Abstract
Cognitive impairment is a common and disabling problem in Parkinson’s disease (PD). Identification of genetic variants that influence the presence or severity of cognitive deficits in PD might provide a clearer understanding of the pathophysiology underlying this important nonmotor feature. We genotyped 1,105 PD patients from the PD Cognitive Genetics Consortium for 249,336 variants using the NeuroX array. Participants underwent assessments of learning and memory (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised [HVLT-R]), working memory/executive function (Letter-Number Sequencing and Trail Making Test [TMT] A and B), language processing (semantic and phonemic verbal fluency), visuospatial abilities (Benton Judgment of Line Orientation [JoLO]), and global cognitive function (Montreal Cognitive Assessment). For common variants we used linear regression to test for association between genotype and cognitive performance with adjustment for important covariates. Rare variants were analyzed using the optimal unified sequence kernel association test. The significance threshold was defined as a false discovery rate corrected P-value (PFDR) of 0.05. Eighteen common variants in 13 genomic regions exceeded the significance threshold for one of the cognitive tests. These included GBA rs2230288 (E326K; PFDR = 2.7 × 10−4) for JoLO, PARP4 rs9318600 (PFDR = 0.006) and rs9581094 (PFDR = 0.006) for HVLT-R total recall, and MTCL1 rs34877994 (PFDR = 0.01) for TMT B-A. Analysis of rare variants did not yield any significant gene regions. We have conducted the first large-scale PD cognitive genetics analysis and nominated several new putative susceptibility genes for cognitive impairment in PD. These results will require replication in independent PD cohorts.
Introduction
Cognitive dysfunction is a common, debilitating, and difficult to treat problem in Parkinson’s disease (PD).(Goldman and Weintraub, 2015, Meireles and Massano, 2012) The rate of decline in overall cognitive function and individual cognitive domains is highly variable for reasons that are not well understood.(Aarsland et al., 2004, Janvin et al., 2006) The identification of genetic variants that contribute to this phenotypic heterogeneity could reveal important insights into the pathophysiology of cognitive impairment in PD.
Using a candidate gene approach in cross-sectional studies, our group and others have identified variants in genes that are associated with either higher (LRRK2) or lower (APOE, GBA) overall cognitive performance among patients with PD.(Alcalay et al., 2012, Alcalay et al., 2015, Mata et al., 2016, Mata et al., 2014, Paul et al., 2016, Srivatsal et al., 2015) Furthermore, we have shown that these genes have a differential impact on specific cognitive domains. For example, in PD patients without dementia the APOEε4 allele is associated with lower performance on word list learning and semantic verbal fluency while GBA variants predict worse scores on tests of working memory/executive function and visuospatial abilities. Other studies have nominated variants in several other genes, including COMT and MAPT, as modifiers of cognitive dysfunction in PD,(Foltynie et al., 2004, Williams-Gray et al., 2009) but these results have not been uniformly replicated.(Hoogland et al., 2010, Mata et al., 2014, Paul et al., 2016)
Large scale genetic studies of cognition in PD have never been conducted. This is in part due to the limited availability of PD cohorts with detailed neuropsychological test data. Here we report the results of an exploratory analysis of genetic risk factors for cognitive impairment in PD in a multisite cohort using the NeuroX array that includes nearly 250,000 genetic markers.
Patients and Methods
Participants
The study population was comprised of 1,226 patients with PD enrolled at six sites from the PD Cognitive Genetics Consortium (PDCGC; Supplementary Materials). All participants met UK PD Society Brain Bank clinical diagnostic criteria for PD (Gibb and Lees, 1988) (modified so that having more than one affected relative was not considered an exclusion criterion), except those from University of California Los Angeles (UCLA) who satisfied clinical diagnostic criteria for PD as described elsewhere (Kang et al., 2005).
Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents were obtained. All study procedures were approved by the institutional review boards at each participating site.
Neuropsychological Assessment
All participants underwent detailed psychometric testing in the on state (if receiving medication). Seven tests that were administered by at least five of the six sites were defined as the “core battery” (Supplementary Table 1). We selected (a priori) nine variables for analysis from the core battery that represent the primary measures most commonly used in a clinical setting.
These “core variables” were: total scores for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA); Letter-Number Sequencing Test (LNST); Trail Making Test (TMT) B-A, semantic and phonemic verbal fluency; Benton Judgment of Line Orientation (JoLO); Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R) total recall; HVLT-R delayed recall; and HVLT-R recognition discrimination index. Data from participants enrolled at four of the six PDCGC sites (Supplementary Materials) were reviewed at diagnostic consensus conferences and participants were classified as demented or nondemented, as previously described (Chahine et al., 2013, Cholerton et al., 2013). The nondemented group included participants with either no cognitive impairment or mild cognitive impairment (MCI).
Genotyping
Genomic DNA was extracted from peripheral blood using standard techniques. All DNA samples were genotyped on the NeuroX array at the Center for Applied Genomics, Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia. NeuroX is a combination of the Illumina Human Exome array v1.1 (242,901 variants) and custom content (24,706 variants) focused on neurologic diseases. The exome array primarily (90%) contains nonsynonymous single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs). While the exome array contains a high proportion of rare variants (82% have a minor allele frequency [MAF] < 0.01), the majority (60%) of variants within the custom content are common and have a MAF between 0.05 and 0.50. Further details on the design and contents of the NeuroX array have been published elsewhere (Nalls et al., 2015) and are also provided in the Supplementary Materials. Automated allele calling was performed with Illumina GenomeStudio Software using a previously developed NeuroX-specific cluster file (Nalls et al., 2015), but none of the cluster plots were manually inspected.
Statistical Analysis
We excluded markers with genotyping call rates < 95% and those that were out of Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) (P < 0.0001). To account for population structure we calculated eigenvectors from principal component analysis (PCA) using SNPStats based on 10,177 markers from the exome array with HapMap3 CEU, CHB, JPT, and YRI population samples as the reference. From visual inspection of scatter plots of the first two principal components we created a subjective boundary to define subjects of European ancestry (Supplementary Fig 1). We performed identity-by-descent estimation with the same markers to assess for erroneous duplicates and cryptic relatedness, using a threshold of Pi-hat > 0.40. The samples genotyped included one known duplicate and nine relatives. All of these individuals were correctly identified and removed, but no other duplicates or relatives were discovered.
Histograms were created for each of the nine core cognitive variables, and for those that were non-normally distributed (MoCA, JoLO, and HVLT-R recognition discrimination index), a squared transformation was employed to improve the fit to normality.
For common variants with MAF > 0.01 we performed linear regression to test for association between genotype and each of the core cognitive variables under an additive model. All models were adjusted for age, sex, disease duration, years of education, site, and the first three principal components from the PCA (to adjust for population structure). For rare variants with MAF < 0.01 we used the optimal unified sequence kernel association test (SKAT-O)(Lee et al., 2012) to test for association between all rare variants within each gene region (computing gene-level P-values) and the same cognitive variables adjusting for the same covariates. Because this was an exploratory study we used a liberal significance threshold defined as a false discovery rate (FDR) corrected P-value (PFDR) of 0.05. For comparison, we also calculated the family wise error rate using the Holm–Bonferroni method (Holm, 1979). All analyses were performed using R 3.2.3.
Results
Of the 1,226 patients in the initial cohort, we excluded 121 individuals for the following reasons: 61 were of non-European ancestry, four failed genotyping, 19 were missing data for one or more covariates, 9 were related to another participant in the cohort, and 28 failed to complete at least half of the cognitive tests (this was done to reduce the influence of floor effects on cognitive test scores in patients with advanced dementia). The characteristics of the 1,105 participants included in the final analysis are summarized in Table 1. In the overall cohort the mean age was 68.8 ± 9.2 years, the mean disease duration was 8.4 ± 5.8 years, and 67.8% of the subjects were male.
Table 1.
Site | N | Male, n (%) | Age, y
|
Disease Duration | Years of Education | Demented, n (%) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
At Testing mean ± SD | At Diagnosis mean ± SD | At Disease Onset mean ± SD | ||||||
Emory University | 142 | 93 (65.5) | 65.2 ± 9.1 | 59.4 ± 9.8 | 57.7 ± 9.8 | 7.5 ± 4.5 | 15.7 ± 2.4 | NA |
PANUC (Portland) | 119 | 105 (88.2) | 68.4 ± 8.0 | 61.0 ± 10.6 | 58.6 ± 11.3 | 9.9 ± 6.7 | 15.6 ± 2.9 | 27 (22.7) |
PANUC (Seattle) | 432 | 279 (64.6) | 67.8 ± 9.5 | 60.7 ± 11.1 | 58.3 ± 11.4 | 9.5 ± 6.6 | 15.8 ± 2.5 | 85 (19.7) |
UCLA | 163 | 94 (57.7) | 72.6 ± 9.5 | 67.2 ± 9.7 | NC | 5.4 ± 2.4 | 14.4 ± 2.8 | NA |
U. Cincinnati | 30 | 24 (80.0) | 63.6 ± 7.7 | 59.9 ± 8.2 | 57.2 ± 8.2 | 6.4 ± 3.2 | 15.3 ± 2.8 | 3 (10.0) |
U. Pennsylvania | 219 | 154 (70.3) | 71.0 ± 7.5 | 64.2 ± 8.4 | 62.6 ± 8.8 | 8.4 ± 5.2 | 15.9 ± 2.4 | 25 (11.4) |
Total | 1105 | 749 (67.8) | 68.8 ± 9.2 | 62.2 ± 10.4 | 59.2 ± 10.6 | 8.4 ± 5.8 | 15.5 ± 2.6 | 140 (17.5) |
NA, not applicable; NC, not collected; PANUC, Pacific Northwest Udall Center; UCLA, University of California, Los Angeles
After restricting the marker set to autosomal single nucleotide substitutions, and excluding variants that failed genotyping (n = 11,350) or were out of HWE (n = 796), 249,336 variants remained available for analysis, of which 46,871 were common and 202,465 were rare. Single-marker analyses of the common variants yielded 18 markers in 13 genomic regions that exceeded the significance threshold for one of the cognitive tests, and none of these variants were significant for more than one test (Table 2). Sixteen of the variants occurred within or in close proximity to a known gene, and two were located within an intergenic region. Across all cognitive measures the strongest associations observed were for transformed JoLO scores and markers within GBA (rs2230288 [E326K]; PFDR = 2.7 × 10−4) and ACSBG2 (rs79266675; PFDR = 8.3 × 10−4), and for TMT B-A and variants in RYR1 (rs55876273; PFDR = 6.4 × 10−4) and IFT140 (rs146128830; PFDR = 6.4 × 10−4). None of the common variants examined were significantly associated with semantic or phonemic verbal fluency, LNST, transformed MoCA, or HVLT-R recognition discrimination index scores. Manhattan plots for the four cognitive variables associated with at least one variant are presented in Fig 1 and plots for the remaining five variables are shown in Supplementary Fig 2. The top 50 variants for each of the core cognitive variables, ranked by P-value, are presented in Supplementary Tables 2–10. Inspection of the QQ plots for each cognitive variable (Supplementary Fig 3) indicated that most of the observed associations lay close to the expected distribution, suggesting that there was no substantial inflation in the test statistics due to population structure or other sources.
Table 2.
Test | Gene Region | Chr | Position (hg19) | SNP | Type | MAF | N | β | SE | P | PFDR | PFWE |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
HVLT-R TR | PARP4 | 13 | 25068808 | rs9318600 | missense | 0.10 | 1039 | 1.88 | 0.37 | 2.9 × 10−7 | 0.0059 | 0.014 |
MDM1 | 12 | 68720534 | rs117673673 | missense | 0.01 | 1040 | −3.62 | 0.71 | 3.7 × 10−7 | 0.0059 | 0.017 | |
PARP4 | 13 | 25082630 | rs9581094 | intronic | 0.15 | 1040 | 1.65 | 0.32 | 3.8 × 10−7 | 0.0059 | 0.018 | |
HVLT-R DR | ALS2CR11 | 2 | 202352480 | rs72939119 | missense | 0.02 | 1038 | 1.66 | 0.31 | 5.7 × 10−8 | 0.0027 | 0.003 |
FAT3 | 11 | 92577659 | rs75081660 | missense | 0.01 | 1038 | 2.07 | 0.43 | 1.4 × 10−6 | 0.032 | 0.06 | |
TMT B-Aa | RYR1 | 19 | 39018347 | rs55876273 | missense | 0.02 | 867 | −34.84 | 6.26 | 2.6 × 10−78 | 6.4 × 10−4 | 0.001 |
IFT140 | 16 | 1652418 | rs146128830 | missense | 0.01 | 867 | −37.05 | 6.67 | 2.7 × 10−08 | 6.4 × 10−4 | 0.001 | |
MTCL1 | 18 | 8806925 | rs34877994 | missense | 0.01 | 867 | −41.13 | 8.27 | 6.7 × 10−7 | 0.0096 | 0.03 | |
MOCS3 | 20 | 49576664 | rs7269297 | missense | 0.01 | 867 | −36.47 | 7.40 | 8.2 × 10−7 | 0.0096 | 0.04 | |
Intergenic | 9 | 97314741 | rs16910061 | intergenic | 0.02 | 867 | −28.13 | 6.07 | 3.6 × 10−6 | 0.034 | 0.17 | |
RASAL3 | 19 | 15565646 | rs56209154 | missense | 0.02 | 867 | −29.23 | 6.46 | 6.0 × 10−6 | 0.047 | 0.28 | |
JoLOb,c | GBAd | 1 | 155206167 | rs2230288 | missense | 0.03 | 919 | −143.61 | 25.16 | 1.2 × 10−8 | 2.7 × 10−4 | 2.8 × 10−4 |
ACSBG2 | 19 | 6141593 | rs79266675 | missense | 0.02 | 919 | 159.86 | 29.67 | 7.1 × 10−8 | 8.3 × 10−4 | 0.003 | |
Intergenic | 9 | 8230433 | rs1984216 | intergenic | 0.43 | 919 | −44.12 | 9.66 | 4.9 × 10−6 | 0.033 | 0.23 |
Key: Chr, chromosome; HVLT-R, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised; DR, delayed recall; TR, total recall; JoLO, Benton Judgment of Line Orientation; MAF, minor allele frequency; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; PFDR, false discovery rate-corrected P-value; PFWE, family wise error-corrected P-value; TMT, Trail Making Test.
Not administered at University of Pennsylvania
Not administered at University of California, Los Angeles
Based on square-transformed scores.
Not shown are results from four SNPs in neighboring genes (rs71628662 [ASH1L], rs12726330 [SLC50A1], rs71630614 [LMNA], rs34372695 [RAB25]) that were each highly correlated with GBA rs2230288 (see Supplementary Materials for details).
One of the variants that failed genotyping on the NeuroX array was rs429358, which differentiates the APOE ε3 and ε4 alleles. Because APOE ε4 is known to influence cognitive performance in PD (Mata et al., 2014, Morley et al., 2012, Paul et al., 2016), we separately genotyped rs429358 on all subjects using a TaqMan assay as previously described (Mata et al., 2014) and analyzed the ε4 allele using the methods employed for all other common variants. After correction for multiple testing, APOE ε4 was not associated with any of the core cognitive variables (Supplementary Table 11). However, it did approach significance for HVLT-R total recall (P = 6.1 × 10−05; PFDR = 0.36) (Supplementary Table 5).
Because some studies have reported that common variants in COMT and MAPT, and rare missense mutations and multiplications of SNCA, are associated with cognitive performance or dementia in PD (Foltynie et al., 2004, Monchi et al., 2016, Williams-Gray et al., 2009), we present the results for these three genes separately. The final dataset included 1 common (rs4680; V158M) and 2 rare variants for COMT, 102 common and 6 rare variants for MAPT, and 45 common and no rare variants for SNCA. However, none of these variants approached significance for any of the cognitive tests in single marker analyses (Supplementary Tables 12–14) or analyses of rare variants using SKAT-O (data not shown).
The analysis of rare variants using a gene-centric approach did not yield any genes that exceeded the significance threshold (data not shown). However, PERP was marginally associated with HVLT-R delayed recall scores (P = 3.7 × 10−7; PFDR = 0.053) based on results for two rare markers.
Discussion
This study represents the first large-scale genetic analysis of cognition in PD. We have nominated several genetic variants as putative susceptibility or protective factors for cognitive impairment in PD, and the vast majority of the genes in which they occur have not previously been linked to PD motor or cognitive phenotypes. A major strength of the study was that the analysis was based on performance on an extensive neuropsychological battery, which provided an opportunity to discern genetic effects within individual cognitive domains. Recent studies suggest that this is a substantially more sensitive approach than relying solely on global screening assessments of cognition, such as the MoCA or Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), since the anticipated effects for each gene likely vary by cognitive domain (Alcalay et al., 2012, Mata et al., 2016, Mata et al., 2014).
Loss-of-function mutations in the GBA gene are well-established risk factors for cognitive impairment and dementia in PD (Alcalay et al., 2012, Mata et al., 2016, Winder-Rhodes et al., 2013, Zokaei et al., 2014). In addition, GBA contains a functional polymorphism, E326K (rs2230288), that decreases glucocerebrosidase activity in vitro (Montfort et al., 2004). In a recent analysis focused on GBA variants, conducted in a cohort that largely overlapped with the one studied here, we found that among PD patients GBA E326K predicted lower performance on tests of working memory/executive function (LNST and TMT B-A) and visuospatial abilities (JoLO) (Mata et al., 2016). In the present study, we again observed that this SNP was associated with lower JoLO scores, and this was one of the strongest associations observed across all markers and all cognitive measures (Table 2). Viewed in the context of an exome-wide analysis, these results provide a new perspective on the importance of GBA-associated cognitive deficits in PD.
While the results of our study will require replication in independent PD cohorts before firm conclusions can be drawn, two of the candidate genes we have nominated are particularly intriguing from a biological standpoint. Two SNPs in the PARP4 gene, one nonsynonymous (rs9318600) and the other intronic (rs9581094), were associated with HVLT-R total recall scores (Table 2). PARP4 (VPARP) catalyzes the reversible addition of multiple branched chains of ADP-ribose to target proteins, including actin (De Maio et al., 2013). Through inhibition of actin polymerization, PARP4 is thought to regulate neuronal plasticity and experiments in animal models suggest that it might play a role in memory consolidation (De Lisa et al., 2012, De Maio et al., 2013). Rs34877994, a nonsynonymous SNP within the MTCL1 gene, was associated with performance on TMT B-A. MTCL1 is preferentially expressed in brain (Nagase et al., 1998) and encodes microtubule crosslinking factor 1. This protein interacts with microtubule affinity regulating kinase 2 (MARK2) to maintain the correct temporal balance between dynamic and stable microtubules.(Sato et al., 2013) MARK2 also phosphorylates microtubule-associated protein tau at multiple sites which alters tau’s affinity for microtubules; (Schwalbe et al., 2013) the MAPT gene, which encodes tau, is a well-established susceptibility for PD.
Our data failed to validate two genes previously implicated in modulating cognitive function in PD. The COMT gene encodes catechol-O-methyltransferase which metabolizes dopamine and other catecholamines. It contains a functional polymorphism, V158M, with nearly equal allele frequencies in populations of European origin. Individuals homozygous for the V allele display higher COMT activity and lower dopaminergic signaling in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) than subjects homozygous for the M allele (Weinberger et al., 2001). Two studies of partially overlapping PD cohorts found that individuals with the high activity V allele performed significantly better on the Tower of London (ToL), a frontostriatally based executive task (Foltynie et al., 2004, Williams-Gray et al., 2009). However, a subsequent study failed to replicate these findings using several measures of attention and executive function including the ToL, TMT B, Stroop Color Word test, and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Hoogland et al., 2010). In the present study of a much larger PD cohort, we did not observe an association between COMT V158M and any of the cognitive variables assessed including two tests of working memory/executive function, LNST and TMT B-A (Supplementary Table 12). This was true even without correction for multiple comparisons. However, our psychometric battery did not include the ToL which limits direct comparisons across studies. MAPT is frequently cited as a risk factor for dementia in PD. This is largely based on sequential publications from a longitudinal study in the UK, which reported that PD patients with the MAPT H1 haplotype experienced a more rapid decline in MMSE score and faster conversion to dementia than patients with the alternate H2 haplotype (Goris et al., 2007, Williams-Gray et al., 2009). Although patients in that study underwent detailed neuropsychological assessments, association tests between the H1 haplotype and change over time in the other cognitive measures were not performed. In contrast, a recent longitudinal study of 246 PD patients from the Parkinson’s Environment and Gene Study observed no association between the MAPT H1 haplotype and change in MMSE score over time (Paul et al., 2016). We recently reported that the MAPT H1 haplotype was not associated with any of the PDCGC core cognitive variables in a PD cohort that largely overlapped with the one studied here (Mata et al., 2014). In the present study, we extend these findings in that despite having much more extensive marker coverage across the MAPT region we still did not observe any significant associations with cognitive performance (Supplementary Table 13).
Prior candidate gene studies have shown that APOE ε4 is associated with lower cognitive performance in PD across multiple cognitive domains (Morley et al., 2012, Paul et al., 2016) and in a previous study from the PDCGC the most significant difference in performance observed between ε4 carriers and non-carriers was for HVLT-R total recall (Mata et al., 2014). In the present study, APOE ε4 did not reach significance for any of the cognitive tests, but for HVLT-R total recall the results were suggestive of association (P = 6.1 × 10−05) and ε4 ranked 8th among all variants tested in single-marker analyses (Supplementary Table 5). One possible explanation for this result is that we used a much more stringent significance threshold in comparison to all previous studies in which no more than a handful of candidate genes were examined.
Bras and colleagues nominated SNCA as a susceptibility locus for dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) (Bras et al., 2014). They reported that the strongest association signal came from variants at the 5′ end of the gene (top SNPs rs894280 and rs7687945) in contrast to a large PD meta-analysis where the peak SNP (rs356182) was located 3′ of SNCA (Nalls et al., 2014). Thus, the authors suggested that PD and DLB might have distinct association profiles. A subsequent case-control study which included both a PD and a DLB cohort replicated this finding (Guella et al., 2016). As shown in Supplementary Table 14, none of the SNCA SNPs in our study were associated with cognitive performance after correction for multiple testing. However, it is interesting to note that among the 45 SNCA SNPs we examined, the two with the lowest P-values for any cognitive measures (HVLT-R total recall, rs894280, P = 6.1 × 10−4; rs7687945, P = 5.4 × 10−4) were also the top-two ranked DLB SNPs reported by Bras et al. In contrast, the highest-ranked PD SNP (rs356182) observed in the meta-analysis by Nalls and colleagues did not display any association signal in our dataset, with P ≥ 0.06 for all cognitive tests (data not shown).
Our study had several limitations. Most importantly, we did not have an independent PD cohort with comparable neuropsychological data available for replication. Given the large number of statistical tests performed, it is likely that some of our findings represent false positive errors. Though the cohort we studied was large in comparison to previous PD cognitive genetic studies, we were still likely underpowered to detect signals for rare variants with small to modest effects. Participants in our study had a higher-than-average level of education, a known contributor to performance across most cognitive measures. Thus, our sample might not be fully representative of all patients with PD. Some of the cognitive measures used rely, in part, on motor function, and thus motor symptoms might have interfered with performance on these tests. However, this was not an issue for HVLT-R and JoLO, which do not require drawing/writing and do not have time limits for responses. Furthermore, we corrected TMT B for motor impairment by subtracting the TMT A score. Therefore, it is unlikely that motor symptoms impacted our findings on the tests (HVLT-R total and delayed recall, TMT B-A, JoLO) for which significant associations were observed. In addition, participants taking medications completed testing in the on state to lessen the impact of motor dysfunction on test performance.
The identification and characterization of genetic risk factors for cognitive impairment in PD has the potential to reveal novel therapeutic targets, but could also have more immediate clinical applications. For example, a priori screening of key genes could be used to better predict prognosis and identify patients who are at risk of faster cognitive decline for earlier interventions. Additionally, PD patients enrolled in clinical trials of neuroprotective agents or cognitive enhancing drugs could be stratified based on genotype to create more homogeneous groups, thus increasing power to detect treatment effects.
Supplementary Material
18 common variants in 13 genomic regions were associated with cognitive performance
Most of the genes identified have not been linked to PD motor or cognitive phenotypes
PARP4, which effects memory in animals, was associated with word list learning
GBA E326K was associated with visuospatial abilities, consistent with previous work
There was no relationship between MAPT and any of the cognitive tests examined
Acknowledgments
The authors thank all participants for participating in this work. We also thank Jacqueline Rick for technical assistance.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Financial Disclosure/Conflict of Interest concerning the research related to the manuscript
None
Disclaimer
The contents do not represent the views of the U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs or the United States Government.
Supplementary Data
Additional Information may be found in the online version of this article at the publisher’s website.
References
- Aarsland D, Andersen K, Larsen JP, Perry R, Wentzel-Larsen T, Lolk A, et al. The rate of cognitive decline in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol. 2004;61(12):1906–11. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.12.1906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alcalay RN, Caccappolo E, Mejia-Santana H, Tang M, Rosado L, Orbe Reilly M, et al. Cognitive performance of GBA mutation carriers with early-onset PD: the CORE-PD study. Neurology. 2012;78(18):1434–40. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318253d54b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alcalay RN, Mejia-Santana H, Mirelman A, Saunders-Pullman R, Raymond D, Palmese C, et al. Neuropsychological performance in LRRK2 G2019S carriers with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2015;21(2):106–10. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.09.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bras J, Guerreiro R, Darwent L, Parkkinen L, Ansorge O, Escott-Price V, et al. Genetic analysis implicates APOE, SNCA and suggests lysosomal dysfunction in the etiology of dementia with Lewy bodies. Hum Mol Genet. 2014;23(23):6139–46. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddu334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chahine LM, Qiang J, Ashbridge E, Minger J, Yearout D, Horn S, et al. Clinical and biochemical differences in patients having Parkinson disease with vs without GBA mutations. JAMA Neurol. 2013;70(7):852–8. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.1274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cholerton BA, Zabetian CP, Quinn JF, Chung KA, Peterson A, Espay AJ, et al. Pacific Northwest Udall Center of excellence clinical consortium: study design and baseline cohort characteristics. J Parkinsons Dis. 2013;3(2):205–14. doi: 10.3233/JPD-130189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Lisa E, De Maio A, Moroz LL, Moccia F, Mennella MR, Di Cosmo A. Characterization of novel cytoplasmic PARP in the brain of Octopus vulgaris. Biol Bull. 2012;222(3):176–81. doi: 10.1086/BBLv222n3p176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Maio A, Natale E, Rotondo S, Di Cosmo A, Faraone-Mennella MR. Vault-poly-ADP-ribose polymerase in the Octopus vulgaris brain: a regulatory factor of actin polymerization dynamic. Comp Biochem Physiol B Biochem Mol Biol. 2013;166(1):40–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpb.2013.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foltynie T, Goldberg TE, Lewis SG, Blackwell AD, Kolachana BS, Weinberger DR, et al. Planning ability in Parkinson’s disease is influenced by the COMT val158met polymorphism. Mov Disord. 2004;19(8):885–91. doi: 10.1002/mds.20118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibb WR, Lees AJ. The relevance of the Lewy body to the pathogenesis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1988;51(6):745–52. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.51.6.745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman JG, Weintraub D. Advances in the treatment of cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2015;30(11):1471–89. doi: 10.1002/mds.26352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goris A, Williams-Gray CH, Clark GR, Foltynie T, Lewis SJ, Brown J, et al. Tau and alpha-synuclein in susceptibility to, and dementia in, Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol. 2007;62(2):145–53. doi: 10.1002/ana.21192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guella I, Evans DM, Szu-Tu C, Nosova E, Bortnick SF, Group SCS, et al. alpha-synuclein genetic variability: A biomarker for dementia in Parkinson disease. Ann Neurol. 2016;79(6):991–9. doi: 10.1002/ana.24664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holm S. A Simple Sequentially Rejective Multiple Test Procedure. Scand J Statist. 6(2):65–70. 6:65–70 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Hoogland J, de Bie RM, Williams-Gray CH, Muslimovic D, Schmand B, Post B. Catechol-O-methyltransferase val158met and cognitive function in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2010;25(15):2550–4. doi: 10.1002/mds.23319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janvin CC, Larsen JP, Aarsland D, Hugdahl K. Subtypes of mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: progression to dementia. Mov Disord. 2006;21(9):1343–9. doi: 10.1002/mds.20974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kang GA, Bronstein JM, Masterman DL, Redelings M, Crum JA, Ritz B. Clinical characteristics in early Parkinson’s disease in a central California population-based study. Mov Disord. 2005;20(9):1133–42. doi: 10.1002/mds.20513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee S, Emond MJ, Bamshad MJ, Barnes KC, Rieder MJ, Nickerson DA, et al. Optimal unified approach for rare-variant association testing with application to small-sample case-control whole-exome sequencing studies. Am J Hum Genet. 2012;91(2):224–37. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2012.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mata IF, Leverenz JB, Weintraub D, Trojanowski JQ, Chen-Plotkin A, Van Deerlin VM, et al. GBA Variants are associated with a distinct pattern of cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2016;31(1):95–102. doi: 10.1002/mds.26359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mata IF, Leverenz JB, Weintraub D, Trojanowski JQ, Hurtig HI, Van Deerlin VM, et al. APOE, MAPT, and SNCA Genes and Cognitive Performance in Parkinson Disease. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(11):1405–12. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.1455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meireles J, Massano J. Cognitive impairment and dementia in Parkinson’s disease: clinical features, diagnosis, and management. Front Neurol. 2012;3:88. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monchi O, Hanganu A, Bellec P. Markers of cognitive decline in PD: The case for heterogeneity. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2016;24:8–14. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montfort M, Chabas A, Vilageliu L, Grinberg D. Functional analysis of 13 GBA mutant alleles identified in Gaucher disease patients: Pathogenic changes and “modifier” polymorphisms. Hum Mutat. 2004;23(6):567–75. doi: 10.1002/humu.20043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morley JF, Xie SX, Hurtig HI, Stern MB, Colcher A, Horn S, et al. Genetic influences on cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2012;27(4):512–8. doi: 10.1002/mds.24946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagase T, Ishikawa K, Suyama M, Kikuno R, Miyajima N, Tanaka A, et al. Prediction of the coding sequences of unidentified human genes. XI. The complete sequences of 100 new cDNA clones from brain which code for large proteins in vitro. DNA Res. 1998;5(5):277–86. doi: 10.1093/dnares/5.5.277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nalls MA, Bras J, Hernandez DG, Keller MF, Majounie E, Renton AE, et al. NeuroX, a fast and efficient genotyping platform for investigation of neurodegenerative diseases. Neurobiol Aging. 2015;36(3):1605 e7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.07.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nalls MA, Pankratz N, Lill CM, Do CB, Hernandez DG, Saad M, et al. Large-scale meta-analysis of genome-wide association data identifies six new risk loci for Parkinson’s disease. Nat Genet. 2014;46(9):989–93. doi: 10.1038/ng.3043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paul KC, Rausch R, Creek MM, Sinsheimer JS, Bronstein JM, Bordelon Y, et al. APOE, MAPT, and COMT and Parkinson’s Disease Susceptibility and Cognitive Symptom Progression. J Parkinsons Dis. 2016 doi: 10.3233/JPD-150762. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sato Y, Akitsu M, Amano Y, Yamashita K, Ide M, Shimada K, et al. The novel PAR-1-binding protein MTCL1 has crucial roles in organizing microtubules in polarizing epithelial cells. J Cell Sci. 2013;126(Pt 20):4671–83. doi: 10.1242/jcs.127845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwalbe M, Biernat J, Bibow S, Ozenne V, Jensen MR, Kadavath H, et al. Phosphorylation of human Tau protein by microtubule affinity-regulating kinase 2. Biochemistry. 2013;52(50):9068–79. doi: 10.1021/bi401266n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Srivatsal S, Cholerton B, Leverenz JB, Wszolek ZK, Uitti RJ, Dickson DW, et al. Cognitive profile of LRRK2-related Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2015;30(5):728–33. doi: 10.1002/mds.26161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinberger DR, Egan MF, Bertolino A, Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Lipska BK, et al. Prefrontal neurons and the genetics of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50(11):825–44. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01252-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams-Gray CH, Evans JR, Goris A, Foltynie T, Ban M, Robbins TW, et al. The distinct cognitive syndromes of Parkinson’s disease: 5 year follow-up of the CamPaIGN cohort. Brain. 2009;132(Pt 11):2958–69. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winder-Rhodes SE, Evans JR, Ban M, Mason SL, Williams-Gray CH, Foltynie T, et al. Glucocerebrosidase mutations influence the natural history of Parkinson’s disease in a community-based incident cohort. Brain. 2013;136(Pt 2):392–9. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zokaei N, McNeill A, Proukakis C, Beavan M, Jarman P, Korlipara P, et al. Visual short-term memory deficits associated with GBA mutation and Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2014;137(Pt 8):2303–11. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.