Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2017 Jul 19;6:e25533. doi: 10.7554/eLife.25533

Ephrin-A/EphA specific co-adaptation as a novel mechanism in topographic axon guidance

Felix Fiederling 1, Markus Weschenfelder 1, Martin Fritz 1, Anne von Philipsborn 1,, Martin Bastmeyer 1, Franco Weth 1,*
Editor: David D Ginty2
PMCID: PMC5517148  PMID: 28722651

Abstract

Genetic hardwiring during brain development provides computational architectures for innate neuronal processing. Thus, the paradigmatic chick retinotectal projection, due to its neighborhood preserving, topographic organization, establishes millions of parallel channels for incremental visual field analysis. Retinal axons receive targeting information from quantitative guidance cue gradients. Surprisingly, novel adaptation assays demonstrate that retinal growth cones robustly adapt towards ephrin-A/EphA forward and reverse signals, which provide the major mapping cues. Computational modeling suggests that topographic accuracy and adaptability, though seemingly incompatible, could be reconciled by a novel mechanism of coupled adaptation of signaling channels. Experimentally, we find such ‘co-adaptation’ in retinal growth cones specifically for ephrin-A/EphA signaling. Co-adaptation involves trafficking of unliganded sensors between the surface membrane and recycling endosomes, and is presumably triggered by changes in the lipid composition of membrane microdomains. We propose that co-adaptative desensitization eventually relies on guidance sensor translocation into cis-signaling endosomes to outbalance repulsive trans-signaling.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.001

Research Organism: Chicken

eLife digest

The human brain contains roughly 100 billion neurons, which are organized into complex networks. But how does the brain establish these networks in the first place? Neurons have long projections known as axons and, in the developing brain, these axons form structures called growth cones at their tips. The growth cones possess finger-like appendages that probe their surroundings in search of signals displayed on the surface of other cells. These signals guide the growth cones to their targets and move the axon tip into a position where it can form connections with other neurons within a particular network.

The signals that growth cones follow are often distributed in concentration gradients so that the levels of a signal may be low at one end of a brain structure and gradually increase to a maximum level at the other end. In the developing visual system, for example, about one million axons from the retina reach their proper targets in visual regions of the brain by reading gradients of signals called ephrins and Ephs. However, when Fiederling et al. studied retinal neurons in a petri dish, they found that the axons became much less sensitive to both signals upon prolonged exposure to them. This unexpected finding raised a new question. If neurons rely upon these gradients for navigation, how do they continue to find their way if they also become less sensitive to those signals over time?

Fiederling et al. used a computer to simulate the events occurring in the developing brain. The simulations were based on the idea that navigating growth cones sense the ratio of ephrins to Ephs, instead of sensing the individual concentrations of these signals. Thus, by keeping the amounts of all involved sensors in strict proportion to each other while continuously re-adjusting them, the axons could still be accurately guided to their targets even though the neurons would become less sensitive to the signals. Experiments in neurons grown in petri dishes confirmed that retinal growth cones do exactly this and regulate the amounts of ephrin and Eph sensors on their outer membranes in a highly coordinated manner using a previously unknown mechanism.

Given that signaling requires energy, the brain may have evolved this system to reduce the costs associated with wiring itself up. The system also offers greater flexibility than guidance based on the absolute concentrations of the signals. If other regions of the brain use a similar mechanism to establish their own wiring patterns, then understanding such basic mechanisms might eventually provide insights into diseases of miswiring such as schizophrenia and autism.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.002

Introduction

The dazzling diversity of functions of the nervous system are brought about by neural networks of definite connectivity, most of which arise during ontogenesis through targeted outgrowth of axons. This is accomplished by growth cones (GCs) at the tips of growing axons (Lowery and Van Vactor, 2009; Vitriol and Zheng, 2012), which sense genetically encoded chemotactic guidance cues (Dickson, 2002; Kolodkin and Tessier-Lavigne, 2011). Such genetic hardwiring endows the individual at prefunctional stages with computational architectures, which have been fitted for survival through the evolution of the whole species.

A well-studied example is the development of the retinotectal projection (Figure 1), which, in non-mammalian vertebrates, connects the retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) of the eye with the midbrain’s optic tectum in a topographic, i.e., neighborhood-preserving manner (Feldheim and O'Leary, 2010; Lemke and Reber, 2005; Weth et al., 2014). Research on this two-dimensional mapping has mainly focused on the anterior-posterior axis, whereby the temporal retina is projected onto the anterior tectum and the nasal retina onto the posterior tectum. The major guidance cues along this axis are EphA receptor tyrosine kinases and their glycosylphosphatidylinositol-(GPI)-anchored ephrin-A ligands (Suetterlin et al., 2012; Triplett and Feldheim, 2012). EphAs and ephrin-As are counter-graded on both the retina and the tectum (temporal retina — EphA high, ephrin-A low; anterior tectum — EphA high, ephrin-A low [McLaughlin and O'Leary, 2005]). The ephrin-A/EphA system can signal in forward (EphA acting as receptor) and in reverse (ephrin-A as receptor) directions (Egea and Klein, 2007; Kania and Klein, 2016; Lisabeth et al., 2013). Both signaling channels act repulsively on RGC GCs. Receptor/ligand interactions typically occur in trans, between different cells, but, when present on the same cell, as on RGCs, they can additionally happen in cis (Hornberger et al., 1999). We have recently proposed a simple but powerful comprehensive computational model (Gebhardt et al., 2012) that includes all possible ephrin-A/EphA interactions in this system (fiber–target, fiber–fiber, cis, each forward and reverse). The model assumes that axon targeting aims to balance all summed reverse signals against all summed forward signals sensed by the GC. There have been diverse further attempts to explain retinotopic mapping (Hjorth et al., 2015; Simpson et al., 2009). Most of them agree, however, that topographic guidance is based on quantitative signaling, whereby the concentrations of sensors and cues bear the topographic information. This assumption has also been corroborated by experimental evidence (Baier and Bonhoeffer, 1992; Hansen et al., 2004; Rosentreter et al., 1998; von Philipsborn et al., 2006b).

Figure 1. The retinotectal projection.

Figure 1.

The topographic projection in the chicken visual system connects RGCs from the retina to the midbrain's optic tectum. The temporal/nasal (t/n) axis of the retina is mapped onto the anterior/posterior (a/p) axis of the tectum, whereas the retinal dorsal/ventral axis projects onto the lateral/medial axis of the tectum. Retinal GCs are guided to their tectal targets by repulsive signals from counter-gradient distributions of tectal ephrin-As (red; p>a) and EphAs (blue; a>p). Detection of these cues is mediated via retinal EphA and ephrin-A sensors, which are expressed in t>n and n>t gradients, respectively.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.003

On the other hand, it has been shown repeatedly that GCs can adapt to chemotactic guidance cues. Xenopus spinal GCs, in turning assays, can adapt to attraction by soluble Netrin-1 or brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (Ming et al., 2002). Ligand-specific desensitization was correlated with decreased Ca2+ signaling, whereas re-sensitization required MAP-kinase activation and local protein synthesis. Xenopus retinal axons in collapse assays were shown to adapt towards the collapse-inducing activities of soluble Sema3A and Netrin-1 (Piper et al., 2005). Here, ligand-specific desensitization and re-sensitization, depended on endocytosis and protein translation, respectively. Notably, however, none of these guidance cues has been involved in topographic mapping. Indeed, it is difficult to imagine how adaptive signal modulation might be compatible with the need for quantitative signaling. First evidence, however, indicates that retinal GCs might in fact be able to adapt to topographic cues (Rosentreter et al., 1998; von Philipsborn et al., 2006b). We therefore decided to investigate this conundrum in more detail.

Introducing a novel adaptation assay (‘gap assay’), we demonstrate that chick retinal GCs adapt towards both ephrin-A forward and EphA reverse topographic signals. Led by computational modeling, we postulate a distinctive novel type of signal modulation (‘co-adaptation’) to reconcile adaptation and topographic accuracy. We prove experimentally that such ephrin-A/EphA specific co-adaptation is indeed realized in retinal GCs. Using SNAP-tagged ephrin-A5 to label sensor surface populations, pharmacological inhibition, and co-localization studies with Rab11, we show that co-adaptation on the cellular level involves trafficking of the guidance sensors between the GC membrane and the recycling endosome. By inducing adaptation by altering the lipid composition of the GC membrane, we provide evidence that repartitioning of guidance sensors between membrane microdomains might drive co-adaptation downstream of ephrin-A/EphA signaling. Eventually we propose, on the basis of our computational model, a hypothetical mechanism of co-adaptation, suggesting that an increase in the efficiency of cis-signaling from endosomes eventually desensitizes GCs against trans-signaling, without altering their topographic identity.

Results

Retinal GCs adapt towards topographic EphA forward and ephrin-A reverse signals

In in-vitro collapse assays (Cox et al., 1990; Kapfhammer et al., 2007) typically ~80% of temporal GCs show a collapsed morphology after 20 min incubation with 0.25 μg/ml ephrin-A5-Fc, whereas control (Fc fragment)-treated GCs mostly remain intact (Figure 2A,B). Surprisingly, however, we observe that temporal GCs recover their morphology, despite the presence of the repulsive cue, after prolonged incubation. After 120 min of incubation with ephrin-A5-Fc, the fraction of collapsed GCs drops from 77.1% (20min) to 32.9% and, thus, to control levels (20 min Fc: 23.3%; 120 min: 27.2%; Figure 2A). This recovery is not caused by a loss of activity of the repulsive cue, as a medium containing 0.25 μg/ml ephrin-A5-Fc that has been on a first culture for 120 min triggers full response when reused on a second culture for 20 min (81.5% collapsed temporal GCs; Figure 2A). Therefore, recovery in the presence of ephrin-A indicates desensitization of RGC GCs towards forward signals.

Figure 2. Growth cone desensitization towards soluble ephrin-A5 and EphA3.

(A) Temporal GCs initially collapse upon application of 250 ng/ml ephrin-A5-Fc (eA5 20 min: 77.1%), but recover their morphology within 120 min in the presence of the cue (eA5 120 min: 32.9% collapsed GCs). Ephrin-A5-Fc is still functional after incubation for 120 min, when re-used on a fresh culture (eA5 recycled 20 min: 81.5% collapsed). Fc controls demonstrate the specificity of the effect of ephrin-A5 (Fc 20 min: 23.3%; Fc recycled 20 min: 20.7%; Fc 120 min: 27.2%). (B) Representative intact and collapsed GC morphology (Phalloidin Alexa488). Scale bar: 10 µm. (C) Soluble EphA3-Fc triggers a collapse when administered at 15 µg/ml (EA3 20 min: 64.4% collapsed), whereas an equimolar concentration of Fc (4.2 µg/ml) does not (Fc 20 min: 24.3%). GCs desensitize towards EphA3-Fc within 120 min (EA3 120 min: 22.6%). Combined data from nasal and temporal GCs. N: number of independent experiments; number of analyzed GCs in brackets. Error bars represent standard deviations. T-test with n.s.: α ≥0.05, ***: α <0.001.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.004

Figure 2—source data 1. Original data underlying bar charts of Figure 2A, C.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.005

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. EphA3 collapse assays and ephrin-A5/EphA3 dissociation constants.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Moderate concentrations of EphA3-Fc (EA3) do not trigger a collapse of nasal or temporal GCs, neither in dimeric nor in an antibody clustered form (Fc, 1 µg/ml, 20 min: 16.1% collapsed; EA3, 2 µg/ml, 5 min: 15.3%; 10 min: 17.3%; 30 min: 22.4%; EA3, 5 µg/ml antibody-clustered (C), 20 min: 21.7% collapsed). Antibody-induced clustering was achieved by preincubation of EphA3 with anti human Alexa Fluor488 goat IgG (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA) in a molar ratio of 2:1 (EphA3:AB) for 1 hr at room temperature. N: number of independent experiments; number of analyzed GCs in brackets. Error bars represent standard deviations. (B) Concentration-dependent binding of ephrin-A5 to EphA3 (see Materials and methods for details). Dissociation constants (KD) of ephrin-A5 and EphA3 decrease abruptly with increasing EphA3 concentrations, indicating a sharp increase in binding affinity, which is potentially due to the concentration-dependent formation of EphA3 clusters in solution. KDs ephrin-A5 binding to: EphA3 3 µg/ml: 36.68 nM; EphA3 15 µg/ml: 0.76 nM; EphA3 33 µg/ml: 1.23 nM; EphA3 100 µg/ml: 0.55 nM. (C) Potential EphA3 clusters are stable for at least one day in solution at a concentration of 11 µg/ml as indicated by a constant KD (fresh EphA3, 11 µg/ml: 3.37 nM; 1 day old EphA3, 11 µg/ml: 3.40 nM). (B, C) N: number of independent experiments. Error bars represent standard deviations. T-test with n.s.: α ≥0.05; *: α <0.05.
Figure 2—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Original data underlying bar charts of Figure 2—figure supplement 1.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.007

To investigate reverse signaling desensitization, we first established EphA3 collapse assays. At high concentrations (15 µg/ml), a specific and significant collapse is triggered (64.4% of GCs; equimolar Fc: 24.3% collapsed; Figure 2C), but this does not occur at lower concentrations (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A). We suggest that the exclusive response to high EphA concentrations is due to an exclusive activity of EphA3 oligomers (n > 2), which might form in concentrated solutions only. Supporting evidence comes from measuring ephrin-A5/EphA3 affinity by biolayer interferometry. Remarkably, binding constants depend on the EphA3 concentration and are dramatically increased at high concentrations (≥15 µg/ml; Figure 2—figure supplement 1B). The assumed oligomers remain stable in solution for at least 1 day (Figure 2—figure supplement 1C). Notably, but similar to the response to ephrin-A5, GCs completely recover after prolonged incubation with EphA3-Fc (22.6% collapsed after 120 min; Fc controls after 120 min: 21.6%; Figure 2C), demonstrating GC desensitization also towards EphA3 reverse signals.

Full adaptability comprises desensitization and re-sensitization. To address the latter and to better approximate the in-vivo situation regarding surface-bound cues, we developed a novel in-vitro adaptation assay, the ‘gap assay’. To this end, we manufactured patterned guidance substrates by protein contact printing (von Philipsborn et al., 2006a), which consisted of two rectangular fields homogeneously covered with the cue and separated by a permissive gap (laminin) of defined width. To ensure the activity of the printed cue, positive controls are performed in which axons grow from homogeneous laminin towards an identical field of the cue. For ephrin-A5-Fc, naїve temporal axons show a robust stop reaction at the boundary to the ephrin field in such controls (98% stopping; Figure 3A). Negative controls using a corresponding field of human Fc fragment (28.8% stopping) demonstrate the specificity of the response (Figure 3C). By contrast, axons starting from a field of homogeneous ephrin-A5-Fc no longer stop and continue growing onto the second ephrin field if the gap is small (Figure 3A'), revealing their desensitization in agreement with the collapse assays. Notably, however, the proportion of GCs that stop in front of the second ephrin field increases with increasing gap sizes. Only 15.3% of temporal axons stop in assays with 50 µm wide gaps, but 43.0% stop after 75 µm, 59.2% after 100 µm and 81.0% after 200 µm wide gaps (Figure 3A'' and C), indicating that GCs regain sensitivity towards forward signaling while growing on the permissive gap.

Figure 3. Growth cone adaptation towards substrate-bound ephrin-A5 and EphA3.

Figure 3.

Subfigures in (A) and (B) each display a cartoon (left) illustrating the experimental setup consisting of explant (thick black strip), axons and printed guidance protein (colored field(s)), the inverted signal of fluorescent phalloidin-stained axonal actin (middle; explant not shown) and the underlying, antibody-labeled substrate (right) are shown in a detailed view of a representative microscopic image (scale: 100 µm). (A) Naïve temporal axons stop in front of a homogeneous field of ephrin-A5-Fc (eA5 15 µg/ml; red), but do not react to an identical boundary when initially grown on ephrin-A5 after a 65 µm wide gap (A'). Stopping behavior returns in axons having crossed a 215 µm wide, ephrin-free gap (A''). (B–B'') Nasal RGC axons show very similar behavior on EphA3-Fc (EA3 15 µg/ml, blue) gap substrates as described for ephrin-A5 gap assays. (C) Quantification of gap assays. Stop reactions were quantified as the percentage of fibers not entering the protein field after the gap. Fc naïve: 28.8% stopping. eA5 naïve: 98.0%, eA5 50 µm: 15.3%, eA5 75 µm: 43.0%, eA5 100 µm: 59.2%, eA5 200 µm: 81.0% stopping. EA3 naïve: 92.6%, EA3 65 µm: 25.0%, EA3 90 µm: 39.9%, EA3 115 µm: 60.1%, EA3 215 µm: 75.8% stopping. Ephrin-A5 gap assay patterns differ slightly from EphA3 patterns in gap width as a result of a modified stamp geometry. N: number of independent experiments. Error bars represent standard deviations. T-test with *: α <0.05, **: α <0.01, ***: α <0.001.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.008

Figure 3—source data 1. Original data underlying bar chart of Figure 3C.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.009

In reverse signaling gap assays, naїve nasal RGC GCs show a clear stop reaction at the boundary to a field of contact printed EphA3 (92.6% stopping; Figure 3B). GCs no longer stop in front of an identical field after a small gap when starting on EphA3 (65 µm: 25.0% stopping; Figure 3B'), indicating desensitization towards reverse signals in accordance with the collapse assays. As in the forward signaling gap assays, they gradually regain sensitivity with increasing gap sizes (90 µm: 39.9% stopping; 115 µm: 60.1% stopping; 215 µm: 75.8% stopping; Figure 3B'' and C), demonstrating re-sensitization towards reverse signaling. Re-sensitization of forward and reverse signaling in gap assays displays very similar dependencies on the overgrown distance hinting at a potential common mechanism.

Together, our results show that topographically mapping retinal GCs robustly adapt towards forward and reverse ephrin-A/EphA signaling. This is puzzling, because topographic guidance is believed to rely on precise quantitative sensing.

Modeling suggests co-adaptation as a novel mechanism for reconciling growth cone adaptation and topographic accuracy

To conceive how adaptation and topography could be reconciled, we made use of our previous computational model (Gebhardt et al., 2012). Briefly, the model (Figure 4A) is exclusively based on ephrin/Eph signaling and assumes that GCs do not distinguish the diverse sources of the impinging signals (target cells, other fibers, GC’s own surface). They just sense total reverse and total forward signaling. GC targeting is then considered as a potential minimization process, comparable to a sphere rolling into a well. To describe this process, a ‘guidance potential’, D, is defined, which is conceived to be proportional to the ratio of total reverse to total forward signaling and to be minimized when both are equal, indicating target arrival. The effective strengths of the signals received by an individual GC depend on its endowment with sensors (ephrins for reverse and Ephs for forward signaling). Thus, the GC’s own ephrin/Eph ratio corresponds to its topographic imprint, determining where on the target area reverse/forward signaling balance will be reached. Therefore, to retain topography in the face of adaptation, which is expected to modulate signal strength, this ratio must remain basically unaltered. In the model, this is achieved by introducing the same multiplicative adaptation factor, a, simultaneously to both reverse and forward signaling. For more mathematical detail see 'Material and methods'.

Figure 4. Modeling growth cone adaptation and topographic mapping.

(A) Fiber terminals are modeled as circular discs bearing Gaussian-shaped distributions of EphAs (RF, blue) and ephrin-As (LF, red), according to their retinal origin, moving on a rectangular target field of unit squares (xT, yT), carrying target-derived guidance cues (LT and RT). Fiber–target and fiber–fiber (cis and trans) interactions and the resulting forward and reverse signals integrate into a guidance potential, D (equation1), which determines the probability, p, of a change in position (only fiber–target interactions are illustrated in the inset). Additionally, D is used to calculate an adaptation coefficient, a (equation2), which simultaneously modulates RF and LF (collectively called S; equation4) through 'co-adaptation'. A resetting force, f (equation3), counteracts a. For mathematical detail see 'Material and methods'. (B) Mapping plots of simulations with 200 fiber terminals using different implementations of adaptation. The anterior-posterior (a-p) position of a terminal on the target is plotted as a function of naso-temporal (n-t) origin (perfect topography is indicated by all terminals targeting the main diagonal). Terminals that are enabled to regulate sensors independently from each other by canonical adaptation are widely scattered across the target field (white circles), whereas co-adapted terminals (regulating both sensors concomitantly) find their topographically correct target positions (gray circles). In the basic model, the potential minimum is at a position where fiber and target ephrin/Eph concentrations match. Therefore, canonical adaptation was implemented in terms of a tendency for GCs to independently match their forward and reverse sensor levels to the receptor and ligand levels, respectively, on the current target position. (C) After implementation of co-adaptation, naïve terminals stop in front of a field of high ephrin-A or EphA (LT = 4, red; RT = 4, blue), respectively, but ignore the same boundary in simulated gap assays with small gap size. In simulations with wider gaps, terminals stop again in front of the second field. Gap size = 20, 40, or 100 units respectively; n = 12; i = 2000; target field: 200 × 8. (D) Co-adaptation predicts that ephrin-A adapted terminals will ignore a field of high EphA after a small gap and vice versa. Gap size = 20; other parameters as in (C).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.010

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Inclusion of co-adaptation does not alter the explanatory power of the computational model.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

For purposes of comparison, this figure, produced using the updated model that includes co-adaptation presented in this paper is structured in parallel to figure 7 in Gebhardt et al. (2012), which did not take adaptation into account. The results are highly similar, demonstrating that the inclusion of co-adaptation does not alter the performance of the original model. (A) 200 simulations of single fibers chosen randomly from the retinal field. Consistent with the experimental evidence in zebrafish, single fibers map topographically, as indicated by the accumulation of terminals on the main diagonal of the mapping plot. (B) EphA3 knock-in, Rki, in every second fiber (blue; ki/+: Rki = 2; ki/ki: Rki = 4; axonal ligand on knock-in fibers was reduced reciprocally to the axonal receptor). Consistent with the experiment, wild-type fibers (gray) are posteriorly displaced, with a more severe effect in homozygotes. (C) Map expansion. 100 fibers chosen from the nasal (n) or temporal (t) half of the retina reach their correct target (yellow) after 3000 iterations. After 30,000 iterations, fibers stably cover the whole tectum (gray). (D) No expansion occurs (gray) when a n half retina grows into a field containing the remnants of a previous full innervation (white). (E) Map compression. 200 fibers from a whole retina form a compressed topographic projection when growing into an anterior tectum half. (F) Mismatch. 100 n fibers form a half-projection, when growing into an anterior, non-matching, tectal half. (G) An additional n projection (gray) displays polarity reversal when growing into a tectal field still containing properly mapped n fibers (white) (n = 100; fiber–fiber interactions were put into effect earlier than usual, i.e., j = 2000 in C(i)).

After implementing this form of concomitant adaptation of two signaling channels, which we term co-adaptation, the model is still able to form an accurate topographic map (Figure 4B, gray circles). By contrast, uncoupled adaptation by independent negative feedback to each channel completely abolishes map formation (Figure 4B, white circles). The updated model reproduces the experimental results of the gap assays (Figure 4C). This is achieved without changing the model’s global explanatory power, described earlier (Figure 4—figure supplement 1; Gebhardt et al., 2012; Weth et al., 2014).

The co-adaptation concept predicts that each channel should be modulated upon its own activation, but counter-intuitively also upon activation of the parallel channel even in the absence of its own ligand. Thus, ephrin-adapted axons should ignore Eph in a ‘double-cue’ gap assay with small gap size. Similarly, Eph-adapted axons should ignore ephrin (simulations in Figure 4D).

EphA forward signaling actually co-adapts ephrin-A reverse signaling and vice versa

To test this prediction, we used double-cue gap assays with substrates comprising different cues on either side of the gap. In ephrin-A5/EphA3 double-cue gap assays, ephrin-A5-adapted temporal GCs, after a small gap (<100 µm), ignore the EphA3 field (26.9% stopping), in front of which they naïvely stop (93.4% stopping; Figure 5A,A' and D). As these GCs have not experienced substrate-derived reverse signals before, their desensitization towards these signals can only be explained through co-regulation of reverse and forward signaling upon activation of the forward channel.

Figure 5. Co-adaptation of retinal growth cones in double-cue gap assays.

(A–C) Naïve axons stop in front of a homogeneous field of EphA3-Fc (A; EA3, blue; nasal axons), ephrin-A5-Fc (B; eA5, red; temporal axons), or Sema3A-Fc (C; S3A, green; temporal axons). Scale: 100 μm. (A'–C') Nasal, eA5-adapted GCs ignore a field of EA3 after a small gap (A') and, vice versa, temporal, EA3-adapted GCs ignore eA5 (B'). By contrast, temporal, eA5-adapted GCs are still strongly repelled by a field of S3A after a small gap, indicating specific co-adaptation of ephrin-A and EphA signals (C'). (D) Quantification of combined data of assays with 65 μm and 90 μm wide gaps grouped in ' < 100 μm' bars; 115 μm and 215 μm grouped in ' > 100 μm' bars. eA5-EA3: naïve: 93.4%, <100μm: 26.9%, >100μm: 76.3% stopping. EA3-eA5: naïve: 91.6%, <100μm: 40.3%, >100μm: 77.1% stopping. eA5-S3A: naïve: 93.7%, <100μm: 81.9% stopping. N: number of independent experiments; error bars indicate standard deviations. T-test with n.s.: α ≥0.05, ***: α <0.001.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.012

Figure 5—source data 1. Original data underlying bar chart of Figure 5D.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.013

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Growth cone adaptation towards Sema3A.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A) Naïve axons stop in front of a homogeneous field of Sema3A-Fc (S3A, green), but predominantly ignore a similar field in gap assays with(B) 65 µm, (C) 115 µm and (D) 215 µm wide gaps. Axons strongly de-fasciculate and begin to branch in the Sema3A-free areas of the gap substrates, but not when growing on the protein fields (detail shown in C', white dotted lines indicate the position of the gap). The reason for this notable effect is currently unknown. Remarkably, the desensitization towards Sema3A in these assays is so strong that re-sensitization is not reached even after the largest gap-size available with our contact-printing stamps (215 µm). (E) S3A-eA5 double-cue gap assay. Sema3A-adapted GCs do not co-adapt towards ephrin-A5 and stop in front of an ephrin-A5 field after a 65 µm wide gap (cf. Figure 5B'). (F) SNAP-ephrin-A5 surface localization on Sema3A-adapted GCs. (G, G') Representative images of SNAP-ephrin-A5 surface-stained GCs growing on laminin or Sema3A, respectively (outline drawn in the actin channel indicated as a white, dotted line). (H) Quantification of SNAP-ephrin-A5 surface staining expressed as fold change in relative intensity compared to controls on laminin (on La: 1; on S3A: 1.07). N: number of independent experiments, number of analyzed GCs in brackets; error bars show standard deviations. T-test with n.s.: α ≥ 0.05. Scale (A–E): 100 µm, (G, G'): 10 µm.
Figure 5—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Original data underlying bar chart of Figure 5—figure supplement 1H.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.015

Conversely, in EphA3/ephrin-A5 double-cue gap assays (<100 µm), nasal GCs co-adapt their forward signal by growing on an EphA3 reverse signaling field (40.3% stopping; naïve: 91.6%; Figure 5B,B' and D). Consistent with the findings in single-cue gap assays, the degree of co-adaptation abates with increasing gap width (eA5–EA3 >100µm: 76.3% stopping; EA3–eA5 >100µm: 77.1% stopping; Figure 5D). Co-adaptation is ephrin/Eph-specific, as for example, ephrin-A5-adapted temporal GCs remain sensitive to other repulsive guidance molecules such as Sema3A (naïve: 93.7% stopping, eA5–S3A <100µm: 81.9% stopping; Figure 5C,C' and D). Although retinal GCs can efficiently desensitize towards Sema3A in gap-assays (Figure 5—figure supplement 1A–D), Sema3A-adapted GCs remain sensitive to ephrin-A5 (Figure 5—figure supplement 1E), further attesting to the orthogonality of these signaling pathways and to the specificity of the co-adaptation mechanism for the ephrin-A/EphA-based topographic mapping system.

Together, these findings, being in full agreement with the predictions of our computational model, prove the existence of a novel mechanism of signal modulation (co-adaptation), which allows for topographic mapping in the presence of CG adaptation.

Desensitization involves clearance of sensors from the GC surface by clathrin-mediated endocytosis

Searching for the molecular underpinnings of co-adaptation, we transfected RGCs with a SNAP-tagged ephrin-A5 expression construct (pSNAP–ephrin-A5–IRES–EGFP; Figure 6—figure supplement 1). The self-labeling SNAP-tag (Gautier et al., 2008) in combination with a membrane-impermeant fluorescent substrate enabled us to specifically label the surface-bound subpopulation of SNAP–ephrin-A5 in naïve and adapted states. SNAP–ephrin-A5 transfected GCs remain sensitive to substrate-bound ephrin-As (see below). This is in contrast to retrovirally transfected native ephrin-A5, which rendered chick RGC GCs insensitive to external ephrin-As (Hornberger et al., 1999). Seemingly, the amount of cis-signaling added through the expression of SNAP–ephrin-A5 is sufficiently low to prevent significant disturbance of the guidance system, supporting the validity of the localization probe. On GCs growing on an EphA3-Fc surface, SNAP–ephrin-A5, being the cognate sensor of EphA3, is dramatically reduced compared to control GCs growing on Fc (Figure 6A,A' and B). Remarkably, SNAP–ephrin-A5 surface levels are also reduced on GCs growing on ephrin-A5-Fc, despite not being detected by this sensor (Figure 6A'' and B). Thus, the reverse signaling sensor is cleared from the surface upon both pure reverse and pure forward signaling, strongly corroborating the co-adaptation concept. By contrast, SNAP–ephrin-A5 surface levels remain unaltered upon adaptation to Sema3A (Figure 5—figure supplement 1G–H), again confirming the specificity of the co-adaptation mechanism to the ephrin-A/EphA system.

Figure 6. Endocytosis of guidance sensors upon growth cone desensitization.

(A) SNAP–ephrin-A5 surface signal is strongly reduced on GCs growing on either homogeneous EphA3-Fc (A') or ephrin-A5-Fc (A'') compared to controls (on Fc), indicating clearance of sensors from the surface upon co-adaptation. Outlines of GCs drawn in the GFP (transfection marker) channel are indicated as white, dotted lines. Scale: 10 µm. (A''') displays a cartoon of the experiment in (A–A'') with axonal SNAP surface labeled ephrin-A5 in magenta and with the corresponding guidance cues (Fc, gray; EA3, blue and eA5, red). (B) Quantification of SNAP–ephrin-A5 surface signal intensities. Relative signal intensity within the GC outline normalized to the control situation is given as fold change (on Fc: 1; on EA3: 0.29; on eA5: 0.32). (C) Effects of Pitstop2 on adaptation towards soluble ephrin-A5. Pitstop2 (P; carrier DMSO) prevents temporal GCs from desensitizing to 0.25 µg/ml ephrin-A5-Fc (eA5+DMSO 120 min: 15.8% collapsed; eA5+P 120 min: 74.3% collapsed). The initial response of GCs towards eA5 is unaltered in the presence of P (eA5+DMSO 20 min: 85.4% collapsed; eA5+P: 83.1% collapsed). Neither P itself, nor its carrier DMSO induces a collapse (Fc+DMSO 20 min: 13.6% collapsed; Fc+P 20 min: 7.5% collapsed). (D) Effects of Pitstop2 on adaptation towards soluble EphA3. Soluble EphA3-Fc (EA3; 15 µg/ml) triggers a collapse on nasal or temporal GCs (EA3 20 min: 64.4% collapsed; Fc [4.2 µg/ml] 20 min: 24.3% collapsed). GCs desensitize towards EA3 within 120 min (EA3 120 min: 22.6% collapsed), but not in the presence of 30 μM Pitstop2 (EA3+P 120 min: 49.8% collapsed). P does not impede GCs from recovering their morphology in general (EA3 20 min, then Fc+P 120 min: 19.3% collapsed). Combined data from nasal and temporal GCs. (B, C, D) N: number of independent experiments, number of analyzed GCs in brackets; error bars represent standard deviations. T-test with n.s.: α ≥0.05, *: α <0.05, ***: α <0.001.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.016

Figure 6—source data 1. Original data underlying the bar charts of Figure 6B—D.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.017

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. SNAP–ephrin-A5 expression constructs pSNAP–ephrin-A5–IRES-FP contains the coding sequence of a fusion protein consisting of the signal sequence of chick ephrin-A5 (60 bp), the SNAP-tag and full-length chick ephrin-A5 downstream of the CAG enhancer/promoter and upstream of an IRES-FP sequence.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

The FP is EGFP in pSNAP–ephrin-A5–IRES-EGFP and dTomato in pSNAP–ephrin-A5–IRES–dTom.

To address the mechanism of surface clearance during co-adaptation, we used the clathrin-mediated endocytosis (CME) inhibitor Pitstop2 (von Kleist et al., 2011). CME is a ubiquitous mechanism for the endocytosis (McMahon and Boucrot, 2011) of numerous receptors including EphAs (Boissier et al., 2013; Yoo et al., 2010). In collapse assays, Pitstop2 does not affect the initial response to the guidance cue (ephrin-A5+DMSO: 85.4%; ephrin-A5+Pitstop2: 83.1% collapsed; Figure 6C), nor does it induce a collapse by itself (Fc+DMSO: 13.6%; Fc+Pitstop2: 7.5% collapsed). Strikingly, however, adaptive recovery after prolonged incubation is completely abolished when Pitstop2 is applied together with ephrin-A5 or EphA3 for 120 min (ephrin-A5+DMSO: 15.8%; ephrin-A5+Pitstop2: 74.3%; EphA3: 22.6%; EphA3+Pitstop2: 49.8% collapsed; Figure 6C and D), indicating that sensor internalization via CME is required for the desensitization of both channels.

Re-sensitization involves replenishment of sensors on the GC surface and is independent of local translation

We next asked how desensitized GCs replenish their surface sensor pools upon re-sensitization. Previously described forms of GC adaptation involve local translation for re-sensitization (Ming et al., 2002; Piper et al., 2005). Thus, we performed ephrin-A5 gap assays in the presence of anisomycin (AIM) to inhibit protein synthesis (Grollman, 1967). Consistent with previous reports (Campbell and Holt, 2001), AIM treatment did not affect axon elongation until, after about 4 hr, cells start to die (data not shown). We used time-lapse imaging and administered AIM just before axons left the first ephrin-A5 field. Interestingly, AIM-treated GCs display normal re-sensitization, as indicated by the unaltered recognition of the second ephrin-A5-Fc field after a 200 µm wide gap (Figure 7A).

Figure 7. Dynamics of SNAP–ephrin-A5 during growth cone re-sensitization.

(A) Exemplary trajectories of five GCs (combined from N = 3 independent experiments) on ephrin-A5 gap patterns (red) in the presence of 40 µM AIM. Positions of GCs (black dots) were marked at the indicated time points (in minutes) after the addition of AIM. GCs stop and pause (I, III) or stop and retract (II, IV, V) upon reaching the second ephrin field, indicating full re-sensitization despite inhibition of local translation. (B–B'') SNAP–ephrin-A5 dynamics upon GC re-sensitization. Representative images of GCs, stained for formerly intracellular SNAP–ephrin-A5 that has been relocated to the membrane. Outlines of GCs drawn in the actin channel are indicated as white, dotted lines. Surface localization of ephrin-A5 (anti-fluorescein signal) is elevated in re-sensitizing GCs (B'', off ephrin-A5) compared to desensitized GCs (B', on ephrin-A5). Scale: 10 μm. (C) Experimental setup with retinal explant (black) on a field of ephrin-A5 (red) and cartoon of the experiment shown in (B–B'') with intracellular SNAP–ephrin-A5 (green), which is detected on the surface (magenta star). (D) Quantification of membrane-relocated SNAP–ephrin-A5 signals. Relative anti-fluorescein signal intensity within the GC outline normalized to the control is given as fold change. Control — 1; on eA5 — 0.85; off eA5 — 1.13. N: number of independent experiments, number of analyzed GCs in brackets; error bars represent standard deviations. T-test with n.s.: α ≥0.05, *: α <0.05.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.019

Figure 7—source data 1. Original data underlying the bar chart in Figure 7D.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.020

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Staining procedure for recycled SNAP–ephrin-A5.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

Extracellular SNAP–ephrin-A5 was blocked by SNAP surface block (gray), before the cell-permeant SNAP cell fluorescein was applied to the living cells (green). After washing, axons were allowed to grow for another 20–22 hr. Then, anti-fluorescein antibody was added to the living cells for 15 min, washed out and, subsequently, explants were fixed and stained for anti-fluorescein (magenta stars).

As an alternative to local translation, the GC membrane might be repopulated with sensors from internal stores during re-sensitization. To check for this possibility, we specifically labeled the intracellular population of SNAP–ephrin-A5 in desensitized GCs growing on ephrin-A5. Subsequently, GCs were given time (20–22 hr) to leave the ephrin-A5 field for re-sensitization (Figure 7C) and were then stained for any formerly intracellular label that had appeared on the GC surface in the meantime (for a detailed staining protocol, see Figure 7—figure supplement 1 and 'Materials and methods'). Notably, GCs that have left the ephrin field show a significantly higher surface staining than GCs that are still growing on the field (fold change in intensity compared to laminin controls: on ephrin-A5 — 0.85; off ephrin-A5 — 1.13; Figure 7B–B’’ and D). This indicates a transport of intracellular sensors to the GC's surface upon re-sensitization. Notably, these results were gained for SNAP–ephrin-A5 on ephrin-A5-Fc substrates. As ephrin-A5 is not a receptor for ephrin-A5-Fc, its regulation has to be assumed to be a result of co-regulation with EphAs, the active sensor in this setting, again supporting the co-adaptation idea.

Co-adaptation implicates temporary storage of guidance sensors in the recycling endosome

Endocytotic clearance and surface repopulation from internal stores upon co-adaptation suggests the involvement of recycling endosomal compartments (Goldenring, 2015). To assess whether ephrin-A5 does indeed traffic through recycling endosomes during co-adaptation, we co-transfected RGCs with pSNAP–ephrin-A5–IRES–dTom and pEGFP–Rab11 (Figure 8A,B,B’ and C,C'). In fact, we find an increased colocalization of SNAP–ephrin-A5 and Rab11 in GCs growing on ephrin-A5 when compared to GCs on laminin (Figure 8B'', C'' and D), suggesting elevated partitioning of these sensors in desensitized GCs into Rab11-positive compartments, from which they can return to the surface upon re-sensitization.

Figure 8. Colocalization of SNAP–ephrin-A5 and Rab11-positive endosomes.

Figure 8.

(A) eGFP–Rab11 localization in a transfected RGC axon (outline drawn in the actin channel indicated as white, dotted line). Rab11-positive vesicular structures are predominantly observed in the axon shaft and the central domain of GCs and, to a lesser extent, are found to stretch into the more distal GC. (B–B'') Representative images of an eGFP–Rab11 (B) and a SNAP–ephrin-A5 (B') co-transfected GC on laminin substrate. Vesicular structures identified by automated image segmentation are indicated in green and red for each channel, respectively. (B'') Colocalization of SNAP–ephrin-A5 and eGFP–Rab11 within the Rab11-positive vesicles is shown as yellow areas. (C–C'') Example images of an eGFP–Rab11- (C) and a SNAP–ephrin-A5-transfected GC (C') growing on ephrin-A5-Fc. (C'') Colocalization of SNAP–ephrin-A5 and eGFP–Rab11 within the Rab11-positive vesicles (yellow areas) increases upon desensitization. (D) Manders' colocalization coefficient (MCC) as a measure of colocalization of SNAP–ephrin-A5 and eGFP–Rab11 within the Rab11-positive vesicles in naïve (on laminin: M2 = 0.55) and adapted (on ephrin-A5: M2 = 0.72) GCs, respectively. N: number of independent experiments, number of analyzed GCs in brackets; error bars indicate standard deviations. T-test with *: α <0.05. Scale: 10 μm.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.022

Figure 8—source data 1. Original data underlying the bar chart of Figure 8D.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.023

Disrupting membrane rafts induces adaptation

In our model, adaption is adjusted in response to the guidance potential experienced by the GC. A potential regulator of co-adaption should therefore be located downstream of both reverse and forward signaling, which together determine the guidance potential, and it should impact on the membrane activity of the signaling sensors. Baba et al. (2009) have shown that Fyn-kinase is activated downstream of ephrin-A reverse signaling, inducing an increase of sphingomyelin in the membrane and a corresponding exclusion of ephrin-A from the cell surface. Sphingomyelin, together with cholesterol, is a major component of the lipid rafts of the cell membrane (Simons and Ikonen, 1997), where ephrin-As have been shown to be localized (Averaimo et al., 2016; Gauthier and Robbins, 2003). As Fyn-kinase also acts downstream of EphA receptors (Ellis et al., 1996; Wu et al., 1997), we first asked whether manipulating the sphingomyelin content of the membrane might impact co-adaptation. Pretreatment with sphingomyelinase (SMase, Neufeld et al., 1996) in collapse assays strongly reduces the primary response of GCs towards ephrin-A5 and EphA3 (ephrin-A5 — 79.6%; ephrin-A5+SMase — 50%; EphA3 — 52.9%; and EphA3+SMase — 23.8% collapsed; Figure 9—figure supplement 1A and B). SMase treatment has been shown to disrupt rafts by replacing them with ceramide-enriched, densely packed solid ordered domains (Chiantia et al., 2006; Goñi and Alonso, 2009). Thus, sphingomyelin overproduction or conversion to ceramide might have the same effect of excluding ephrin-As from rafts. To investigate the role of these membrane microdomains in co-adaptation more closely, and to avoid the intricacies of SMase effects, we used methyl-β-cyclodextrin (MβCD, [Mahammad and Parmryd, 2015]) to disrupt rafts by extracting cholesterol from the GC membrane in further adaptation assays. In forward signaling collapse assays with 0.25 µg/ml ephrin-A5-Fc, co-application of 2 mg/ml MβCD does not significantly alter the primary response after 20 min (80.3% without, 77.4% with MβCD, Figure 9A). After 1 hr of incubation, however, significantly more GCs have recovered in the presence of MβCD than without (74.2% collapse without, 45% with MβCD, Figure 9A). In reverse signaling collapse assays with 15 µg/ml EphA3, a corresponding desensitizing effect is already seen after 20 min (78.2% collapsed without, 51.6% collapsed with MβCD, Figure 9B) and remains significant after 1 hr (28.3% collapse without, 14.5% with MβCD, Figure 9B). In double-cue gap-assays with a gap size actually allowing for substantial re-sensitization (115 µm), a significantly higher percentage of EphA3- or ephrin-A5-adapted axons are still desensitized when MβCD is present compared to controls; these axons overgrow the boundary and enter the field of ephrin-A5 or EphA3, respectively, after the gap (EA3-eA5: 80.7% stopping without MβCD, 46,3% stopping with MβCD [Figure 9C,C’ and E]; eA5-EA3: 70.9% stopping without MβCD, 23.4% stopping with MβCD [Figure 9D,D’ and E]). Thus, extraction of cholesterol from the GC membrane, which is thought to result in the disintegration of lipid rafts, strongly enhances desensitization in both types of adaptation assays.

Figure 9. Disintegration of lipid microdomains induces co-adaptive growth cone desensitization.

(A) Ephrin-A5-Fc and (B) EphA3-Fc collapse assays. Methyl-beta-cyclodextrin (MβCD, 2 mg/ml)-treated GCs show enhanced desensitization (reduced collapse rates) when exposed to soluble ephrin-A5-Fc (0.25 μg/ml) or EphA3-Fc (15 μg/ml) compared to controls without MβCD. (A) GCs collapsed after 20 min incubation: Fc (0.25 μg/ml),16.7%; Fc+MβCD, 11.4%; eA5, 80.3%; and eA5+MβCD, 77.4%. GCs collapsed after after 60 min incubation: Fc, 12.1%; Fc+MβCD, 13.4%; eA5, 74.2%; and eA5+MβCD, 45.0%. (B) GCs collapsed after 20 min incubation: Fc (4.2 μg/ml), 22.8%; Fc+MβCD, 24.4%; EA3, 78.2%; and EA3+MβCD, 51.6%. GCs collapsed after 60 min incubation: Fc, 14.9%; Fc+MβCD, 10.4%; EA3, 28.3%; and EA3+MβCD, 14.5%. N: number of independent experiments, number of analyzed GCs in brackets; error bars show standard deviations. T-test with n.s.: α ≥0.05, *: α <0.05, ***: α <0.001. (C) EA3–eA5 and (D) eA5–EA3 double-cue gap assays. After 115 µm wide gaps, untreated GCs predominantly stop, whereas MβCD-treated GCs (C', D') ignore the protein field after the gap. (E) Quantification of double-cue gap assays: EA3–eA5, 80.7%; EA3–eA5+MβCD, 46.3% stopping; eA5–EA3, 70.9%; and eA5–EA3+MβCD, 23.6% stopping. N: number of independent experiments, error bars show standard deviations, T-test with *: α <0.05, ***: α <0.001.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.024

Figure 9—source data 1. Original data underlying the bar charts in Figure 9A, B, E.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.025

Figure 9.

Figure 9—figure supplement 1. Disintegration of lipid microdomains by sphingomyelinase desensitizes growth cones towards soluble ephrin-A5 and EphA3.

Figure 9—figure supplement 1.

(A) Ephrin-A5-Fc and (B) EphA3-Fc collapse assays. In accordance with MβCD experiments, Sphingomyelinase (SMase, 400mU/ml)-treated GCs show reduced collapse rates when exposed to soluble ephrin-A5-Fc (0.25 μg/ml) or EphA3-Fc (15 μg/ml) compared to controls without SMase. (A) Fc (0.25 μg/ml), 19.5%; Fc+SMase, 27.5%; eA5, 79.6%; and eA5+SMase, 50% collapsed. (B) Fc (4.2 μg/ml), 22.5%; Fc+SMase, 27.7%; EA3, 52.9%; and EA3+SMase, 23.8% collapsed after 20 min incubation. SMase was applied during a 30 min pre-incubation and thereafter together with Fc or ephrin-A5. N: number of independent experiments, number of analyzed GCs in brackets; error bars show standard deviations. T-test with n.s.: α ≥ 0.05, *: α < 0.05, **: α < 0.01, ***: α < 0.001.
Figure 9—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Original data underlying the bar charts of Figure 9—figure supplement 1.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.027

Modeling suggests that co-adaptation is needed for tectal entry

We have previously shown that our model, through the inclusion of fiber–fiber in addition to fiber–target interactions, displays a substantial degree of mapping plasticity even in the absence of any adaptation (Weth et al., 2014). Thus, in accordance with experimental observations, a half-retinal projection expands to a full tectum, a full retinal projection compresses to a half-tectum, and a half-projection can be forced to map onto a mismatching tectal half (Goodhill and Richards, 1999). This is due to fiber–fiber interactions, by virtue of which the axons introduce their own mapping apparatus into the target field, eventually overwriting the target cues. Why then should co-adaptation be needed? Generally speaking, signaling comes at the expense of energy. Adaptation is continuously acting to reduce the guidance potential and, thus, the energetic costs of organizing the connectome, which are presumably limiting to the development of the nervous system as the mature brain is the most highly ordered biological structure we know of. A more specific advantage of the adaptation of retinal GCs can be envisaged from corresponding simulations (Figure 10). Without adaptation, the majority of axons would be unable to overcome the steep boundary of reverse signaling at the entrance of the target field. By contrast, when we deliberately desensitize the fiber population through the co-adaptation mechanism before entering the target field, they easily overcome the boundary and automatically re-adjust their sensitivities on the target field. To this end, a pre-adapting signal would be required before the target. Fiber–fiber interactions enabled by the intimate contact of the fibers in the optic tract are unlikely to be the preadapting factor, as it has been show that fiber–fiber interactions are required neither for target entry nor for the actual mapping (Gosse et al., 2008). This suggests the existence of still elusive biochemical preadapting factors, which should be present in the optic tract, but absent from the target.

Figure 10. Modeling tectal innervation.

Figure 10.

Co-adaptation enables fiber terminals to enter a tectal target field initially and allows correct mapping therein (gray circles), whereas non-adapted terminals (white circles) fail to enter a tectal target field in simulations with 200 GCs placed in front of the target. The graded distributions of EphAs (blue) and ephrin-As (red) on the target field are symbolized by the colored wedges. RF and LF of adapted terminals are initially deflected by a factor of 30.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.028

Discussion

The surprising finding of this study is that GCs of chick RGCs can adapt towards forward and reverse ephrin-A/EphA signals, which by their quantities provide the critical cues for topographic guidance. Inspired by computational modeling, we suggest that the seemingly incompatible properties of topographic accuracy and adaptation can be reconciled by a distinctive novel mode of strictly parallelized modulation of the signaling channels, termed co-adaptation. Experimentally, we demonstrate that co-adaptation is implemented in retinal GCs. The cellular mechanism of co-adaptation is based on the trafficking of guidance sensors between the GC membrane and internal stores, including the recycling endosome. Finally, we provide evidence indicating that repartitioning of guidance sensors between membrane microdomains might be a critical first step in co-adaptation. According to modeling, co-adaptation could have a particular role during tectal entry, allowing axons to overcome the steep reverse signaling boundary at this location.

Co-adaptation preserves the ephrin-A/EphA ratio, the genetic imprint of topographic identity

It has repeatedly been shown before that GCs can adapt to axonal guidance cues that act as global attractants or repellents (Ming et al., 2002; Piper et al., 2005). Adaptation in these cases might be a means of adjusting the dynamic range of the sensors. By contrast, adaptation against topographic cues comes unexpectedly, as these cues have been thought to act by virtue of their concentrations within graded distributions since the first proposition of gradient guidance (Sperry, 1963). Concentration gradients provide two kinds of information for chemotactic guidance: positional information, via the local concentration, c(x), and directional information, via the derivative, dc(x)/dx. Adaptation amounts to deleting the positional information. This is what is found in bacterial chemotaxis (Parkinson et al., 2015). At every location in the gradient, the CheR methyltransferase desensitizes chemotaxis receptors so that upon relocation of the bacterium, only the incremental change of the cue concentration is detected. This exclusive readout of the gradient’s directional information enables the bacteria to reach the source or the sink of an attractant or repellent, respectively, but precludes the identification of intermediate targets. The same would hold true for the topographic guidance of GCs.

In our previously published computational model of topographic guidance (Gebhardt et al., 2012), the topographic identity of the GCs is encoded in the ratio of ephrin-As to EphAs that they express. This is because this ratio determines the ratio of instantaneous reverse to forward signals received by the GC, which, according to the model, has to be balanced upon target approach. Thus, retaining topographic identity in the face of adaptation requires that these sensing channels are co-modulated concomitantly by the same factor. We challenged this idea of co-adaptation experimentally by testing the strong prediction that, through co-adaptive coupling, a signaling channel should be modulated even in the absence of its cognate signal, as long as the coupled channel is activated and modulated. We show that this is what happens in retinal GCs with regard to reverse and forward ephrin-A/EphA signaling. The mechanism is specific to the ephrin/Eph system, and GCs remain sensitive to other guidance signals throughout. To the best of our knowledge, co-adaptation is a novel signaling phenomenon not described previously. Canonical adaptation can be conceived as a negative feedback to channel activation. Co-adaptation, by contrast, also modulates coupled, non-activated signaling channels.

It should be noted that the existence of co-adaptation in retinal GCs along the way supports our modeling assumption that topographic identity is encoded in the ephrin-A/EphA ratio. It also corroborates the occasionally disputed role of reverse signaling in topographic mapping. Why should a specific co-adaptation mechanism for the two channels exist, if only one was needed for mapping?

The cellular mechanism of co-adaptation involves the shuttling of a controlled subpopulation of membrane-bound sensors on a special trafficking route

According to our findings, co-adaptation in retinal GCs is based on vesicular trafficking. Desensitization is correlated with surface clearance of ephrin-As and EphAs by CME, whereas re-sensitization implies replenishment of ephrin-As and EphAs from recycling endosomal compartments. In contrast to the transport of ephrin-As, the trafficking of EphA receptors has been described in detail (Sabet et al., 2015). Receptors that have been auto-phosphorylated as a result of ligand binding, as well as receptors that are spontaneously auto-phosphorylated in the absence of a ligand, are internalized into Rab5-positive early endosomes. Ligand binding, as opposed to spontaneous auto-phosphorylation, is thought to induce clustering (Janes et al., 2012; Nikolov et al., 2013) and additional phosphorylation. The latter recruits ubiquitinylating enzymes, eventually diverting the stable receptor/ligand complexes to the late endosomal/lysosomal compartment for degradation. Unliganded, spontaneously auto-phosphorylated receptors, however, are trafficked to the recycling endosome, bringing them into apposition with tyrosine phosphatase PTP1B on the cytosolic face of the endoplasmic reticulum membrane. After dephosphorylation, the receptors are recycled to the plasma membrane. Despite being triggered by ligand binding, co-adaptation undoubtedly necessitates the trafficking of unbound receptors. It is unlikely, however, that these correspond to the spontaneously auto-phosphorylated subpopulation described above. This is because spontaneous auto-phosphorylation is expected to occur at constant frequency, whereas the degree of co-adaptation must vary with the impinging signal. Thus, we propose that the sensors trafficked during co-adaptation form an independent subpopulation, whose size is adjusted to the required degree of adaptation and which is shuttled on a pathway that is not identical with the routes described above. As co-adaptation requires the shuttling of the proper absolute number of sensors in the topographically appropriate ratio, the question arises as to how this subpopulation might be selected for trafficking.

Repartitioning between membrane lipid microdomains as the potential mechanism allocating sensors to the co-adaptation pathway

The degree of co-adaptation required in the model is determined by the instantaneous guidance potential, D, measured by a GC, i.e., downstream of the integration of reverse and forward signals. Sphingomyelin has been shown to increase in the cell membrane upon activation of Fyn-kinase, downstream of ephrin-A/EphA signaling, and to induce a concomitant decrease of membrane ephrin-As (Baba et al., 2009). This sphingomyelin appeared to be a promising candidate regulator of co-adaptation. In fact, SMase pretreatment desensitizes retinal GCs to both ephrin-A forward and EphA reverse signaling.

Sphingomyelin, together with cholesterol, is thought to be a key organizer of lipid rafts, which are liquid-ordered microdomains of the cell membrane (Róg and Vattulainen, 2014; Simons and Sampaio, 2011). We therefore went on to test the effects of MβCD, an efficient scavenger of cholesterol, on GC co-adaptation. In fact, we find a strong enhancement of the desensitization of forward and reverse signaling in both collapse and gap co-adaptation assays upon application of MβCD. Thus, manipulation of both major components of lipid rafts yields the same effect, supporting the idea that these membrane microdomains are involved in the observed desensitization. Formally, our experiments cannot rule out the possibility that lipid raft disruption simply impedes primary signaling and is thus unrelated to the co-adaptation mechanism. However, the fact that ephrin-A reverse signaling impacts on the relevant lipid composition of the membrane strongly suggests the idea that raft microdomain alteration is a genuine element of the GC co-adaptation mechanism.

Raft disintegration would lead to a release of ephrin-As, which have been shown to be localized in membrane rafts (Averaimo et al., 2016; Gauthier and Robbins, 2003), into the liquid disordered domain of the membrane. There is substantial evidence that EphAs might also reside in lipid rafts (Boscher et al., 2012; Chakraborty et al., 2012; Foster et al., 2003; Gu et al., 2012; Inder et al., 2012; Kim et al., 2012; Yang et al., 2010; Yi et al., 2013; Zheng et al., 2011), potentially driven by the lipid-interactions of their second fibronectin type III domain (Chavent et al., 2016), although these rafts seem to be separate from the ephrin-A-containing microdomains (Kao and Kania, 2011; Marquardt et al., 2005). Therefore, we suggest that a similar dispersion from microdomains might also happen to EphAs upon variation of the sphingomyelin content of the membrane (Figure 11). These processes would eventually generate an intermixed subpopulation of dispersed ephrin-As and EphAs in the non-raft membrane.

Figure 11. Proposed mechanism of co-adaptation.

Figure 11.

(A) EphAs (blue) and ephrin-As (red), located in membrane lipid microdomains (dark gray), signal in trans due to external cues. Trans forward (FWD) and reverse (REV) signals integrate into the guidance potential, D, and elicit repulsion when not balanced. Trans signaling also promotes sphingomyelin (SM) synthesis, and when incorporated into the membrane, this phospholipid might proportionally solubilize EphAs and ephrin-As from their respective domains. (B) Dispersed, unbound sensors are destined for constitutive clathrin-mediated endocytosis (CME) and might increase cis signaling upon internalization, as anti-parallel orientation is favored in high-curvature membrane vesicles. Enhanced cis signals desensitize GCs to trans signals. Sensitivity returns with the recycling of unbound sensors, and degraded receptor/ligand complexes should constitutively be replaced through protein synthesis in the long run.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.029

Notably, raft-located proteins are typically internalized by clathrin-independent endocytosis (Sandvig et al., 2011). By contrast, we observe CME for both ephrin-As and EphAs during co-adaptation, supporting the idea that the endocytosed subpopulation of molecules might not be raft localized. If, as we propose, alteration of the sphingomyelin content induced the same fraction of ephrin-As and EphAs to re-partition from their respective microdomains to the liquid-disordered phase of the membrane, the dispersed mixture eventually would have the same ratio of ephrin-As and EphAs as the whole membrane. It could thus be endocytotically removed from the surface upon co-adaptation without altering the topographically relevant ephrin-A/EphA ratio. The same would be hardly conceivable if membrane clearance involved only ephrin-A/EphA ligand receptor complexes, as their ratio would be stoichiometric instead of depending on the topographic imprint of the respective cell. The disperse subpopulation would probably not be activated, as activated sensors should stay in their respective microdomains due to increased clustering caused by ligand binding (Janes et al., 2012; Nikolov et al., 2013). Thus, the co-adaptive subpopulation would not be expected to carry any phosphorylation tag to trigger endocytosis. We instead suggest that the disperse distribution by itself might destine for constitutive endocytosis (Figure 11).

Increased cis-signaling from endosomes as the potential core mechanism of co-adaptation

Albeit highly intuitive that desensitization and re-sensitization might involve sensor endocytosis and recycling, respectively, our computational modeling suggests unexpected twists on the interpretation of sensor trafficking. Surprisingly, according to the model, the sensor surface concentration is irrelevant to adaptation. This can be seen, for example, when an individual axon is growing on a homogeneous ephrin target. Then, the model equation reduces to

D=|ln1LT/LF+1|

which includes the ephrin on the fiber terminal, LF, and on the target, LT, but not on the receptor actually detecting the trans signal. In fact, the model suggests that the axonal ligand representing cis-signaling is the relevant parameter determining the guidance potential. As discussed in the previous paper (Gebhardt et al., 2012), we assume cis-interactions to be signal transducing instead of signal inactivating. That it is, in fact, cis-signaling that governs co-adaptation in the model can be seen more easily when the model equation for an individual axon is expressed conceptually in terms of signaling channels (REV: reverse, FWD: forward):

D=|lnREVtrans+REVcisFWDtrans+FWDcis|

Upon introduction of an adaptation factor, a, which was originally meant to equally modulate all sensor concentrations, the model equation becomes

D=|lna*REVtrans+a2*REVcisa*FWDtrans+a2*FWDcis|

The square in front of the cis-signaling is due to the fact that, in contrast to trans-signaling, cis-signaling involves both axonal sensors. This obviously reduces to

D=|lnREVtrans+a*REVcisFWDtrans+a*FWDcis|

Thus, effectively, only the efficiency of cis-signaling is modulated during co-adaptation. Figuratively, this compares to a situation in which we listen to music via earphones (cis-signaling) to suppress external acoustic signals (trans-signaling).

The proposed type of signal transducing cis-interactions has the steric requirement that receptor and ligand be aligned, as in trans-signaling, in an anti-parallel orientation (Himanen et al., 2001). This will normally occur only occasionally, when filopodia collide or when membrane ruffles are formed. Thus, the major effect of endocytosis in co-adaptation (Figure 11) might be to provide high membrane curvature (around the vesicle lumen) to increase the efficiency of cis-signaling from endosomes (Schmick and Bastiaens, 2014).

In summary, the proposed mechanisms could explain how co-adaptation might reconcile targeting accuracy in topographically mapping GCs with their adaptability. Thus, our study reveals that the primary task of forming a proper topographic map, despite being based on genetic hardwiring, is achieved through an astonishingly flexible mechanism.

Material and methods

Reagents were obtained from Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA unless explicitly stated.

RGC explant cultures

Retinae of embryonic day 6 to 7 (E6–E7) chick embryos were dissected in ice-cold Hanks' balanced salt solution (HBSS), sucked onto a nitrocellulose filter and cut into 250–300 μm wide naso-temporal strips. Explants were placed on cover slips coated with 20 µg/ml mouse natural laminin in HBSS (1 hr at 37°C) and grown in F12 medium containing 0.4% methylcellulose, 5% fetal calf and 2% chicken serum (F12-MC) at 37°C and 4% CO2 for 20–24 hr.

Collapse assays

The medium on explant cultures was replaced with warm F12-MC containing recombinant human ephrin-A5-Fc, mouse EphA3-Fc (both RnD Systems, Minneapolis, MN, USA) or human Fc fragment (Calbiochem, San Diego, CA, USA). For inhibitor experiments, 30 µM Pitstop2 (Abcam, Cambridge, UK) or 400 mU/ml sphingomyelinase (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA) in F12-MC was applied during a 15 or 30 min pre-incubation, respectively, and thereafter together with ephrin-A5-Fc, EphA3-Fc or Fc. Methyl-β-cyclodextrin (2 mg/ml; Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA) was administered together with ephrin-A5-Fc, EphA3-Fc or Fc. After incubation for 20 or 120 min, cultures were fixed and stained with Alexa488 or Alexa568-phalloidin and the percentage of collapsed GCs was counted.

Gap assays

Gap patterns were produced by direct contact printing as described previously (von Philipsborn et al., 2006a). In short, a PDMS stamp (Elastosil RT 625; Wacker Chemie, Munich, Germany) comprising the gap pattern was covered with protein solution for 2 hr at 37°C, rinsed in H2O, dried under a stream of nitrogen gas and then stamped onto an epoxysilanized and poly-L-lysine (PLL)-covered glass coverslip (Ø 18 mm, VWR, Radnor, PA, USA) for 15 min at 37°C (cleaned coverslips in 1% [3-glycidoxypropyl]-trimethoxysilane [abcr, Karlsruhe, Germany] in pure EtOH, pH 5.5 for 5 min, air dried and covered with 200 μg/ml PLL [Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA] in PBS overnight). The substrate was then covered with laminin, rinsed in H2O, covered with F12 and kept at 37°C until imminent usage.

For single-cue gap assays 15 μg/ml ephrin-A5-Fc, 15 μg/ml EphA3-Fc or 25 μg/ml mouse semaphorin3A-Fc in PBS were used. For double-cue gap assays, both fields of the stamp were physically separated using a piece of ParafilmTM and each was covered with a different protein solution (combinations of 15 μg/ml ephrin-A5-Fc, 15 μg/ml EphA3-Fc and 25 μg/ml mouse semaphorin3A-Fc in PBS). The pattern orientation was indicated on the coverslip. Patterns were stained with Alexa-conjugated anti-human goat IgGs. Quantification of gap assays was performed using a custom-written MATLAB (RRID:SCR_001622) code (Weth, 2017) to count fibers in and immediately behind the gap. In short, grayscale images of axons were thresholded and two ROIs were drawn in parallel to the gap (ROI one in the gap, ROI two directly behind the gap; both ~20 μm wide and over the complete lateral extension of the gap). Both ROIs were then evaluated, counting signal peaks in each pixel row, and the percentage of stopping fibers was calculated from mean counts in ROI one and ROI 2. Methyl-β-cyclodextrin (2 mg/ml; Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA) was directly added to the culture medium. For experiments with anisomycin (AIM, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA), 40 µM AIM was added to the culture medium when axons were just about to leave the first ephrin-A5 field, and GCs were tracked using time-lapse imaging.

Expression constructs

To construct pSNAP–ephrin-A5–IRES–EGFP (Figure 6—figure supplement 1), we first reverse transcribed and PCR amplified the message for full-length Gallus gallus ephrin-A5 (NCBI [RRID:SCR_006472] NM_205184.2) using primers starting 16 bp upstream of the start and ending 27 bp downstream of the stop codon from total RNA of the E7 chick tectum. Using SdaI and NotI linkers, the amplification product was inserted between the respective sites of the pSNAP-tag(m) vector (New England Biolabs, Ipswich, MA, USA). In addition, a synthetic double-stranded DNA encoding the first 20 amino acids of chick ephrin-A5 (signal peptide) was inserted into the EcoRV site of the same construct to create a continuous open reading frame for a fusion protein containing the ephrin-A5 signal peptide, the SNAP-tag and full-length ephrin-A5. This coding sequence was amplified from the construct using the primers starting 8 bp upstream of the start codon and ending 36 bp downstream of the stop codon, and was inserted into the SmaI site of the expression vector pCIG2, containing a CAG enhancer/promotor upstream and IRES–EGFP downstream of the insert (Hand et al., 2005). For pSNAP-ephrin-A5-IRES-dTom, the EGFP coding sequence was excised using MscI from pSNAP-ephrin-A5-IRES-EGFP and replaced by the coding sequence for dTomato amplified from pCAGJC-dTomato (Lee et al., 2013) using MscI primer linkers starting 4 bp upstream and ending 33 bp downstream of the dTomato coding sequence. pEGFP-Rab11 was a kind gift of Dr. Esther Stoeckli (University of Zurich). It contains the coding sequence of human Rab11 N-terminally fused to EGFP using the pEGFP-C3 vector (TaKaRa Bio USA, Mountain View, CA, USA), which contains the CMV enhancer/promotor (Alther et al., 2016).

Electroporation

For transgenic SNAP-ephrin-A5 expression, dissected retinae were cut and treated with 100 µl ice-cold Accutase solution for 5 min at room temperature and electroporated using 330 ng/µl pSNAP-ephrin-A5-IRES-EGFP/dTomato plasmid in 0.5x PBS and CUY700P20 electrodes (NepaGene, Ichikawa-City, Japan; 15V, 5 × 50 ms, 950 ms off time). Because of higher transfection efficiency, we later switched to whole-eye electroporation as described in Vergara et al., 2013 using 1 µg/µl pSNAP-ephrin-A5-IRES-EGFP/dTomato and 1 µg/µl pEGFP–Rab11.

SNAP labeling

All SNAP labeling reagents were purchased from New England Biolabs, Ipswich, MA, USA and administered at 1 µg/ml in warm F12-MC for 40 min at 37°C. For Surface SNAP-ephrin-A5 staining, SNAP Surface488 reagent was followed by washing with warm F12-MC. After fixation, cells were treated with sodium borohydride solution (2 × 5 min, about 250 mM in PBS) to remove background and stained with anti-AlexaFluor488 rabbit IgG (RRID:AB_221544) and anti-rabbit AlexaFluor647 goat IgG (Jackson ImmunoResearch, Suffolk, UK) to improve signal/noise. For recycling assays (Figure 7—figure supplement 1) surface SNAP-ephrin-A5 was blocked using SNAP Surface Block. Intracellular SNAP-ephrin-A5 was labeled with SNAP Cell Fluorescein followed by washing with warm F12-MC. Cultures were grown for another 20–22 hr before application of anti-fluorescein mouse IgG in pre-warmed F12-MC to the living cells for 15 min. After washing, cells were fixed and stained with anti-mouse AlexaFluor647 goat IgG for formerly intracellular SNAP-ephrin-A5 now relocated to the cell surface. Images were taken using the ApoTome module on a Zeiss AxioimagerZ1 microscope (Zeiss, Oberkochen, Germany).

Colocalization

Colocalization of SNAP–ephrin-A5 with EGFP–Rab11 was evaluated by calculating the Manders' colocalization coefficients M1 and M2 using the Fiji (RRID:SCR_002285) plugin Coloc2 (https://github.com/fiji/Colocalisation_Analysis/, V2.0.2) after image segmentation to isolate vesicular structures within GCs. Segmentation was performed using Squassh (Rizk et al., 2014) in Fiji with parameters: regul.=0.05; min.obj.int.=0.09; subpix.seg=true; excl.z-edge=true; localint.est.=auto; noise=gauss; PSFstd.dev.xy=0.7, z = 0.8; removeint.=0; and removesize = 2.

Simulations

All simulations were performed using MATLAB 8.4 (RRID:SCR_001622, The MathWorks, Natrick, MA, USA). Except for the introduction of co-adaptation (see below), all modeling conditions were as previously described (Gebhardt et al., 2012). Briefly, the projection target, T, a rectangular array of unit squares (XT,maxXT,max=508), displays the guidance cues along the x-axis (exponential counter-distributions of ephrin-As [LT] and EphAs [RT] for simulations of tectal innervation and mapping, step-functions for simulation of gap assays). Fiber terminals, F, are modeled as circular discs with a diameter of about seven units, carrying ligands (LF for terminal under consideration, Lf, for interacting terminals), and receptors (RF and Rf, respectively) in Gaussian-shaped distributions, according to their origin in the retina, which also bears exponential counter-distributions of LF and RF, and from which the terminals are equally sampled. On the target field, terminals are allowed to overlap freely while performing a random walk, which is biased by the tendency to minimize a guidance potential, D. In every iteration, i, with current terminal center position (xT,yT),DF,i(xT,yT) is calculated from total EphA forward and total ephrin-A reverse signals, both comprising fiber–target, fiber–fiber and cis interactions (assumed to be signal transducing in both directions). All signals are calculated from mass action for the corresponding receptors and ligands over all unit increments of the target and of other terminals overlapped by the terminal under consideration. All interactions are weighted equally (with constants set to one), except trans fiber signals, whose influence, C(i), increases with iteration number to conceptually reflect the developmental increase in terminal number and size. Thus,

DF,i(xT,yT)=|ln(xT,yTLF(xT,yT)[RT(xT,yT)+RF(xT,yT)+C(i) Rf(xT,yT)]xT,yTRF(xT,yT)[LT(xT,yT)+LF(xT,yT)+C(i) Lf(xT,yT)])| (1)

with the nominator representing total reverse and the denominator total forward signals instantaneously impinging on the terminal. A GC has reached its target position, when both signaling channels are balanced (total reverse/total forward = 1) and, therefore, when D is minimized (abs(ln(1))=0)). For more detail, see Gebhardt et al. (2012).

We updated this model to include co-adaptation by introducing a common adaptation coefficient, a(i), and a Hookian resetting force, f(i), modulating both ligands and receptors (collectively called sensors, S) on the fiber terminals at every iteration, i+1, depending on the recent history (h) of D:

S(i+1)=a(i) S(i)+f(i) (2)

with

a(i)=1+ln(1+μ(k=1hk(D(ih+k))k=1hk)) (3)

and

f(i)=λ(S(0)S(i)) (4)

μ and λ are adjusting parameters. Unless explicitly stated, a unique set of parameters was used in all simulations: number of terminals n = 200; iterations i = 30000; μ = 0.006; λ = 0.0045; and h = 10. For the simulation of in-vitro assays (Figure 4), where the area density of terminals is sparse, and for the analysis of tectal entry (Figure 10), where a role of fiber–fiber interactions has been excluded experimentally, the disproportionate increase of the weight of fiber–fiber interactions was switched off (C0 = 1). On target fields without guidance cues (gap assays and in front of the tectum in the analysis of tectal entry), the random walk of the terminals was given a slight inherent bias to avoid prolonged dwelling (qx = 0.37). For details on C0 and qx and further parameters of the basic model, see Gebhardt et al. (2012). Code is available via GitHub (Weth, 2017). A copy is archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/RTP_Co-adapt_Model.

Measurement of ephrin-A5/EphA3 dissociation constants

KDs were measured using the BLItz biolayer interferometer (Pall ForteBio, Menlo Park, USA) at different EphA concentrations (cf. Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Streptavidin-coated biosensors were loaded with 5 µg/ml biotinylated human ephrin-A5-Fc (RnD Systems, Minneapolis, MN, USA) and the binding kinetics were measured using concentration series of 0, 3, 15, 33 and 100 µg/ml EphA3-Fc all in BLItz kinetics buffer (loading — 120 s; baseline — 30 s; association — 120 s; and dissociation —120 s). KDs were calculated from on and off rates derived by software from local curve fits corrected for start of association and dissociation.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Dr. Juergen Loeschinger and Dr. Friedrich Bonhoeffer for their support in the early phases of the project, Dr. Christoph Gebhardt for initial contributions and ongoing discussions on the modeling of GC adaptation, Gabi Gerdon for help with the collapse assays, and Dr. Esther Stoeckli for kindly providing the pEGFP–Rab11 plasmid. We are particularly grateful to the reviewers for their thoughtful comments, which substantially improved the paper.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Karlsruhe School of Optics and Photonics DFG GSC 21 to Felix Fiederling.

  • Baden-Württemberg Stiftung State Scholarship to Felix Fiederling.

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft BA 1034/14-3 to Markus Weschenfelder, Martin Fritz, Martin Bastmeyer, Franco Weth.

  • Studienstiftung des Deutschen Volkes PhD Scholarship to Anne von Philipsborn.

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft Open Access Publishing Fund of KIT to Franco Weth.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

FF, Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

MW, Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology.

MF, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization.

AvP, Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology.

MB, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition.

FW, Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Visualization, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

References

  1. Alther TA, Domanitskaya E, Stoeckli ET. Calsyntenin 1-mediated trafficking of axon guidance receptors regulates the switch in axonal responsiveness at a choice point. Development. 2016;143:994–1004. doi: 10.1242/dev.127449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Averaimo S, Assali A, Ros O, Couvet S, Zagar Y, Genescu I, Rebsam A, Nicol X. A plasma membrane microdomain compartmentalizes ephrin-generated cAMP signals to prune developing retinal axon arbors. Nature Communications. 2016;7:12896. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12896. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baba A, Akagi K, Takayanagi M, Flanagan JG, Kobayashi T, Hattori M. Fyn tyrosine kinase regulates the surface expression of glycosylphosphatidylinositol-linked ephrin via the modulation of sphingomyelin metabolism. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2009;284:9206–9214. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M809401200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baier H, Bonhoeffer F. Axon guidance by gradients of a target-derived component. Science. 1992;255:472–475. doi: 10.1126/science.1734526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Boissier P, Chen J, Huynh-Do U. EphA2 signaling following endocytosis: role of Tiam1. Traffic. 2013;14:1255–1271. doi: 10.1111/tra.12123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Boscher C, Zheng YZ, Lakshminarayan R, Johannes L, Dennis JW, Foster LJ, Nabi IR. Galectin-3 protein regulates mobility of N-cadherin and GM1 ganglioside at cell-cell junctions of mammary carcinoma cells. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2012;287:32940–32952. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M112.353334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Campbell DS, Holt CE. Chemotropic responses of retinal growth cones mediated by rapid local protein synthesis and degradation. Neuron. 2001;32:1013–1026. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00551-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chakraborty S, Veettil MV, Bottero V, Chandran B. Kaposi's sarcoma-associated herpesvirus interacts with EphrinA2 receptor to amplify signaling essential for productive infection. PNAS. 2012;109:E1163–E1172. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1119592109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chavent M, Seiradake E, Jones EY, Sansom MS. Structures of the EphA2 receptor at the membrane: role of lipid interactions. Structure. 2016;24:337–347. doi: 10.1016/j.str.2015.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chiantia S, Kahya N, Ries J, Schwille P. Effects of ceramide on liquid-ordered domains investigated by simultaneous AFM and FCS. Biophysical Journal. 2006;90:4500–4508. doi: 10.1529/biophysj.106.081026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cox EC, Müller B, Bonhoeffer F. Axonal guidance in the chick visual system: posterior tectal membranes induce collapse of growth cones from the temporal retina. Neuron. 1990;4:31–37. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(90)90441-H. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dickson BJ. Molecular mechanisms of axon guidance. Science. 2002;298:1959–1964. doi: 10.1126/science.1072165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Egea J, Klein R. Bidirectional eph-ephrin signaling during axon guidance. Trends in Cell Biology. 2007;17:230–238. doi: 10.1016/j.tcb.2007.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Ellis C, Kasmi F, Ganju P, Walls E, Panayotou G, Reith AD. A juxtamembrane autophosphorylation site in the eph family receptor tyrosine kinase, Sek, mediates high affinity interaction with p59fyn. Oncogene. 1996;12:1727–1736. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Feldheim DA, O'Leary DD. Visual map development: bidirectional signaling, bifunctional guidance molecules, and competition. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 2010;2:a001768. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a001768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Foster LJ, De Hoog CL, Mann M. Unbiased quantitative proteomics of lipid rafts reveals high specificity for signaling factors. PNAS. 2003;100:5813–5818. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0631608100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gauthier LR, Robbins SM. Ephrin signaling: one raft to rule them all? one raft to sort them? one raft to spread their call and in signaling bind them? Life Sciences. 2003;74:207–216. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2003.09.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gautier A, Juillerat A, Heinis C, Corrêa IR, Kindermann M, Beaufils F, Johnsson K. An engineered protein tag for multiprotein labeling in living cells. Chemistry & Biology. 2008;15:128–136. doi: 10.1016/j.chembiol.2008.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gebhardt C, Bastmeyer M, Weth F. Balancing of ephrin/Eph forward and reverse signaling as the driving force of adaptive topographic mapping. Development. 2012;139:335–345. doi: 10.1242/dev.070474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Goldenring JR. Recycling endosomes. Current Opinion in Cell Biology. 2015;35:117–122. doi: 10.1016/j.ceb.2015.04.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Goodhill GJ, Richards LJ. Retinotectal maps: molecules, models and misplaced data. Trends in Neurosciences. 1999;22:529–534. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(99)01469-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gosse NJ, Nevin LM, Baier H. Retinotopic order in the absence of axon competition. Nature. 2008;452:892–895. doi: 10.1038/nature06816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Goñi FM, Alonso A. Effects of ceramide and other simple sphingolipids on membrane lateral structure. Biochimica Et Biophysica Acta (BBA) - Biomembranes. 2009;1788:169–177. doi: 10.1016/j.bbamem.2008.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Grollman AP. Inhibitors of protein biosynthesis. II. Mode of action of anisomycin. The Journal of Biological Chemistry. 1967;242:3226–3233. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gu MX, Fu Y, Sun XL, Ding YZ, Li CH, Pang W, Pan S, Zhu Y. Proteomic analysis of endothelial lipid rafts reveals a novel role of statins in antioxidation. Journal of Proteome Research. 2012;11:2365–2373. doi: 10.1021/pr300098f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hand R, Bortone D, Mattar P, Nguyen L, Heng JI, Guerrier S, Boutt E, Peters E, Barnes AP, Parras C, Schuurmans C, Guillemot F, Polleux F. Phosphorylation of Neurogenin2 specifies the migration properties and the dendritic morphology of pyramidal neurons in the neocortex. Neuron. 2005;48:45–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.08.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hansen MJ, Dallal GE, Flanagan JG. Retinal axon response to ephrin-as shows a graded, concentration-dependent transition from growth promotion to inhibition. Neuron. 2004;42:717–730. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Himanen JP, Rajashankar KR, Lackmann M, Cowan CA, Henkemeyer M, Nikolov DB. Crystal structure of an eph receptor-ephrin complex. Nature. 2001;414:933–938. doi: 10.1038/414933a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hjorth JJ, Sterratt DC, Cutts CS, Willshaw DJ, Eglen SJ. Quantitative assessment of computational models for retinotopic map formation. Developmental Neurobiology. 2015;75:641–666. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hornberger MR, Dütting D, Ciossek T, Yamada T, Handwerker C, Lang S, Weth F, Huf J, Wessel R, Logan C, Tanaka H, Drescher U. Modulation of EphA receptor function by coexpressed ephrinA ligands on retinal ganglion cell axons. Neuron. 1999;22:731–742. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80732-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Inder KL, Zheng YZ, Davis MJ, Moon H, Loo D, Nguyen H, Clements JA, Parton RG, Foster LJ, Hill MM. Expression of PTRF in PC-3 cells modulates cholesterol dynamics and the actin cytoskeleton impacting secretion pathways. Molecular & Cellular Proteomics. 2012;11:M111.012245. doi: 10.1074/mcp.M111.012245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Janes PW, Nievergall E, Lackmann M. Concepts and consequences of eph receptor clustering. Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology. 2012;23:43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2012.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kania A, Klein R. Mechanisms of ephrin-Eph signalling in development, physiology and disease. Nature Reviews Molecular Cell Biology. 2016;17:240–256. doi: 10.1038/nrm.2015.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kao TJ, Kania A. Ephrin-mediated cis-attenuation of eph receptor signaling is essential for spinal motor axon guidance. Neuron. 2011;71:76–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kapfhammer JP, Xu H, Raper JA. The detection and quantification of growth cone collapsing activities. Nature Protocols. 2007;2:2005–2011. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2007.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kim YM, Kim J, Heo SC, Shin SH, Do EK, Suh DS, Kim KH, Yoon MS, Lee TG, Kim JH. Proteomic identification of ADAM12 as a regulator for TGF-β1-induced differentiation of human mesenchymal stem cells to smooth muscle cells. PLoS One. 2012;7:e40820. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kolodkin AL, Tessier-Lavigne M. Mechanisms and molecules of neuronal wiring: a primer. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 2011;3:a001727. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a001727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lee SI, Zhang W, Ravi M, Weschenfelder M, Bastmeyer M, Levine JM. Atypical protein kinase C and Par3 are required for proteoglycan-induced axon growth inhibition. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:2541–2554. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3154-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lemke G, Reber M. Retinotectal mapping: new insights from molecular genetics. Annual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology. 2005;21:551–580. doi: 10.1146/annurev.cellbio.20.022403.093702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lisabeth EM, Falivelli G, Pasquale EB. Eph receptor signaling and ephrins. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 2013;5:a009159. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a009159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lowery LA, Van Vactor D. The trip of the tip: understanding the growth cone machinery. Nature Reviews Molecular Cell Biology. 2009;10:332–343. doi: 10.1038/nrm2679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Mahammad S, Parmryd I. Cholesterol depletion using methyl-β-cyclodextrin. Methods in Molecular Biology. 2015;1232:91–102. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-1752-5_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Marquardt T, Shirasaki R, Ghosh S, Andrews SE, Carter N, Hunter T, Pfaff SL. Coexpressed EphA receptors and ephrin-A ligands mediate opposing actions on growth cone navigation from distinct membrane domains. Cell. 2005;121:127–139. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2005.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. McLaughlin T, O'Leary DD. Molecular gradients and development of retinotopic maps. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2005;28:327–355. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. McMahon HT, Boucrot E. Molecular mechanism and physiological functions of clathrin-mediated endocytosis. Nature Reviews Molecular Cell Biology. 2011;12:517–533. doi: 10.1038/nrm3151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Ming GL, Wong ST, Henley J, Yuan XB, Song HJ, Spitzer NC, Poo MM. Adaptation in the chemotactic guidance of nerve growth cones. Nature. 2002;417:411–418. doi: 10.1038/nature745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Neufeld EB, Cooney AM, Pitha J, Dawidowicz EA, Dwyer NK, Pentchev PG, Blanchette-Mackie EJ. Intracellular trafficking of cholesterol monitored with a cyclodextrin. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 1996;271:21604–21613. doi: 10.1074/jbc.271.35.21604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Nikolov DB, Xu K, Himanen JP. Eph/ephrin recognition and the role of Eph/ephrin clusters in signaling initiation. Biochimica Et Biophysica Acta (BBA) - Proteins and Proteomics. 2013;1834:2160–2165. doi: 10.1016/j.bbapap.2013.04.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Parkinson JS, Hazelbauer GL, Falke JJ. Signaling and sensory adaptation in escherichia coli chemoreceptors: 2015 update. Trends in Microbiology. 2015;23:257–266. doi: 10.1016/j.tim.2015.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Piper M, Salih S, Weinl C, Holt CE, Harris WA. Endocytosis-dependent desensitization and protein synthesis-dependent resensitization in retinal growth cone adaptation. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:179–186. doi: 10.1038/nn1380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Rizk A, Paul G, Incardona P, Bugarski M, Mansouri M, Niemann A, Ziegler U, Berger P, Sbalzarini IF. Segmentation and quantification of subcellular structures in fluorescence microscopy images using squassh. Nature Protocols. 2014;9:586–596. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2014.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Rosentreter SM, Davenport RW, Löschinger J, Huf J, Jung J, Bonhoeffer F. Response of retinal ganglion cell axons to striped linear gradients of repellent guidance molecules. Journal of Neurobiology. 1998;37:541–562. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4695(199812)37:4&#x0003c;541::AID-NEU4&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Róg T, Vattulainen I. Cholesterol, sphingolipids, and glycolipids: what do we know about their role in raft-like membranes? Chemistry and Physics of Lipids. 2014;184:82–104. doi: 10.1016/j.chemphyslip.2014.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Sabet O, Stockert R, Xouri G, Brüggemann Y, Stanoev A, Bastiaens PI. Ubiquitination switches EphA2 vesicular traffic from a continuous safeguard to a finite signalling mode. Nature Communications. 2015;6:8047. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Sandvig K, Pust S, Skotland T, van Deurs B. Clathrin-independent endocytosis: mechanisms and function. Current Opinion in Cell Biology. 2011;23:413–420. doi: 10.1016/j.ceb.2011.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Schmick M, Bastiaens PI. The interdependence of membrane shape and cellular signal processing. Cell. 2014;156:1132–1138. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Simons K, Ikonen E. Functional rafts in cell membranes. Nature. 1997;387:569–572. doi: 10.1038/42408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Simons K, Sampaio JL. Membrane organization and lipid rafts. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 2011;3:a004697. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a004697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Simpson HD, Mortimer D, Goodhill GJ. Chapter 1 Theoretical Models of Neural Circuit Development. 2009;87:1–51. doi: 10.1016/S0070-2153(09)01201-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sperry RW Chemoaffinity in the orderly growth of nerve fiber patterns and connections. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1963;50:703–710. doi: 10.1073/pnas.50.4.703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Suetterlin P, Marler KM, Drescher U. Axonal ephrinA/EphA interactions, and the emergence of order in topographic projections. Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology. 2012;23:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2011.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Triplett JW, Feldheim DA. Eph and ephrin signaling in the formation of topographic maps. Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology. 2012;23:7–15. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2011.10.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Vergara MN, Gutierrez C, O'Brien DR, Canto-Soler MV. Ex vivo electroporation of retinal cells: a novel, high efficiency method for functional studies in primary retinal cultures. Experimental Eye Research. 2013;109:40–50. doi: 10.1016/j.exer.2013.01.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Vitriol EA, Zheng JQ. Growth cone travel in space and time: the cellular ensemble of cytoskeleton, adhesion, and membrane. Neuron. 2012;73:1068–1081. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. von Kleist L, Stahlschmidt W, Bulut H, Gromova K, Puchkov D, Robertson MJ, MacGregor KA, Tomilin N, Tomlin N, Pechstein A, Chau N, Chircop M, Sakoff J, von Kries JP, Saenger W, Kräusslich HG, Shupliakov O, Robinson PJ, McCluskey A, Haucke V. Role of the clathrin terminal domain in regulating coated pit dynamics revealed by small molecule inhibition. Cell. 2011;146:471–484. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.06.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. von Philipsborn AC, Lang S, Bernard A, Loeschinger J, David C, Lehnert D, Bastmeyer M, Bonhoeffer F. Microcontact printing of axon guidance molecules for generation of graded patterns. Nature Protocols. 2006a;1:1322–1328. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. von Philipsborn AC, Lang S, Loeschinger J, Bernard A, David C, Lehnert D, Bonhoeffer F, Bastmeyer M. Growth cone navigation in substrate-bound ephrin gradients. Development. 2006b;133:2487–2495. doi: 10.1242/dev.02412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Weth F, Fiederling F, Gebhardt C, Bastmeyer M. Chemoaffinity in topographic mapping revisited--is it more about fiber-fiber than fiber-target interactions? Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology. 2014;35:126–135. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2014.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Weth F. RTP_Co-adapt_Model. 13c245fhttps://github.com/FrancoWeth/RTP_Co-adapt_Model Github. 2017
  70. Wu C, Butz S, Ying Y, Anderson RG. Tyrosine kinase receptors concentrated in caveolae-like domains from neuronal plasma membrane. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 1997;272:3554–3559. doi: 10.1074/jbc.272.6.3554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Yang W, Di Vizio D, Kirchner M, Steen H, Freeman MR. Proteome scale characterization of human S-acylated proteins in lipid raft-enriched and non-raft membranes. Molecular & Cellular Proteomics. 2010;9:54–70. doi: 10.1074/mcp.M800448-MCP200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Yi JS, Mun DG, Lee H, Park JS, Lee JW, Lee JS, Kim SJ, Cho BR, Lee SW, Ko YG. PTRF/cavin-1 is essential for multidrug resistance in Cancer cells. Journal of Proteome Research. 2013;12:605–614. doi: 10.1021/pr300651m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Yoo S, Shin J, Park S. EphA8-ephrinA5 signaling and clathrin-mediated endocytosis is regulated by Tiam-1, a Rac-specific guanine nucleotide exchange factor. Molecules and Cells. 2010;29:603–609. doi: 10.1007/s10059-010-0075-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Zheng YZ, Boscher C, Inder KL, Fairbank M, Loo D, Hill MM, Nabi IR, Foster LJ. Differential impact of caveolae and caveolin-1 scaffolds on the membrane raft proteome. Molecular & Cellular Proteomics. 2011;10:M110.007146. doi: 10.1074/mcp.M110.007146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2017 Jul 19;6:e25533. doi: 10.7554/eLife.25533.030

Decision letter

Editor: David D Ginty1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Ephrin-A/EphA specific co-adaptation as a novel mechanism in topographic axon guidance" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers and a Reviewing Editor, and the evaluation has been overseen by the Reviewing Editor and Marianne Bronner as the Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The Reviewers and Reviewing Editor have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

Fiederlin and co-workers address a fundamental problem in topographic map formation; the ability of axons to adapt to external guidance cues (de/re-sensitisation) on the one hand, and the requirement of growth cones for quantitative signaling to enable them to orient within a guidance gradient on the other hand. The present study using in vitro signaling assays and computational modeling in an attempt to reconcile this conundrum and advance the field.

The authors have worked for some time on a (molecular) understanding of topographic mapping in the chick retinotectal projection in vitro, in particular focussing on the EphA/ephrin-A family. The new and exciting finding of the present study is the identification and characterization of co-adaptation between forward and reverse ephrin-A/EphA signaling modules; Adaptation in forward signaling is accompanied by adaptation in reverse signaling, thereby maintaining topographically important information and a balance of forward and reverse signaling. The authors' findings are supported and complemented by computational modeling approaches, which in turn provide further insights into potential mechanisms of co-adaptation. Overall, the experiments are of high quality and well documented, and the study is of general interest because it provides a clearer understanding of the molecular mechanisms of topographic map formation.

Essential revisions:

1) The modeling predictions of disrupting co-adaptation are intriguing but remain untested experimentally. We would like to see additional in vitro manipulations showing that co-adaptation is disrupted following treatment with sphingomyelinase. One possible experiment to address this would be the Figure 5 co-adaptation assay repeated in the presence of sphingomyelinase.

2) The interplay (or lack thereof) between the Eph/ephrin system and Sema3A is an interesting feature of the system and should be expanded to elevate the scope and impact of the work. What is the behavior of retinal axons in a gap assay with Sema3A only? What is the outcome of a gap assay in which a Sema3A source is followed – after the gap – by an ephrin-A5 substrate? Does Sema3A alter ephrinA or EphA surface localization?

eLife. 2017 Jul 19;6:e25533. doi: 10.7554/eLife.25533.031

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) The modeling predictions of disrupting co-adaptation are intriguing but remain untested experimentally. We would like to see additional in vitro manipulations showing that co-adaptation is disrupted following treatment with sphingomyelinase. One possible experiment to address this would be the Figure 5 co-adaptation assay repeated in the presence of sphingomyelinase.

To respond to this point, we have included a complete new set of experiments. The results are presented in the new Figure 9, explained in the Results paragraph “Disrupting membrane rafts induces adaptation” and discussed further in the Discussion paragraph “Repartitioning between membrane lipid microdomains as a potential mechanism of allocating sensors to the co-adaptation pathway”. The old Figure 9 has been moved to the supplement (now Figure 9—figure supplement 1). Corresponding additions have been made to the Materials and methods paragraphs “Collapse assays” and “Gap assays”.

Briefly, we turned from sphingomyelinase to methyl-β-cyclodextrin (MβCD) to manipulate lipid rafts and to study their influence on co-adaptation. SMase generates ceramide, which by itself forms new lipid microdomains with cholesterol (Chiantia et al., 2006; Goni and Alonso, 2009), potentially confounding the interpretation of the results. In addition, the activity of SMase in overnight culture media is hard to control. MβCD, in contrast, is a well-established drug to disrupt lipid rafts by cholesterol scavenging (Mahammad and Parmryd, 2015). We applied MβCD in collapse as well as in co-adaption gap assays. In both cases, we find a profound desensitizing effect of the drug, confirming the previous SMase data. In both forward and reverse signaling collapse assays, axons regenerate faster in the presence of the drug, corresponding to more efficient desensitization. In ephrin-A5 – EphA3 as well as in EphA3 – ephrin-A5 co-adaptation gap assays, axons also are more desensitized, indicated by overgrowing larger gaps. These results strongly support the proposed mechanism of co-adaptation (Figure 11), predicting that raft disintegration would release ephrin-As and EphAs into the liquid-disordered phase of the membrane, from where they are constitutively endocytosed to desensitize the growth cone.

2) The interplay (or lack thereof) between the Eph/ephrin system and Sema3A is an interesting feature of the system and should be expanded to elevate the scope and impact of the work. What is the behavior of retinal axons in a gap assay with Sema3A only? What is the outcome of a gap assay in which a Sema3A source is followed – after the gap – by an ephrin-A5 substrate? Does Sema3A alter ephrinA or EphA surface localization?

To answer these questions, we have included another new set of experiments. The results are shown in the new Figure 5—figure supplement 1. They are eluded to in the Results paragraphs “EphA forward signaling in fact co-adapts ephrin-A reverse signaling and vice versa” (second paragraph) and “Desensitization involves clearance of sensors from the GC surface by clathrin-mediated endocytosis” (first paragraph). Necessary additions have been made in the Materials and methods paragraph “Gap assays”.

Briefly, retinal axons display very efficient desensitization towards Sema3A in gap-assays. The desensitization is so efficient that we do not see obvious re-sensitization even with the largest gaps (215µm) available with our contact-printing stamps. Along the way, we observed an interesting behavior of the RGC axons on the Sema3A gap substrates: As soon as the adapted axons leave the Sema3A pedestal and continue to grow on laminin in the gap, they display a strong tendency to defasciculate and to form branches. Dwelling deeper into this phenomenon is, however, beyond the scope of this manuscript.

In Sema3A – ephrin-A5 double-cue gap assays, we observe no co-adaptation. Correspondingly, the SNAP-ephrin-A5 surface localization probe is not internalized on Sema3A. These observations clearly indicate that the two guidance systems (ephrin-A/EphA and Sema3A) do not interplay in retinal growth cones and display an orthogonal behavior. This finding supports the idea, that co-adaptation is a specific feature of the topographic guidance system.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 2—source data 1. Original data underlying bar charts of Figure 2A, C.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.005

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.005
    Figure 2—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Original data underlying bar charts of Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.007

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.007
    Figure 3—source data 1. Original data underlying bar chart of Figure 3C.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.009

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.009
    Figure 5—source data 1. Original data underlying bar chart of Figure 5D.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.013

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.013
    Figure 5—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Original data underlying bar chart of Figure 5—figure supplement 1H.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.015

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.015
    Figure 6—source data 1. Original data underlying the bar charts of Figure 6B—D.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.017

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.017
    Figure 7—source data 1. Original data underlying the bar chart in Figure 7D.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.020

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.020
    Figure 8—source data 1. Original data underlying the bar chart of Figure 8D.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.023

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.023
    Figure 9—source data 1. Original data underlying the bar charts in Figure 9A, B, E.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.025

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.025
    Figure 9—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Original data underlying the bar charts of Figure 9—figure supplement 1.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25533.027

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.25533.027

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES