Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2017 Aug 4;12(8):e0181865. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0181865

Anatomy and behavioral function of serotonin receptors in Drosophila melanogaster larvae

Annina Huser 1, Melanie Eschment 1, Nazli Güllü 1, Katharina A N Collins 1, Kathrin Böpple 1, Lyubov Pankevych 1, Emilia Rolsing 1, Andreas S Thum 1,2,3,*
Editor: Hiromu Tanimoto4
PMCID: PMC5544185  PMID: 28777821

Abstract

The biogenic amine serotonin (5-HT) is an important neuroactive molecule in the central nervous system of the majority of animal phyla. 5-HT binds to specific G protein-coupled and ligand-gated ion receptors to regulate particular aspects of animal behavior. In Drosophila, as in many other insects this includes the regulation of locomotion and feeding. Due to its genetic amenability and neuronal simplicity the Drosophila larva has turned into a useful model for studying the anatomical and molecular basis of chemosensory behaviors. This is particularly true for the olfactory system, which is mostly described down to the synaptic level over the first three orders of neuronal information processing. Here we focus on the 5-HT receptor system of the Drosophila larva. In a bipartite approach consisting of anatomical and behavioral experiments we describe the distribution and the implications of individual 5-HT receptors on naïve and acquired chemosensory behaviors. Our data suggest that 5-HT1A, 5-HT1B, and 5-HT7 are dispensable for larval naïve olfactory and gustatory choice behaviors as well as for appetitive and aversive associative olfactory learning and memory. In contrast, we show that 5-HT/5-HT2A signaling throughout development, but not as an acute neuronal function, affects associative olfactory learning and memory using high salt concentration as a negative unconditioned stimulus. These findings describe for the first time an involvement of 5-HT signaling in learning and memory in Drosophila larvae. In the longer run these results may uncover developmental, 5-HT dependent principles related to reinforcement processing possibly shared with adult Drosophila and other insects.

Introduction

The biogenic amine serotonin (5-HT) exerts an essential role in a wide range of insect behaviors by its action as neurotransmitter, neuromodulator, and/or neurohormone (reviewed by [1]). Accordingly, for the adult fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster it was shown that 5-HT signaling is involved in chemosensation [2, 3], aggression [4, 5], mating [6], feeding [7, 8], and locomotion [7, 9].

As in vertebrates, 5-HT acts as natural ligand for a group of G protein-coupled receptors and ligand-gated ion channels found in the central and peripheral nervous systems [1012]. 5-HT receptors mediate both excitatory and inhibitory function. In the Drosophila genome five different 5-HT G protein-coupled receptors have been previously identified, called 5-HT1A, 5-HT1B, 5-HT2A, 5-HT2B, and 5-HT7 [1318]. 5-HT receptor expression was found in distinct adult fly brain regions including the mushroom bodies (MB), central complex, optic lobes and antennal lobes (AL) [1924]. Consequently, differential 5-HT receptor requirement was reported for aggression (5-HT1A and 5-HT2A), sleep (5-HT1A), feeding (5-HT1B and 5-HT2A), circadian entrainment and behavior (5-HT1B and 5-HT2A), and courtship and mating (5-HT7) [8, 1921, 23, 25, 26]. Moreover, 5-HT/5-HT receptor signaling was shown to be required for learning and memory as 5-HT1A, 5-HT2A, and 5-HT7 were reported to be involved in short and long term association [27]. Additionally, anesthesia-resistant memory formation was supposed to be mediated through 5-HT1A receptors expressed in the mushroom body [28].

At the larval stage only a few studies have addressed the anatomical organization and functional relevance of 5-HT receptors. It was described that putative 5-HT1A, 5-HT1B, 5-HT2A, and 5-HT7 positive cells can be found in the larval central nervous system (CNS) [19, 21, 23, 29]. This allows 5-HT to specifically regulate certain aspects of larval physiology and behavior. In detail, distinct larval 5-HT receptor function was reported for locomotion (5-HT1A, 5-HT1B, 5-HT2A, 5-HT2B, and 5-HT7), reduced light avoidance (5-HT1A), juvenile-to-adult transition (5-HT7), feeding (5-HT2A), and modulation of heart rate frequency (5-HT2A and 5-HT2B) [13, 3034].

However, these studies often focus on a single type of 5-HT receptor and thus miss a comprehensive side-by-side investigation of each receptor on the anatomical and behavioral level. Accordingly, one emphasis of this study was to investigate the organization of several different 5-HT receptors within the larval brain in parallel. To this end we used the Gal4-UAS system that allows for reproducible expression of effector genes in defined subsets of potential 5-HT receptor cells [3537]. Based on the limitation of available Gal4 lines we focused our analysis on four of the five 5-HT receptors: 5-HT1A-Gal4, 5-HT1B-Gal4, 5-HT2A-Gal4 (also often called 5-HT2Dro-Gal4 [21]), and 5-HT7-Gal4. Please note that 5-HT2B was only recently identified [13] and it is not part of this study. Thereby, we provide an initial analysis of larval 5-HT receptor system based on genetic tools that are state of the art and widely used in the field. We want to emphasize that it is not clear for all 5-HT receptor Gal4 lines if and how reliable the Gal4 expression reflects the endogenous receptor gene expression patterns. Due to technical limitations antibody and in-situ information is almost completely unavailable. Consequently, in the longer run, more sophisticated genetic tools have to be established and co-localization studies have to be performed.

Here, we applied a two-part approach by analyzing the expression patterns and the behavioral function of potential 5-HT receptor cells in Drosophila larvae. The behavioral assays include measurements for naïve olfactory and gustatory preferences performed via simple choice tests on agarose filled test plates [38]. In addition, a more advanced design allows to study associative olfactory learning and memory [3947]. Presenting an odor (the conditioned stimulus (CS)) simultaneously with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US) induces experience dependent avoidance of the CS. Conversely, if the same CS is paired with an appetitive US, animals can be trained to develop a preference for the CS. Thus, depending on previous experience, the same odor can trigger either avoidance or attraction [41]. Taken together, a comprehensive set of behavioral assays to analyze larval chemosensory behavior is at hand that allows investigating simple choice behavior and associative olfactory learning and memory in Drosophila larvae.

In an earlier study, we could show that 5-HT positive cells are per se neither necessary for naïve gustatory, olfactory, or light preferences nor for associative olfactory learning and memory (using fructose and electric shock as positive and negative reinforcers, respectively) [48]. However, this does not exclude a specific contribution of distinct 5-HT receptors for chemosensory behaviors. Different 5-HT receptors act antagonistically. 5-HT7 was shown to activate adenylyl cyclases and increases cAMP levels, whereas 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptors inhibit cAMP production [14, 15, 18]. Thus, ablation of the entire 5-HT system can affect both assisting and inhibitory functions of antagonistic 5-HT receptors at the same time. We have therefore expanded our analysis on the role of 5-HT signaling on larval chemosensory behavior by focusing on distinct 5-HT receptor functions.

Materials and methods

Flies

Flies were maintained on standard Drosophila medium at 25°C or 19°C under 12h light /dark conditions. All 5-HT receptor specific lines, 5-HT1A-Gal4 [29], 5-HT1B-Gal4 [49] (Bloomington Stock Center no. 24240), 5-HT2A-Gal4 [21, 50] (Bloomington Stock Center no. 19367), 5-HT7-Gal4 [23], were kindly provided by Charles Nichols. Note that the 5-HT2A-Gal4 construct is an enhancer trap piggyBac construct in the 5-HT2A locus that reduces its expression by nearly 90% [21]. TRH-GAL4 [78] was kindly provided by Serge Birman. For behavioral experiments, wild type Canton-S (WT CS) flies and the effector lines UAS-hid,rpr [51, 52] and UAS-shits [53] (Bloomington Stock Center no. 44222) were used. w1118 flies (kindly provided by Martin Heisenberg) were crossed with UAS- and Gal4 lines to obtain heterozygous controls. To visualize the Gal4 expression pattern, we used UAS-mCD8::GFP [54] (kindly provided by Hiromu Tanimoto) and UAS-myr::tomato [55, 56] (Bloomington Stock Center no. 32221). In all cases five to six day old feeding third instar larvae were used.

Immunostaining

Experiments were performed as described before [57, 58]. Third instar larvae were put on ice and dissected in phosphate buffered saline (PBS). Brains were fixed in 3.6% formaldehyde (Merck, Darmstadt) in PBS for 30 min. After washing with PBT (PBS with 3% Triton-X 100, Sigma Aldrich, St. Louis, MO), brains were blocked with 5% normal goat serum (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) in PBT for one to two hours and then incubated for two days with first antibodies at 4°C. Before applying the secondary antibodies for one or two days at 4°C, brains were washed with PBT. Finally, brains were again washed, mounted in Vectashield (Vector Laboratories) between two cover slips and stored at 4°C in darkness. This protocol was used for data presented in Figs 1A, 1AI, 1AII, 1B, 1BI, 1BVI, 1C, 1CIV and 2.

Fig 1. Gal4 expression patterns of four potential 5-HT receptor lines.

Fig 1

5-HT1A-, 5-HT1B-, 5-HT7-, and 5-HT2A-Gal4 positive cells are shown in A, B, C, and D, respectively. (A-C) Gal4 lines were crossed with UAS-mCD8::GFP to analyze their expression patterns (green; anti-GFP staining) in addition to reference labeling of the central nervous system (CNS) (magenta; anti-ChAT/anti-FasII double-staining). In (D) 5-HT2A-Gal4 was crossed with UAS-myr::tomato to visualize its expression pattern (green; anti-dsRed staining) within the larval CNS (magenta; anti-ChAT/anti-FasII double-staining). For all four lines the first column shows a frontal view onto a z-projection of the entire CNS. In addition, for each line representative z-projections of close-ups of the ventral nerve cord (VNC), one hemisphere (HEMI), the suboesophageal ganglion (SOG), one antennal lobe (AL), the calyx (CA) of the mushroom body (MB), the lobes of the MB, and the larval optic neuropil (LON) are shown from left to right. Below each close-up only the GFP channel is shown as an inverted black and white image to visualize innervation patterns with higher contrast and no neuropil background. White arrows highlight aspects of the expression patterns that are further described in the results. Additional abbrevations: VL vertical lobe, ML medial lobe, PED peduncle; Scale bars: 50 μm (in A, B, C, D) and 20 μm (in all other panels).

Fig 2. Co-expression of 5-HT1A-, 5-HT1B-, and 5-HT7-Gal4 with anti-5-HT and anti-TH.

Fig 2

Expression patterns of 5-HT1A-, 5-HT1B-, and 5-HT7-Gal4 crossed with UAS-mCD8::GFP are visualized using a triple staining protocol. Anti-GFP (green) was used to label Gal4 positive cells, anti-ChAT/anti-FasII (blue) was used as a reference labeling for the CNS, and anti-5-HT (red, in A, C, E) or anti-TH (red, in B, D, F) was used to identify 5-HT or dopaminergic cells, respectively. 5-HT2A-Gal4 was not included in this experiment due to the low number of cell bodies inside the CNS (Fig 1D). There was no co-staining detected between 5-HT receptor Gal4 expression patterns and anti-TH (B, D, F). The same is true for the co-expression analysis with anti-5-HT (A, C, E); the only exception was a single pair of neurons in the terminal segment of the VNC labeled by 5-HT1A-Gal4 (arrowhead in AII). (A-F) show frontal views onto z-projections of the entire CNS (left) and representative z-projections of close-ups of one or both hemispheres and SOG (right top) and abdominal ganglion (right bottom). Scale bars: 50 μm (left) and 20 μm (right top and bottom).

In addition we used a second protocol developed at the HHMI Janelia research campus (https://www.janelia.org/project-team/flylight/protocols). Larval tissues were dissected, fixed and washed as described above. After blocking with 3% normal goat serum (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) in PBT for 1 hr, tissues were incubated for two days with first antibodies at 4°C. After multiple rinses in PBT, tissues were incubated 2 days at 4°C in the cocktail of secondary antibodies. Nervous systems were then washed two to three times in PBT, mounted on poly-L-lysine (Sigma-Aldrich) coated coverslips and then transferred to a coverslip staining jar (Electron Microscopy Sciences) to dehydrate through a graded ethanol series. Afterwards tissues were cleared in xylene, and mounted in DPX (Sigma). This protocol was used for data presented in Fig 1AIII–1AVII, 1BII–1BV, 1BVII, 1CI–1CIII, 1CV and 1DVII.

Images were taken with Zeiss confocal laser microscopes LSM 550 and LSM 780. The resulting image stacks were projected and analyzed with ImageJ (NIH; https://imagej.nih.gov/ij/) software. Contrast and brightness adjustment as well as rotation and organization of images were performed in Photoshop (Adobe Systems Inc., San Jose, CA).

Immunofluorescence antibodies

Anti-GFP (polyclonal/rabbit/A6455/Molecular Probes/1:1000), anti-GFP-FITC (polyclonal/goat/ ab6662/abcam/1:1000) and anti-DsRed (polyclonal/rabbit/632496/Clonetech/1:200) were used for visualizing Gal4 lines expression patterns. Two different mouse antibodies for staining the neuropil (anti-ChAT (monoclonal/mouse/ChAT4B1/DSHB/1:100)) and the axonal tracts (anti-Fasciclin II (monoclonal/1d4 anti-Fas/DSHB/1:50)) were applied to provide landmarks within the larval CNS. 5-HT cells were visualized with anti-5-HT (polyclonal/rabbit/S5545/Sigma Aldrich /1:1000), dopaminergic cells with anti-TH (monoclonal/mouse/22941/Immuno-Star/1:500), respectively.

As secondary antibodies goat anti-rabbit IgG Alexa Fluor 488 (polyclonal/A11008/Molecular Probes/ 1:200), goat anti-mouse IgG Alexa Fluor 647 (polyclonal/A21235/Molecular Probes/1:200), goat anti-mouse IgG Alexa Fluor 405 (polyclonal/A31553/Molecular Probes/1:200), goat anti-mouse IgG Cy3 (polyclonal/A10521/Molecular Probes/1:200), and goat anti-rabbit IgG Cy5 (polyclonal/A10523/Molecular Probes/1:200) were used.

Behavioral experiments

Five to six day old feeding third instar larvae were used for all behavioral experiments. The assays were performed either at 22°C or at 35°C using UAS-hid,rpr and UAS-shits effector lines, respectively. In case of UAS-shits, larvae were additionally incubated for 2 min in a water bath at 37°C prior to behavioral experiments. For further details on experimental protocols we added the following description and refer to earlier studies [3840, 43, 57, 5962].

Associative olfactory learning and memory

Experiments were conducted on test plates filled with a thin layer of 2.5% agarose containing either pure agarose or agarose plus reinforcer. We used 1.5 M sodium chloride (SALT) and 2.0 M D-fructose (FRU). As olfactory stimuli, we used 10 μl amyl acetate (AM, diluted 1:250 in paraffin oil) and benzaldehyde (BA, undiluted). Odorants were loaded into custom-made Teflon containers with perforated lids. Learning and memory were tested by exposing a first group of 30 animals to BA, while crawling on agarose medium containing sugar as a positive reinforcer or high salt concentration as a negative reinforcer. After 5 min, larvae were transferred to a fresh test plate in which they were allowed to crawl on pure agarose medium for 5 min while being exposed to AM. A second group of larvae received the reciprocal training. Immediately, after three training cycles, larvae were transferred onto test plates on which AM and BA were presented on opposite sides. Please note that for aversive olfactory learning and memory the test plate included the same high salt concentration as respective training plates. After 5 min, individuals were counted on the AM side (#AM), the BA side (#BA), and in a neutral zone. By subtracting the number of larvae on the BA side from the number of larvae on the AM side divided by the total number of counted individuals (#TOTAL), we calculated a preference index for each training group:

PREFAM+/BA=(# AM - # BA)/# TOTAL (1a)
PREFAM/BA+=(# AM -  # BA)/# TOTAL (1b)

To measure specifically the effect of associative learning and memory we calculated the associative performance index (PI) as the difference in preference between the reciprocally trained larvae:

PI=(PREFAM+/BA - PREFAM/BA+)/2 (2)

Negative PIs thus represent aversive olfactory learning and memory, whereas positive PIs indicate appetitive olfactory learning and memory. Division by 2 ensures scores are bound within (-1; 1).

Chemosensory preference

For gustatory preference tests, 2.5% agarose (Sigma Aldrich Cat. No.: A5093, CAS No.: 9012-36-6) solution was boiled in a microwave oven and filled as a thin layer into test plates (85 mm diameter, Cat. No.: 82.1472, Sarstedt, Nümbrecht). After cooling, the agarose was removed from half of the plate. The empty half was filled with 2.5% agarose solution containing sodium chloride (SALT, Sigma Aldrich Cat. No.: S7653, CAS No.: 7647-14-5; 2.0 M and 1.5 M) and D-fructose (FRU, Sigma Aldrich Cat. No.: 47740, CAS No.: 57-48-7; 2.0 M). Assay plates were used at the same day shortly after preparation to avoid diffusion of the stimuli from one side to the other. Groups of 30 larvae were placed in the middle of the plate, allowed to crawl for 5 min, and then counted on the stimulus containing side, the agarose only side, and a neutral zone. The neutral zone covers 1 cm from top to bottom of the Petri dish between the left and right sides. It thereby separates both halfs and covers the transition from pure agarose to agarose plus gustatory stimulus. By subtracting the number of larvae on the pure agarose side (#nS) from the number of larvae on the stimulus side (#S) divided by the total number of counted larvae (#TOTAL), a preference index for the respective chemosensory stimulus was calculated:

PREF=(#S - #nS)/#TOTAL

Negative PREF values indicate avoidance, whereas positive PREF values represent attractiveness.

For olfactory preference tests, a similar assay was used except that olfactory stimuli were presented in custom-made Teflon containers with perforated lids presented on only pure agarose containing test plates. As olfactory stimuli amyl acetate (AM, Fluka Cat. No.: 46022; CAS No.: 628-63-7; diluted 1:250 in paraffin oil, Fluka Cat. No.: 76235, CAS No.: 8012-95-1) and benzaldehyde (BA, Fluka Cat. No.: 12010, CAS No.: 100-52-7; undiluted) were used.

Acutely blocking synaptic output with shibirets

To acutely block synaptic output of defined sets of cells we used UAS-shits1 [53]. Immediately before the experiment, larvae were incubated for 2 min in a water-bath at 37°C. The behavioral experiments were then performed as described before, at a restrictive temperature of about 35°C in a custom made chamber.

Statistical methods

Statistical analysis and visualizations were done with R (version 2.15.2), R studio (version 0.98.1028) and Adobe Photoshop (version CC 2015.5). Behavioral data are visualized as box plots with medians (middle lines), 25% / 75% percentiles (box boundaries), and 10% / 90% percentiles (whiskers). Sample size in each case is n = 10–20. Kruskal-Wallis tests (KWT) were performed and, in case of significance, followed by Wilcoxon rank-sum tests (WRT); Bonferroni corrections were used for multiple comparisons as applicable (indicated with BWRT). Likewise, Wilcoxon signed-ranked tests (WST) were used to compare values against chance level. Visualization of statistical evaluations: if only n.s. is shown the initial KWT did not suggest for a difference between the three groups (p ≥ 0.05). When differences between each group are shown this provides the results of the BWRT as the initial KWT suggested for significance (p < 0.05). P values were rounded to three decimal places except for cases that would have resulted in zero. Here additional decimal places are given. Further details including raw data can be found in S1 Table.

Results

Expression patterns of four specific 5-HT receptor Gal4 lines in the larval brain

To analyze the cellular anatomy of different 5-HT receptor Gal4 lines in the CNS of third instar larvae of Drosophila, we crossed 5-HT1A-Gal4, 5-HT1B-Gal4, and 5-HT7-Gal4 with UAS-mCD8::GFP. We used an anti-GFP antibody to identify details of the cellular innervation and morphology and anti-Fasciclin II (FasII) / anti-Choline Acetyltransferase (ChAT) background staining (Fig 1), which label axonal tracts [63] and neuropils [64], respectively. The double labeling approach thus allows us to map the different 5-HT receptor Gal4 expression patterns into a common neuropil reference. This approach was hitherto not applied to all of these lines at the larval stage. Please note, to clearly disentangle 5HT2A-Gal4 dependent expression from marker expression we crossed 5-HT2A-Gal4 with UAS-myr::tomato and labelled specifically Gal4 positive cells via an anti-DsRed antibody [65]. This is necessary as the enhancer trap piggyBac construct carries a “3xP3-EYFP” marker that is also recognized by the anti-GFP antibody [50, 66, 67]. The “3xP3-EYFP” marker is expressed under the control of the endogenous Pax6 gene and thus on its own drives expression in the larval visual system and brain [68, 69].

5-HT1A-Gal4

5-HT1A-Gal4 expression was found throughout the larval CNS including both brain hemispheres and the ventral nerve cord (VNC) (Fig 1A). A closer inspection revealed innervation of the protocerebrum (arrow in Fig 1AII and 1AVI), the AL (arrow in Fig 1AIV), to a lesser degree of the suboesophageal ganglion (SOG; Fig 1AIII), and mushroom body calyx (CA; arrow in Fig 1AV). In contrast, the MB lobes and larval optic neuropil (LON) were not labeled (Fig 1AVI and 1AVII). Characteristic for the 5-HT1A-Gal4 expression pattern was a row of cell bodies localized close to the midline in each thoracic and abdominal segment, (Fig 1A and 1AI) as well as a set of about 15 cell bodies dorsolateral of the CA innervating different protocerebral regions (Fig 1AV). In addition, several neurons were labled per thoracic and abdominal segment that leave the brain and potentially target the peripheral system of the larvae (arrows in Fig 1A). The obtained expression pattern is comparable to the ones described before [15, 29].

5-HT1B-GAL4

5-HT1B-Gal4 also showed expression in extended regions throughout the whole larval CNS, including both hemispheres and the VNC (Fig 1B). A closer inspection revealed innervation in cells at the tip and midline of the SOG (arrows in 1BIII). A small number of only about 20, likely embryonic-born Kenyon cells (KC) [70], innervate the entire MB at the CA and at the surface of the MB peduncle, vertical and medial lobe (arrows in Fig 1BV and 1BVI). No expression was found in the AL and LON (Fig 1BIV and 1BVII). Besides the pronounced staining along the midline, a set of cells with somata in the lateral abdominal segments project to the terminal plexus of the VNC (arrows in Fig 1BI). These 5-HT1B positive cells were reported to express the peptide hormone leucokinin and play a role in larval turning behavior [71]. An additional characteristic of the 5-HT1B-Gal4 line is its massive expression laterally in both hemispheres that give rise to the adult optic lobes after metamorphosis (arrows in Fig 1BII and 1BVII) [72]. Our results are in line with earlier findings of the expression patterns of 5-HT1B [19, 71].

5-HT7-Gal4

5-HT7-Gal4 expression was detected throughout the whole larval CNS including both hemispheres and the VNC (Fig 1C). We could not find innervation within the MB lobes and the LON by potential 5-HT7 cells (Fig 1CVI and 1CVII). Yet, innervation was detected in the VNC, SOG, AL and CA (arrows in Fig 1CI–1CV). Seven cells with somata in close proximity to the AL densely innervate the AL (Fig 1CIV). These are likely projection neurons as they follow the antennocerebral tract an innervate the CA (arrows in Fig 1CV). The peduncle of the MB was weakly innervated by a MB extrinsic neuron called BL neuron (right arrow in Fig 1CVI) [70]. In addition, 5-HT7-Gal4 shows dense innervation within the SOG potentially co-localizing with the 5-HT positive SE0 clusters [34, 48, 73]. These cells were reported to be involved in feeding and also in linking the external environment with the internal endocrine system [34, 73]. Overall, the observed expression is consistent with the one reported before [23].

5-HT2A-Gal4

5-HT2A-Gal4 showed an expression pattern that is remarkably different when compared to the other three driver lines (Fig 1D). There are nearly no somata detectable within the larval CNS (Fig 1DI–DVII). Yet, there is strong labelling within the SOG, likely by axonal terminals from neurons of the peripheral nervous system that enter the brain via different nerves (arrows in Fig 1DIII). However, due to technical limitations we were not able to clearly localize these cells outside of the CNS. We are nonetheless convinced that these cells are 5-HT2A-Gal4 positive due to their initial description by Nichols [21] using the same Gal4 line. In addition, we found a second type of brain innervation from the periphery, this time into the LON (arrow in Fig 1DVII). Please note that the larval MB was not innervated. Detectable staining was limited to adjacent fiber bundles (lower arrow in Fig 1DVI) and weak innervation of the dorsal protocerebrum (upper arrow in Fig 1DVI).

In summary, the four analyzed Gal4 expression patterns suggest that 5-HT receptors may be expressed broadly throughout the brain in partially overlapping patterns including primary olfactory and gustatory brain centers (AL and SOG). Distinct receptors may be limited to particular types of cells like peripheral sensory neurons that project to the SOG (5-HT2A-Gal4) and MB Kenyon cells that are known to be involved in learning and memory (5-HT1B-Gal4). Yet, these results are based on Gal4 expression patterns that may not represent the endogenous gene expression patterns and therefore have to be handled with care.

Co-localization of 5-HT receptor Gal4 lines with serotonin and Tyrosine-Hydroxylase

To examine if the larval 5-HT system is controlled in an auto-regulatory manner [7476], we crossed 5-HT1A-, 5-HT1B-, and 5-HT7-Gal4 with UAS-mCD8::GPF, used the same primary antibody mixture as before (anti-ChAT/anti-FasII and anti-GPF), but added anti-5-HT (Fig 2A, 2C and 2E). We did not analyze 5-HT2A-Gal4 due to the low number of stained somata within the larval brain (Fig 1D), but rather afferent projections of peripheral neurons that have their somata outside the CNS. All three driver lines exhibited no co-labeling of Gal4 positive and anti-5-HT staining (Fig 2A, 2C and 2E). This indicates that 5-HT receptors may localize mostly postsynaptically in the larva. This interpretation matches with earlier results that have analyzed 5-HT/5-HT receptor co-localization [23, 29]. Yet, we also found one exception: consistent with previous work [29] a single pair of 5-HT1A stained cells in the VNC co-localized with 5-HT immunolabeling (Fig 2AII, arrowheads).

In addition, we also analyzed if potential 5-HT receptor cells are dopaminergic. It was shown that different aminergic systems directly connect on each other. Octopaminergic neurons for example signal a specific aspect of sugar reinforcement directly onto dopaminergic neurons that express an α-adrenergic-like octopamine receptor called OAMB [77]. To address if the larval serotonergic system can directly act on dopaminergic neurons we applied a triple staining approach. We used anti-TH for the visualization of dopaminergic neurons, anti-GFP to label 5-HT receptor positive cells and anti-ChAT/anti-FasII for a reference staining of the larval CNS (Fig 2B, 2D and 2F). For the tested lines, 5-HT1A-, 5-HT1B-, and 5-HT7-Gal4, we did not detect co-localization with dopamine synthesizing cells. Therefore, it is rather unlikely that the 5-HT system directly signals as a neurotransmitter on dopaminergic neurons.

Ablation of 5-HT2A-Gal4 positive cells during development impairs aversive associative olfactory learning and memory

To examine if larvae lacking potential 5-HT receptor cells are able to associate an odor with a positive or negative gustatory stimulus, we utilized a well-established standard paradigm (reviewed in [38]). As olfactory stimuli we used benzaldehyde (BA) and amyl acetate (AM). As appetitive and aversive gustatory unconditioned stimuli we used fructose (FRU, 2.0 M) and high sodium chloride concentration (SALT, 1.5 M), respectively. This is possible, because ablation of potential 5-HT receptor cells did neither change naïve olfactory responses towards amyl acetate (diluted 1:250 in paraffin oil), benzaldehyde (undiluted), heptanol (undiluted), and nonanol (undiluted) (S1 Fig); nor gustatory responses to sodium chloride (2M and 1.5M for 5-HT2A-Gal4), fructose (2M), arabinose (2M), and sorbitol (2M) (S2 and S3 Figs).

Ablation of 5-HT1A-Gal4, 5-HT1B-Gal4, and 5-HT7-Gal4 positive cells throughout development did not change appetitive olfactory learning and memory (Fig 3B, KWT p = 0.061; Fig 3C, KWT p = 0.007; BWRT p = 0.189 compared to 5-HT1B-Gal4/+ and p = 0.052 compared to UAS-hid,rpr/+; Fig 3D, KWT p = 0.061) as well as aversive olfactory learning and memory (Fig 3F, KWT p = 0.233; Fig 3G, KWT p = 0.006; BWRT p = 1.000 compared to 5-HT1B-Gal4/+ and p = 0.016 compared to UAS-hid,rpr/+; Fig 3H, KWT p = 0.303).

Fig 3. Ablation of potential 5-HT2A receptor cells throughout development impairs aversive olfactory learning and memory.

Fig 3

5-HT1A-, 5-HT1B-, 5-HT7-, and 5-HT2A-Gal4 lines were crossed with UAS-hid,rpr to genetically induce apoptosis in potential 5-HT receptor cells. In addition, Gal4 lines and UAS-hid,rpr were crossed with w1118 to obtain heterozygous genetic control larvae. (A) provides a color scheme for the three different groups used in each experiment. Appetitive olfactory learning and memory using fructose reinforcement is shown at the top (B-E). Aversive olfactory learning and memory is shown at the bottom (F-I). In most cases, experimental larvae and genetic control groups behaved similar. However, ablation of 5-HT2A-Gal4 positive cells specifically impaired aversive olfactory learning and memory (I), while leaving appetitive olfactory learning and memory intact (E). Sample size (n = 10–14) is indicated at the bottom of each box plot. Differences against zero are given at the top of each box plot. Differences between all three groups or individual groups are shown at the bottom of the panel. Visualization of statistical evaluations: if only n.s. is shown the initial Kruskal-Wallis test (KWT) did not suggest for a difference between the three groups (p > 0.05). When differences between each group are shown this provides the results of the Wilcoxon rank-sum tests with Bonferroni corrections (BWRT) as the initial KWT suggested for singnificance (p < 0.05). *** (p < 0.001), ** (p < 0.01), * (p < 0.05), n.s. (not significant p ≥ 0.05).

Yet, 5-HT2A-Gal4/UAS-hid,rpr experimental larvae showed impaired aversive olfactory learning and memory (Fig 3I; WST p = 0.131), which was significantly different compared to both genetic controls (Fig 3I; KWT p = 0.010; BWRT p = 0.021 compared to 5-HT2A-Gal4/+, p = 0.034 compared to UAS-hid,rpr/+). Appetitive olfactory learning and memory did not differ among the three tested genotypes (Fig 3E; KWT p = 0.606). Repetition of the experiments with increased sample size gave rise to the same results (S4 Fig).

In summary, we thus conclude that 5-HT1A-, 5-HT1B-, and 5-HT7-Gal4 positive cells were not necessary for appetitive and aversive olfactory learning and memory. Yet, for 5-HT2A-Gal4 we got a different result; unlike appetitive olfactory learning and memory, aversive olfactory learning and memory was impaired.

5-HT2A receptor function throughout development is necessary for aversive olfactory learning and memory

To investigate if the memory impairment seen for 5-HT2A-Gal4/UAS-hid,rpr experimental larvae was due to changes in the 5-HT2A receptor function, we next tested a homozygous hypomorphic 5-HT2A receptor mutant. In detail, Nichols [21] showed via quantitative real-time PCR analysis for this line that 5-HT2A expression is reduced by about 90% compared to wild type (Oregon-R) and w1118 flies.

Homozygous 5-HT2A mutant larvae showed a similar reduction for aversive olfactory learning and memory when compared to wild-type control (WT CS) larvae (Fig 4B; WRT p = 0.001). Again, the behavioral phenotype was specific for aversive olfactory learning and memory and did not affect appetitive olfactory learning and memory (Fig 4C; WRT p = 1.000 compared to WT CS). In addition, we performed control experiments with 5-HT2A mutant and WT CS larvae to test for proper olfactory and gustatory chemotaxis. This was necessary to exclude perturbing defects in task-relevant sensory-motor abilities. 5-HT2A receptor mutants displayed olfactory AM and BA preferences as well as gustatory SALT and FRU preferences that did not significantly differ from wild type controls (Fig 4D, 4E, 4F and 4G; WRT p = 0.173, p = 0.154, p = 0.148, p = 0.516, respectively).

Fig 4. Impaired 5-HT2A receptor function throughout development impairs aversive olfactory learning and memory.

Fig 4

Homozygous 5-HT2A receptor gene mutants and wild-type control larvae (WT CS) were used to analyze aversive (B) and appetitive (C) olfactory learning and memory, olfactory amyl acetate (AM, in D) and benzaldehyde (BA in E) preferences and gustatory sodium chloride (SALT, in F) and fructose (FRU, in G) preferences. (A) provides a color scheme for the two different groups used in each experiment. Whereas mutant larvae showed olfactory and gustatory preferences as well as appetitive olfactory learning and memory comparable to WT CS larvae, aversive olfactory learning and memory was significantly reduced. Sample size (n = 11–17) is indicated at the bottom of each box plot. Differences against zero are given at the top of each box plot. Differences between mutant and wild type larvae are shown at the bottom of the panel. *** (p < 0.001), ** (p < 0.01), * (p < 0.05), n.s. (not significant p ≥ 0.05).

Based on these results we conclude that 5-HT2A receptor function throughout development is necessary for aversive olfactory learning and memory using high salt concentration as a unconditioned stimulus.

Ablation of the 5-HT/5-HT2A receptor system throughout development specifically impairs aversive odor-salt learning and memory

In an earlier study we had shown that larvae lacking 5-HT cells are able to establish an association between an odor and a punishing stimulus [48]. At first sight the behavioral phenotypes shown in Figs 3I and 4B, therefore, appear to be contradictory. Yet, in our initial experiments we used electric shock instead of high salt concentration as unconditioned stimulus. Thus, it is possible that manipulation of the 5-HT system does not affect aversive olfactory learning and memory in general but is rather restricted to gustatory high salt concentration. To investigate whether this is the case we utilized the same approach as used in Huser et al. (2012). We crossed the TRH-Gal4 line [78] that expresses in most of the larval 5-HT cells (Fig 5A; [48]) as the tryptophan hydroxylase (TRH) gene was reported to catalyse the rate-limiting step of 5-HT synthesis from tryptophan to 5-hydroxy-tryptophan [1]. Experimental TRH-Gal4/UAS-hid,rpr larvae having most of their 5-HT cells ablated throughout development [48] did not show aversive odor-salt learning and memory (Fig 5B; WST p = 0.762). The behavior was different from both genetic controls (Fig 5B; KWT p = 0.002; BWRT p = 0.002 compared to TRH-Gal4/+, p = 0.038 compared to UAS-hid,rpr/+). The specificity for the unconditioned stimulus is further supported by a second experiment. Also 5-HT2A-Gal4/UAS-hid,rpr experimental larvae that received odor-electric shock training performed similar as genetic controls and were not impaired in learning and memory (Fig 5C and 5D; KWT p = 0.470). We thus conclude that 5-HT/5-HT2A receptor signaling throught development is necessary for aversive olfactory learning and memory reinforced by high salt concentration.

Fig 5. 5-HT/5-HT2A signaling throughout development specifically impairs odor-salt learning and memory.

Fig 5

(A) The TRH-Gal4 line was crossed with UAS-mCD8::GFP to visualize its expression pattern (green; anti-GFP staining) in addition to a reference labeling of the CNS (magenta; anti-ChAT/anti-FasII double-staining). (B) The TRH-Gal4 line was crossed with UAS-hid,rpr to genetically induce apoptosis in 5-HT cells. In addition, the Gal4 line and UAS-hid,rpr were crossed with w1118 to obtain heterozygous genetic control larvae. Above the panel a color scheme describes the three different groups used in the experiment. Ablation of 5-HT cells completely abolished aversive olfactory learning and memory reinforced by high salt concentration. (C) shows an overview on the experimental procedure that was used in larvae to test for odor-electric shock learning and memory. (D) Ablation of 5-HT2A-Gal4 positive cells via UAS-hid,rpr did not impair odor-electric shock learning and memory. Sample size (n = 15) is indicated at the bottom of each box plot. Differences against zero are given at the top of each box plot. Differences between the groups are shown at the bottom of the panel. Visualization of statistical evaluations: if only n.s. is shown the initial KWT did not suggest for a difference between the three groups (p > 0.05). When differences between each group are shown this provides the results of the BWRT as the initial KWT suggested for significance (p < 0.05). *** (p < 0.001), ** (p < 0.01), * (p < 0.05), n.s. (not significant p ≥ 0.05). Scale bar: 50 μm.

Acute blockage of neuronal output of 5-HT and 5-HT2A receptor cells does not impair odor-salt learning and memory

To address if 5-HT function is acutely required for odor-salt learning and memory we used the temperature-sensitive dominant negative form of dynamin UAS-shibirets (shits) [53]. Thereby, we specifically disrupted synaptic vesicle recycling during training and testing but not during development.

An acute block of neurotransmission of 5-HT2A-Gal4 and TRH-Gal4 positive cells did not affect odor-salt learning and memory (Fig 6C and 6D; WST p = 0.001 and p = 0.020). For both experiments we did not observe a difference between the three particular genotypes (Fig 6C and 6D; KWT p = 0.414 and p = 0.655).

Fig 6. Acute blockage of synaptic output of 5-HT cells and potential 5-HT2A receptor cells does not affect chemosensory behavior.

Fig 6

The 5-HT2A-Gal4 and TRH-Gal4 lines were crossed with UAS-shits to genetically interfere with synaptic transmission only during training and testing but not during development. In addition, the Gal4 lines and UAS-hid,rpr were crossed with w1118 to obtain heterozygous genetic control larvae. (A) provides a color scheme for the different groups used in each experiment. (B) shows the temperature regime that was applied to block synaptic output at the restrictive temperature of 35°C specifically during training (30 min) and testing (5 min). Immediately before the experiment, larvae were incubated (2 min) in a water-bath at 37°C. Aversive olfactory learning and memory reinforced by high salt concentration (C and D), olfactory preferences for AM and BA (E–H) and gustatory preferences for SALT (I and J) were analyzed. In none of the cases experimental larvae behaved significantly different compared to both genetic control groups. We thus reason that blockage of synaptic output of 5-HT cells and 5-HT2A-Gal4 positive cells does not impair all tested chemosensory behaviors. Sample size (n = 12–18) is indicated at the bottom of each box plot. Differences against zero are given at the top of each box plot. Differences between all three groups or individual groups are shown at the bottom of the panel, except for SALT, where it is placed above the box plots. Visualization of statistical evaluations: if only n.s. is shown the initial KWT did not suggest for a difference between the three groups (p > 0.05). When differences between each group are shown this provides the results of the BWRT as the initial KWT suggested for singnificance (p < 0.05). *** (p < 0.001), ** (p < 0.01), * (p < 0.05), n.s. (not significant p ≥ 0.05).

In addition, experimental larvae were tested for olfactory AM and BA preferences and gustatory SALT preference. 5-HT2A-Gal4/UAS-shits were attracted by both odors (Fig 6E and 6G; WST p = 0.001 and p = 0.048) and avoided SALT (Fig 6I; WST p = 0.033). For all experiments there was no difference between experimental and control larvae (Fig 6E, 6G and 6I; KWT p = 0.560, KWT p = 0.660, KWT p = 0.127, respectively). The same results were observed with TRH-Gal4 (Fig 6F, 6H and 6J; KWT p = 0.017; BWRT p = 0.029 compared to TRH-Gal4/+, p = 0.622 compared to UAS-shits/+; KWT p = 0.597, KWT p = 0.251, respectively). We thus conclude that acute blockage of neurotransmission of the 5-HT signaling system did not affect odor-salt learning and memory reinforced by high salt concentration as well as tested naïve chemosensory preferences.

Discussion

Ablation of 5-HT/5-HT2A receptor signaling during development impairs aversive olfactory learning and memory reinforced by high salt concentration

Serotonin is a biogenic amine, an important neuroactive molecule within the CNS of several insect species. Serotonin, dopamine, histamine, octopamine, and tyramine are amines which have been extensively studied in Drosophila (reviewed in [1, 79, 80]. Each one of the five aminergic neuronal systems—including the serotonergic system—exhibits a stereotypic pattern of a small number of neurons that are widely distributed in the larval and adult CNS [1]. Aminergic neurons have attracted much attention in recent years. Thus, the detailed anatomy and various behavioral functions for many aminergic neurons were identified—in numerous cases even at single cell resolution (only focusing on the Drosophila larvae: [48, 57, 58, 62, 81]).

In contrast, much less is known on the receptor side. Due to the lack of specific antibodies and genetic tools to anatomically describe and functionally manipulate aminergic receptor cells only limited access and insight is given. Further, similar to vertebrates, for each amine different receptor genes were identified that in turn often couple to different signaling pathways and thereby provide a substrate for complex multi-dimensional functions (reviewed in [1, 82]). This complexity is additionally complicated by differences in the nomenclature (5-HT7 or 5-HT7Dro was also called 5-HT-dro1; 5-HT1A or 5-HT1ADro was also called 5-HT-dro2A [10]) and the identification of new receptor genes (5-HT2B was only recently described [13]).

In this study we used state of the art Gal4 lines to anatomically describe and functionally evaluate the role of 5-HT1A, 5-HT1B, 5-HT2A, and 5-HT7 receptor cells with respect to larval chemosensory behaviors [2123, 26, 27, 2932, 71]. Due to the lack of specific antibodies this approach is limited as it is not possible to validate the correctness of each Gal4 expression pattern. We found that putative 5-HT/5-HT2A receptor signaling during development is necessary for odor-salt learning and memory. The finding is based on three mutually supportive results. First, ablation of potential 5-HT2A receptor cells throughout development specifically impairs odor-salt learning and memory (Fig 3). Second, 5-HT2A receptor mutant larvae show the same impairment specifically for odor-salt learning and memory (Fig 4). Third, ablation of 5-HT cells throughout development abolishes specifically odor-salt learning and memory (Fig 5), while leaving appetitive odor-sugar and aversive odor-electric-shock learning and memory intact [48]. Thereby, we describe for the first time a potential involvement of the 5-HT system in larval nervous system development underlying learning and memory. The mode of action, however, of the putative 5-HT/5-HT2A receptor signaling is related to the development of the animal and independent of an acute neuronal function (Fig 6).

5-HT and 5-HT2A receptor cells are dispensable for most of the chemosensory behaviors tested

In addition, we found that potential 5-HT1A, 5-HT1B, and 5-HT7 receptor cells are not necessary for any of the tested chemosensory behaviors (Fig 3, S1 and S2 Figs). However, given the missing verification for each of the three Gal4 lines expression patterns this result has to be interpreted carefully. Moreover, 5-HT2A receptor cells were not required for olfactory and gustatory preferences and appetitive olfactory learning and memory (Fig 3, S1, S2, S3 and S4 Figs). Overall, it is remarkable that the obtained behavioral phenotypes are limited, particularly as several studies revealed that 5-HT receptors are essential for different aspects of larval behavior. For locomotion, for instance, 5-HT1B function was required within a small set of abdominal leucokinin positive neurons to suppress rearing [71]. RNAi-dependent knock-down of 5-HT1B, 5-HT2A, and 5-HT7 receptors within the MB Kenyon cells increased the distance larvae crawled within 140 seconds [32]. Furthermore, 5-HT2A and 5-HT7 receptor mutant larvae showed a reduced number of body-wall contractions [31]. Thus, it seems that larval locomotion requires the entire set of 5-HT receptors within the CNS to organize different aspects of the motor program. Nevertheless, 5-HT receptor function is not limited to locomotion. Pan-neuronal overexpression of 5-HT1A, but not 5-HT1B, 5-HT2A, and 5-HT7, increased the time larvae spend in the light [33]. In addition, 5-HT2A mutants and pan-neuronal knock-down of 5-HT2A reduces larval feeding [13]. This means that individual 5-HT receptors are also important for different kinds of larval behavior. For 5-HT2A this at least includes locomotion, feeding, and odor-salt learning and memory. Future work needs to address whether different sets of cells regulate distinct behavioral outputs or if a single set of cells has multiple functions. Interestingly, in our hands genetic interference with 5-HT receptor function did not affect olfactory and gustatory preferences nor appetitive olfactory learning and memory (Fig 3, S1, S2, S3 and S4 Figs). Thus, defects in certain aspects of locomotion did not prevent larvae from making chemosensory choices, at least within the test interval of 5 minutes that we have applied. Loss or knock-down of 5-HT receptor function does not completely compromise larval crawling and therefore still allows larvae to orientate within their chemosensory environment over longer time periods. In the future, recently established automated locomotion tracking techniques can be implemented that allow to reconstruct and evaluate larval runs and turns with high spatial and temporal resolution [8387]. This will allow to evaluate if 5-HT receptor function is necessary for immediate chemosensory responses or if particular aspects of the chemosensory behavioral output are changed (for example turn rate and run distance).

Interestingly, although 5-HT2A function is required for feeding [13], larvae with missing 5-HT2A cells or reduced 5-HT2A expression are able to associate an odor with a food reward (Figs 3 and 4 and S4 Fig). This might suggest that at least two different systems process gustatory information in the larval CNS: one pathway modulates feeding and depends on 5-HT function, a second pathway is important for appetitive learning and memory and independent of 5-HT and its receptors. Indeed, it was shown that 5-HT function affects all feeding related motor patterns, including head tilting, mouth hook movement, and pharyngeal and esophagus movements [73, 88]. Whether the processing of the sweetness and/or the nutritional value of sugars contributes differently to these proposed pathways is not clear [45]. A similar dual system was recently also found for the processing of bitter quinine [89].

The developmental effect of 5-HT/5-HT2A receptor signaling is specific for odor-salt learning and memory

We have shown that 5-HT cells and 5-HT2A receptor function is necessary for odor-salt learning and memory during development. During learning olfactory stimuli are sensed by only 21 olfactory receptor neurons, which are housed in a single sensillum at the head of the larva, the dorsal organ [9095]. The olfactory information from a given olfactory receptor neuron is further conveyed by 21 uniglomerular and 14 multiglomerular projection neurons from the AL to the lateral horn and the CA region of the MB [93, 9699]. Here, intrinsic MB Kenyon cells provide a substrate for synaptic plasticity as olfactory information converges with gustatory reward and punishment signals from different sets of dopaminergic neurons [57, 62, 100, 101]. Further downstream only a limited number of about two dozen MB output neurons [70] transfer the information onto premotor centers to trigger learned behavior.

Given that 5-HT cells and 5-HT2A receptor function is dispensable for appetitive olfactory learning and memory (Figs 3 and 4 and S4 Fig) and given that 5-HT cells [48] as well as 5-HT2A-Gal4 positive cells (Fig 5) are not necessary for aversive odor-electric shock learning and memory, we assume that 5-HT/5-HT2A receptor signaling is specifically necessary for salt reinforcement processing. Otherwise, due to the overlapping neuronal circuits (including the olfactory circuit, the mushroom body, and the premotor and motor centers), more general impairments in learning and memory would have occurred.

Where in the reinforcing pathway does 5-HT/5-HT2A receptor signaling become effective? Unfortunately, very little is known about the neuronal pathways signaling aversive salt reinforcement. In Drosophila larvae there is no evidence for direct 5-HT input onto the MB [48]. In adults, however, 5-HT positive neurons innervate the MB lobes and CA, termed DPM (dorsal paired medial) and CSD (contralaterally projecting serotonin-immunoreactive deutocerebral) neuron [102, 103]. Yet, the DPM neuron is not present at the larval stage [70] and the CSD neuron only innervates both AL and the lateral protocerebrum but misses the adult specific innervation of the CA and lateral horn [103].

Instead, 5-HT2A-Gal4 line expression nearly exclusively innervates the SOG (Fig 1). Most of the innervation comes from cells having their somata outside the CNS. Unfortunately, due to technical limitations we were not able to clearly localize them. However, TRH-Gal4 has a pronounced expression within the SOG (Fig 5 [48]). Thus, although we favor the hypothesis that 5-HT/5-HT2A receptor signaling, which is necessary for salt reinforcement can be attributed to the SOG, future work is needed for validation. In addition, we cannot exclude that the 5-HT2A receptor Gal4 expression pattern may be misleading, given that we were able to show that acute blockage of 5-HT/5-HT2A receptor neurotransmission does not impair odor-salt learning and memory (Fig 6). Thus, neuromodulatory or developmental processes may underlie 5-HT/5-HT2A receptor function. Indeed, 5-HT2A receptor gene expression pattern markedly changes over larval development [21]. Therefore, it is possible that the impairment of odor-salt learning and memory is based on cellular functions, which are no longer included in the expression pattern of third instar larvae that we used for our analysis. This renders a localization of the cellular effects rather difficult.

5-HT and 5-HT2A receptor signaling regulate developmental processes

Before adopting their roles as neurotransmitters in the mature CNS, neuroactive substances function in the establishment of neural networks [104]. This is also true for 5-HT that can serve as a neurotransmitter and a neuromodulator in all animal phyla studied (reviewed in [105]). In vertebrates it was shown that 5-HT modulates different developmental events, including neuronal migration, cell differentiation, and synaptogenesis (reviewed in [106]). In invertebrates 5-HT regulates—among other processes—cell division in mollusca and the development of the AL during metamorphosis in moths [107, 108]. For Drosophila larvae it was shown that dopa decarboxylase mutants that are devoid of 5-HT and dopamine increase the extent of branching of 5-HT projections to the proventriculus and midgut, similar to larvae in which neuronal 5-HT synthesis was constitutively knocked-down [76, 109]. In addition, constitutively knocked-down 5-HT synthesis showed an increased number and size of varicosities in 5-HT fiber projections to the proventriculus [76]. This kind of autoregulation for the organization of 5-HT varicosities was also described in the larval CNS in 5-HT neurons of the A7 segment of the abdominal ganglion [76, 110].

Developmental functions of 5-HT signaling are not limited to larval stages but also include embryonic development. High levels of 5-HT2A receptor expression occur already at 3 hours of embryonic development and match with the seven-stripe pattern of the pair-rule gene Fushi tarazu [16]. 5-HT signaling through the 5-HT2A receptor triggers changes in cell adhesiveness that are necessary for normal germband extension during gastrulation [111, 112].

In summary, there is good evidence that 5-HT/5-HT2A receptor signaling serves several developmental functions, including the regulation of neuronal connectivity in addition to their classic role in synaptic transmission. Future work needs to address how this might affect unconditioned stimulus processing using high salt concentration. In the longer run this may uncover 5-HT dependent organizational principles of reinforcement processing shared with adult Drosophila and other insects.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Ablation of potential 5-HT receptor cells does not alter olfactory preferences towards four different odors.

5-HT1A-, 5-HT1B-, 5-HT7-, and 5-HT2A-Gal4 lines were crossed with UAS-hid,rpr to genetically induce apoptosis in potential 5-HT receptor cells. In addition, Gal4 lines and UAS-hid,rpr were crossed with w1118 to obtain heterozygous genetic control larvae. (A) provides a color scheme for the three different groups used in each experiment. Naïve olfactory preferences for amyl acetate (AM, in B, C, D, E), benzaldehyde (BA, in F, G, H, I), heptanol (HEP, in J, K, L, M), and nonanol (NON, in N, O, P, Q) were analyzed. In none of the cases experimental larvae behaved significantly different from both genetic control groups. We thus reason that ablation of potential 5-HT receptor cells does not impair the ability of the larvae to detect olfactory cues. The sample size (n = 11–16) is indicated under each box plot. Differences against random distribution are given at the top of each box plot. Differences between all three groups or individual groups are shown at the bottom of the panel. *** (p < 0.001), ** (p < 0.01), * (p < 0.05), n.s. (not significant p ≥ 0.05).

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Ablation of potential 5-HT receptor cells does not alter gustatory preferences towards four different tastants.

5-HT1A-, 5-HT1B-, 5-HT7-, and 5-HT2A-Gal4 lines were crossed with UAS-hid,rpr to genetically induce apoptosis in potential 5-HT receptor cells. In addition, Gal4 lines and UAS-hid,rpr were crossed with w1118 to obtain heterozygous genetic control larvae. (A) provides a color scheme for the three different groups used in each experiment. Naïve gustatory preferences for sodium chloride (SALT, in B, C, D, E), fructose (FRU, in F, G, H, I), arabinose (ARA, in J, K, L, M), and sorbitol (SOR, in N, O, P, Q) were analyzed. In none of the cases experimental larvae behaved significantly different to both genetic control groups. We thus reason that ablation of potential 5-HT receptor cells does not impair the ability of the larvae to detect gustatory stimuli. Sample size (n = 13–20) is indicated under each box plot. Differences against random distribution are given at the top of each panel. Differences between all three groups or individual groups are shown at the bottom of the panel, except for SALT, where it is placed above the box plots. *** (p < 0.001), ** (p < 0.01), * (p < 0.05), n.s. (not significant p ≥ 0.05).

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Ablation of potential 5-HT2A receptor cells does not alter gustatory preferences towards 1.5 M sodium chloride.

5-HT2A-Gal4 was crossed with UAS-hid,rpr to genetically induce apoptosis in potential 5-HT2A receptor cells. In addition, the Gal4 line and UAS-hid,rpr were crossed with w1118 to obtain heterozygous genetic control larvae. Naïve gustatory preferences for 1.5 M sodium chloride (SALT) was analyzed. Experimental larvae behaved at the same level as both genetic control groups. We thus reason that ablation of potential 5-HT2A receptor cells does not impair the ability of the larvae to detect 1.5 M sodium chloride. Sample size (n = 15) is indicated under each box plot. Differences against random distribution are given at the top of each panel. Differences between all three groups or individual groups are shown above the box plots. n.s. indicates that the initial KWT did not suggest for a difference between the three groups (p > 0.05). *** (p < 0.001), ** (p < 0.01), n.s. (not significant p ≥ 0.05).

(TIF)

S4 Fig. Ablation of potential 5-HT2A receptor cells throughout development impairs aversive olfactory learning and memory.

5-HT2A-Gal4 was crossed with UAS-hid,rpr to genetically induce apoptosis in potential 5-HT2A receptor cells. In addition, the Gal4 line and UAS-hid,rpr were crossed with w1118 to obtain heterozygous genetic control larvae. (A) provides a color scheme for the three different groups used in each experiment. (B) Appetitive olfactory learning and memory using fructose reinforcement is shown at the top. (C) Aversive olfactory learning and memory is shown at the bottom. (B) For appetitive olfactory learning experimental larvae and genetic control groups behaved similar. Yet, ablation of 5-HT2A-Gal4 positive cells throughout development specifically impaired aversive olfactory learning and memory (C). Sample size (n = 17–20) is indicated at the bottom of each box plot. Differences against zero are given at the top of each box plot. Differences between all three groups or individual groups are shown at the bottom of the panel. Visualization of statistical evaluations: if only n.s. is shown the initial KWT did not suggest for a difference between the three groups (p ≥ 0.05). When differences between each group are shown this provides the results of the BWRT as the initial KWT suggested for significance (p > 0.05). *** (p < 0.001), ** (p < 0.01), * (p < 0.05), n.s. (not significant p ≥ 0.05).

(TIF)

S1 Table. Raw data behavior.

(XLSX)

S1 File. Supplemental material and methods.

(PDF)

S2 File. Movie 1: 5-HT1A-GAL4 expression.

(AVI)

S3 File. Movie 2: 5-HT1A-GAL4 expression (GFP).

(AVI)

S4 File. Movie 3: 5-HT1B-GAL4 expression.

(AVI)

S5 File. Movie 4: 5-HT1B-GAL4 expression (GFP).

(AVI)

S6 File. Movie 5: 5-HT7-GAL4 expression.

(AVI)

S7 File. Movie 6: 5-HT7-GAL4 expression (GFP).

(AVI)

S8 File. Movie 7: 5-HT2A-GAL4 expression.

(AVI)

S9 File. Movie 8: 5-HT2A-GAL4 expression (dsRed).

(AVI)

Acknowledgments

We thank Margarete Ehrenfried for fly care and maintenance. We thank Tilman Triphan, Wolf Hütteroth, Dennis Pauls, and William Collins for comments on the manuscript. In addition, we thank Annekathrin Widmann, Anna Rist, and Astrid Rohwedder for technical advises. The anatomical analyses were supported by the Bioimaging Facility of the University of Konstanz. This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (TH1584/ 1–1 and TH1584/3-1 to A.S.T), the Swiss National Science Foundation (31003A132812/1 to A.S.T.), the Baden Württemberg Stiftung (to A.S.T.), the Bioimaging Center and Zukunftkolleg of the University of Konstanz (to A.S.T.). A.H. is supported by the International Max Planck Research School for Organismal Biology.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (TH1584/ 1-1 and TH1584/3-1 to A.S.T), the Swiss National Science Foundation (31003A132812/1 to A.S.T.), the Baden Württemberg Stiftung (to A.S.T.), the Bioimaging Center and Zukunftkolleg of the University of Konstanz (to A.S.T.). A.H. is supported by the International Max Planck Research School for Organismal Biology. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Monastirioti M. Biogenic amine systems in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. Microscopy research and technique. 1999;45(2):106–21. Epub 1999/05/20. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Zhang X, Gaudry Q. Functional integration of a serotonergic neuron in the Drosophila antennal lobe. eLife. 2016;5 Epub 2016/08/31. 10.7554/eLife.16836 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Dacks AM, Green DS, Root CM, Nighorn AJ, Wang JW. Serotonin modulates olfactory processing in the antennal lobe of Drosophila. Journal of neurogenetics. 2009;23(4):366–77. 10.3109/01677060903085722 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Alekseyenko OV, Lee C, Kravitz EA. Targeted manipulation of serotonergic neurotransmission affects the escalation of aggression in adult male Drosophila melanogaster. PloS one. 2010;5(5):e10806 Epub 2010/06/04. 10.1371/journal.pone.0010806 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Alekseyenko OV, Chan YB, Fernandez MP, Bulow T, Pankratz MJ, Kravitz EA. Single serotonergic neurons that modulate aggression in Drosophila. Current biology: CB. 2014;24(22):2700–7. 10.1016/j.cub.2014.09.051 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Pooryasin A, Fiala A. Identified Serotonin-Releasing Neurons Induce Behavioral Quiescence and Suppress Mating in Drosophila. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2015;35(37):12792–812. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1638-15.2015 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Neckameyer WS, Coleman CM, Eadie S, Goodwin SF. Compartmentalization of neuronal and peripheral serotonin synthesis in Drosophila melanogaster. Genes, brain, and behavior. 2007;6(8):756–69. Epub 2007/03/23. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00307.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ro J, Pak G, Malec PA, Lyu Y, Allison DB, Kennedy RT, et al. Serotonin signaling mediates protein valuation and aging. eLife. 2016;5 Epub 2016/08/31. 10.7554/eLife.16843 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kamyshev NG, Smirnova GP, Savvateeva EV, Medvedeva AV, Ponomarenko VV. The influence of serotonin and p-chlorophenylalanine on locomotor activity of Drosophila melanogaster. Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior. 1983;18(5):677–81. Epub 1983/05/01. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Tierney AJ. Structure and function of invertebrate 5-HT receptors: a review. Comparative biochemistry and physiology Part A, Molecular & integrative physiology. 2001;128(4):791–804. Epub 2001/04/03. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Barnes NM, Sharp T. A review of central 5-HT receptors and their function. Neuropharmacology. 1999;38(8):1083–152. 10.1016/S0028-3908(99)00010-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hoyer D, Hannon JP, Martin GR. Molecular, pharmacological and functional diversity of 5-HT receptors. Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior. 2002;71(4):533–54. Epub 2002/03/13. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Gasque G, Conway S, Huang J, Rao Y, Vosshall LB. Small molecule drug screening in Drosophila identifies the 5HT2A receptor as a feeding modulation target. Scientific reports. 2013;3:srep02120 Epub 2013/07/03. 10.1038/srep02120 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Witz P, Amlaiky N, Plassat JL, Maroteaux L, Borrelli E, Hen R. Cloning and characterization of a Drosophila serotonin receptor that activates adenylate cyclase. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1990;87(22):8940–4. Epub 1990/11/01. ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Saudou F, Boschert U, Amlaiky N, Plassat JL, Hen R. A family of Drosophila serotonin receptors with distinct intracellular signalling properties and expression patterns. The EMBO journal. 1992;11(1):7–17. Epub 1992/01/01. ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Colas JF, Launay JM, Kellermann O, Rosay P, Maroteaux L. Drosophila 5-HT2 serotonin receptor: coexpression with fushi-tarazu during segmentation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1995;92(12):5441–5. Epub 1995/06/06. ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hauser F, Cazzamali G, Williamson M, Blenau W, Grimmelikhuijzen CJ. A review of neurohormone GPCRs present in the fruitfly Drosophila melanogaster and the honey bee Apis mellifera. Progress in neurobiology. 2006;80(1):1–19. Epub 2006/10/31. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2006.07.005 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Blenau W, Thamm M. Distribution of serotonin (5-HT) and its receptors in the insect brain with focus on the mushroom bodies: lessons from Drosophila melanogaster and Apis mellifera. Arthropod structure & development. 2011;40(5):381–94. Epub 2011/01/29. 10.1016/j.asd.2011.01.004 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Yuan Q, Lin F, Zheng X, Sehgal A. Serotonin modulates circadian entrainment in Drosophila. Neuron. 2005;47(1):115–27. Epub 2005/07/06. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.027 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Yuan Q, Joiner WJ, Sehgal A. A sleep-promoting role for the Drosophila serotonin receptor 1A. Current biology: CB. 2006;16(11):1051–62. Epub 2006/06/07. 10.1016/j.cub.2006.04.032 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Nichols CD. 5-HT2 receptors in Drosophila are expressed in the brain and modulate aspects of circadian behaviors. Developmental neurobiology. 2007;67(6):752–63. 10.1002/dneu.20370 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Kahsai L, Carlsson MA, Winther AM, Nassel DR. Distribution of metabotropic receptors of serotonin, dopamine, GABA, glutamate, and short neuropeptide F in the central complex of Drosophila. Neuroscience. 2012;208:11–26. Epub 2012/03/01. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.02.007 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Becnel J, Johnson O, Luo J, Nassel DR, Nichols CD. The serotonin 5-HT7Dro receptor is expressed in the brain of Drosophila, and is essential for normal courtship and mating. PloS one. 2011;6(6):e20800 Epub 2011/06/16. 10.1371/journal.pone.0020800 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Sizemore TR, Dacks AM. Serotonergic Modulation Differentially Targets Distinct Network Elements within the Antennal Lobe of Drosophila melanogaster. Scientific reports. 2016;6:37119 Epub 2016/11/16. 10.1038/srep37119 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Johnson O, Becnel J, Nichols CD. Serotonin 5-HT(2) and 5-HT(1A)-like receptors differentially modulate aggressive behaviors in Drosophila melanogaster. Neuroscience. 2009;158(4):1292–300. Epub 2008/12/02. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.10.055 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Liu Y, Luo J, Carlsson MA, Nassel DR. Serotonin and insulin-like peptides modulate leucokinin-producing neurons that affect feeding and water homeostasis in Drosophila. The Journal of comparative neurology. 2015;523(12):1840–63. Epub 2015/03/04. 10.1002/cne.23768 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Johnson O, Becnel J, Nichols CD. Serotonin receptor activity is necessary for olfactory learning and memory in Drosophila melanogaster. Neuroscience. 2011;192:372–81. Epub 2011/07/14. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.06.058 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lee PT, Lin HW, Chang YH, Fu TF, Dubnau J, Hirsh J, et al. Serotonin-mushroom body circuit modulating the formation of anesthesia-resistant memory in Drosophila. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108(33):13794–9. Epub 2011/08/03. 10.1073/pnas.1019483108 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Luo J, Becnel J, Nichols CD, Nassel DR. Insulin-producing cells in the brain of adult Drosophila are regulated by the serotonin 5-HT1A receptor. Cellular and molecular life sciences: CMLS. 2012;69(3):471–84. Epub 2011/08/06. 10.1007/s00018-011-0789-0 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Majeed ZR, Stacy A, Cooper RL. Pharmacological and genetic identification of serotonin receptor subtypes on Drosophila larval heart and aorta. Journal of comparative physiology B, Biochemical, systemic, and environmental physiology. 2014;184(2):205–19. Epub 2013/12/29. 10.1007/s00360-013-0795-7 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Majeed ZR, Abdeljaber E, Soveland R, Cornwell K, Bankemper A, Koch F, et al. Modulatory Action by the Serotonergic System: Behavior and Neurophysiology in Drosophila melanogaster. Neural plasticity. 2016;2016:7291438 Epub 2016/03/19. 10.1155/2016/7291438 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Silva B, Goles NI, Varas R, Campusano JM. Serotonin receptors expressed in Drosophila mushroom bodies differentially modulate larval locomotion. PloS one. 2014;9(2):e89641 Epub 2014/03/04. 10.1371/journal.pone.0089641 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Rodriguez Moncalvo VG, Campos AR. Role of serotonergic neurons in the Drosophila larval response to light. BMC neuroscience. 2009;10:66 Epub 2009/06/25. 10.1186/1471-2202-10-66 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Shimada-Niwa Y, Niwa R. Serotonergic neurons respond to nutrients and regulate the timing of steroid hormone biosynthesis in Drosophila. Nature communications. 2014;5:5778 Epub 2014/12/17. 10.1038/ncomms6778 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Duffy JB. GAL4 system in Drosophila: a fly geneticist's Swiss army knife. Genesis (New York, NY: 2000). 2002;34(1–2):1–15. Epub 2002/09/27. 10.1002/gene.10150 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Brand AH, Perrimon N. Targeted gene expression as a means of altering cell fates and generating dominant phenotypes. Development (Cambridge, England). 1993;118(2):401–15. Epub 1993/06/01. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Venken KJ, Simpson JH, Bellen HJ. Genetic manipulation of genes and cells in the nervous system of the fruit fly. Neuron. 2011;72(2):202–30. Epub 2011/10/25. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.021 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gerber B, Stocker RF. The Drosophila larva as a model for studying chemosensation and chemosensory learning: a review. Chem Senses. 2007;32(1):65–89. 10.1093/chemse/bjl030 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Apostolopoulou AA, Widmann A, Rohwedder A, Pfitzenmaier JE, Thum AS. Appetitive associative olfactory learning in Drosophila larvae. Journal of visualized experiments: JoVE. 2013;(72). 10.3791/4334 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Gerber B, Stocker RF, Tanimura T, Thum AS. Smelling, tasting, learning: Drosophila as a study case. Results and problems in cell differentiation. 2009;47:139–85. 10.1007/400_2008_9 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Schleyer M, Saumweber T, Nahrendorf W, Fischer B, von Alpen D, Pauls D, et al. A behavior-based circuit model of how outcome expectations organize learned behavior in larval Drosophila. Learn Mem. 2011;18(10):639–53. 10.1101/lm.2163411 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Scherer S, Stocker RF, Gerber B. Olfactory learning in individually assayed Drosophila larvae. Learn Mem. 2003;10(3):217–25. 10.1101/lm.57903 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Gerber B, Hendel T. Outcome expectations drive learned behaviour in larval Drosophila. Proceedings Biological sciences / The Royal Society. 2006;273(1604):2965–8. 10.1098/rspb.2006.3673 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Aceves-Pina EO, Quinn WG. Learning in normal and mutant Drosophila larvae. Science (New York, NY). 1979;206(4414):93–6. 10.1126/science.206.4414.93 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Rohwedder A, Pfitzenmaier JE, Ramsperger N, Apostolopoulou AA, Widmann A, Thum AS. Nutritional value-dependent and nutritional value-independent effects on Drosophila melanogaster larval behavior. Chem Senses. 2012;37(8):711–21. 10.1093/chemse/bjs055 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Honjo K, Furukubo-Tokunaga K. Induction of cAMP response element-binding protein-dependent medium-term memory by appetitive gustatory reinforcement in Drosophila larvae. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2005;25(35):7905–13. Epub 2005/09/02. 10.1523/jneurosci.2135-05.2005 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Honjo K, Furukubo-Tokunaga K. Distinctive neuronal networks and biochemical pathways for appetitive and aversive memory in Drosophila larvae. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2009;29(3):852–62. Epub 2009/01/23. 10.1523/jneurosci.1315-08.2009 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Huser A, Rohwedder A, Apostolopoulou AA, Widmann A, Pfitzenmaier JE, Maiolo EM, et al. The serotonergic central nervous system of the Drosophila larva: anatomy and behavioral function. PloS one. 2012;7(10):e47518 Epub 2012/10/20. 10.1371/journal.pone.0047518 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Bellen HJ, Levis RW, Liao G, He Y, Carlson JW, Tsang G, et al. The BDGP gene disruption project: single transposon insertions associated with 40% of Drosophila genes. Genetics. 2004;167(2):761–81. Epub 2004/07/09. 10.1534/genetics.104.026427 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Hacker U, Nystedt S, Barmchi MP, Horn C, Wimmer EA. piggyBac-based insertional mutagenesis in the presence of stably integrated P elements in Drosophila. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2003;100(13):7720–5. Epub 2003/06/13. 10.1073/pnas.1230526100 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Abbott MK, Lengyel JA. Embryonic head involution and rotation of male terminalia require the Drosophila locus head involution defective. Genetics. 1991;129(3):783–9. ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.White K, Tahaoglu E, Steller H. Cell killing by the Drosophila gene reaper. Science (New York, NY). 1996;271(5250):805–7. Epub 1996/02/09. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Kitamoto T. Conditional modification of behavior in Drosophila by targeted expression of a temperature-sensitive shibire allele in defined neurons. Journal of neurobiology. 2001;47(2):81–92. Epub 2001/04/06. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Lee T, Luo L. Mosaic analysis with a repressible cell marker for studies of gene function in neuronal morphogenesis. Neuron. 1999;22(3):451–61. Epub 1999/04/10. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Chen Y, Akin O, Nern A, Tsui CY, Pecot MY, Zipursky SL. Cell-type-specific labeling of synapses in vivo through synaptic tagging with recombination. Neuron. 2014;81(2):280–93. Epub 2014/01/28. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.021 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Pfeiffer BD, Jenett A, Hammonds AS, Ngo TT, Misra S, Murphy C, et al. Tools for neuroanatomy and neurogenetics in Drosophila. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105(28):9715–20. Epub 2008/07/16. 10.1073/pnas.0803697105 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Selcho M, Pauls D, Han KA, Stocker RF, Thum AS. The role of dopamine in Drosophila larval classical olfactory conditioning. Plos One. 2009;4(6):e5897 10.1371/journal.pone.0005897 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Selcho M, Pauls D, Huser A, Stocker RF, Thum AS. Characterization of the octopaminergic and tyraminergic neurons in the central brain of Drosophila larvae. The Journal of comparative neurology. 2014. Epub 2014/04/23. 10.1002/cne.23616 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Schipanski A, Yarali A, Niewalda T, Gerber B. Behavioral analyses of sugar processing in choice, feeding, and learning in larval Drosophila. Chemical senses. 2008;33(6):563–73. Epub 2008/05/31. 10.1093/chemse/bjn024 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Niewalda T, Singhal N, Fiala A, Saumweber T, Wegener S, Gerber B. Salt processing in larval Drosophila: choice, feeding, and learning shift from appetitive to aversive in a concentration-dependent way. Chem Senses. 2008;33(8):685–92. 10.1093/chemse/bjn037 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Rohwedder A, Selcho M, Chassot B, Thum AS. Neuropeptide F neurons modulate sugar reward during associative olfactory learning of Drosophila larvae. The Journal of comparative neurology. 2015;523(18):2637–64. Epub 2015/08/04. 10.1002/cne.23873 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Rohwedder A, Wenz NL, Stehle B, Huser A, Yamagata N, Zlatic M, et al. Four Individually Identified Paired Dopamine Neurons Signal Reward in Larval Drosophila. Current biology: CB. 2016;26(5):661–9. Epub 2016/02/16. 10.1016/j.cub.2016.01.012 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Nassif C, Noveen A, Hartenstein V. Early development of the Drosophila brain: III. The pattern of neuropile founder tracts during the larval period. J Comp Neurol. 2003;455(4):417–34. 10.1002/cne.10482 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Colomb J, Grillenzoni N, Ramaekers A, Stocker RF. Architecture of the primary taste center of Drosophila melanogaster larvae. J Comp Neurol. 2007;502(5):834–47. 10.1002/cne.21312 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Matz MV, Fradkov AF, Labas YA, Savitsky AP, Zaraisky AG, Markelov ML, et al. Fluorescent proteins from nonbioluminescent Anthozoa species. Nature biotechnology. 1999;17(10):969–73. 10.1038/13657 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kubala MH, Kovtun O, Alexandrov K, Collins BM. Structural and thermodynamic analysis of the GFP:GFP-nanobody complex. Protein science: a publication of the Protein Society. 2010;19(12):2389–401. Epub 2010/10/15. 10.1002/pro.519 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Webb CM, Cameron EM, Sundberg JP. Fluorescence-labeled reporter gene in transgenic mice provides a useful tool for investigating cutaneous innervation. Veterinary pathology. 2012;49(4):727–30. Epub 2011/07/23. 10.1177/0300985811414033 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Horn C, Jaunich B, Wimmer EA. Highly sensitive, fluorescent transformation marker for Drosophila transgenesis. Development genes and evolution. 2000;210(12):623–9. Epub 2001/01/11. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Berghammer AJ, Klingler M, Wimmer EA. A universal marker for transgenic insects. Nature. 1999;402(6760):370–1. Epub 1999/12/10. 10.1038/46463 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Pauls D, Selcho M, Gendre N, Stocker RF, Thum AS. Drosophila larvae establish appetitive olfactory memories via mushroom body neurons of embryonic origin. J Neurosci. 2010;30(32):10655–66. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1281-10.2010 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Okusawa S, Kohsaka H, Nose A. Serotonin and downstream leucokinin neurons modulate larval turning behavior in Drosophila. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2014;34(7):2544–58. Epub 2014/02/14. 10.1523/jneurosci.3500-13.2014 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Hofbauer A, Campos-Ortega JA. Proliferation pattern and early differentiation of the optic lobes in Drosophila melanogaster. Roux's archives of developmental biology. 1990;198(5):264–74. 10.1007/BF00377393 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Schoofs A, Huckesfeld S, Surendran S, Pankratz MJ. Serotonergic pathways in the Drosophila larval enteric nervous system. Journal of insect physiology. 2014;69:118–25. Epub 2014/06/08. 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2014.05.022 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Pineyro G, Blier P. Autoregulation of serotonin neurons: role in antidepressant drug action. Pharmacol Rev. 1999;51(3):533–91. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Chen J, Condron BG. Branch architecture of the fly larval abdominal serotonergic neurons. Developmental biology. 2008;320(1):30–8. Epub 2008/06/20. 10.1016/j.ydbio.2008.03.038 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Neckameyer WS. A trophic role for serotonin in the development of a simple feeding circuit. Developmental neuroscience. 2010;32(3):217–37. Epub 2010/08/18. 10.1159/000304888 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Burke CJ, Huetteroth W, Owald D, Perisse E, Krashes MJ, Das G, et al. Layered reward signalling through octopamine and dopamine in Drosophila. Nature. 2012;492(7429):433–7. Epub 2012/10/30. 10.1038/nature11614 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Sitaraman D, LaFerriere H, Birman S, Zars T. Serotonin is critical for rewarded olfactory short-term memory in Drosophila. Journal of neurogenetics. 2012;26(2):238–44. Epub 2012/03/23. 10.3109/01677063.2012.666298 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Nall A, Sehgal A. Monoamines and sleep in Drosophila. Behavioral neuroscience. 2014;128(3):264–72. Epub 2014/06/03. 10.1037/a0036209 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Blenau W. Cellular actions of biogenic amines. Archives of insect biochemistry and physiology. 2005;59(3):99–102. 10.1002/arch.20072 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Selcho M, Pauls D, El Jundi B, Stocker RF, Thum AS. The role of octopamine and tyramine in Drosophila larval locomotion. J Comp Neurol. 2012;520(16):3764–85. 10.1002/cne.23152 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Blenau W, Baumann A. Molecular and pharmacological properties of insect biogenic amine receptors: lessons from Drosophila melanogaster and Apis mellifera. Archives of insect biochemistry and physiology. 2001;48(1):13–38. Epub 2001/08/24. 10.1002/arch.1055 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Gomez-Marin A, Partoune N, Stephens GJ, Louis M, Brembs B. Automated tracking of animal posture and movement during exploration and sensory orientation behaviors. PloS one. 2012;7(8):e41642 Epub 2012/08/23. 10.1371/journal.pone.0041642 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Risse B, Thomas S, Otto N, Lopmeier T, Valkov D, Jiang X, et al. FIM, a novel FTIR-based imaging method for high throughput locomotion analysis. PloS one. 2013;8(1):e53963 Epub 2013/01/26. 10.1371/journal.pone.0053963 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Berh D, Risse B, Michels T, Otto N, Jiang X, Klambt C. A FIM-based Long-Term in-vial Monitoring System for Drosophila Larvae. IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering. 2016. Epub 2016/11/17. 10.1109/tbme.2016.2628203 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Gershow M, Berck M, Mathew D, Luo L, Kane EA, Carlson JR, et al. Controlling airborne cues to study small animal navigation. Nature methods. 2012;9(3):290–6. Epub 2012/01/17. 10.1038/nmeth.1853 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Ohyama T, Jovanic T, Denisov G, Dang TC, Hoffmann D, Kerr RA, et al. High-throughput analysis of stimulus-evoked behaviors in Drosophila larva reveals multiple modality-specific escape strategies. Plos One. 2013;8(8):e71706 10.1371/journal.pone.0071706 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Schoofs A, Huckesfeld S, Schlegel P, Miroschnikow A, Peters M, Zeymer M, et al. Selection of motor programs for suppressing food intake and inducing locomotion in the Drosophila brain. PLoS biology. 2014;12(6):e1001893 Epub 2014/06/25. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001893 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Apostolopoulou AA, Mazija L, Wust A, Thum AS. The neuronal and molecular basis of quinine-dependent bitter taste signaling in Drosophila larvae. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience. 2014;8:6 Epub 2014/01/31. 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00006 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Singh RN, Singh K. Fine structure of the sensory organs of Drosophila melanogaster Meigen larva (Diptera: Drosophilidae). International Journal of Insect Morphology and Embryology. 1984;13(4):255–73. 10.1016/0020-7322(84)90001-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Python F, Stocker RF. Adult-like complexity of the larval antennal lobe of D. melanogaster despite markedly low numbers of odorant receptor neurons. The Journal of comparative neurology. 2002;445(4):374–87. Epub 2002/03/29. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Fishilevich E, Domingos AI, Asahina K, Naef F, Vosshall LB, Louis M. Chemotaxis behavior mediated by single larval olfactory neurons in Drosophila. Current biology: CB. 2005;15(23):2086–96. Epub 2005/12/08. 10.1016/j.cub.2005.11.016 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Berck ME, Khandelwal A, Claus L, Hernandez-Nunez L, Si G, Tabone CJ, et al. The wiring diagram of a glomerular olfactory system. eLife. 2016;5 10.7554/eLife.14859 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Kreher SA, Kwon JY, Carlson JR. The molecular basis of odor coding in the Drosophila larva. Neuron. 2005;46(3):445–56. Epub 2005/05/11. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.007 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Oppliger FY, P MG, Vlimant M. Neurophysiological and behavioural evidence for an olfactory function for the dorsal organ and a gustatory one for the terminal organ in Drosophila melanogaster larvae. Journal of insect physiology. 2000;46(2):135–44. Epub 2003/05/29. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Ramaekers A, Magnenat E, Marin EC, Gendre N, Jefferis GS, Luo L, et al. Glomerular maps without cellular redundancy at successive levels of the Drosophila larval olfactory circuit. Current biology: CB. 2005;15(11):982–92. Epub 2005/06/07. 10.1016/j.cub.2005.04.032 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Masuda-Nakagawa LM, Gendre N, O'Kane CJ, Stocker RF. Localized olfactory representation in mushroom bodies of Drosophila larvae. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106(25):10314–9. 10.1073/pnas.0900178106 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Thum AS, Leisibach B, Gendre N, Selcho M, Stocker RF. Diversity, variability, and suboesophageal connectivity of antennal lobe neurons in D. melanogaster larvae. J Comp Neurol. 2011;519(17):3415–32. 10.1002/cne.22713 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Masuda-Nakagawa LM, Awasaki T, Ito K, O'Kane CJ. Targeting expression to projection neurons that innervate specific mushroom body calyx and antennal lobe glomeruli in larval Drosophila. Gene Expr Patterns. 2010;10(7–8):328–37. 10.1016/j.gep.2010.07.004 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Widmann A, Artinger M, Biesinger L, Boepple K, Peters C, Schlechter J, et al. Genetic Dissection of Aversive Associative Olfactory Learning and Memory in Drosophila Larvae. PLoS genetics. 2016;12(10):e1006378 Epub 2016/10/22. 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006378 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Schroll C, Riemensperger T, Bucher D, Ehmer J, Voller T, Erbguth K, et al. Light-induced activation of distinct modulatory neurons triggers appetitive or aversive learning in Drosophila larvae. Current biology: CB. 2006;16(17):1741–7. 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.023 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Waddell S, Armstrong JD, Kitamoto T, Kaiser K, Quinn WG. The amnesiac gene product is expressed in two neurons in the Drosophila brain that are critical for memory. Cell. 2000;103(5):805–13. Epub 2000/12/15. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Roy B, Singh AP, Shetty C, Chaudhary V, North A, Landgraf M, et al. Metamorphosis of an identified serotonergic neuron in the Drosophila olfactory system. Neural development. 2007;2:20 Epub 2007/10/26. 10.1186/1749-8104-2-20 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Herlenius E, Lagercrantz H. Neurotransmitters and neuromodulators during early human development. Early Human Development. 2001;65(1):21–37. 10.1016/S0378-3782(01)00189-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Buznikov GA, Lambert HW, Lauder JM. Serotonin and serotonin-like substances as regulators of early embryogenesis and morphogenesis. Cell and tissue research. 2001;305(2):177–86. Epub 2001/09/08. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Gaspar P, Cases O, Maroteaux L. The developmental role of serotonin: news from mouse molecular genetics. Nature reviews Neuroscience. 2003;4(12):1002–12. Epub 2003/11/18. 10.1038/nrn1256 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Glebov K, Voronezhskaya EE, Khabarova MY, Ivashkin E, Nezlin LP, Ponimaskin EG. Mechanisms underlying dual effects of serotonin during development of Helisoma trivolvis (Mollusca). BMC developmental biology. 2014;14:14 Epub 2014/03/15. 10.1186/1471-213X-14-14 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Oland LA, Kirschenbaum SR, Pott WM, Mercer AR, Tolbert LP. Development of an identified serotonergic neuron in the antennal lobe of the moth and effects of reduction in serotonin during construction of olfactory glomeruli. Journal of neurobiology. 1995;28(2):248–67. Epub 1995/10/01. 10.1002/neu.480280210 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Budnik V, Wu CF, White K. Altered branching of serotonin-containing neurons in Drosophila mutants unable to synthesize serotonin and dopamine. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 1989;9(8):2866–77. Epub 1989/08/01. . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Sykes PA, Condron BG. Development and sensitivity to serotonin of Drosophila serotonergic varicosities in the central nervous system. Developmental biology. 2005;286(1):207–16. Epub 2005/08/27. 10.1016/j.ydbio.2005.07.025 ; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Colas JF, Launay JM, Maroteaux L. Maternal and zygotic control of serotonin biosynthesis are both necessary for Drosophila germband extension. Mechanisms of development. 1999;87(1–2):67–76. Epub 1999/09/24. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Colas JF, Launay JM, Vonesch JL, Hickel P, Maroteaux L. Serotonin synchronises convergent extension of ectoderm with morphogenetic gastrulation movements in Drosophila. Mechanisms of development. 1999;87(1–2):77–91. Epub 1999/09/24. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Fig. Ablation of potential 5-HT receptor cells does not alter olfactory preferences towards four different odors.

5-HT1A-, 5-HT1B-, 5-HT7-, and 5-HT2A-Gal4 lines were crossed with UAS-hid,rpr to genetically induce apoptosis in potential 5-HT receptor cells. In addition, Gal4 lines and UAS-hid,rpr were crossed with w1118 to obtain heterozygous genetic control larvae. (A) provides a color scheme for the three different groups used in each experiment. Naïve olfactory preferences for amyl acetate (AM, in B, C, D, E), benzaldehyde (BA, in F, G, H, I), heptanol (HEP, in J, K, L, M), and nonanol (NON, in N, O, P, Q) were analyzed. In none of the cases experimental larvae behaved significantly different from both genetic control groups. We thus reason that ablation of potential 5-HT receptor cells does not impair the ability of the larvae to detect olfactory cues. The sample size (n = 11–16) is indicated under each box plot. Differences against random distribution are given at the top of each box plot. Differences between all three groups or individual groups are shown at the bottom of the panel. *** (p < 0.001), ** (p < 0.01), * (p < 0.05), n.s. (not significant p ≥ 0.05).

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Ablation of potential 5-HT receptor cells does not alter gustatory preferences towards four different tastants.

5-HT1A-, 5-HT1B-, 5-HT7-, and 5-HT2A-Gal4 lines were crossed with UAS-hid,rpr to genetically induce apoptosis in potential 5-HT receptor cells. In addition, Gal4 lines and UAS-hid,rpr were crossed with w1118 to obtain heterozygous genetic control larvae. (A) provides a color scheme for the three different groups used in each experiment. Naïve gustatory preferences for sodium chloride (SALT, in B, C, D, E), fructose (FRU, in F, G, H, I), arabinose (ARA, in J, K, L, M), and sorbitol (SOR, in N, O, P, Q) were analyzed. In none of the cases experimental larvae behaved significantly different to both genetic control groups. We thus reason that ablation of potential 5-HT receptor cells does not impair the ability of the larvae to detect gustatory stimuli. Sample size (n = 13–20) is indicated under each box plot. Differences against random distribution are given at the top of each panel. Differences between all three groups or individual groups are shown at the bottom of the panel, except for SALT, where it is placed above the box plots. *** (p < 0.001), ** (p < 0.01), * (p < 0.05), n.s. (not significant p ≥ 0.05).

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Ablation of potential 5-HT2A receptor cells does not alter gustatory preferences towards 1.5 M sodium chloride.

5-HT2A-Gal4 was crossed with UAS-hid,rpr to genetically induce apoptosis in potential 5-HT2A receptor cells. In addition, the Gal4 line and UAS-hid,rpr were crossed with w1118 to obtain heterozygous genetic control larvae. Naïve gustatory preferences for 1.5 M sodium chloride (SALT) was analyzed. Experimental larvae behaved at the same level as both genetic control groups. We thus reason that ablation of potential 5-HT2A receptor cells does not impair the ability of the larvae to detect 1.5 M sodium chloride. Sample size (n = 15) is indicated under each box plot. Differences against random distribution are given at the top of each panel. Differences between all three groups or individual groups are shown above the box plots. n.s. indicates that the initial KWT did not suggest for a difference between the three groups (p > 0.05). *** (p < 0.001), ** (p < 0.01), n.s. (not significant p ≥ 0.05).

(TIF)

S4 Fig. Ablation of potential 5-HT2A receptor cells throughout development impairs aversive olfactory learning and memory.

5-HT2A-Gal4 was crossed with UAS-hid,rpr to genetically induce apoptosis in potential 5-HT2A receptor cells. In addition, the Gal4 line and UAS-hid,rpr were crossed with w1118 to obtain heterozygous genetic control larvae. (A) provides a color scheme for the three different groups used in each experiment. (B) Appetitive olfactory learning and memory using fructose reinforcement is shown at the top. (C) Aversive olfactory learning and memory is shown at the bottom. (B) For appetitive olfactory learning experimental larvae and genetic control groups behaved similar. Yet, ablation of 5-HT2A-Gal4 positive cells throughout development specifically impaired aversive olfactory learning and memory (C). Sample size (n = 17–20) is indicated at the bottom of each box plot. Differences against zero are given at the top of each box plot. Differences between all three groups or individual groups are shown at the bottom of the panel. Visualization of statistical evaluations: if only n.s. is shown the initial KWT did not suggest for a difference between the three groups (p ≥ 0.05). When differences between each group are shown this provides the results of the BWRT as the initial KWT suggested for significance (p > 0.05). *** (p < 0.001), ** (p < 0.01), * (p < 0.05), n.s. (not significant p ≥ 0.05).

(TIF)

S1 Table. Raw data behavior.

(XLSX)

S1 File. Supplemental material and methods.

(PDF)

S2 File. Movie 1: 5-HT1A-GAL4 expression.

(AVI)

S3 File. Movie 2: 5-HT1A-GAL4 expression (GFP).

(AVI)

S4 File. Movie 3: 5-HT1B-GAL4 expression.

(AVI)

S5 File. Movie 4: 5-HT1B-GAL4 expression (GFP).

(AVI)

S6 File. Movie 5: 5-HT7-GAL4 expression.

(AVI)

S7 File. Movie 6: 5-HT7-GAL4 expression (GFP).

(AVI)

S8 File. Movie 7: 5-HT2A-GAL4 expression.

(AVI)

S9 File. Movie 8: 5-HT2A-GAL4 expression (dsRed).

(AVI)

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES