Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2017 Mar 13;27(9):3716–3724. doi: 10.1007/s00330-017-4768-1

The effect of hippocampal function, volume and connectivity on posterior cingulate cortex functioning during episodic memory fMRI in mild cognitive impairment

Janne M Papma 1,, Marion Smits 2, Marius de Groot 2,3, Francesco U Mattace Raso 4, Aad van der Lugt 2, Henri A Vrooman 2,3, Wiro J Niessen 2,3,5, Peter J Koudstaal 1, John C van Swieten 1, Frederik M van der Veen 6, Niels D Prins 7
PMCID: PMC5544779  PMID: 28289940

Abstract

Objectives

Diminished function of the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) is a typical finding in early Alzheimer’s disease (AD). It is hypothesized that in early stage AD, PCC functioning relates to or reflects hippocampal dysfunction or atrophy. The aim of this study was to examine the relationship between hippocampus function, volume and structural connectivity, and PCC activation during an episodic memory task-related fMRI study in mild cognitive impairment (MCI).

Method

MCI patients (n = 27) underwent episodic memory task-related fMRI, 3D-T1w MRI, 2D T2-FLAIR MRI and diffusion tensor imaging. Stepwise linear regression analysis was performed to examine the relationship between PCC activation and hippocampal activation, hippocampal volume and diffusion measures within the cingulum along the hippocampus.

Results

We found a significant relationship between PCC and hippocampus activation during successful episodic memory encoding and correct recognition in MCI patients. We found no relationship between the PCC and structural hippocampal predictors.

Conclusions

Our results indicate a relationship between PCC and hippocampus activation during episodic memory engagement in MCI. This may suggest that during episodic memory, functional network deterioration is the most important predictor of PCC functioning in MCI.

Key Points

PCC functioning during episodic memory relates to hippocampal functioning in MCI.

PCC functioning during episodic memory does not relate to hippocampal structure in MCI.

Functional network changes are an important predictor of PCC functioning in MCI.

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s00330-017-4768-1) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Keywords: Mild cognitive impairment, Episodic memory task-related functional MRI, Diffusion tensor imaging, Posterior cingulate cortex, Hippocampus

Introduction

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a clinical construct that identifies individuals with cognitive impairment and a high risk of dementia [13]. Although MCI is a heterogeneous condition, most MCI patients exhibit the amnestic phenotype with episodic memory deficits as the sole or most prominent characteristic [4]. In those patients, Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common clinical endpoint [1, 5], and the amnestic deficits are considered to be the consequence of neuropathological changes affecting the medial temporal lobe (MTL) early in the disease process. While structural and functional MRI studies concerning memory functioning in MCI and AD, initially focused on the MTL or specifically hippocampus [6], current neuroimaging studies examine patterns of deterioration in global functional and structural brain circuits or networks, e.g. the Papez circuit [7, 8], or the more recently described default mode network (DMN) [912]. Within these networks though, the effects of medial temporal lobe degeneration on network functioning, or functioning of specific network nodes, is still unclear [8, 13].

The posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) is an important network node, showing hypometabolism and hypoperfusion in the MCI stage [1419], predictive for further cognitive decline into clinical AD [14, 20, 21]. Given its network connection with the hippocampus, several studies debated whether changes in PCC functioning might reflect deterioration of the hippocampus in early disease stages [13, 20, 2230]. From these studies we can grossly deduce three theories, namely that PCC functioning in MCI might reflect: (1) functional changes in the hippocampus [9, 24, 3133]; (2) structural changes in the hippocampus [13, 14, 2428]; or (3) degeneration of white matter tracts subserving a part of the connection between the hippocampus and PCC [29, 30, 3436]. It is known that the hippocampus has multiple efferent connections, and communication between the hippocampus and PCC also runs through the thalamus [8, 22], or several other nodes, as the functional path length between the hippocampus and PCC is shown to alter in MCI in relationship with cognitive decline [37]. Meanwhile, there are a number of clinical studies that have specifically indicated cingulum disruption to subserve memory problems due to a disconnection between the hippocampus and PCC [34, 3840]. As the PCC is a key hub, and PCC dysfunctioning is shown to be an indicator for AD prodromal stages, examining the relationship between PCC functioning and hippocampus functioning, hippocampus structure and structural connectivity in MCI will provide further insight into the origin of PCC dysfunctioning in MCI. In this multimodal MRI study we performed analyses on the association between PCC functioning during task-related episodic memory fMRI and (1) hippocampus activation (episodic memory fMRI), (2) hippocampus volume (automatic segmentation of hippocampus), and (3) structural connections subserving partly the connection between the hippocampus and PCC (diffusion tensor imaging; DTI), in patients with MCI.

Material and methods

Participants

Thirty-three MCI patients underwent episodic memory task-related fMRI, structural MRI, DTI and extensive neuropsychological assessment. MCI patients were recruited from 2009–2011 on the basis of the criteria of Petersen [4], as criteria for MCI due to AD by Albert et al. [1] were not published at time of recruitment. Exclusion criteria for all participants were: history of neurological or psychiatric disorders negatively affecting cognition (e.g. major stroke, cerebral tumour or depression) and contraindication for MRI (e.g. metal implants, claustrophobia).

All participants gave informed consent to the protocol of the study, which was approved by the medical ethics committee of the Erasmus MC. On the basis of fMRI task performance (as specified below) or movement during scanning, we excluded six MCI patients, and thus eventually included 27 MCI patients in our analyses.

Structured interview

Data on demographics were collected during a structured interview. We assessed level of education by means of a Dutch education scale, with a range from 1 (less than 6 years of elementary school) to 7 (academic degree) [41]. We administered the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) as a global cognitive screening method.

Neuropsychological assessment

All participants underwent an extensive neuropsychological assessment. For every neuropsychological test, we calculated z-scores using the mean and standard deviation from a control group of 15 healthy elderly (mean age 70.6 years, mean MMSE 28.9), and subsequently constructed compound scores for cognitive domains (see Supplementary Material and Methods).

MRI acquisition protocol

We performed functional and structural MR imaging on a 3.0T MRI scanner with an 8-channel head coil (HD platform, GE Healthcare, Milwaukee, WI, US). All participants underwent an MRI protocol including 3D T1w MRI, 2D T2-FLAIR, DTI and fMRI during an episodic memory task. Details on MRI sequence parameters can be found in the Supplementary Material and Methods.

Automated MRI brain tissue segmentation and volumetric analysis of hippocampi

We automatically classified brain tissue on MRI into cerebrospinal fluid, grey matter, normal appearing white matter, white matter hyperintensities (WMH), based on intensities of the 3D T1-weighted and 2D T2-FLAIR MRI scans [4244]. We segmented the hippocampus using the 3D T1-weighted image by means of an automated method as described previously [45, 46]. Blinded for clinical information, we visually inspected the results of all automated segmentations, and if necessary (one case), the automated segmentations were manually corrected using FSLview as part of FSL [45]. An experienced neurologist (NDP) and neuroradiologist (MS) visually evaluated the occurrence and location of thalamic lacunar infarcts on the basis of 2D T2-FLAIR, though we acknowledge the low sensitivity of T2-FLAIR for the detection of thalamic lesions [47].

DTI data processing and probabilistic tractography

We applied an extensive diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) data processing method, in which we used the eigenvalues of the fitted diffusion tensors to provide measures of fractional anisotropy (FA), mean diffusivity (MD), axial diffusivity (AxD) and radial diffusivity (RD). These measures and methods are described in detail in a study by Papma et al. [42]. To rule out the effects of vascular-related macrostructural white matter damage in the DTI data, we restricted DTI analyses to the normal appearing white matter by mapping the individual WMH segmentation masks on the DTI maps and excluding voxels originating from WMH from our analyses, as described previously [42]. We performed automated probabilistic tractography for the cingulum along the hippocampus (CGH). These analyses were performed in subject native space by means of Probtrackx, available in FSL. We used standard space seed, target, stop and exclusion masks, placed in accordance with the protocols described by Wakana et al. [48, 49]. To adopt the region from Wakana et al. for probabilistic tractography, additional masks were included to prevent an over segmentation of the CGH (also see [50]). Seed and target masks are slightly larger than the masks indicated in Wakana, in order to be robust against slight misalignment introduced in the registration step of the tractography. Further information on our approach is publicly available on the FSL website (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/AutoPtx). The masks were transferred to subject-native space using nonlinear registration obtained with default settings for FA images in FNIRT. The tract density image for each tract was normalized by division with the total number of fibre paths recorded in the tract density image. These images were then thresholded at 0.005 to yield binary segmentations.

Functional MRI paradigm

We engaged episodic memory by means of an event-related verbal episodic memory task including separate encoding and recognition runs, based on the fMRI paradigm of Daselaar et al. [51]. Verbal stimuli consisted of emotionally neutral words, visually presented to participants. During encoding, participants were asked to indicate a positive or negative emotion related to the presented stimulus in order to improve consolidation, by means of respectively a right-or left-hand button press. Words presented in the recognition run consisted of words presented during the encoding run, intermixed with new words. Participants were asked to indicate whether they recognized a word or not by means of respectively a right or left-hand button press. Both the encoding and recognition runs included baseline stimuli consisting of the words ‘left’, indicating a left-hand press, and ‘right’, indicating a right-hand press. Stimuli that elicited no reaction were excluded from further analyses. Details on the task design can be found in the Supplementary Material and Methods.

Functional MRI in-scanner task performance

If participants correctly recognized a stimulus, this was considered ‘correct recognition’ in the recognition run, and consequently ‘successful encoding’ in the encoding run. We examined task performance by calculating d prime, a measure indicating the ability to discriminate between target and distractor words in episodic memory tasks [52]. D prime was calculated as (Z-score false alarm) minus (Zscore correct recognition). Lower d prime scores reflect lower sensitivity in discriminating target from distractor items. Participants were excluded from the study if their task performance during recognition was equal to or below chance level, if the number of missed stimuli exceeded the number of correct recognition stimuli, or if the number of false alarm stimuli exceeded the number of correct rejections. This was done in order to be able to attribute functional MRI changes to potential neurodegenerative processes rather than task performance [53].

Functional MRI data analysis

We analysed fMRI data using Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM8; Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK) implemented in Matlab R2013b (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA). Details on data preprocessing in SPM can be found in the Supplementary Material and Methods. For encoding and recognition analyses we used separate design matrices. In the encoding design matrix we included baseline stimuli and successfully encoded stimuli as separate parameters, as well as realignment parameters. In a first level analysis we created individual ‘successful encoding versus baseline’ contrast maps. These contrast maps were entered in a second level analysis where we performed an activation and deactivation within group analysis. In the design matrix for the recognition phase we entered regressors for baseline stimuli and correct recognition. In a first level analysis we created the ‘correct recognition versus baseline’ contrast on an individual level, and subsequently entered the contrast maps in a second level analysis where we performed an activation and deactivation within-group analysis. Significance was tested at p <0.05 with family wise error (FWE) correction for multiple comparisons, as well as at a more lenient threshold of p < 0.001 not corrected for multiple comparisons, and a cluster size of at least 20 voxels. In a post hoc analysis we added a covariate for MMSE, to test to what extent the severity of disease influences results. Anatomical labelling of significantly activated clusters was performed using WFU Pickatlas software extension to SPM8 (Functional MRI laboratory - Wake Forest University School of Medicine, Winston Salem). A priori defined regions of interest (ROIs) of the bilateral PCC, left and right hippocampus, and left and right parahippocampal gyrus were created using AAL masks. Within these ROIs we extracted mean beta values for successful encoding and correct recognition using Marsbar, and subsequently exported these to SPSS (version 21.0 for Windows) for stepwise linear regression analyses.

Statistical analysis

Demographic and neuropsychological data was analysed using SPSS version 21.0 for Windows. To determine whether (1) hippocampus activation, (2) volume or (3) white matter connectivity was associated with PCC activation during successful episodic memory encoding and correct recognition in MCI, we performed a stepwise linear regression analysis with the PCC beta values during successful encoding and correct recognition as dependent variable and the independent variables: beta values of left and right hippocampus (respectively during successful encoding and correct recognition), left and right hippocampal volume in ml, mean FA, MD, AxD and RD of the left and right CGH. Post hoc, we performed the same analysis, but added the left and right parahippocampal gyrus activation to the independent variables. To reduce the risk of overfitting associated with a stepwise regression analysis, we performed a Bonferroni adjustment for the analyses [54].

Results

Participant characteristics, neuropsychological profile and MRI characteristics

Demographic, neuropsychological and MRI characteristics of MCI patients are displayed in Tables 1, 2 and 3. As expected, MCI patients had lowered Z-scores on neuropsychological tests for immediate and delayed memory recall (>2 SD for delayed recall, Table 2), but also performed worse on tests for executive function and language than controls (Table 2). One MCI patient showed a lacunar infarct in the lateral right thalamus.

Table 1.

Demographic characteristics of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) patients

MCI (n = 27)
Age, years 73.9 (4.9)
Sex, women (%) 5 (18.5)
Education 5.6 (1.0)
MMSE 27.5 (2.0)

Values are unadjusted means (standard deviation) or number of participants (percentages)

MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination

Table 2.

Neuropsychological assessment and fMRI performance of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) patients

MCI
(n = 27)
Memory immediate recall −1.39 (0.66)
Memory delayed recall −2.49 (2.15)
Processing speed −0.46 (0.94)
Executive functioning −1.14 (1.85)
Language −0.83 (1.39)
Visuospatial ability 0.04 (1.42)
Visuoconstructive ability −0.17 (1.85)
D prime episodic memory during fMRI 1.69 (0.71)

Neuropsychological results displayed as mean z-scores for cognitive domains (standard deviation), relative to a group of n = 15 controls

fMRI functional MRI

Table 3.

Neuroimaging results

MCI
(n = 27)
Left hippocampal volume in ml 2.8 (0.5)
Right hippocampal volume in ml 2.9 (0.4)
Grey matter volume in ml 457.4 (68.5)
White matter volume in ml 412.8 (52.2)
White matter hyperintensity volume in ml (log)1 3.2 (2.8-3.6)
Left FA CGH 0.36 (0.03)
Left MD CGH2 1.05 (0.09)
Left AxD CGH2 1.47 (0.13)
Left RD CGH2 0.84 (0.08)
Right FA CGH 0.39 (0.03)
Right MD CGH2 1.04 (0.09)
Right AxD CGH2 1.49 (0.11)
Right RD CGH2 0.82 (0.08)
Beta values PCC during successful encoding -1.16 (2.10)
Beta values left hippocampus during successful encoding -0.08 (1.71)
Beta values right hippocampus during successful encoding -0.22 (1.36)
Beta values PCC during correct recognition -0.39 (1.68)
Beta values left hippocampus during correct recognition 0.07 (1.53)
Beta values right hippocampus during correct recognition 0.08 (1.04)

Values are unadjusted means (standard deviation)

1 median (interquartile range)

2 MD, AxD and Rd shown as 10-3 mm2/s. In case of median testing the Mann Whitney U-test was used

MCI mild cognitive impairment, FA fractional anisotropy, MD mean diffusivity, AxD axial diffusivity, RD radial diffusivity, CGH cingulate along the hippocampus, PCC posterior cingulate cortex

Functional MRI results

Within group activation analyses for memory encoding at a threshold of p <0.05 with FWE correction for multiple comparisons, showed left inferior and medial frontal gyrus and left cingulate gyrus activation for MCI patients. The activation pattern was extended to the right hemisphere when using a more lenient threshold of p < 0.001, without correction for multiple comparisons (Fig. 1, Supplementary Results Table 1) and included the bilateral medial and inferior frontal gyrus, the left middle temporal gyrus, the left PCC, the left postcentral gyrus and the bilateral inferior occipital gyrus. Within-group deactivation analyses for memory encoding showed no deactivation at a threshold of p < 0.05 with FWE correction for multiple comparisons, and right postcentral gyrus, bilateral inferior parietal gyrus, right precuneus, right PCC and the right mid cingulate gyrus deactivation at a lenient threshold (Fig. 1, Supplementary Results Table 1). Upon visual inspection, the recognition memory activation pattern was more extensive, with activation in the right inferior frontal gyrus, the left insula, the bilateral middle and right inferior occipital gyrus, the bilateral inferior and right superior parietal gyrus (Fig. 1, Supplementary Results Table 1), however lacking PCC activation (Fig. 1). During recognition we found hippocampal and parahippocampal gyrus activation in MCI patients, in clusters smaller than 20 voxels, displayed in Fig. 2. Within-group deactivation during recognition was found in the middle temporal gyrus (Fig. 1, Supplementary Results Table 1). In a post hoc analysis we added the MMSE as a covariate. Results were similar to the analyses without this covariate (see Supplementary Results Table 2).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Activation and deactivation patterns in successful encoding versus baseline and correct recognition versus baseline, lenient threshold without correction for multiple comparisons. Coronal view in posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) region

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Activation of the hippocampus extending into the parahippocampal gyrus in mild cognitive impairement (MCI) patients during recognition, lenient threshold without multiple comparisons

Stepwise linear regression analyses of PCC

Table 4 summarizes the results for the stepwise regression model. A model with right hippocampal activation was significantly associated with PCC activation during successful encoding, while left hippocampal activation was significantly related to PCC activation during correct recognition (Table 4). We performed a post hoc analysis in which we also included the left and right parahippocampal gyrus activation respectively during encoding and recognition. In this second stepwise regression analysis, the right and left hippocampus remained the most important predictors for PCC activation during respectively encoding and recognition. The p values associated with left and right parahippocampal gyrus volumes in the model of PCC activation during encoding were left parahippocampal gyrus: (T 0.334) p = 0.741, and right parahippocampal gyrus: (T 0.586) p = 0.618; and during recognition left parahippocampal gyrus: (T 0.555) p = 0.584, and right parahippocampal gyrus: (T 1.493) p = 0.149.

Table 4.

Stepwise regression model in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) patients

Dependent variable Predictors Beta value Sig R2
PCC beta values encoding 1. Right hippocampus beta values encoding 0.960 0.0005 0.390
PCC beta values recognition 1. Left hippocampus beta values recognition 0.853 <0.0001 0.607

Significant predictors in a stepwise regression model. Beta values are unstandardized coefficients. Bonferroni corrected threshold of p < 0.0041

PCC posterior cingulate cortex

Discussion

Our results indicate an association between PCC activation and hippocampus activation during successful episodic memory encoding and correct recognition in MCI patients. We found no relationship between structural hippocampal predictors, such as the CGH subserving a part of the connection between the PCC and hippocampus or hippocampal volumes, and PCC activation. While this suggests that episodic memory decline in MCI can best be explained as a functional network disorder, structural and functional connectivity between the PCC and hippocampus is known to be quite complex in MCI and AD [8, 37].

The finding of a relationship between hippocampal and PCC activation in the present study is supported by several functional neuroimaging studies in healthy individuals, MCI or dementia patients [31, 5557]. The absence of a relationship between structural hippocampal measures and PCC activation though, contrasts other studies. In these studies hippocampal volume was related to either perfusion within the PCC [13, 25], fMRI task-induced PCC deactivation [58] or functional connectivity between both structures [27]. One reason for the difference in findings may be methodological, as our study was performed in an episodic memory-related fMRI setting, which enables us to investigate hippocampal and PCC activation while these structures are engaged in episodic memory. From the current clinical literature we can grossly deduce three theories that have been examined in previous studies, concerning the effects of disease-related changes in the hippocampus on PCC functioning. First, a theory concerning a relationship between PCC dysfunctioning and hippocampal grey matter atrophy via the degeneration of white matter bundles that subserve a part of the connections between the hippocampus and PCC [26, 38, 59]. This is supported by several studies showing a relationship between hippocampal volume and PCC metabolism mediated through cingulum bundle disruption [26, 59, 60]. Second, the theory that independent white matter tract degeneration disconnects the hippocampus from other structures, and subsequently affects episodic memory [29, 30, 36, 39, 40, 61]. Third, a theory concerning the deterioration of functional networks, in which brain regions synchronically function less well [12, 55]. As we found a relationship between hippocampal and PCC activation, our results can only support the functional network deterioration theory. However, we acknowledge that the structural and functional connectivity between the PCC and hippocampus subserving episodic memory in MCI is more complex [8, 37].

We found that the most important predictor of PCC activation during successful episodic memory encoding was right hippocampal activation, whereas left hippocampal activation was a significant predictor for PCC functioning during episodic memory recognition. This pattern of hemispheric specialization for different phases of episodic memory has been presented in the literature, with right temporal lobe dysfunction having the greatest effect on acquisition of new learning, and left temporal lobe dysfunction on retention or retrieval [62, 63]. In particular in case of verbal episodic memory engagement it was shown that other regions can be involved besides the PCC and hippocampus, like the medial and inferior frontal gyrus and the insula, and as stated below, the thalamus [32, 6467].

Increasing evidence suggests that while the hippocampus is an important node in memory functioning, other nodes within the mnemonic system or Papez circuit or DMN can also lead to memory dysfunctioning [8]. As expected, the PCC is one of these nodes, but recent evidence shows that thalamus functioning is critical in memory functioning [8]. With its numerous connections as a relay system, the thalamus subserves connections between the hippocampus and PCC. Through this interdependent relationship with the PCC, early thalamus damage or dysfunction may also be explanatory for the earliest PCC imaging evidence of early AD [8]. In the current study one MCI patients showed a thalamus infarct in light of relatively mild medial temporal lobe atrophy (left MTA 1, right MTA 2) as evaluated on 2D T2 FLAIR. We acknowledge the limitations of T2 FLAIR to detect thalamic lesions [47], and the possibility of underestimating the number and the role of thalamic lesions in the current study. Therefore, future studies should include (1) T2-weighted images in order to examine thalamic lesions, and (2) evaluate anterior thalamic functioning when elucidating influence on PCC functioning during memory tasks in MCI.

In this study we cannot rule out a direct relationship between PCC function and neuropathological changes within this region. One study suggested that amyloid burden disrupts functional networks in healthy elderly [33]; however, PCC amyloid deposition in MCI was not found to be related to functional changes within the PCC region [68] nor to clinical status or AD disease progression [6973]. Furthermore, the PCC region is relatively unaffected by neurofibrillary tangle (NFT) formation in early AD disease stages [74, 75]. Hippocampal atrophy, on the other hand, is related to NFT formation [76], directly reflected in episodic memory impairment at the MCI stage. As one study claims that PCC volume loss and cingulum bundle deterioration secondary to MTL atrophy both influence PCC functioning [60], the role of PCC degeneration in the face of relative late NFT formation [74, 75] could be obtained as a sequential process of functional decline followed by structural decline later in the disease process.

Strengths of our study are the use of an event-related fMRI design that enabled us to investigate PCC and hippocampus functioning during episodic memory component processes such as successful encoding and correct recognition. A drawback of the current study is the inability to use the research criteria for MCI due to AD in the present study [1], as our participants were included in the years 2009–2011, before these criteria were established. All MCI patients though presented with amnestic MCI, which is known to have a high rate of conversion to clinical AD [7779]. We did not include AD biomarkers at the time, since PiB-PET was still in its infancy, and lumbar puncture was considered too invasive by our medical ethics committee for patients in a pre- or early clinical stage of dementia. Furthermore, although the number of MCI patients in this study was comparable to other studies using memory-related fMRI in MCI [64], our study may suffer from a lack of power. This could explain the fact that hippocampus and PCC activation or deactivation in both the encoding and recognition tasks was minimal. This is an important limitation of our study, in which a priori hypotheses considered the relationship between PCC and hippocampus activation.

In the present study, we found a relationship between activation in the PCC and hippocampus during successful episodic memory encoding and correct recognition in MCI. We found no evidence for the influence of often referred structural hippocampal predictors, on PCC functioning. Our results suggest that in MCI, PCC functioning is foremost influenced by hippocampal functioning during episodic memory, reflecting possibly a pattern in which functional changes precede or exceed structural changes.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

ESM 1 (134.8KB, docx)

(DOCX 134 kb)

Acknowledgements

The scientific guarantor of this publication is Dr. Janne Papma, Erasmus MC. The authors of this manuscript declare no relationships with any companies whose products or services may be related to the subject matter of the article. This work was supported by the Brain Foundation of The Netherlands (project number H07.03 to Niels D. Prins). Janne M. Papma received financial support from the Netherlands Alzheimer Foundation. No complex statistical methods were necessary for this paper. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects (patients) in this study. Institutional Review Board approval was obtained. Some of the study subjects have been previously reported in three previous studies:

- Papma JM et al. (2014) Cerebral small vessel disease affects white matter microstructure in mild cognitive impairment. Human Brain Mapping 35(6): 2836-2851

- IJsselstijn L et al. (2013) Serum proteomics in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Proteomics 13 (16)

- Papma JM et al. (2012) The influence of cerebral small vessel disease on default mode network deactivation in mild cognitive impairment. Neuroimage: Clinical 16(2): 33-42

Methodology: prospective, case-control study, performed at one institution.

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s00330-017-4768-1) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

  • 1.Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bruscoli M, Lovestone S. Is MCI really just early dementia? A systematic review of conversion studies. Int Psychogeriatr. 2004;16:129–140. doi: 10.1017/S1041610204000092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999;56:303–308. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004;256:183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, et al. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2001;58:1985–1992. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.12.1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Jack CR, Petersen RC, Xu YC, et al. Prediction of AD with MRI-based hippocampal volume in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 1999;52:1397–1397. doi: 10.1212/WNL.52.7.1397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Papez JW. A proposed mechanism of emotion. Arch Neurol Psychiatr. 1937;38:9. doi: 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1937.02260220069003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Aggleton JP, Pralus A, Nelson AJ, Hornberger M. Thalamic pathology and memory loss in early Alzheimer's disease: moving the focus from the medial temporal lobe to Papez circuit. Brain. 2016;139:1877–1890. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Greicius MD, Srivastava G, Reiss AL, Menon V. Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer's disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:4637–4642. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308627101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Seeley WW, Crawford RK, Zhou J, Miller BL, Greicius MD. Neurodegenerative diseases target large-scale human brain networks. Neuron. 2009;62:42–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98:676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Rombouts SA, Barkhof F, Goekoop R, Stam CJ, Scheltens P. Altered resting state networks in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;26:231–239. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Guedj E, Barbeau EJ, Didic M, et al. Effects of medial temporal lobe degeneration on brain perfusion in amnestic MCI of AD type: deafferentation and functional compensation? Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging. 2009;36:1101–1112. doi: 10.1007/s00259-009-1060-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Chetelat G, Desgranges B, De La Sayette V, Viader F, Eustache F, Baron JC. Mild cognitive impairment Can FDG-PET predict who is to rapidly convert to Alzheimer’s disease? Neurology. 2003;60:1374–1377. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000055847.17752.E6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ishii K, Mori T, Hirono N, Mori E. Glucose metabolic dysfunction in subjects with a clinical dementia rating of 0.5. J Neurol Sci. 2003;215:71–74. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(03)00206-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Nestor PJ, Fryer TD, Ikeda M, Hodges JR. Retrosplenial cortex (BA 29/30) hypometabolism in mild cognitive impairment (prodromal Alzheimer's disease) Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18:2663–2667. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02999.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Frings L, Dressel K, Abel S, et al. Reduced precuneus deactivation during object naming in patients with mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease, and frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010;30:334–343. doi: 10.1159/000320991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Jagust W, Gitcho A, Sun F, Kuczynski B, Mungas D, Haan M. Brain imaging evidence of preclinical Alzheimer's disease in normal aging. Ann Neurol. 2006;59:673–681. doi: 10.1002/ana.20799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Johnson SC, Schmitz TW, Moritz CH, et al. Activation of brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer's disease: the effect of mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27:1604–1612. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Huang C, Wahlund L-O, Svensson L, Winblad B, Julin P. Cingulate cortex hypoperfusion predicts Alzheimer's disease in mild cognitive impairment. BMC Neurol. 2002;2:9. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-2-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Petrella JR, Wang L, Krishnan S, et al. Cortical deactivation in mild cognitive impairment: high-field-strength functional MR imaging 1. Radiology. 2007;245:224–235. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2451061847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Aggleton JP, Brown MW. Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal-anterior thalamic axis. Behav Brain Sci. 1999;22:425–444. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Chételat G, Desgranges B, de la Sayette V, et al. Dissociating atrophy and hypometabolism impact on episodic memory in mild cognitive impairment. Brain. 2003;126:1955–1967. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kim J, Kim Y-H, Lee J-H. Hippocampus–precuneus functional connectivity as an early sign of Alzheimer's disease: a preliminary study using structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Brain Res. 2013;1495:18–29. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Jobst KA, Smith AD, Barker CS, et al. Association of atrophy of the medial temporal lobe with reduced blood flow in the posterior parietotemporal cortex in patients with a clinical and pathological diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55:190–194. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.3.190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Villain N, Fouquet M, Baron J-C, et al. Sequential relationships between grey matter and white matter atrophy and brain metabolic abnormalities in early Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2010;133:3301–3314. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Gili T, Cercignani M, Serra L, et al. Regional brain atrophy and functional disconnection across Alzheimer's disease evolution. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2011;82:58–66. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2009.199935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Yakushev I, Schreckenberger M, Müller MJ, et al. Functional implications of hippocampal degeneration in early Alzheimer’s disease: a combined DTI and PET study. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging. 2011;38:2219–2227. doi: 10.1007/s00259-011-1882-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bai F, Zhang Z, Watson DR, Yu H, Shi Y, Yuan Y. Abnormal white matter independent of hippocampal atrophy in amnestic type mild cognitive impairment. Neurosci Lett. 2009;462:147–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Zhuang L, Sachdev PS, Trollor JN, et al. Microstructural white matter changes, not hippocampal atrophy, detect early amnestic mild cognitive impairment. PLoS One. 2013;8:e58887. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Huijbers W, Pennartz CM, Cabeza R, Daselaar SM. The hippocampus is coupled with the default network during memory retrieval but not during memory encoding. PLoS One. 2011;6:e17463. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Clement F, Belleville S, Mellah S. Functional neuroanatomy of the encoding and retrieval processes of verbal episodic memory in MCI. Cortex. 2010;46:1005–1015. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Hedden T, Van Dijk KRA, Becker JA, et al. Disruption of functional connectivity in clinically normal older adults harboring amyloid burden. J Neurosci. 2009;29:12686–12694. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3189-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Huang H, Fan X, Weiner M, et al. Distinctive disruption patterns of white matter tracts in Alzheimer's disease with full diffusion tensor characterization. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33:2029–2045. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Pengas G, Hodges JR, Watson P, Nestor PJ. Focal posterior cingulate atrophy in incipient Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2010;31:25–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rémy F, Vayssière N, Saint-Aubert L, Barbeau E, Pariente J. White matter disruption at the prodromal stage of Alzheimer's disease: relationships with hippocampal atrophy and episodic memory performance. NeuroImage: Clin. 2015;7:482–492. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Tijms BM, Wink AM, de Haan W, et al. Alzheimer's disease: connecting findings from graph theoretical studies of brain networks. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34:2023–2036. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bozzali M, Giulietti G, Basile B, et al. Damage to the cingulum contributes to Alzheimer's disease pathophysiology by deafferentation mechanism. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33:1295–1308. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Delano-Wood L, Stricker NH, Sorg SF, et al. Posterior cingulum white matter disruption and its associations with verbal memory and stroke risk in mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;29:589. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-102103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Chang Y-L, Chen T-F, Shih Y-C, Chiu M-J, Yan S-H, Tseng WY. Regional cingulum disruption, not gray matter atrophy, detects cognitive changes in amnestic mild cognitive impairment subtypes. J Alzheimers Dis JAD. 2015;44:125–138. doi: 10.3233/JAD-141839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Verhage F. Intelligentie en leeftijd: onderzoek bij Nederlanders van twaalf tot zevenenzeventig jaar [intelligence and age: research on Dutch people aged twelve to seventy-seven years old] Assen: Van Gorcum; 1964. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Papma JM, de Groot M, de Koning I, et al. Cerebral small vessel disease affects white matter microstructure in mild cognitive impairment. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:2836–2851. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.de Boer R, Vrooman HA, van der Lijn F, et al. White matter lesion extension to automatic brain tissue segmentation on MRI. Neuroimage. 2009;45:1151–1161. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Vrooman HA, Cocosco CA, van der Lijn F, et al. Multi-spectral brain tissue segmentation using automatically trained k-Nearest-Neighbor classification. Neuroimage. 2007;37:71–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.den Heijer T, van der Lijn F, Koudstaal PJ, et al. A 10-year follow-up of hippocampal volume on magnetic resonance imaging in early dementia and cognitive decline. Brain. 2010;133:1163–1172. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.van der Lijn F, den Heijer T, Breteler MMB, Niessen WJ. Hippocampus segmentation in MR images using atlas registration, voxel classification, and graph cuts. Neuroimage. 2008;43:708–720. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Bastos Leite AJ, van Straaten EC, Scheltens P, Lycklama G, Barkhof F. Thalamic lesions in vascular dementia: low sensitivity of fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) imaging. Stroke. 2004;35:415–419. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000109226.67085.5A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Wakana S, Caprihan A, Panzenboeck MM, et al. Reproducibility of quantitative tractography methods applied to cerebral white matter. Neuroimage. 2007;36:630–644. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Wakana S, Jiang H, Nagae-Poetscher LM, Van Zijl PCM, Mori S. Fiber tract–based atlas of human white matter anatomy 1. Radiology. 2004;230:77–87. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2301021640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.de Groot M, Vernooij MW, Klein S, et al. Improving alignment in Tract-based spatial statistics: evaluation and optimization of image registration. Neuroimage. 2013;76:400–411. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Daselaar S, Veltman DJ, Rombouts S, Raaijmakers JGW, Jonker C. Neuroanatomical correlates of episodic encoding and retrieval in young and elderly subjects. Brain. 2003;126:43–56. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Snodgrass JG, Corwin J. Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1988;117:34. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.117.1.34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Price CJ, Friston KJ. Scanning patients with tasks they can perform. Hum Brain Mapp. 1999;8:102–108. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:2/3&#x0003c;102::AID-HBM6&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-J. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Mundfrom DJ, Perrett JJ, Schaffer J, Piccone A, Roozeboom M. Bonferroni adjustments in tests for regression coefficients. Multiple Linear Regression Viewpoints. 2006;32:1–6. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Celone KA, Calhoun VD, Dickerson BC, et al. Alterations in memory networks in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: an independent component analysis. J Neurosci. 2006;26:10222–10231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2250-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Jin G, Li K, Hu Y, et al. Amnestic mild cognitive impairment: functional MR imaging study of response in posterior cingulate cortex and adjacent precuneus during problem-solving tasks. Radiology. 2011;261:525–533. doi: 10.1148/radiol.11102186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Dunn CJ, Duffy SL, Hickie IB, et al. Deficits in episodic memory retrieval reveal impaired default mode network connectivity in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. NeuroImage: Clin. 2014;4:473–480. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Miettinen PS, Pihlajamäki M, Jauhiainen AM, et al. Structure and function of medial temporal and posteromedial cortices in early Alzheimer’s disease. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;34:320–330. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07745.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Villain N, Desgranges B, Viader F, et al. Relationships between hippocampal atrophy, white matter disruption, and gray matter hypometabolism in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci. 2008;28:6174–6181. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1392-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Choo ILH, Lee DY, Oh JS, et al. Posterior cingulate cortex atrophy and regional cingulum disruption in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2010;31:772–779. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Zhang Y, Schuff N, Jahng GH, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging of cingulum fibers in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2007;68:13–19. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000250326.77323.01. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Jones-Gotman M, Zatorre RJ, Olivier A, et al. Learning and retention of words and designs following excision from medial or lateral temporal-lobe structures. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35:963–973. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00024-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Kennepohl S, Sziklas V, Garver KE, Wagner DD, Jones-Gotman M. Memory and the medial temporal lobe: hemispheric specialization reconsidered. Neuroimage. 2007;36:969–978. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Terry DP, Sabatinelli D, Puente AN, Lazar NA, Miller LS. A meta‐analysis of fMRI activation differences during episodic memory in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. J Neuroimaging. 2015;25:849–860. doi: 10.1111/jon.12266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Tomadesso C, Perrotin A, Mutlu J et al (2015) Brain structural, functional, and cognitive correlates of recent versus remote autobiographical memories in amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. NeuroImage: Clinical [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 66.Dannhauser TM, Shergill SS, Stevens T, et al. An fMRI study of verbal episodic memory encoding in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Cortex. 2008;44:869–880. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Trivedi MA, Murphy CM, Goetz C, et al. fMRI activation changes during successful episodic memory encoding and recognition in amnestic mild cognitive impairment relative to cognitively healthy older adults. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;26:123–137. doi: 10.1159/000148190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Huijbers W, Mormino EC, Schultz AP et al (2015) Amyloid-β deposition in mild cognitive impairment is associated with increased hippocampal activity, atrophy and clinical progression. Brain:awv007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 69.De Souza LC, Corlier F, Habert M-O, et al. Similar amyloid-β burden in posterior cortical atrophy and Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2011;134:2036–2043. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Chang YT, Huang CW, Chang YH, et al. Amyloid burden in the hippocampus and default mode network: relationships with gray matter volume and cognitive performance in mild stage Alzheimer disease. Medicine (Baltimore) 2015;94:e763. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000000763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Arriagada PV, Growdon JH, Hedley-Whyte ET, Hyman BT. Neurofibrillary tangles but not senile plaques parallel duration and severity of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1992;42:631–639. doi: 10.1212/WNL.42.3.631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Ingelsson M, Fukumoto H, Newell KL, et al. Early Aβ accumulation and progressive synaptic loss, gliosis, and tangle formation in AD brain. Neurology. 2004;62:925–931. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000115115.98960.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Markesbery WR, Schmitt FA, Kryscio RJ, Davis DG, Smith CD, Wekstein DR. Neuropathologic substrate of mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:38–46. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.1.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 1991;82:239–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Delacourte A, David JP, Sergeant N, et al. The biochemical pathway of neurofibrillary degeneration in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1999;52:1158–1158. doi: 10.1212/WNL.52.6.1158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.DeCarli C, Frisoni GB, Clark CM, et al. Qualitative estimates of medial temporal atrophy as a predictor of progression from mild cognitive impairment to dementia. Arch Neurol. 2007;64:108–115. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.1.108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Schmidtke K, Hermeneit S. High rate of conversion to Alzheimer's disease in a cohort of amnestic MCI patients. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008;20:96–108. doi: 10.1017/S1041610207005509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Sarazin M, Berr C, De Rotrou J, et al. Amnestic syndrome of the medial temporal type identifies prodromal AD: a longitudinal study. Neurology. 2007;69:1859–1867. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000279336.36610.f7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Fischer P, Jungwirth S, Zehetmayer S, et al. Conversion from subtypes of mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer dementia. Neurology. 2007;68:288–291. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000252358.03285.9d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

ESM 1 (134.8KB, docx)

(DOCX 134 kb)


Articles from European Radiology are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES