Skip to main content
Translational Psychiatry logoLink to Translational Psychiatry
. 2017 Jul 25;7(7):e1182. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.151

The DCDC2 deletion is not a risk factor for dyslexia

T S Scerri 1, E Macpherson 2, A Martinelli 2, W C Wa 3, A P Monaco 4, J Stein 5, M Zheng 6, C Suk-Han Ho 6, C McBride 7, M Snowling 8, C Hulme 9, M E Hayiou-Thomas 10, M M Y Waye 3,11, J B Talcott 12, S Paracchini 2,*
PMCID: PMC5538127  PMID: 28742079

Abstract

Dyslexia is a specific impairment in learning to read and has strong heritability. An intronic deletion within the DCDC2 gene, with ~8% frequency in European populations, is increasingly used as a marker for dyslexia in neuroimaging and behavioral studies. At a mechanistic level, this deletion has been proposed to influence sensory processing capacity, and in particular sensitivity to visual coherent motion. Our re-assessment of the literature, however, did not reveal strong support for a role of this specific deletion in dyslexia. We also analyzed data from five distinct cohorts, enriched for individuals with dyslexia, and did not identify any signal indicative of associations for the DCDC2 deletion with reading-related measures, including in a combined sample analysis (N=526). We believe we conducted the first replication analysis for a proposed deletion effect on visual motion perception and found no association (N=445 siblings). We also report that the DCDC2 deletion has a frequency of 37.6% in a cohort representative of the general population recruited in Hong Kong (N=220). This figure, together with a lack of association between the deletion and reading abilities in this cohort, indicates the low likelihood of a direct deletion effect on reading skills. Therefore, on the basis of multiple strands of evidence, we conclude that the DCDC2 deletion is not a strong risk factor for dyslexia. Our analyses and literature re-evaluation are important for interpreting current developments within multidisciplinary studies of dyslexia and, more generally, contribute to current discussions about the importance of reproducibility in science.

Introduction

Dyslexia, or reading disability (RD), is a common neurodevelopmental disorder, with prevalence estimates ranging between 5–10% among school-age children in most countries.1, 2 While a significant genetic component has been firmly established for this condition, only a small number of specific genetic factors have been identified to date.3 These include variants within the DCDC2 gene, located on chromosome 6 (at 6p22.3). Single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) within DCDC2 showed association with both dyslexia and reading abilities in independent studies,4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10 but the functional mechanisms underlying these associations remain unclear. Attention has increasingly focussed on an intronic deletion of 2,445 bp within DCDC2 as a potential functional element (Table 1).

Table 1. Previous studies that analyzed the DCDC2 deletion for association with dyslexia.

Reference Country Sample Type of analysis Allele Quantitative phenotype P-value Deletion Frequency
Meng et al., 2005 USA 153 nuclear families Quantitative Deletion+STR rare alleles Homonym Choice 0.00002 8.5%
          Untimed PIAT Word Recognition 0.023  
          Word Recognition 0.04  
          OC 0.0035  
          OC 0.0385  
               
Harold et al., 2006 England 264 nuclear families Quantitative Deletion   Not associated 10%
    126 severe families subgroup     IWR 0.03 9%
          READ 0.04  
          PA 0.05  
  Wales 350 cases Categorical Deletion   Not associated 9% cases
    273 controls   Deletion+STR rare alleles   Not associated 9% controls
               
Brkanac et al., 2007 USA 191 trios Quantitative Deletion SWE 0.05 7.5%
          Spelling 0.04  
               
Ludwig et al., 2008 Germany 396 trios Categorical Deletion+STR rare alleles   Not associated 8.6%
               
Wilcke et al., 2009 Germany 72 cases Categorical Deletion   P<0.01 10% cases
    25 severe cases         8% severe cases
    184 controls         4% controls
               
Marino et al., 2012 Italy 303 nuclear families Quantitative Deletion SWR 0.05 6%
        Deletion PD 0.04  
        STR allele 10 Memory Composite Score 0.00001  
        Deletion+STR rare alleles Memory Composite Score 0.00001  
               
Powers et al., 2016 UK 89 cases Categorical Deletion Severe RD Not associated 8.3%
    4428 controlsa     Severe LI    

Abbreviations: IWR, irregular word reading; LI, language impairment; OC, orthographic coding; PA, phonological awareness; PD, non-word reading; PIAT, Peabody individual achievement test; READ, single-word reading; RD, reading disability; SWE, sight word efficiency.

a

The samples sizes were estimated by combining information from different studies;11, 12 these studies reported association between the 5 and 6 STR alleles with reading-related measures.

The original study describing the DCDC2 deletion reported associations between this locus and quantitative measures.4 However, such associations were detected only when the deletion was analyzed in combination with a pool of 10 rare alleles of a short tandem repeat (STR; BV677278) marker at the same locus.4 Analyses of the deletion combined with the STR alleles, failed to replicate the association with dyslexia as a binary category.13, 14 A separate study reported an association with a composite memory score, but the association appeared to be driven by one particular STR allele (allele 10 with frequency of 5%) rather than the deletion itself.15 This STR has been suggested to be a regulatory element.11, 16 Powers et al. (2013)11 reported that two 6-SNP haplotypes (allele frequency<5%)) were associated with reading and language phenotypes in a large epidemiological sample (the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC)) stratified for a severe phenotype (⩾2 s.d. below the mean on a phoneme-deletion task). This stratification resulted in the comparison of 89 cases with the remaining 5,225 individuals who scored above the −2 s.d. diagnostic threshold. These two rare haplotypes were in linkage disequilibrium (LD) with alleles 5 and 6 of the STR. In a follow up study, allele 5 (3.6% frequency) and 6 (4.7% frequency), but not the deletion, showed association with reading and language measures.12 Meng et al. (2011)16 showed that STR alleles 3, 4 and 5 have differential effects on gene expression regulation. In summary, there is no clear consensus as to which STR alleles are associated with dyslexia and regulate gene expression. Rare alleles, such as the STR variants, can increase the likelihood of false positives because their analysis is typically conducted in very small samples.17 Up to 40 alleles have been described for the STR,11, 13 and therefore it is expected that some alleles would lead to significant P-values purely by chance.

The deletion has also been analyzed without the inclusion of the STR marker. Weak associations have been reported for various reading-related traits with significance values in the range of 0.02<P<0.0514, 15, 18 (Table 1; Supplementary Table 1). Lack of replication has also been reported.12, 14 Wilcke et al. (2009) reported the most compelling association to date (P<0.01) in a case/control study of a German cohort (Table 1). In this sample, the deletion had a frequency of 10% in cases (8% in severe cases) and 4% in controls. This lower frequency in the controls, however, is not consistent with what is observed in other studies of European samples. In fact, the deletion had a frequency of 8.6% among the parents of probands with dyslexia in a different German sample.13 A frequency of 9% was reported for both cases with dyslexia and controls in a UK cohort,14 similar to the 8.3% frequency reported in a UK epidemiological sample.12

Taken together, these studies do not support a role of the DCDC2 deletion as a specific risk factor for dyslexia. Nevertheless, the DCDC2 deletion is becoming increasingly used in research aimed at identifying the neuronal correlates of dyslexia. For example, the deletion has been suggested to directly affect the development of brain structures relevant to language and reading, through quantitative imaging studies of anatomical variability in healthy individuals.19, 20 The deletion has also been linked to functional mechanisms, such as reduced sensitivity to visual coherent motion.21, 22 Perception of motion is an important component of visual development, for which impairment has been demonstrated to occur in several developmental disorders,23 including dyslexia.24 Observed associations between visual motion sensitivity and reading skills contributed to the development of the magnocellular hypothesis for dyslexia.25 However, motion perception deficits may be restricted to subgroups of individuals with dyslexia, and typically those with the most severe phenotypes who might also present comorbidities with other developmental disorders.26 Cicchini et al.22 reported that a decreased ability to detect and discriminate visual motion correlated with the presence of the DCDC2 deletion and suggested that motion sensitivity was more strongly associated with the presence of the deletion than with the presence of dyslexia. A major limitation of these imaging and behavioral studies, however, is the very small sample size, which suffers from both low power of detecting real effects as well as the increased chance of generating false positives.17

The aim of the present study was to provide additional data to evaluate the role of the DCDC2 deletion as a contributor to risk for dyslexia and impairment on related measures. In addition to a re-evaluation of the published literature (Table 1), we report new association analyses for the deletion in four distinct cohorts characterized for reading measures, including, for the first time, to our knowledge, a cohort of Asian origin. We also present a replication analysis for association between the DCDC2 deletion and a measure of visual motion sensitivity. The evidence we have gathered does not support an association between the DCDC2 deletion and dyslexia or quantitative measures of reading abilities. This work is important for the interpretation of a developing body of literature and for framing the direction of future research studies of dyslexia. Moreover, our results are relevant to the current discussion about the importance of reproducibility in science,27 including the observation that it may be very easy to canonize weak findings as scientific facts unless a substantial number of negative findings are published.28

Materials and methods

Study participants

Our analysis included four distinct UK cohorts, referred to here as the Oxford Family Dyslexia cohort, Oxford Cases Dyslexia Cohort, Aston Dyslexia cohort, York cohort, and a fifth cohort, the Hong Kong cohort from the Chinese-English Twin Study of Biliteracy (Table 2).

Table 2. Frequency of the DCDC2 deletion reported in this study.

Cohort Structure Recruitment DCDC2 deletion frequency
Oxford Family Dyslexia 219 families Clinical—children with reading difficulties 8.9% parents (N=414) 9.8% probands (N=153)a
       
Oxford Cases Dyslexia 272 singletons Clinical—children with reading difficulties 6.1%
       
Aston Dyslexia 105 singletons Clinical—children with reading difficulties 7.6%
       
York 103 families Longitudinal—children with language difficulties, family history of dyslexia and typically developing 7.4% parents (N=174) 7.7% all probands (N=103) 5.5% RD group (N=18) 10.7% TD group (N=72)
       
Combined 203 singletons Individuals scoring READ<-1.5s.d. in the above cohorts 9.6%
       
Hong Kong 220 singletons/twins Typically developing 37.6%
       
1000 Genomes 503 singletons 504 singletons General population 8.05% Europe 33.5% East Asia

Abbreviations: RD, reading difficulties; TD, typically developing.

a

The number of probands is lower than the number of families because of missing data.

The Oxford Family Dyslexia cohort was recruited by research clinics in Oxford and Reading. Genotype and phenotype data were available for 219 families (445 siblings, age range 6–27 years). Parental genotypes were also available. The same research clinics recruited the Oxford Cases Dyslexia Cohort, a collection of 272 unrelated individuals (age range 8–18 years). The psychometric measures used to assess these cohorts included the British Ability Scales (BAS) single-word reading (READ) and spelling (SPELL) tests,29 irregular word reading (OC-irreg)30 and phonological decoding (PD).30 An orthography measure using the forced choice task (OC-choice)31 and phoneme awareness (PA)32 scores were available for the Family cohort. Both cohorts have been described before33 and the Family cohort was also previously analyzed for association between the DCDC2 deletion and reading abilities.14 Here, we analyzed the Oxford Family cohort for association with a visual motion (VMOT) measure described previously34 (See also Supplementary Materials for details) and as part of a combined sample (see below).

The Aston Dyslexia cohort includes 105 unrelated individuals, recruited through the Aston Dyslexia and Developmental Assessment Unit in Birmingham, UK (age range 7–16 years). Children attending this educational clinic have been referred for assessments of reading difficulties by local schools. On the basis of their educational assessments, children are invited back to participate in research studies. The phenotypes obtained for this cohort included single-word reading (READ) and single-word spelling (SPELL), measured by standardised assessments including using the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test,35 the British Abilities Scales, and the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE).36 Sub-scales of TOWRE were used to measure pseudo-word reading (or phonological decoding, TOWRE_PD) and sight word efficiency (TOWRE-SWE).

The York cohort is a longitudinal cohort designed to study language and literacy development37 and has been analyzed previously for genetic studies.38 DNA was available for 103 families and a total of 318 individuals. Of these, phenotype data were available for N=103 probands and N=41 siblings. Phenotypes were available from the same children at different time points. Reading abilities were measured at the ages of 4½, 5½, 6 and 8 years, representing the years when reading skills are in ascendancy. At age 5½ years, the measure was word reading from the York Assessment of Reading Comprehension (READ);39 at age 6 years, the measures were word reading efficiency from the Test of Word Reading Efficiency Word (TOWRE_READ) and Pseudoword (TOWRE_PD) tests36 and spelling from the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT-SPELL);35 at age 8 years the measures were irregular word reading (IWR) from the Diagnostic Test of Word Reading Processes (DTWRP),40 phonological awareness measured by a phoneme-deletion task (PA) and rapid automatic naming (RAN).41 The factor scores were latent variables derived following confirmatory factor analysis: a phonology factor score (PHON_FS) was derived from word recall and phoneme awareness tasks at 4½ years and from phoneme awareness (deletion) at ages 5½, 6 and 8 years; a literacy factor score (LIT_FS) was derived from measures of single-word reading, reading accuracy, word and non-word reading fluency and spelling at age 8 years. At age 8 years, children were assigned to a formal diagnosis of dyslexia when they scored >1.5 s.d. below the control group mean on the literacy composite measure. This approach identified N=18 children with dyslexia, including N=9 who also had language impairment (LI). Another N=11 had LI only and N=72 children were classified as typical developing (TD).

Exclusion criteria for the UK cohorts were non-European ethnicity, signs of other neurological conditions in the probands or performance IQ<80.

The Hong Kong cohort included 220 unrelated individuals who were not selected for reading difficulties (age range 6–10.5 years old).42 Study participants were recruited either as singletons (N=104) or as twin pairs with only one twin per pair retained for the study (N=116). All participants speak Cantonese as their first language and have not been diagnosed with either intellectual disabilities or neurological conditions. Three reading-related phenotypes were available from a standardized test battery described previously: Chinese Word Reading (CWR), Chinese one-minute (COM) word reading and Chinese Digit Rapid Automatized Naming (CDRAN).43 Mean scores are shown in Supplementary Table 2.

DNA was extracted from blood or buccal swabs using standard procedures, and from saliva using Oragene kit (prepITL2P) (DNA Genotek, Ottawa, Canada). Ethical approval was obtained from the local ethical research committees, including the Oxfordshire Psychiatric Research Ethics Committee, the NHS Research Ethics (Yorkshire & The Humber – Humber Bridge), the University of York Department of Psychology Ethics Committee and the Aston University Ethics Committee, and the Joint Chinese University of Hong Kong-New Territories East Cluster Clinical Research Ethics Committee. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants and/or their caregivers.

Genotype and statistical analysis

Genotype data were generated using a previously described protocol4 and checked for Mendelian errors and deviation from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium using PEDSTATS.44 Family-based cohorts were tested using the Quantitative Transmission Disequilibrium Test (QTDT) under the total association model45 after estimating the identical-by-descent (IBD) sharing with MERLIN.46 No population stratification was observed for the UK cohorts following analysis of genome-wide genotype data.47, 48 Singletons analysis was conducted with PLINK (Purcell et al., 2007) under an allelic model in each cohort as well as in combined samples including the Oxford Cases cohort, the Aston cohort and the probands of the Oxford Family cohort. Analysis was also conducted in a subgroup derived from this combined sample for meeting a stringent criteria of dyslexia defined by a score below −1.5 s.d. from the normative population mean for a single-word reading test. Power calculations were conducted with the genetic Power Calculator49 (Supplementary Material).

Results

The DCDC2 deletion and reading abilities

The frequency of the deletion ranged 6.1–9.8% in the UK cohorts (Table 2), which is consistent with the range reported previously, including that for UK controls (9% Table 1). Differences in frequency across the cohorts and in specific subgroups likely resulted from sampling bias and illustrates how small samples can potentially affect this type of analysis. For example, the longitudinal design of the York cohort enabled the identification of children with dyslexia on the basis of very detailed assessments. The overall frequency of the deletion, across the 103 probands was 7.7%, similar to that observed in the parents (7.4%, N=174). On the basis of the final diagnosis, the deletion had a frequency of 10.7% (N=72) in the TD group compared to 5.5% in the RD (N=18) group (Table 2). The deletion frequency in a combined group of individuals (N=203) from the UK cohorts meeting a stringent definition of dyslexia (READ<−1.5 s.d. from the standardised population mean) was 9.6%. Strikingly, the deletion had a frequency of 37.6% (N=220; Table 2) in the Hong Kong cohort which was sampled from a population with typical literacy achievement (Supplementary Table 2). Given the substantial difference in allele frequency between the UK and Hong Kong population we queried the data from the 1000 Genomes project.50 The deletion (ID=Esv3608367) presents a global frequency of 11.5% across 2504 individuals, and consistently with our data, shows a frequency of 8.05% and 33.53% in the European and East Asian populations (Table 2; Supplementary Table 3). The frequency observed in the European population is important in interpreting the results by Wilcke et al. (2009) that were based on comparisons with a control cohort of German origin that presented a deletion frequency of 4% (Table 1).

The DCDC2 deletion was tested for association with quantitative reading-related measures in the Oxford Cases, Aston, York and Hong Kong cohorts. The DCDC2 deletion was tested for association with reading measures in the Oxford Family Dyslexia cohort previously;14 no associations were detected in the entire cohort, and only marginally significant associations were observed in the subgroup of 126 families selected for severity of the phenotype (Supplementary Methods and Supplementary Table 1). We then conducted quantitative association analyses in the combined sample including individuals with directly comparable phenotypes. We used a combined sample including probands from the Oxford Family cohorts and the Oxford Cases (N=425) which had four comparable phenotypes. We then analyzed this sample with the addition of the Aston cohort (N=526) which was characterised with the same READ and SPELL phenotypes. In both combined samples we analyzed subgroups of individuals who met a stringent definition of dyslexia defined by a score below -1.5 s.d. from the standardised mean for READ of the population. No association was detected with any of the phenotypes tested (Supplementary Table 4).

The DCDC2 deletion and visual motion

The distribution of VMOT thresholds (threshold=1/sensitivity) approximated normality across N=701 individuals (including individuals for whom genotype data were not available) with a mean of 18.1 (s.d.=12.2) in the Oxford Dyslexia cohort. VMOT thresholds showed a small, but statistically significant correlation with READ (r=−0.16; one-tailed P=1.0 × 10−5), indicating that lowered motion sensitivity is associated with poorer reading scores.

We did not identify patterns of association between the DCDC2 deletion and VMOT thresholds, either in the whole cohort or in the same subset of 126 families selected for severity as described previously14 (Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary Material). The subgroup analysis has the limitation of reducing the power of the analysis by decreasing the sample size, but was aimed at addressing the alternative hypothesis that the effect of the deletion on visual motion abilities is restricted to individuals within the lower tail of the normal distribution of reading ability.26

We stratified the READ and VMOT scores for the DCDC2 deletion genotypes across all siblings and for the probands only (Table 3). As expected, the mean scores for the probands showed lower performance for both READ and VMOT compared to the whole data set, but their raw scores were similarly distributed across the three genotype groups (Supplementary Figure 1). Accordingly, genetic association analysis run under different linear models (dominant, recessive and genotypic conducted using PLINK) did not show any trends of association (data not shown).

Table 3. Mean score of READ and VMOT stratified by the DCDC2 deletion genotype in the Oxford dyslexia cohort.

Number of deletion alleles READ (s.d.;N)
VMOT % (s.d.;N)
  All Probands All Probands
0 46.2 (9.7; 417) 38.6 (6.8; 126) 18.8 (12.0; 374) 20.7 (13.5; 112)
1 45.0 (10.8; 73) 36.6 (7.6; 24) 17.5 (9.8; 66) 21.3 (14.2; 22)
2 46.4 (14.5; 7) 36.7 (8.7; 3) 16.7 (6.9; 6) 19.2 (5.8; 3)

Abbreviations: READ, single-word reading; VMOT, visual motion. The READ scores are standardized with a population mean of 50 and s.d.=10. VMOT scores are in % coherent motion at detection threshold. Poorer reading is represented by lower standard scores; reduced sensitivity to VMOT is indicated by higher scores. See Supplementary Figure 1 for the distribution of the raw scores.

Discussion

With this study, we aimed to investigate the role of the DCDC2 deletion in contributing to dyslexia or measures of reading and component skills. Our work was prompted by recent studies that based their hypotheses and research designs on the assumption that the DCDC2 deletion is an established susceptibility factor for dyslexia. However, as our re-evaluation of the literature shows, the accumulated evidence in support of this assumption is statistically weak (Table 1). Most prior studies either did not show association with the deletion4, 12, 13, 14 or showed only moderate signals of association (0.01<P-values<0.5)15, 18, 51 (Table 1). This latter group included also our previous study that reported a weak association signal (P=0.04) with one phenotype (IWR) in a subgroup of families (Supplementary Table 1). Only one study reported a significantly higher frequency of the deletion in cases with dyslexia (10%) compared to controls (4%).7 However, through comparisons with other studies12, 13, 14 and with the data derived from the 1000 Genome project for European reference populations (Supplementary Table 3), we suggest that the low frequency reported in the control cohort analyzed by Wilcke et al.7 most likely resulted from an artifact. Stronger association signals were observed only when the deletion was combined with different rare STR alleles.4, 12, 15 We did not test the STR in this study because our focus was to address the role of the DCDC2 deletion specifically as a reliable marker for dyslexia. Our goal was to verify the theoretical foundation of the design of imaging19, 20 and behavioral21, 22 studies for which the deletion alone, and not in combination with STR alleles, has been used as a dyslexia marker. These studies suggested that the deletion is directly associated with structural brain changes and sensory deficits regardless of the presence of dyslexia, implying a causative effect for this genetic marker. Furthermore, the STR harbors at least 40 (mostly extremely rare) alleles, making it challenging to distinguish genuine signals from noise and to test specific hypotheses. In fact, associations so far have been reported for different STR alleles (i.e. deletion+pool of rare alleles, allele 10, allele 5 and allele 6; Table 1).

We tested the deletion for association with reading abilities in four cohorts from the UK analysed both separately and as combined samples, including analysis in a subgroup meeting stringent criteria for dyslexia. The Oxford Family Dyslexia cohort, analyzed previously,14 was re-analyzed here as part of this approach and for association with a visual motion test. Furthermore, we tested a cohort recruited in Hong Kong, which represents the first cohort of non-European ancestry to be analyzed for this marker in the context of reading abilities. None of our analyses showed any statistically (or nominally) significant associations between the deletion and reading measures. Power calculations indicate that our largest sample used for genetic analysis (N=526) has the power to detect only relatively large effects (>1.5% of the phenotypic variance; Supplementary Material). Although this is larger than what would generally be predicted for common genetic factors contributing to complex traits, our data are important for the interpretation of the current literature. The analysis in the Hong Kong cohort revealed a frequency of 37.6% which is consistent with frequency data reported for the 1000 Genomes Project Asian populations. The Hong Kong cohort is representative of the general Hong Kong population of Chinese origin and is not enriched for individuals presenting reading difficulties, as demonstrated by the distribution of their language and reading abilities scores (Supplementary Table 2). It is debated whether dyslexia in Chinese is the manifestation of a different deficit compared to European populations as a result of culture.52 However both behavioral53 and neuroimaging54 studies predict commonalities in the neuronal markers for dyslexia that can be dissociated from language-specific effects in both English and Chinese individuals. The incidence of dyslexia in Hong Kong, similar to the UK populations, is about 10%.2 However, visual processing may be more relevant for decoding Chinese compared to alphabetic scripts because of the complex visual composition of Chinese characters.55 The role of the visual magnocellular pathway in reading has been suggested to be particularly relevant for decoding Chinese scripts.56 The visual motion perception tests that compared groups on the basis of the DCDC2 deletion genotypes were designed to assess the role of this pathway in dyslexia.21, 22 Genetic association analysis for VMOT measures in the Hong Kong cohort, or other Chinese speaking groups, would allow direct comparisons with these studies.

We tested for effects of the deletion on a visual motion task (Table 3; Supplementary Table 1) in the Oxford Dyslexia cohort and found no associations, thereby failing to replicate the findings by Cicchini et al (2015),22 albeit we used a different version of the VMOT test (see Supplementary Material for details). Cicchini and colleagues used a contrast sensitivity paradigm with a very short presentation time that varied the contrast of a moving stimulus to the detection threshold. Our VMOT task involved much longer presentation times for random dot kinematograms (RDK) in which the signal to noise ratio was manipulated to the detection threshold. Although we cannot directly compare the two tasks, the observation that the probands in our sample have lower sensitivity to motion compared to the entire sample (Table 3) provides cross-validation of previous work with this task in dyslexia57 and as a correlate with reading skills in the normal population.34 The sample we used for the VMOT analysis was substantially larger (N=374 non-carriers, N=66 heterozygotes; N=6 deletion homozygotes; Table 3) compared to the analysis reported by Cicchini and colleagues in 11 deletion carriers and 10 non-carriers. Therefore, while we cannot exclude an effect dependent on specific phenotypes or of smaller size, our study provides a new data set for the interpretation of previously reported results. Regardless of the differences in the tests used, our genetic analysis does not support the design of behavioral studies based on the stratification for DCDC2 deletion genotypes.

While the DCDC2 deletion is interesting for the potential regulatory effect, other DCDC2 markers, mainly SNPs, showed more consistent associations across independent studies.4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10 Our current study, therefore, does not undermine the overall evidence in support of DCDC2 as a candidate gene for dyslexia or reading abilities.

Replication studies are essential but present particular challenges. The acquisition of cognitive measures is only possible through expensive and time consuming one-on-one sessions, making it extremely difficult to obtain large sample sizes. Heterogeneity of phenotypic and behavioral measures makes direct comparison across studies very difficult. Overall, these observations reinforce the importance of collecting high quality cognitive data in general population samples to enable follow up studies of genetic associations reported for neurodevelopmental disorders.58

In summary, through both a re-evaluation of the published literature and new genetic association analyses, our results show that there is no strong evidence in support of the DCDC2 deletion as a risk factor for dyslexia. These data are important for guiding the future direction of dyslexia research and more generally highlight the caveats associated with over-generalizing from analyses conducted in small sample sizes and from misinterpretations of the literature. Most importantly, we provide a useful example of the importance of publishing null results toward avoiding the potential of canonizing weak evidence as fact.

Acknowledgments

SP is a Royal Society University Research Fellow. EM was the recipient of a Wolfson Intercalated Award. Support to the analysis was provided by the St Andrews Bioinformatics Unit funded by the Wellcome Trust [grant 105621/Z/14/Z]. Analysis of the York cohort was supported by a Wellcome Trust Programme Grant to MJS [WT082032MA]. Assessment of the Aston cohort was supported by funding from The Waterloo Foundation to JBT and SP [797–1720]. The work on the Chinese-English Twin Study of Biliteracy was supported by a Hong Kong Collaborative Research Fund (CUHK8/CRF/13G) award to CM, CH, and MW, The Chinese University of Hong Kong VCF2014023 fund awarded to CM, and a SFC/RGC Hong Kong/Scotland Collaborative Partnership award to SP and CM (2014_HK-Scot-0020; X-CUHK404/14). We are grateful to Dianne Newbury and Dorothy Bishop for useful comments during the preparation of this manuscript.

Footnotes

Supplementary Information accompanies the paper on the Translational Psychiatry website (http://www.nature.com/tp)

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Information

References

  1. Katusic SK, Colligan RC, Barbaresi WJ, Schaid DJ, Jacobsen SJ. Incidence of Reading Disability in a Population-Based Birth Cohort, 1976–1982, Rochester, Minn. Mayo Clin Proc 2001; 76: 1081–1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Chan DW, Ho CS, Tsang S, Lee S, Chung KKH. Prevalence, gender ratio and gender differences in reading‐related cognitive abilities among Chinese children with dyslexia in Hong Kong. Educ Stud 2007; 33: 249–265. [Google Scholar]
  3. Paracchini S, Diaz R, Stein J. Chapter two – advances in dyslexia genetics—new insights into the role of brain asymmetries. Adv Genet 2016; 96: 53–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Meng H, Smith SD, Hager K, Held M, Liu J, Olson RK et al. DCDC2 is associated with reading disability and modulates neuronal development in the brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2005; 102: 17053–17058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Couto JM, Gomez L, Wigg K, Ickowicz A, Pathare T, Malone M et al. Association of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with a candidate region for reading disabilities on chromosome 6p. Biol Psychiatry 2009; 66: 368–375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Schumacher J, Anthoni H, Dahdouh F, Konig IR, Hillmer AM, Kluck N et al. Strong genetic evidence of DCDC2 as a susceptibility gene for dyslexia. Am J Hum Genet 2006; 78: 52–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Wilcke A, Weissfuss J, Kirsten H, Wolfram G, Boltze J, Ahnert P. The role of gene DCDC2 in German dyslexics. Ann Dyslexia 2009; 59: 1–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Newbury DF, Paracchini S, Scerri TS, Winchester L, Addis L, Richardson AJ et al. Investigation of dyslexia and SLI risk variants in reading-and language-impaired subjects. Behav Genet 2011; 41: 90–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Zhang Y, Li J, Song S, Tardif T, Burmeister M, Villafuerte SM et al. Association of DCDC2 polymorphisms with normal variations in reading abilities in a chinese population. PLoS ONE 2016; 11: e0153603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Scerri TS, Morris AP, Buckingham LL, Newbury DF, Miller LL, Monaco AP et al. DCDC2, KIAA0319 and CMIP are associated with reading-related traits. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 70: 237–245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Powers NR, Eicher JD, Butter F, Kong Y, Miller LL, Ring SM et al. Alleles of a polymorphic ETV6 binding site in DCDC2 confer risk of reading and language impairment. Am J Hum Genet 2013; 93: 19–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Powers NR, Eicher JD, Miller LL, Kong Y, Smith SD, Pennington BF et al. The regulatory element READ1 epistatically influences reading and language, with both deleterious and protective alleles. J Med Genet 2016; 53: 163–171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ludwig KU, Schumacher J, Schulte-Korne G, Konig IR, Warnke A, Plume E et al. Investigation of the DCDC2 intron 2 deletion/compound short tandem repeat polymorphism in a large German dyslexia sample. Psychiatr Genet 2008; 18: 310–312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Harold D, Paracchini S, Scerri T, Dennis M, Cope N, Hill G et al. Further evidence that the KIAA0319 gene confers susceptibility to developmental dyslexia. Mol Psychiatry 2006; 11(1061): 1085–1091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Marino C, Meng H, Mascheretti S, Rusconi M, Cope N, Giorda R et al. DCDC2 genetic variants and susceptibility to developmental dyslexia. Psychiatr Genet 2012; 22: 25–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Meng H, Powers NR, Tang L, Cope NA, Zhang PX, Fuleihan R et al. A dyslexia-associated variant in DCDC2 changes gene expression. Behav Genet 2011; 41: 58–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Button KS, Ioannidis JPA, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ESJ et al. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 2013; 14: 365–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brkanac Z, Chapman NH, Matsushita MM, Chun L, Nielsen K, Cochrane E et al. Evaluation of candidate genes for DYX1 and DYX2 in families with dyslexia. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2007; 144B: 556–560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Meda SA, Gelernter J, Gruen JR, Calhoun VD, Meng H, Cope NA et al. Polymorphism of DCDC2 reveals differences in cortical morphology of healthy individuals-a preliminary voxel based morphometry study. Brain Imaging Behav 2008; 2: 21–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Marino C, Scifo P, Della Rosa PA, Mascheretti S, Facoetti A, Lorusso ML et al. The DCDC2/intron 2 deletion and white matter disorganization: Focus on developmental dyslexia. Cortex 2014; 57: 227–243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gori S, Mascheretti S, Giora E, Ronconi L, Ruffino M, Quadrelli E et al. The DCDC2 intron 2 deletion impairs illusory motion perception unveiling the selective role of magnocellular-dorsal stream in reading (dis)ability. Cereb Cortex 2015; 25: 1685–1695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cicchini GM, Marino C, Mascheretti S, Perani D, Morrone MC. Strong motion deficits in dyslexia associated with DCDC2 gene alteration. J Neurosci 2015; 35: 8059–8064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hadad B, Schwartz S, Maurer D, Lewis TL. Motion perception: a review of developmental changes and the role of early visual experience. Front Integr Neurosci 2015; 9: 49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Benassi M, Simonelli L, Giovagnoli S, Bolzani R. Coherence motion perception in developmental dyslexia: a meta-analysis of behavioral studies. Dyslexia 2010; 16: 341–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Stein J, Walsh V. To see but not to read; the magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends Neurosci 1997; 20: 147–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Talcott JB, Witton C, Stein JF. Probing the neurocognitive trajectories of children’s reading skills. Neuropsychologia 2013; 51: 472–481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Baker M. 1,500 scientists lift the lid on reproducibility. Nature 2016; 533: 452–454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Nissen SB, Magidson T, Gross K, Bergstrom CT, Arbesman S, Baker M et al. Publication bias and the canonization of false facts. Elife 2016; 5: 452–454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Elliot C, Murray D, Pearson L. British abilities scales. NFER-Nelson: Windsor 1983.
  30. Castles A, Coltheart M. Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition 1993; 47: 149–180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Olson Forsberg H, Wise BRK Genes, environment, and the development of orthographic skills In: Berninger VW (ed) The Varieties of Orthographic Knowledge, 1: Theoretical and Developmental Issues. Kluwer Academic: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1994; 27–71. [Google Scholar]
  32. Frederickson N, Frith U, Reason R. Phonological Assessment Battery (Manual and Materials). NFER-Nelson: Windsor, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  33. Scerri TS, Paracchini S, Morris A, MacPhie IL, Talcott J, Stein J et al. Identification of candidate genes for dyslexia susceptibility on chromosome 18. PLoS ONE 2010; 5: e13712. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Talcott JB, Witton C, Hebb GS, Stoodley CJ, Westwood EA, France SJ et al. On the relationship between dynamic visual and auditory processing and literacy skills; results from a large primary-school study. Dyslexia 2002; 8: 204–225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Wechsler D. Wechsler Individual Achievement Test. 2nd edn. The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  36. Wagner R, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA Test of Word Reading Efficiency 2nd edn. Pearson Assessment: Austin, 2011.
  37. Nash HM, Hulme C, Gooch D, Snowling MJ. Preschool language profiles of children at family risk of dyslexia: continuities with specific language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2013; 54: 958–968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Pettigrew KA, Frinton E, Nudel R, Chan MTM, Thompson P, Hayiou-Thomas ME et al. Further evidence for a parent-of-origin effect at the NOP9 locus on language-related phenotypes. J Neurodev Disord 2016; 8: 24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hulme C, Stothard S, Clarke P, Bowyer-Crane C, Harrington A, Truelove E et alYARC York assessment of reading for comprehension. Early reading. GL Assessment; 2009.
  40. Stainthorp R Diagnostic test of word reading processes. GL Assessment 2012.
  41. Hulme C, Nash HM, Gooch D, Lervag A, Snowling MJ. The foundations of literacy development in children at familial risk of dyslexia. Psychol Sci 2015; 26: 1877–1886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Wong SWL, Ho CS-H, McBride C, Chow BW-Y, Waye MMY, Chen J et al. Less is more in Hong Kong: investigation of biscriptal and trilingual development among Chinese twins in a (relatively) small city. Twin Res Hum Genet 2016; 13: 1–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ho CS-H, Chan D, Chung K, Tsang S-M, Lee S-H, Cheng RW-Y. The Hong Kong Test of Specific Learning Difficulties in Reading and Writing for Primary School Students Second. Hong Kong Specific Learning Difficulties Research Team: Hong Kong, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  44. Wigginton JE, Abecasis GR. PEDSTATS: descriptive statistics, graphics and quality assessment for gene mapping data. Bioinformatics 2005; 21: 3445–3447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Abecasis GR, Cardon LR, Cookson WO. A general test of association for quantitative traits in nuclear families. Am J Hum Genet 2000; 66: 279–292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Abecasis GR, Cherny SS, Cookson WO, Cardon LR. Merlin—rapid analysis of dense genetic maps using sparse gene flow trees. Nat Genet 2002; 30: 97–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Pettigrew KA, Reeves E, Leavett R, Hayiou-Thomas ME, Sharma A, Simpson NH et al. Copy number variation screen identifies a rare de novo deletion at chromosome 15q13.1-13.3 in a child with language impairment. PLoS One 2015; 10: e0134997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Brandler WM, Morris AP, Evans DM, Scerri TS, Kemp JP, Timpson NJ et al. Common variants in left/right asymmetry genes and pathways are associated with relative hand skill. PLoS Genet 2013; 9: e1003751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Purcell S, Cherny SS, Sham PC. Genetic power calculator: design of linkage and association genetic mapping studies of complex traits. Bioinformatics 2003; 19: 149–150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Auton A, Abecasis GR, Altshuler DM, Durbin RM, Abecasis GR, Bentley DR et al. A global reference for human genetic variation. Nature 2015; 526: 68–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Cope N, Harold D, Hill G, Moskvina V, Stevenson J, Holmans P et al. Strong evidence that KIAA0319 on chromosome 6p is a susceptibility gene for developmental dyslexia. Am J Hum Genet 2005; 76: 581–591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Siok WT, Perfetti CA, Jin Z, Tan LH. Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture. Nature 2004; 431: 71–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ziegler JC, Goswami U. Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychol Bull 2005; 131: 3–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hu W, Lee HL, Zhang Q, Liu T, Geng LB, Seghier ML et al. Developmental dyslexia in Chinese and English populations: dissociating the effect of dyslexia from language differences. Brain 2010; 133: 1694–1706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. McBride-Chang C, Lam F, Lam C, Chan B, Fong CY-C, Wong TT-Y et al. Early predictors of dyslexia in Chinese children: familial history of dyslexia, language delay, and cognitive profiles. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2011; 52: 204–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Zhao J, Qian Y, Bi H-Y, Coltheart M, Rauschecker AM, Bar M et al. The visual magnocellular-dorsal dysfunction in Chinese children with developmental dyslexia impedes Chinese character recognition. Sci Rep 2014; 4: 7068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Hansen PC, Stein JF, Orde SR, Winter JL, Talcott JB. Are dyslexics’ visual deficits limited to measures of dorsal stream function? Neuroreport 2001; 12: 1527–1530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Paracchini S. Dissection of genetic associations with language-related traits in population-based cohorts. J Neurodev Disord 2011; 3: 365–373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information

Articles from Translational Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES