a "net", for photoreception, not a high acuity system for detailed analysis. The original "splash" regarding ipRGCs impacting eye-specific segregation (and retinal waves) was published by Renna et al., 2011. The results presented here are different but the novelty resides in the "200" aspect which, while interesting, is hard to set in context. What if it had been 300 or 500? Is the number past 1 relevant?

Our point is not that 200 cells is a magic number, but rather that the subset of ipRGCs, which project exclusively to circadian brain regions (the SCN and the IGL) and have intraretinal axonal collaterals, is sufficient to mediate circadian clock maturation and refinement of retinal ganglion cell projections to the LGN. The role of light in the maturation of the circadian clock is surprising; however, the role of this subset of ipRGCs in that process is logical, since they obviously project to the SCN. Yet, the experiments with Opn4Cre/+; Pou4f2zDTA/+ mice are critical to show that ipRGCs are not functioning within the LGN to mediate eye-specific segregation (see point 9 below). In the original submission, we implied that this subset must be acting within the retina to mediate eye-specific segregation. With the addition of recordings of retinal waves in WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice (see next point) we have corroborated this conclusion. We have also modified the text to better emphasize that 200 cells is not the key point, but rather one specialized subset mediates both of these seemingly distinct development processes.

9) Given the retinal wave phenotype in the Renna et al., 2011 paper, do the authors have any evidence relevant to this phenotype?

Renna et al. demonstrated that light (detect by melanopsin) alters duration of bursting during retinal waves. However, independent of light ipRGCs also depolarized during retinal waves. Thus, we have now included an assessment of retinal wave in darkness in P6 WT and DTA/DTA mice using multielectrode recordings. These data are now presented in new Figure 5E-H and raw numbers and p values are shown in new Supplementary file 2. Spiking properties of RGCs during waves were significantly altered in DTA/DTA mice. Wave-associated bursts were significantly longer in duration, and had higher firing rates, more total spikes and shorter inter-burst intervals than they did in wild-type controls. There were also significantly more spikes outside of bursts. Correlated spiking activity between neurons was very similar between the genotypes, as measured by the spike time tiling. These data indicate that ipRGCs are critical for normal retinal wave activity, even in darkness, and together with our anatomical findings, allow us to suggest that ipRGCs mediate eye-specific segregation of RGC projections to the dLGN by regulating the spiking properties of conventional RGCs during retinal waves.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

This revision is much improved by additional data dealing with the major issues with the original submission and some changes in presentation. Phenotypes for the various mouse lines are clear and well described. We reiterate however that a weakness of the study design is that two important bits of information for interpreting the data are fundamentally unknowable – what happens to ipRGCs of all types in the DTA/DTA and aDTA/aDTA animals, and whether there are off target effects on other cells in the retina and/or brain of these transgenes. The authors have addressed these questions with more data that are consistent with their preferred interpretation, that the DTA/DTA ablation of ipRGCs occurs earlier and is more extensive than in aDTA/aDTA, and that there is no off target effect. We would prefer a more balanced coverage of these critical questions, the authors present the positive evidence for their interpretation but do not give the limitations of their data appropriate coverage. A paragraph in the Discussion describing what they cannot know and what alternative explanations for their data this allows would be helpful for the reader.

We have accepted the suggestion and have added the following paragraph to the Discussion:

“A key method in this study was the use of two different versions of diphtheria toxin subunit A that have different potencies, the attenuated versus the full strength forms. [...] Together, although we cannot fully eliminate the possibility of off target effects of the DTA line, we believe this is unlikely.”

One note to authors: the reply to other reviews has in several places, a "we told you before, in the previous version" tone. This is a source of opaqueness in the paper too; we suggest the authors consider the possibility that the reviewers misunderstood in part because the presentation was not clear and/or there are a lot of conditional mutants. This is not a simple and straightforward study, which requires like any challenging conversation, more clarity, not less. We recommend they consider how the reader will interpret their findings and think hard about striving for more clarity about novelty and methodology for the non-expert.

We truly apologize if we presented our responses as “we told you before.” We agree that this is a large study with many different mutant lines and manipulations, in addition to covering two different areas, circadian and vision. In our previous response, we aimed to highlight the areas where the concerns raised by the reviewers have been addressed in our original submission. We certainly strive to improve the clarity of our publications. Therefore, we have made several changes throughout the manuscript, which we hope improve the clarity of this study.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. Description of all mouse lines.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22861.017

    elife-22861-supp1.docx (138.7KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.22861.017
    Supplementary file 2. Values and statistics for retinal wave recordings.

    Properties of spontaneous retinal activity in P6 WT and Opn4DTA/DTA mice. Recordings were done in darkness. * represent statistically significant differences after Student's T-tests with a Holm-Bonferroni correction, α = 0.05, m = 22. Significance is determined when the p-value is less than or equal to the Holm-Bonferroni corrected p-value.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22861.018

    elife-22861-supp2.docx (202.9KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.22861.018

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES