Significance
There are many examples of costly cooperation in humans. Although other great apes have been shown to engage in a number of cooperative behaviors, there is no reliable experimental evidence that they will sacrifice resources to benefit others. Here, we show that chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) return favors to conspecifics who have previously assisted them in acquiring food; crucially, they even do this at a material cost to themselves and especially when the conspecific incurred a risk in providing the assistance. Chimpanzees are thus capable of engaging in materially costly reciprocal interactions commonly considered unique to humans.
Keywords: cooperation, prosociality, chimpanzees, reciprocity
Abstract
Humans regularly provide others with resources at a personal cost to themselves. Chimpanzees engage in some cooperative behaviors in the wild as well, but their motivational underpinnings are unclear. In three experiments, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) always chose between an option delivering food both to themselves and a partner and one delivering food only to themselves. In one condition, a conspecific partner had just previously taken a personal risk to make this choice available. In another condition, no assistance from the partner preceded the subject’s decision. Chimpanzees made significantly more prosocial choices after receiving their partner’s assistance than when no assistance was given (experiment 1) and, crucially, this was the case even when choosing the prosocial option was materially costly for the subject (experiment 2). Moreover, subjects appeared sensitive to the risk of their partner’s assistance and chose prosocially more often when their partner risked losing food by helping (experiment 3). These findings demonstrate experimentally that chimpanzees are willing to incur a material cost to deliver rewards to a conspecific, but only if that conspecific previously assisted them, and particularly when this assistance was risky. Some key motivations involved in human cooperation thus may have deeper phylogenetic roots than previously suspected.
The motivation to benefit others even at a cost to the self is commonly seen as a hallmark of human cooperation (1–4). In their natural habitats, chimpanzees—human’s closest living evolutionary relatives together with bonobos (Pan paniscus) (5)—also engage in a number of cooperative activities, such as teaming up with partners in fights, grooming with others reciprocally, and even sharing meat after successful hunts (6–10). Observational studies with captive animals have shown further that, over the long-term, chimpanzees reciprocally groom and share food with others (11–14). Bonobos also cooperate in a number of contexts (15–18), suggesting that such cooperative tendencies have deep phylogenetic roots.
It is unclear, however, in how far these cooperative acts in chimpanzees are based on prosocial motives in the sense that they are intended to benefit a partner. Coalitions in fights and mutual grooming, for instance, provide immediate benefits to both partners (19), whereas meat sharing most often occurs in response to threats and/or harassment from beggars (20, 21). The psychological mechanisms underlying chimpanzee cooperation and in how far they overlap with those seen in humans is thus currently unresolved (19, 20, 22–26).
Laboratory experiments have attempted to clarify the situation. Numerous studies suggest that when there is no material cost (i.e., solely a small energetic cost), chimpanzees will help both humans and other chimpanzees attain their goals, for example, by fetching out-of-reach objects and tools (27, 28) or by flipping a latch to enable a conspecific to obtain food (29); however, they appear to do this only if they themselves have no chance whatsoever to obtain any food in the situation, and it has been argued that helping rarely occurs in the absence of active solicitation or harassment by the partner (26, 30). A recent study has also argued that experimental design features (e.g., subjects’ motivation to engage with novel tasks) should be considered as potential alternative explanations (31).
Furthermore, several studies have shown that when presented with one option delivering food to both themselves and a conspecific and another option only delivering food to themselves, chimpanzees choose completely randomly between these alternatives, which has been interpreted as suggesting that chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of conspecifics even when being prosocial is entirely noncostly (32–34). Although some studies show somewhat more mixed (but not easily interpretable) results concerning cost-free prosocial choices in chimpanzees (35, 36), the picture is very clear when it comes to materially costly prosociality: to date there is no reliable experimental evidence that chimpanzees are willing to deliver resources to conspecifics when doing so entails a cost for themselves.
However, previous experiments may have failed to elicit the kind of social context in which chimpanzee prosociality naturally occurs. One possibility, for instance, is that costly prosocial acts are contingent on others’ prior helpful behaviors (37, 38) and therefore could not be detected in experiments stripped of any prior cooperative interaction. Indeed, several studies have suggested reciprocal patterns of sharing behaviors in the animal kingdom (39–42), although the underlying psychological mechanisms are not always clear. Although chimpanzees did not show such reciprocal prosocial patterns in some experiments (43, 44), this can at least partly be explained by the subjects’ lack of understanding of experimental setup and the choice contingencies (see refs. 26 and 45 for recent methodological critiques). Others found a slight increase of cooperation with a helpful conspecific in a helping paradigm (46), but chimpanzees did not preferentially favor an individual who had assisted them over one who had not.
In the current study, we therefore aimed to create a situation in which chimpanzee subjects thoroughly understood the experimental setup and clearly recognized whether a conspecific partner had previously assisted them to access food; subsequently, they had the opportunity to return that favor by providing their partner with resources. In each of three experiments, subjects (n = 6) always chose between a prosocial option that equally benefited both themselves and a conspecific partner and an option that only benefitted themselves. What differed between conditions was that this choice was either made available to the subject by a prosocial act of the partner or without her assistance.
In the risky assistance condition (RA) of experiment 1, the partner first rejected an option (option A; Fig. 1A) that would have terminated the trial while delivering two pieces of food to herself and leaving the subject empty-handed (unbeknownst to the subject, the partner was trained to always reject option A). Instead, the partner pulled on a rope which—while precluding her from directly accessing any food—unhinged a latch whereby the aforementioned choice was opened up for the subject (options B and C; Fig. 1B). Before the test, subjects had extensive experience with the apparatus and demonstrated in several training steps that they were sensitive to the payoffs associated with each choice as well as the apparent choices available to the partner. Hence, from the subject’s perspective the partner risked getting nothing for herself but instead assisted the subject in obtaining food. In the no-assistance condition (NA), the partner was present but did not act at all. The experimenter unhinged the latch and the subject faced the same choice as in RA but without having received assistance from the partner. In both conditions, subjects faced the choice between a prosocial 2/2 option (subject’s payoff/partner’s payoff) and a 2/0 option benefitting only the subject (Table 1).
Table 1.
Experiment/condition | Option | Subject’s payoff | Partner’s payoff |
Experiment 1 | A | •• | |
B | •• | ||
C | •• | •• | |
Experiment 2 | A | ••• | |
B | •••• | ||
C | ••• | ••• | |
Experiment 3 RA and NA | A | •• | |
B | •• | ||
C | •• | •• | |
Experiment 3 NRA | A | ||
B | •• | ||
C | •• | •• |
Dots represent pieces of food.
Results
Chimpanzees chose the 2/2 option in 76.4% of trials in RA and in 50.7% of trials in NA (P = 0.031, exact Wilcoxon signed-rank test, V = 21, n = 6; Fig. 2; all Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were two-tailed). Moreover, in RA, four of six subjects chose the 2/2 option significantly above chance (P < 0.05), whereas no subject did so in NA (chance comparisons were based on two-tailed binomial tests that were corrected for multiple testing using false discovery rates to keep type 1 error rates at the nominal level of 0.05) (47).
Experiment 2 was identical to experiment 1 except that choosing the prosocial option was now costly for the subject—that is, after the partner made the choice available to the subject, the subject chose between a prosocial 3/3 option and a selfish 4/0 option (Table 1), which meant that they had to give up one piece of food to deliver a benefit to their partner. Chimpanzees chose the 3/3 option in 43.8% of trials in RA compared with 17.4% of trials in NA (P = 0.031, exact Wilcoxon signed-rank test, V = 21, n = 6). In a preference test before and after experiment 2, subjects further demonstrated that they clearly discriminated between three and four pieces of food and showed a strong and stable preference for the higher quantity. This finding indicates that subjects often went against their preference in RA and were willing to give up 25% of their payoff to reward their partner’s cooperative effort.
In experiment 3 we examined further whether subjects appreciated that their partner’s assistance was potentially costly. RA and NA conditions were virtually the same as in experiment 1, but in a new nonrisky assistance condition (NRA), the partner provided assistance without having to forego any resources (i.e., there was no food present in the rejected option A; Table 1). Hence, from the subject’s point of view, the partner had no choice but to provide assistance to obtain any food. The new NRA condition thus provides an especially subtle contrast because the partner assisted the subject just as in the RA condition—only without incurring any material cost or risk to do so.
The number of 2/2 choices differed significantly between conditions [P = 0.011, Friedman test, χ2(2) = 8.96]. Subjects chose the 2/2 option in 77.8% of trials in RA and in 53.5% of trials in NA—virtually replicating the results from experiment 1 with the exception of one subject (Fig. 2). The proportion of 2/2 choices in NRA (67.4%) was intermediate as would be expected if nonrisky assistance was interpreted as less costly than risky assistance but still more worthy of reward than if no assistance was given. Indeed, post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed a significant difference between choices in NRA and NA (P = 0.031, exact Wilcoxon signed-rank test, V = 21, n = 6). The difference between the RA and NA as well as the difference between RA and NRA approached statistical significance (exact Wilcoxon signed-rank test, V = 15, n = 6, P = 0.063 and V = 19, n = 6, P = 0.094, respectively). Moreover, five of six subjects chose the 2/2 option significantly above chance in RA, whereas only one subject—who predominantly chose the prosocial option regardless of condition—did so in the other two conditions (Fig. 2).
To test for potential order effects, we fitted a generalized linear mixed model for each experiment (SI Materials and Methods for detailed model descriptions), which revealed that neither the test predictors session and trial number nor the interaction of these predictors with the experimental condition significantly affected subjects’ tendency to choose the prosocial option in any of the three experiments: experiment 1, χ2(4) = 5.71, P = 0.222; experiment 2, χ2(4) = 5.31, P = 0.257; experiment 3, χ2(6) = 6.13, P = 0.409.
SI Materials and Methods
Procedure.
Basic training procedure before experiments.
Partner training.
P in room 1 was trained to always make options B and C available in room 2. The vertical sliding door in room 1 could be invisibly rigged in a way that the door could only be moved down; this meant that only the offer rope leading to room 2 could be accessed but not the string of option A. This blocking was always done when P was in room 1 so that she never experienced the possibility to choose option A. Test subjects, however, never experienced a blocked vertical sliding door at any point in the experiment. P was always only present in room 1 with access to the vertical sliding door. E started a partner training trial by removing the cover of the mesh panel so that P could make options B and C available in room 2. Whenever P did so, she was rewarded by E. P reliably performed this action whenever the cover was removed from the first trial on and did so throughout all experiments.
Apparatus experience.
Each of the 12 initial S got access to both rooms 1 and 2. Apparatus experience sessions consisted of 11 trials.
Trials 1 and 2: In the first two trials, the vertical sliding door in room 1 was already moved up so that the only possible action for S was to pull the string of option A. There was one piece of food in each cup of option A [1/1]. When S pulled the string, the two cups moved toward the respective rooms and E would give one piece of food to S on each side.
Trials 3 and 4: In the third and fourth trial, E put one piece of food into one cup of either option B or C [0/1]. S did not have access to the vertical sliding door in room 1 in these trials. A trial started as soon as S sat in front of the horizontal sliding door in room 2. E then pulled the offer rope with the attached latch, opening up options B and C for S who then had to choose the correct option to receive the food (option B in trial 3 and option C in trial 4).
Trials 5 and 6: In the fifth trial, both cups of option B [1/1] and in the sixth trial, both cups of option C were baited [1/1]. In both of these trials, the vertical sliding door in room 1 was already moved down so that the only possible action for S in room 1 was to pull the offer rope. Hence, S had to make options B and C available, choose the correct one of these options, and then collect the food in both rooms; this was done for S to learn how to transfer the choice by pulling the rope and that food would be delivered to both rooms.
Trials 7 and 8: In the seventh and eighth trial (identical to warm-up trials in the one-solution criterion), E removed the offer rope and the string of option A from the vertical sliding door and showed S a piece of food on the top and the bottom side of the transparent door, respectively. In these trials, the vertical sliding door was in the middle position and allowed S a choice between the top and bottom. S received the food when she correctly moved the sliding door down (trial 7) or up (trial 8) to gain access to the food behind it. When S chose incorrectly, E would move the sliding door back into the middle position and repeat the trial until S performed the correct action. This procedure was done to familiarize S with the movement of the vertical sliding door.
Trials 9–11: In these trials, the apparatus was in the starting position (Fig. 1A). All options A, B, and C were therefore accessible when the correct choices were made. In the ninth trial, both cups of option A were baited [1/1], whereas options B and C contained no rewards [0/0]. S thus had to choose option A and discard the offer rope to get the food. In the 10th and 11th trial, the two cups of either option B or C were baited [1/1], whereas the other options contained no food [0/0]. S received the food when they discarded option A, made options B and C available, and chose the correct one of the two options in room 2.
Criterion to pass on to the next prerequisite step was 10 correct choices in these 11 trials (trials 7 and 8 were always correct as they were repeated until S chose correctly); this was not as strict a criterion as later prerequisite criteria (see below), because E called S to the correct room and showed S what to do when she tried incorrect options or lost interest. The apparatus experience familiarized S with the different options and actions that could be performed on the apparatus on both sides.). Six of 12 initial S managed to operate the apparatus and passed to experiment 1.
Prerequisite criteria.
Experiments 1, 2, and 3: One solution criterion.
S had access to both sides of the apparatus. Before each session, S received two warm-up trials to remind S how to operate the vertical sliding door (analogous to trials 7 and 8 of the apparatus experience). After the warm-up, sessions consisted of 12 trials with the apparatus in the starting position. In each trial, only one piece of food was put into one of the six food cups (Table S1). In half of the trials (A trials), one cup of option A was baited (either [1/0] or [0/1]). In the other half of the trials (B/C trials), the food was in one of the cups of options B or C (either [1/0] or [0/1]). S passed criterion when they managed to access the food in at least 10 of 12 trials in two consecutive sessions and never made more than one mistake per trial type (i.e., A or B/C trials) in one session. All six S passed this criterion, demonstrating that they understood how to operate the apparatus to access each of the six available food cups. The procedure of the one-solution criterion was identical in all three experiments.
Table S1.
Condition | Option | Cage 2 | Cage 1 |
One solution criterion (B/C trial) | A | ||
B | |||
C | • | ||
Best solution criterion, RA and NA in experiments 1 and 3 | A | •• | |
B | •• | ||
C | •• | •• | |
Dependence experience 1 | A | • | • |
B | • | ||
C | • | • | |
Dependence experience 2 | A | • | |
B | • | • | |
C | • | • | |
Quantity preference | A | ||
B | ••• | ||
C | •••• | ||
Best solution criterion, RA and NA in experiment 2 | A | ••• | |
B | •••• | ||
C | ••• | ••• | |
Attention criterion (A trial) | A | ••• | |
B | • | ||
C | • | • | |
Attention criterion (B/C trial) | A | ||
B | • | ||
C | • | • | |
NRA in experiment 3 | A | ||
B | •• | ||
C | •• | •• |
Dots represent amount of food pieces.
Experiments 1, 2, and 3: Best solution criterion.
S had access to both sides of the apparatus. In experiments 1 and 3, option A always contained two rewards [2/0], whereas either option B contained four [2/2] and option C two [0/2] or vice versa (B [0/2] and C [2/2]). In experiment 2, option A always contained three rewards ([3/0]), whereas either option B contained [3/3] and option C [0/4], or vice versa (B [0/4] and C [3/3]). These payoffs were equivalent to the payoffs in the respective experimental conditions (Table S1). S had to maximize their food intake by always discarding option A and then choosing either option B or C in the other room, depending on where the higher quantity of food pieces was. S passed the criterion when they chose correctly in 10 of 12 trials in two consecutive sessions. All six S passed the criterion.
If S made systematic mistakes in the best solution criterion, for example, always choosing option A, E successively decreased the payoffs in that option and only successively increased them again when S began to choose the correct options. In that way, S were trained to pay attention to the payoffs in all options, inhibit going for the first accessible option, and choose the maximum number of food rewards. This adjustment of payoffs was necessary for three S in experiment 1 (Frodo: nine sessions; Sandra: seven sessions; Kara: one session), one S in experiment 2 (Frodo: seven sessions), and one S in experiment 3 (Frodo: one session). Note that the NA conditions in the experimental conditions controlled for the possibility of general learning effects (i.e., if S learned in the best solution criterion to always choose the option with the highest total amount of rewards, they should have chosen this option in both the RA and NA conditions regardless of P’s prior behavior).
Experiments 1, 2, and 3: Dependence experiences 1 and 2.
In dependence experience 1, S were in room 1. The vertical sliding door was moved down and the string of option A was removed so that option A was visibly not accessible. The offer rope was not attached; options B and C were closed with a latch without a rope, which meant that there was no action at all that S could perform from room 1. Payoffs in the inaccessible option A were always [1/1]. In half of the trials, payoffs in option B were [1/1] and in option C [0/1] and in the other half of trials vice versa (B [0/1] and C [1/1]; Table S1). There was a human test helper (H) in room 2. S received one session consisting of 12 trials in which S could first see and assess the situation before E removed the latch from the horizontal sliding door to open up the choices for H. In a prearranged pattern, in half of the trials H chose the [1/1] option, and in the other half of trials H chose the [0/1] option. S had no way of influencing H’s choice and experienced that receiving a food reward or not depended on the choices made in room 2. This condition provided S with experience of the position her partner would be in at test.
In dependence experience 2, S were in room 2 like in the experimental conditions. H was in room 1 with the apparatus in the starting position. S received one session consisting of 12 trials in which H was given access to the vertical sliding door. There was one reward in option A ([1/0]), whereas there were two in both option B and C ([1/1]; Table S1). Hence, choosing option A meant that only H would get a reward, whereas making options B and C available resulted in one reward being delivered to both H and S regardless of S’ choice between B and C. In a prearranged pattern, H chose option A in half of the trials and made options B and C available in the other half. This condition made S experience that if she was located in room 2 she depended on the choices made in room 1, as would later be the case at test.
The procedure of dependence experience 1 and 2 was identical in all three experiments.
Experiment 2: Quantity preference.
In the quantity preference, S were in room 2 and the offer rope was not in place. Options B and C were closed with a latch without a rope. Sessions consisted of 12 trials in which the payoffs were [0/3] in option B and [0/4] in option C, or vice versa (B [0/4] and C [0/3]; Table S1). E removed the latch from the horizontal sliding door so that S could choose between options B and C, or, in other words, between three and four pieces of food. Criterion was met when S chose the four pieces of food in all 12 trials of one session or when S chose the four pieces of food in at least 10/12 trials in two consecutive sessions. Three S met criterion in the first session, two more S in two sessions, whereas one S (Frodo) initially did not show a quantity preference (Table S2); for him, the difference between options was increased (four vs. two in two sessions) to raise his motivation to pay attention to the payoffs and the food was elevated within the food cups so that the quantities were easier to see. This last S passed criterion (again with four vs. three) after five sessions, and for him the food rewards were then always elevated within the cups in all prerequisite and test conditions of experiment 2.
Table S2.
Subject | Kara | Kofi | Lobo | Lome | Frodo | Sandra |
Sex | ♀ | ♂ | ♂ | ♂ | ♂ | ♀ |
Age at start of E1 (y, mo) | 8, 11 | 9, 0 | 10, 2 | 12, 11 | 20, 9 | 21, 3 |
Rank in relation to partner* | Lower | Similar | Similar | Higher | Higher | Lower |
Social bond with partner* | Weak | Weak | Medium-weak | Weak | Medium-weak | Strong |
Genetic relationship with partner | Paternal half-sister | Paternal half-brother | Paternal half-brother | Paternal half-brother | Paternal half-brother | Sister |
Condition order in E1 and E2 | RA → NA | NA → RA | NA → RA | RA → NA | NA → RA | RA → NA |
No. of sessions to pass apparatus experience | 1 | 4 | 3 | 4 | 14 | 1 |
No. of sessions to pass one solution criterion in E1 | 3 | 2 | 12 | 9 | 18 | 7 |
No. of sessions to pass best solution criterion in E1 | 5 | 4 | 4 | 11 | 12 | 10 |
No. of trials with (2/2) choices in RA in E1 | 20/24 | 19/24 | 17/24 | 17/24 | 18/24 | 19/24 |
No. of trials with (2/2) choices in NA in E1 | 15/24 | 14/24 | 9/24 | 13/24 | 10/24 | 12/24 |
No. trials with choice of higher quantity in quantity preference before E2 | 12/12 | 21/24 | 21/24 | 12/12 | 20/24† | 12/12 |
No. of sessions to pass one solution criterion in E2 | 3 | 4 | 2 | 2 | 12 | 5 |
No. of sessions to pass best solution criterion in E2 | 2 | 2 | 2 | 9 | 11 | 6 |
No. of trials with (3/3) choices in RA in E2 | 9/24 | 12/24 | 9/24 | 12/24 | 13/24 | 8/24 |
No. of trials with (3/3) choices in NA in E2 | 4/24 | 2/24 | 4/24 | 3/24 | 10/24 | 2/24 |
No. trials with choice of higher quantity in quantity preference after E2 | 21/24 | 22/24 | 12/12 | 12/12 | 17/24 | 22/24 |
Condition order in E3 | RA → NA → NRA | NRA → NA → RA | NA → NRA → RA | RA → NRA → NA | NA → RA → NRA | NRA → RA → NA |
No. of sessions to pass one solution criterion in E3 | 2 | 2 | 2 | 4 | 10 | 8 |
No. of sessions to pass best solution criterion in E3 | 4 | 2 | 3 | 2 | 6 | 2 |
No. of sessions to pass attention criterion in E3 | 2 | 2 | 4 | 2 | 2 | 2 |
No. of trials with (2/2) choices in RA in E3 | 20/24 | 17/24 | 18/24 | 19/24 | 20/24 | 18/24 |
No. of trials with (2/2) choices in NRA in E3 | 15/24 | 16/24 | 15/24 | 14/24 | 22/24 | 15/24 |
No. of trials with (2/2) choices in NA in E3 | 10/24 | 12/24 | 13/24 | 11/24 | 20/24 | 11/24 |
Based on estimations by zookeepers.
Subject received five sessions. Results are shown for the last two sessions.
The quantity preference ensured that S could recognize the difference of four vs. three pieces of food and demonstrated that they preferred the bigger quantity. S received the quantity preference condition before the other prerequisite criteria and again after all testing of experiment 2 was finished to confirm that their preference for the larger amount of food was consistent and not influenced by the test (results in Table S2).
Experiment 3: Attention criterion.
S had access to rooms 1 and 2. Sessions consisted of 12 trials with the apparatus initially in the starting position. In option A there were either three ([3/0]) or no rewards ([0/0]), whereas there were two rewards in option B ([1/1]) and one in option C ([0/1]), or vice versa (B [0/1] and C [1/1]; Table S1). In half of the trials (A trials) it was correct to choose option A to maximize food rewards (three pieces), and in the other half of trials (B/C trials) it was correct to discard option A and choose either option B or C (two pieces). S passed criterion when they chose correctly in at least 10 of 12 trials in two consecutive sessions with the stipulation that there could not be more than one mistake in A trials and more than one mistake in B/C trials. All S met criterion.
The attention criterion was conducted between the best solution criterion and the dependence experience sessions; this ensured that S paid attention to the payoffs in option A, which was important for the experimental conditions.
Analysis.
All tests of condition differences were exact and two-tailed. Chance comparisons were based on binomial tests (two-tailed) and we used false discovery rates to control for multiple testing, which is standard procedure for keeping type 1 error rates at the nominal level of 0.05 when repeatedly testing-related hypotheses. To test for order effects, we fitted separate generalized linear mixed models with binomial error structure for each experiment. The test predictors were session number, trial number, and the interactions of these predictors with the experimental condition (allowing for the possibility that order effects may have been more prevalent in one of the conditions). We also included the random effects of subject ID (to control for the fact that we had multiple observations from each subject) as well as the random slopes of session and trial number nested within subject ID (57). We compared this full model to a null model not including the test predictors but retaining the main effect of condition, the random effect of subject ID, and the random slopes (58). This full-null model comparison was not significant for any of the three experiments [experiment 1, χ2(4) = 5.71, P = 0.222; experiment 2, χ2(4) = 5.31, P = 0.257; experiment 3, χ2(6) = 6.13, P = 0.409], suggesting that session number, trial number, or their interaction with condition did not significantly affect subject’s choices.
Discussion
The tendency to engage in costly behaviors aimed at benefiting others is a central component of human cooperation. The findings of the current study demonstrate experimentally that under some circumstances chimpanzees are also motivated to deliver benefits to conspecifics. In the absence of any prior interaction (NA conditions), chimpanzee subjects’ behavior was consistent with previous studies finding no prosociality in chimpanzees in similar prosocial choice paradigms (32–34). Indeed, when choosing the prosocial option was materially costly, subjects’ prosocial choices in the NA condition were close to zero (Fig. 2). In contrast, however, after they had received assistance from a partner, subjects provided their partner with resources in return, even at a material cost to themselves. Across the three experiments, subjects tended to reward risky assistance most often, nonrisky assistance somewhat less, and no assistance least often, suggesting that they understood the other's motivation and distinguished genuine prosociality from potentially self-serving behaviors.
We found material reciprocation where other researchers have not (43), most likely because we made sure before testing that subjects understood everything about the apparatus and the contingencies facing both the partner and themselves (SI Materials and Methods and Table S1). Given the careful counterbalancing scheme and as indicated by the session and trial number analysis, our findings cannot be explained by local enhancement, order effects, or learning. Moreover, previous research has demonstrated the importance of stable social bonds in primate cooperation (48–50). In the current experiments, however, increases in prosocial choices after receiving risky assistance were highly consistent across subjects who differed in rank and their social relationship with the partner (Table S2). These results are therefore unlikely to be explained by variation in prior long-term relationships (although future research should look at this more systematically). The current findings thus complement previous work highlighting the long-term nature of chimpanzee reciprocation of social services (14, 51) by showing that chimpanzees can also integrate recent single events into contingent cooperative short-term decisions, even when this involves making a material sacrifice.
One possibility is that chimpanzees strategically chose the prosocial option in RA conditions to motivate their partner to continue to provide assistance on future trials. However, if the prosocial option were only used to motivate future assistance, subjects’ behaviors should have been the same in the RA and NRA conditions in experiment 3. A more probable explanation is instead that chimpanzees’ choices were mediated by affiliative reactions in response to their partner’s cooperative efforts; this would correspond to theoretical accounts suggesting that chimpanzees may engage in some form of “emotional bookkeeping” such that individuals make social decisions based on emotional states associated with particular partners (14, 51, 52), which may also be consistent with empirical findings showing that chimpanzees show elevated oxytocin levels—a hormone involved in social bonding—in response to cooperative interactions (48, 53), and this may mediate individuals’ propensity to engage in cooperative activities with particular partners in the future. Hence, in the current experiments, subjects may have interpreted the partner’s assistance (and thus her decision to deviate from a payoff-maximizing strategy) as an invitation to contribute to a mutually desirable cooperative outcome (54), triggering positive affect toward the partner and motivating reciprocation.
It should further be noted that in contrast to previous experimental studies on animal reciprocity (39–42), there was a distinct cognitive dimension to the current task—that is, chimpanzees did not merely respond to the outcome of the partner’s actions but rather took into account their partner’s decisions (and decision alternatives). This finding is particularly clear in experiment 3 in which the partner’s actions and the outcomes of these actions were identical in both assistance conditions, but subjects tended to reward the partner more if they had incurred a risk in assisting them than when the same action could be interpreted as self-serving. This kind of consideration of a partner’s decision-making and thus her cooperative intentions as a basis for reciprocal prosocial behavior is a central contribution of the current study.
One limitation of this study was the small sample size; this was the result of the strict prerequisite criteria at training (SI Materials and Methods), which, while reducing the number of subjects permitted to the test, ensured that all tested subjects thoroughly understood the test situation and the consequences of their choices. Although the resulting small sample was unfortunate, we think the strict training protocol was a key advantage of the study and the clarity and consistency of the results across three separate experiments further strengthen our confidence in these findings. However, a valuable extension to the current study would be to conduct similar experiments with different groups of chimpanzees and, if possible, with larger samples. Furthermore, similar studies with bonobos—who display greater prosociality than chimpanzees in several contexts (55)—would be highly insightful for our understanding of the evolution of human cooperation and the sociocognitive makeup of humans’ last common ancestor with the Pan species.
Finally, the cooperative inclinations of chimpanzees revealed in the current experiments suggest that observed species differences in cooperative propensities between humans and chimpanzees may at least partly be due to factors other than the motivation to reward others' prosocial actions. For example, competition and lack of social tolerance appear to fundamentally constrain chimpanzees’ abilities to sustain cooperative interactions in some contexts (56), and it would be important to ascertain in how far these factors also impact their propensity to engage in material reciprocation. Here again, the comparison with bonobos would be instructive given that bonobos show considerably greater social tolerance and less intragroup aggression than chimpanzees (55).
In conclusion, the current findings demonstrate experimentally that chimpanzees are willing to incur a material cost to themselves to deliver a material reward to a conspecific, but they do this only if that conspecific previously incurred a risk to assist them. This finding suggests that some of the key motivations crucial to human cooperation may have deeper phylogenetic roots than previously suspected.
Materials and Methods
Subjects.
We initially started the test with 12 socially housed chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) at the Wolfgang Köhler Primate Research Center in Leipzig, Germany. Six test subjects met our prerequisites and advanced to the testing phases of all three studies (two females and four males; average age = 13.1 y; age range = 8.9–21.3 y at the beginning of data collection). For further details, see Table S2. We trained an additional female chimpanzee (age 11.8 y at the beginning of data collection, middle to low ranking) to act as a partner during the test (SI Materials and Methods). All subjects have previously participated in studies on cooperation; however, as far as we can ascertain, they have not been paired with the same partner in similar prosocial choice tasks.
Ethical Statement.
The study complied with the European and World Associations of Zoos and Aquariums Ethical Guidelines and was approved by the joint ethical committee of the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology and Leipzig Zoo. Chimpanzees were neither food- or water-deprived and could participate or refuse to participate in the study by their own choice.
Apparatus.
Between room 1 on the left and the opposite room 2 on the right, a table (70 × 90 cm) was placed that could be accessed from both sides through mesh panels. On the table, there were three different options, A, B, and C, that each held two round cups (diameter 8 cm). At the beginning of all trials in all conditions, all six cups were in the middle of the table, out of reach from the rooms. Each option could be chosen by pulling a string, resulting in one of the two cups to move toward room 1 and the other one toward room 2 (Fig. 1). Throughout the methods we will refer to rewards in the two cups of each option in square brackets (e.g., [1/0] or [1/1]) with the first number always referring to the cup accessible from room 1 and the second number to the cup accessible from room 2. Only one of the three options could be chosen per trial. Option A could only be accessed from room 1, whereas options B and C could only be chosen from room 2; to emphasize this, the two cups in option A were blue, and the four cups in option B and C were green. To pull the string attached to option A, a transparent vertical sliding door had to be operated. Moving the sliding door upwards made it possible to access option A while simultaneously precluding access to options B and C. If the sliding door was instead moved down, option A was blocked and a rope (the “offer rope”; 4.5 m long) running over a hook at the ceiling to the mesh panel of the opposite room became accessible. The rope was attached to a latch in a transparent horizontal sliding door that blocked access to options B and C from room 2. The latch was in the middle of the door and thus equally distant from both options. When the offer rope was pulled from room 1 long enough the latch was removed, which enabled subjects in room 2 to choose between options B and C (this action will henceforth be called “making options B and C available”). Making available options B and C was very salient: the rope had to be pulled for several seconds, which resulted in the latch conspicuously springing out of the horizontal sliding door and then swinging from the ceiling, and this ensured that a chimpanzee watching from room 2 would clearly see and hear the removal of the latch from the other side. Once subjects chose option B or C, the other option became inaccessible.
In the starting position, the vertical sliding door was in the middle position, the end of the offer rope was in place behind the vertical sliding door in room 1, and the attached latch blocked the horizontal sliding door in room 2 (Fig. 1A). All options, A, B, and C, were thus accessible when the appropriate choices were made on both sides. In room 1, subjects could either choose option A or they could discard option A and instead make options B and C available in room 2. In room 2, subjects could then choose between options B and C (Fig. 1B). Different payoffs of food rewards could be placed into the six cups to manipulate the optimal choices. A trial always started with the experimenter (E) removing a transparent Perspex cover in front of the mesh panel in room 1 to allow access to the vertical sliding door at option A. Access to the horizontal sliding door in room 2 was always unobstructed but with the latch in place, it could not be operated.
Procedure.
We used strict prerequisite criteria to ensure that only subjects with a thorough understanding of the apparatus contingencies and consequences of their choices were tested (for detailed descriptions of all prerequisite steps, see SI Materials and Methods and Table S1).
In all prerequisite criteria and experimental conditions, different trial types or different payoff locations were randomized and counterbalanced (with the stipulation that there were never more than two identical trials in a row) to preclude local enhancement and order effects. Rewards throughout all conditions were banana pellets. Individual data and number of sessions per subject of all conditions can be found in Table S2. All trials of all conditions were video recorded and live coded.
Basic Training Procedure Before Experiments.
We trained a chimpanzee partner (P) to always make options B and C available in room 2. Subjects (S) first experienced all parts of the apparatus and the different actions that could be performed on both sides (SI Materials and Methods). Six of 12 initial S managed to operate the apparatus and passed to experiment 1.
Experiment 1.
Prerequisite criteria.
S first had to pass the one solution criterion (SI Materials and Methods) in which the door between rooms was open so that they had access to both sides of the apparatus. In each trial, there was one piece of food in one of the six food cups, and S had to demonstrate that they understood how to operate the apparatus on both sides to access each of the available cups. The next step was the best solution criterion (SI Materials and Methods) in which the cups were baited equivalently to the later experimental conditions. Again, the door between rooms was open so that S could operate the apparatus on both sides. To pass this criterion, S had to demonstrate apparatus understanding by maximizing their food intake. All six S passed these criteria steps. Last, S received dependence experience 1 and 2 (SI Materials and Methods). Here, S experienced that whether they received food depended on the choices made by their partner in the opposite room (in dependence experience 1 S was in room 1 and a human partner in room 2, and the reverse was the case in dependence experience 2).
RA.
In RA, S were in room 2, and the trained conspecific P was in room 1. Option A always contained two food rewards [2/0], and either option B contained four [2/2] and option C two [0/2] or vice versa (B [0/2] and C [2/2]) (Table 1). The vertical door was rigged (SI Materials and Methods for details) so that P could never choose option A. However, this manipulation was invisible to S. With the apparatus in the starting position, P made options B and C available to S in each trial. S could then choose between one option delivering rewards only to themselves ([0/2]) and another option delivering rewards to both herself and P ([2/2]). Hence, S had the opportunity to provide a reward to her partner at no material cost to herself. Option B or C were the prosocial option in half of the trials of each session.
NA.
NA was equivalent to RA with regards to the positions of P and S as well as the payoffs. In contrast to RA, however, P did not perform any action. The vertical sliding door was already moved down and the string of option A was removed so that option A could (visibly for S) not be accessed by P. The offer rope was not attached; options B and C were closed with a latch without a rope. At the start of each trial, E waited a few seconds for S to see and assess the situation and then removed the latch from the horizontal sliding door to give S the choice between the [0/2] and the [2/2] option. In both conditions, the time interval between two trials was roughly a little over 1 min.
Subjects received two sessions of 12 trials per condition. Half of the S first received RA and then NA, whereas the other half completed the experiment in the reverse order. Between conditions, there was a test break of 17–20 d (median = 19 d) to avoid carryover effects between conditions. Whether a subject started with RA or NA was assigned randomly.
Experiment 2.
Prerequisite criteria.
In experiment 2 the same six S were tested again with the same P. S had to pass the same prerequisite criteria (one solution criterion, best solution criterion, dependence experience 1 and 2) to confirm thorough apparatus understanding. However, unlike in experiment 1, different payoffs were used at test and the best solution criterion was adjusted to match the new test payoffs. In addition, there were quantity preference sessions before and after testing to ensure that S could recognize the difference of four vs. three pieces of food and demonstrated that they preferred the bigger quantity (SI Materials and Methods). All six subjects passed all criteria.
RA and NA (Movies S1 and S2).
The procedures in both conditions were identical to the procedures in experiment 1 but the payoffs were different: option A always contained three rewards ([3/0]), whereas either option B contained [3/3] and option C [0/4], or vice versa (Table 1); this means that in experiment 2, S had to incur a cost to provide a benefit to their partner (i.e., by choosing [3/3] over [0/4]).
The same three S as in experiment 1 first received the RA and then NA, whereas the other half completed the experiment in the reverse order. Between conditions, there was a test break of 16–19 d (median = 17 d) to avoid carryover effects.
Experiment 3.
Prerequisite criteria.
In experiment 3, again the same six S were tested with the same P. S had to pass the same prerequisite criteria with the same payoffs as in experiment 1 (one solution criterion, best solution criterion, dependence experience 1 and 2) to confirm thorough apparatus understanding. There was also an additional attention criterion (SI Materials and Methods) between the best solution criterion and dependence conditions in which S had access to both rooms. In some trials S had to choose option A, whereas in the other trials they had to reject option A to make options B and C available to maximize their food intake. S were thus trained to pay additional attention to the payoffs in option A, which was crucial for the subsequent experimental conditions. All six subjects passed all criteria. In addition to RA and NA, there was also a nonrisky assistance condition.
RA.
RA was identical in every aspect to RA in experiment 1 (see above).
NRA.
NRA was identical to RA with the critical difference that no rewards were present in option A ([0/0]; Table 1). P therefore performed the same actions as in RA but from the point of view of S she did not risk losing food by discarding option A.
NA.
NA was identical to NA in experiment 1 (see above) except for one difference: whereas P remained passive, E now made options B and C available to S by mimicking P’s actions in RA and NRA. At the start of each trial, E pulled the offer rope himself to remove the latch. Functionally this condition was identical to NA in experiments 1 and 2 (P was only present and could not act at all, whereas E opened up the choice for S). However, the timing as well as the perceptual information that S received in the new NA procedure closely matched the RA and NRA conditions. It thus controlled for the possibility that S had learned (for unspecified reasons) to only choose prosocially (i.e., the [2/2] option) when the offer rope was used to remove the latch.
Each S received the three conditions in a different order, assigned randomly. As previously, test breaks of 15–19 d (median = 16.5 d) were introduced between conditions to avoid carryover effects.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank R. Pieszek for help with the apparatus and S. Tüpke for help with the figures.
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1700351114/-/DCSupplemental.
References
- 1.Henrich J, et al. “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behav Brain Sci. 2005;28:795–815, discussion 815–855. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Fehr E, Fischbacher U. The nature of human altruism. Nature. 2003;425:785–791. doi: 10.1038/nature02043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Gintis H, Bowles S, Boyd R, Fehr E, editors. Moral Sentiments and Material Interests. MIT Press; Cambridge, MA: 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 4.West SA, Griffin AS, Gardner A. Social semantics: Altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. J Evol Biol. 2007;20:415–432. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01258.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Prüfer K, et al. The bonobo genome compared with the chimpanzee and human genomes. Nature. 2012;486:527–531. doi: 10.1038/nature11128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Muller MN, Mitani JC. Conflict and cooperation in wild chimpanzees. In: Slater P, et al., editors. Advances in the Study of Behavior. Vol 35. Academic; Cambridge, MA: 2005. pp. 275–331. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Duffy KG, Wrangham RW, Silk JB. Male chimpanzees exchange political support for mating opportunities. Curr Biol. 2007;17:R586–R587. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gomes CM, Mundry R, Boesch C. Long-term reciprocation of grooming in wild West African chimpanzees. Proc Biol Sci. 2009;276:699–706. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.1324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mitani JCC, Watts DP. Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat? Anim Behav. 2001;61:915–924. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Gomes CM, Boesch C. Wild chimpanzees exchange meat for sex on a long-term basis. PLoS One. 2009;4:e5116. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.de Waal FBM. Food sharing and reciprocal obligations among chimpanzees. J Hum Evol. 1989;18:433–459. [Google Scholar]
- 12.de Waal FBM. The chimpanzee’s service economy: Food for grooming. Evol Hum Behav. 1997;18:375–386. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Jaeggi AV, Stevens JMG, Van Schaik CP. Tolerant food sharing and reciprocity is precluded by despotism among bonobos but not chimpanzees. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2010;143:41–51. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.21288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Jaeggi AV, De Groot E, Stevens JMG, Van Schaik CP. Mechanisms of reciprocity in primates: Testing for short-term contingency of grooming and food sharing in chimpanzees and bonobos. Evol Hum Behav. 2013;34:69–77. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Tan J, Hare B. Bonobos share with strangers. PLoS One. 2013;8:e51922. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Surbeck M, Hohmann G. Social preferences influence the short-term exchange of social grooming among male bonobos. Anim Cogn. 2015;18:573–579. doi: 10.1007/s10071-014-0826-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Tokuyama N, Furuichi T. Do friends help each other? Patterns of female coalition formation in wild bonobos at Wamba. Anim Behav. 2016;119:27–35. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Yamamoto S. Non-reciprocal but peaceful fruit sharing in wild bonobos in Wamba. Behaviour. 2015;152:335–357. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Clutton-Brock T. Cooperation between non-kin in animal societies. Nature. 2009;462:51–57. doi: 10.1038/nature08366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Gilby IC. Meat sharing among the Gombe chimpanzees: Harassment and reciprocal exchange. Anim Behav. 2006;71:953–963. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Stevens JR, Stephens DW. Food sharing: A model of manipulation by harassment. Behav Ecol. 2002;13:393–400. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Brosnan SF, de Waal FBM. A proximate perspective on reciprocal altruism. Hum Nat. 2002;13:129–152. doi: 10.1007/s12110-002-1017-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Schino G, Aureli F. The relative roles of kinship and reciprocity in explaining primate altruism. Ecol Lett. 2010;13:45–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2009.01396.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Stevens JR, Hauser MD. Why be nice? Psychological constraints on the evolution of cooperation. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8:60–65. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Silk JB, Brosnan SF, Henrich J, Lambeth SP, Shapiro SJ. Chimpanzees share food for many reasons: The role of kinship, reciprocity, social bonds and harassment on food transfers. Anim Behav. 2013;85:941–947. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Jensen K. The prosocial primate – a critical review. Adv Stud Behav. 2016;48:387–441. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Warneken F, Hare B, Melis AP, Hanus D, Tomasello M. Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young children. PLoS Biol. 2007;5:e184. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Yamamoto S, Humle T, Tanaka M. Chimpanzees’ flexible targeted helping based on an understanding of conspecifics’ goals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:3588–3592. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1108517109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Melis AP, et al. Chimpanzees help conspecifics obtain food and non-food items. Proc Biol Sci. 2011;278:1405–1413. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1735. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Schmelz M, Call J. The psychology of primate cooperation and competition: A call for realigning research agendas. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016;371:20150067. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Tennie C, Jensen K, Call J. The nature of prosociality in chimpanzees. Nat Commun. 2016;7:13915. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Silk JB, et al. Chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelated group members. Nature. 2005;437:1357–1359. doi: 10.1038/nature04243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Jensen K, Hare B, Call J, Tomasello M. What’s in it for me? Self-regard precludes altruism and spite in chimpanzees. Proc Biol Sci. 2006;273:1013–1021. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Vonk J, et al. Chimpanzees do not take advantage of very low cost opportunities to deliver food to unrelated group members. Anim Behav. 2008;75:1757–1770. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Claidière N, et al. Selective and contagious prosocial resource donation in capuchin monkeys, chimpanzees and humans. Sci Rep. 2015;5:7631. doi: 10.1038/srep07631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.House BR, Silk JB, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ. Task design influences prosociality in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) PLoS One. 2014;9:e103422. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Trivers RL. The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol. 1971;46:35–57. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Axelrod R, Hamilton WD. The evolution of cooperation. Science. 1981;211:1390–1396. doi: 10.1126/science.7466396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Wilkinson GS. Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat. Nature. 1984;308:181–184. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Carter GG, Wilkinson GS. Food sharing in vampire bats: Reciprocal help predicts donations more than relatedness or harassment. Proc Biol Sci. 2013;280:20122573. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Rutte C, Taborsky M. Generalized reciprocity in rats. PLoS Biol. 2007;5:e196. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Suchak M, de Waal FB. Monkeys benefit from reciprocity without the cognitive burden. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:15191–15196. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1213173109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Brosnan SF, et al. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) do not develop contingent reciprocity in an experimental task. Anim Cogn. 2009;12:587–597. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0218-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Yamamoto S, Tanaka M. Do chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) spontaneously take turns in a reciprocal cooperation task? J Comp Psychol. 2009;123:242–249. doi: 10.1037/a0015838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Tan J, Kwetuenda S, Hare B. Preference or paradigm? Bonobos show no evidence of other-regard in the standard prosocial choice task. Behaviour. 2015;152:521–544. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Melis AP, Hare B, Tomasello M. Do chimpanzees reciprocate received favours? Anim Behav. 2008;76:951–962. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc B. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Wittig RM, et al. Food sharing is linked to urinary oxytocin levels and bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proc Biol Sci. 2014;281:20133096. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.3096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Silk JB, et al. The benefits of social capital: Close social bonds among female baboons enhance offspring survival. Proc Biol Sci. 2009;276:3099–3104. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. The evolutionary origins of friendship. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:153–177. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Schino G, Aureli F. Reciprocal altruism in primates: partner choice, cognition and emotions. In: Brockmann HJ, et al., editors. Advances in the Study of Behavior. Vol 39. Academic; Cambridge, MA: 2009. pp. 45–69. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Tomasello M. A Natural History of Human Morality. Harvard Univ Press; Cambridge, MA: 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Crockford C, et al. Urinary oxytocin and social bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proc Biol Sci. 2013;280:20122765. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Kabalak A, Smirnova E, Jost J. Non-cooperative game theory in biology and cooperative reasoning in humans. Theory Biosci. 2015;134:17–46. doi: 10.1007/s12064-015-0210-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Hare B. Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annu Rev Psychol. 2017;68:155–186. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Melis AP, Hare B, Tomasello M. Engineering cooperation in chimpanzees: Tolerance constraints on cooperation. Anim Behav. 2006;72:275–286. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Schielzeth H, Forstmeier W. Conclusions beyond support: Overconfident estimates in mixed models. Behav Ecol. 2009;20:416–420. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arn145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Forstmeier W, Schielzeth H. Cryptic multiple hypotheses testing in linear models: Overestimated effect sizes and the winner’s curse. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2011;65:47–55. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1038-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.